Spelling Instruction and its Effect on Word Recognition and Reading Ability Shannon Mullins Abstract The purpose of this paper is to test and understand the effects of appropriate spelling instruction on word recognition and reading ability of first grade students. In my opinion, spelling instruction has taken a “back seat” within the classroom. With tight curriculum pacing expectations, assessment inundations, and scheduling constraints, teachers have been forced to take “short cuts” with the delivery of what seems like a “less important” curriculum to students. I believe that spelling instruction has been an unfortunate recipient of this rule of thought and is a topic of study that is often overlooked for its value and significance. This paper will show that appropriate developmental spelling instruction is very beneficial and extremely necessary to students of all ability levels and can have a significant impact on their word recognition and reading ability. This writing will reflect on the positive impact spelling /word study instruction has on the development of word recognition skills and reading ability of all students. To benchmark a child’s understanding of orthographic knowledge, students in a single first grade classroom will be pre-tested using word recognition and spelling inventory assessments. After pre-testing each child, the students will be grouped according to their spelling ability. Each group of students will receive specific spelling instruction through the use of word study to target the errors made within their own spelling. Word study is a way for students to practice and learn the appropriate use of specific spelling patterns they have used but confused within their own spelling through sorting word patterns found within a given group of words. After a period of time, the students will be given the same word recognition and spelling inventories as a posttest to see if the targeted word study instruction made a difference within their own spelling of given words and in recognizing words in and out of context. My hope is to find that through accurate word study instruction, the students under my evaluation will develop a solid sense of the way letters work in words and will be able to transfer that knowledge to their own spelling. I anticipate finding that through this direct route of instruction, the students will be able to recognize words in and out of context more successfully. Area of Focus The purpose of this study is to describe the effects of targeted word study instruction on a child’s ability to spell words correctly, to recognize words in isolation quickly, and to observe the impact orthography has on the reading ability of early elementary students. Research Question Do first grade elementary students receiving word study instruction acquire better word recognition skills and develop stronger reading abilities than other first grade students? Review of Literature Treiman and Cassar (1996) argued that young children are capable of correctly spelling words using morphemes. Larkin and Snowling (2008) extend the work of Treiman and Cassar (1996) and Bourassa, Treiman, and Kessler (2006) in their study to support findings that children in early elementary school can use their understanding of the morphological structure of words to increase their accuracy among spelling attempts. In this study, Larkin and Snowling (2008) state that phonological awareness alone is not enough to ensure spelling acquisition within the English language. The subjects in this study were tested on word recognition using one and two-syllable spelling words. The study concluded that early grade school subjects showed no evidence of using morphology to assist them in the spelling of one and two syllable words. One possible explanation for why the results did not support the other research findings is the two-morpheme words used in the spelling test were all regular past tense verbs. This may have enabled the students to adopt a strategy of consistently adding the “ed” ending in their spelling attempts inappropriately, Larkin and Snowling (2008). Another reason for the inconsistent findings could be due to the developmental readiness of the subjects in the study. Children at this early stage of spelling development rely heavily on phonetic spelling as opposed to orthographic spelling strategies, which are found in a later developmental spelling phase. In summary, there is support for a developmental progression of spelling among early elementary students who most often use a phonetic form of spelling and lead up to an understanding of the orthographic rules found within the spelling of words. Bryant and Bindman (2006) state that in order for children to read and write successfully they must grasp the morphemic structure of the words they are learning to use. The spelling of words in the English language, as well as in many other alphabetic languages, require children to have a firm understanding of the morphemic structure of the words used within that language. This is necessary because the pattern of spelling and the sound of a word can change pending the language rule and the context in which the word is used. Students need to make sense of the spellings that are not specified in the word’s phonological representation, Bryant and Bindman (2006). Therefore, children must have a firm understanding of the morphemic and phonological structure of the alphabetic language they are learning. Bryant and Bindman (2006) suggest that the understanding of an alphabetic language in terms of literacy development is bi-directional. For example, when spelling and reading words, one must consider how the word sounds and what they know morphologically and phonologically about the spelling of the words in and out of context before comprehending the word. Bryant and Bindman (2006) conducted two longitudinal studies that support a student’s understanding of the morphologic structure of an alphabetic language. The subjects in these studies had not been exposed to the morphological or phonological representation of words within the structure of a classroom setting. However, their exposure to literacy through environmental means did impact their morphological and phonological awareness. The first study measured a subject’s ability to represent morphology in spelling the appropriate use of the “ed” ending in past regular verbs. This task was used as a predictor of morphological awareness and was assessed through an oral analogy task. Bryant and Bindman (2006) found that a subject’s awareness of morphology was a strong predictor of their ability to spell words that cannot be spelled simply by phonological representation. In the second study, three measures of morphological spelling were used to predict the subject’s morphological awareness using a pseudo-word interpretation task in and out of context. Subjects were asked to manipulate the pseudo-words through their morphologic understanding to interpret their meaning. The results showed a high correlation and supported the validity of morphological awareness. In their longitudinal studies, Bryant and Bindman (2006) found a connection between morphological awareness and its effect on a student’s spelling and reading of words. They found that in order for children to make sense of how a word is spelled and read, a strong morphological awareness was necessary. It allowed a subject to use morphemes as a new tool for thinking about how to spell and read words found within the language they are learning. Cassar and Treiman (1997) found evidence that some orthographic knowledge is displayed earlier in the spelling development of young children. From a child’s initial understanding of print they use many strategies for spelling. Children pull from their limited knowledge of language in an attempt to spell words. In other words, children use what they know from their experience with language and where they are developmentally in an attempt to spell words. Caessar and Treiman (1997) conducted several experiments investigating children’s knowledge of double letters using three orthographic practices with the position of double consonants. They used non-words and position to test a child’s understanding of the conventions of doubling. The results of their study showed a developmental trend in acquiring these orthographic forms of spelling. They saw that a knowledge of position was present as early as kindergarten, consonant doubling was present in first grade, and at sixth grade and above children showed a clear understanding of short vowels and medial doubling. In summary, these results support stage development in spelling acquisition as well as suggesting that some orthographic knowledge emerges earlier in the course of spelling development. Ehri (1979) studied the beginning reader’s ability to use orthography as a representational system for storing speech sounds in memory. She wanted to assess how important this ability might be as children learn to read. Ehri (1979) developed what is called the Word Amalgamation Theory. This theory is described when various identities are combined to form a single unit in lexical memory. The identities found within the lexicon are abstract word units made up of the following: phonological identity - how words sound and are articulated, syntactic identity - grammatical roles of words within sentences, and semantic identity - the meaning of the word. Amalgamation is the process by which these identities are combined to form a single unit in lexical memory. This is important because the more knowledge a reader has about how print maps speech, the easier the reading process will become for them. The larger task for the early reader is to learn how to incorporate print into their existing knowledge of letter sounds successfully. Ehri used beginning readers to examine the process of how orthographic forms were established as images symbolizing sounds within memory. She taught young readers visual cues to four sets of words, some with just squiggles, some with the beginning letter, some with misspellings, and some with CVC word patterns. According to Ehri’s findings, when all three identities of a word are amalgamated in the lexicon, word recognition moves from taking print to sound and accessing the word’s meaning using phonology to recognize the word as a single unit. With the word bonded by their identities in memory through amalgamation, the pronunciation and meaning of the word are recognized simultaneously instead of separately. The sound of the word no longer stands in the way of word recognition because the amalgamation of the identities is the “glue” for word recognition. Printed word learning is therefore complete due to a word’s orthographic image being formed in lexical memory and the image being amalgamated with its phonological, syntactic, and semantic identities. In conclusion, sounds accompanied by spelling support were learned much faster than sounds prompted by initial letters without spellings, misspellings, and squiggles. Perfetti states that reading comprehension depends upon the availability of highly developed skills in early literacy development, including the ability to convert print into the language code, Perfetti (1975). Through his research, Perfetti developed what is known as the Verbal Efficiency Theory which places great emphasis on lower level processing skills, mainly decoding. Perfetti tested and proved that the automaticity of lower level decoding allows the reader’s attention to be placed on higher order comprehension processing instead of decoding words to understand text. The automaticity of decoding words should be an effortless task for any reader, freeing up their ability to focus on text comprehension instead of word recognition. The reading process is an interdependent process in which the processors in the brain act simultaneously or interdependently of each other. When one process is slower at work, it inhibits the other processors to do their job effectively. If one aspect of the reading process is requiring too much attention from the reader, the other processes suffer, causing a lapse in the understanding of what is being read. A reader requires considerable processing capacity to decode a single word. If he struggles with this process, his comprehension of text will suffer. Perfetti studied the relationship of decoding skills using high frequency words, low frequency words and nonsense words. He used a word identification task to measure the subject’s ability to decode words automatically. He used good readers and readers struggling with comprehension to compare the rate at which they decode the words in a flashed setting. His results proved that both struggling and good readers decoded high frequency words at about the same speed. The big difference was the ability subjects had in decoding low-frequency (rare) and made-up words. The skilled readers were always significantly higher in decoding lowfrequency and nonsense words. This proved Perfetti’s theory, the more automatic decoding and spelling patterns are for students; the more they are able to focus on text comprehension. In summary, a person’s reading comprehension skill affects how well they read words because of their decoding skill ability. Highly skilled readers will take less time to read known sight words and non-words while poor readers will need more time. Poor readers may know as many known sight words, but will struggle with the non-word list due to their lack of decoding ability. Description of Intervention or Innovation This is a descriptive study and there is no intervention or innovation. Data Collection Methods Quantitative data collection will be used in gathering research data for this study. Pre-tests in the form of word recognition in isolation, Schlagal spelling inventory, and word recognition in context assessments will be used to benchmark each subject’s word recognition, spelling, and contextual reading abilities. Students will be assessed individually with word recognition in isolation and word recognition in context. The students will be assessed as a whole group for the Schlagal spelling inventory. After pre-test data has been collected and evaluated, subjects will be grouped according to their developmental spelling ability. A word study treatment will be used to teach spelling patterns that are being used but confused by the subjects within word recognition in isolation and in spelling. The treatment will continue for an eighteen week period. After the treatment period is completed, the subjects will be assessed using the word recognition in isolation, contextual reading, and spelling assessments to see if any orthographic gains were made in these three areas. Data analysis and Interpretation This is a descriptive study. There is no data to analyze or interpret at this point. Action Plan From the information I have gathered from reading research articles, I plan to implement the strategies mentioned in the data collection portion of this action research paper. I feel confident, based on the research I have read, that implementing appropriate word study instruction to my students will prove to be fruitful and will begin to close the literacy gap among those children I teach. References Larkin, R. F., & Snowlin, M. J. (2008). Morphologic spelling development. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 24(4), 363 – 376. Bryant, N. P., & Bindman, M. (2006). The effects of learning to spell on children’s awareness of morphology. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19,767-787. Cassar, M., & Treiman, R. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 631644. Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1979). The mnemonic value of orthography among beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(1), 26-40. Perfetti, C., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(4), 461-469.