References Cited 1 References Cited Andruski, J. E., Blumstein, S. E., & Burton, M. (1994). The effects of subphonetic differences on lexical access. Cognition, 52, 163-187 Bishop, J., & Keating, P. (2010). Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s own range: Fundamental frequency, voice quality and speaker sex. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 108, 113-140. Church, B. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1994). Perceptual specificity of auditory priming: Implicit memory for voice intonation and fundamental frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 521-533. Creel, S. C., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Heeding the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access. Cognition, 106, 633-664. Creelman, C. D. (1957). Case of the unknown talker. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 655. Cutler, A. (1997). The comparative perspective on spoken-language processing. Speech Communication, 21, 3-15. Dolson, M. (1994). The pitch of speech as a function of linguistic community. Music Perception, 11, 321-331. Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680-698. Goldinger, S. D. (1996). Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1166-1183. Grosjean, F., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (1996). A guide to spoken word recognition paradigms: Introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 553-558. Honorof, D. N., & Whalen, D. H. (2005). Perception of pitch location within a speaker’s F0 range. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117, 2193-2200. Honorof, D. N., & Whalen, D. H. (2010). Identification of speaker sex from one vowel across a range of fundamental frequencies,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 30953104. Jackson, A., & Morton, J. (1984). Facilitation of auditory recognition. Memory & Cognition, 12, 568-574. Johnson, K. A. (2005). Speaker normalization in speech perception. In D. B. Pisoni & R. E. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 363-389). Malden: Blackwell. Kwan, T. W. (2001). Chinese Character Frequency Statistics for Hong Kong, Mainland China and Taiwan: A Trans-Regional, Diachronic Survey. Hong Kong: Chinese University of Hong Kong. Retrieved from http://humanum.arts.cuhk.edu.hk/Lexis/chifreq/ Ladefoged, P., & Broadbent, D. E. (1957). Information conveyed by vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 98-104. Laver, J. (1994). Principles of Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Leather, J. (1983). Speaker normalization in perception of lexical tone. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 373-382. Lee, C.-Y. (2007). Does horse activate mother? Processing lexical tone in form priming. Language and Speech, 50, 101-124. Lee, C.-Y. (2009). Identifying isolated, multispeaker Mandarin tones from brief acoustic input: A perceptual and acoustic study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 1125- References Cited 2 1137. Lee, C.-Y., Dutton, L., & Ram, G. (2010). The role of speaker gender identification in F0 height estimation from multispeaker, brief speech segments. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 384-388. Lee, C.-Y., & Lee, Y.-F. (2010). Perception of musical pitch and lexical tones by Mandarinspeaking musicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 526-535. Lee, C.-Y., Lee, Y.-F., & Shr, C-L. (2011). Perception of musical and lexical tones by Taiwanese-speaking musicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127, 481-490. Lee, C.-Y. & Zhang, Y. (submitted). Processing speaker variability in repetition and semantic/associative priming. Manuscript submitted to Language and Speech. Luce, P. A., & Lyons, E. A. (1998). Specificity of memory representations for spoken words. Memory & Cognition, 26, 708-715. Mattys, S. L., & Liss, J. M. (2008). On building models of spoken-word recognition: When there is as much to learn from natural “oddities” as artificial normality. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1235-1242. McLennan, C. T., & Luce, P. A. (2005). Examining the time course of indexical specificity effects in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 306-321. Mertus, J. A. (2000) The Brown Lab Interactive Speech System [computer software]. Providence: Brown University. Retrieved from http://www.mertus.org/Bliss/index.html Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1976). Meaning, memory structure, and mental processes. Science, 192, 27-33. Moore, C. B., & Jongman, A. (1997). Speaker normalization in the perception of Mandarin Chinese tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1864-1877. Pisoni, D. B. (1997). Some thoughts on "normalization" in speech perception. In K. Johnson & J. W. Mullennix (Eds.), Talker Variability in Speech Processing (pp. 9-32). San Diego: Academic Press. Schacter, D. L., & Church, B. A. (1992). Auditory priming: Implicit and explicit memory for words and voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 915-930. Vitevitch, M. S., & Donoso, A. (2011). Processing indexical information requires time: Evidence from change deafness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1484-1493. Wong, P. C. M., & Diehl, R. L. (2003). Perceptual normalization for inter- and intratalker variation in Cantonese level tones. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 413-421. Zhou, N., Zhang, W., Lee, C.-Y., & Xu, L. (2008). Lexical tone recognition with an artificial neural network. Ear and Hearing, 29, 326-335. Zwitserlood, P. (1996). Form priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 589-596.