Norms and Identities among Representatives and Constituents Many important aspects of our lives are managed by representatives. While representatives are often involved in important decisions, negotiations, and conflicts, organizational and behavioral research has not yet characterized the unique features and implications of representatives. The current symposium aims to fill this gap, providing an overview of how norms and identity are reflected in and influenced by representatives and constituents in negotiations, marketing, and politics. In the first presentation, Aaldering and Steinel discuss the use of unethical tactics in representative negotiations. They show that the norms communicated by the constituency are used by representatives as justification for the use of unethical behavior through the process of moral disengagement. The negative implications of non-normative behavior by representatives, is further discussed in the second presentation, by Cheshin, Amit, and Van Kleef. They show that displays of non-normative emotional intensity by customer representatives reduce customers trust in the representative as well as customers satisfaction with the service and product. Further focusing on the detrimental aspects of incongruency, in the third presentation Moran and Ritov highlight an identity-driven gap in representative-negotiations. They suggest that constituents and representatives may differ in the emotionality assigned to issues in the negotiations. The incongruence between the value and the emotionality of the issues leads to less efficient outcomes. In the forth presentation, Amit and Arieli focus on social identity in the political arena. They present a model of nested social attributions and suggest that constituents monitor and interpret their representatives' behavior using social attributions. They further show that social identity influences these attributions. The four presentations provide a wide overview of contexts in which representatives play an important role. The presentations vary in context, providing insights to important processes in negotiations, marketing, and politics. The first two presentations highlight the importance of constituents' norms on the behavior of the representative and on the perceptions of the constituency. The last two presentations point to the importance of identity gaps inherent to the representative-constituency context. Looking at the impact on both representatives (1st & 3rd presentations) and constituents (2nd & 4th presentations), we stress to the importance of understanding the special characteristics of contexts involving representatives. The symposium will end with an integrative discussion across the four presentations and beyond led by Eran Halperin. Why Representatives Use Unethical Tactics in Negotiations Hillie Aaldering, University of Amsterdam Wolfhang Steinel, Leiden University Representatives' negotiation behavior is strongly influenced by constituency pressures (De Dreu et al., 2013; Druckman, 1977). Depending on the norm communicated by their constituency, representatives can take a cooperative or competitive course of action during the negotiations (Steinel et al., 2010). Negotiators are often willing to bend the truth when they believe this can give them an advantage in the negotiation. The present studies investigate to what extent and why representatives a) engage in unethical behavior and b) use constituency pressures as an explanation for their behavior. The first study showed that representatives were more willing to use unethical tactics when a majority (versus minority) of their constituency endorse unethical values and that the prevalence of such tactics was very high (80%). The second study corroborates these findings in a lab experiment, where representatives were more likely to send unethical messages to a negotiation opponent when the majority of their constituency endorsed unethical (vs. ethical) values. Furthermore, representatives were more motivated to serve their unethical (rather than ethical) constituency when they themselves had a low (rather than high) personal moral identity. A recurring finding in negotiation literature is that representatives set higher demands, make fewer concessions and in general follow a competitive course throughout the negotiation compared to negotiators to act on their own behalf (De Dreu, Aaldering, & Saygi, 2014). One main explanation for this effect is that representatives aim to serve the wishes of their constituency, and believe that the best way to do is to set high demands. In line with this explanation, we investigated in experiment 3 and 4 to what extent representatives use the wishes of their constituency as justification for their unethical behavior. One form of justifying unethical behavior is by moral disengagement, the deactivation of moral selfregulatory processes (Detert et al., 2008). We expected that a constituency with unethical values could serve as reason for representatives to morally disengage and in turn increase unethical behavior. Results of the third study firstly replicate the finding that representatives are most motivated to serve their constituency when the constituencies' values are congruent with their personal moral identity. Secondly, representatives reported higher moral disengagement when a majority versus a minority of their constituency endorsed unethical values, which increased their unethical behavior (i.e., lying about important negotiation information). The same effect was found for post hoc justification, where representatives were explicitly asked to what extent their unethical actions were influenced by their constituencies’ wishes. The fourth study, a laboratory experiment, replicated the mediating role of moral disengagement and explicit justification. This study furthermore showed that unethical behavior already increases when only half of the constituency endorses unethical values. In sum, it seems that representatives use their constituency as justification for their unethical behavior, both implicit (moral disengagement) and explicit (study 2, 3 and 4) and that unethical values endorsed by the constituency ‘free’ a representative to undertake unethical acts (study 3 and 4). Importantly, unethical behavior seems to be the norm, rather than the exception (all studies) and at least a majority of constituency members endorsing ethical values are needed to decrease representatives’ use of unethical behavior (study 1 and 4). Finally, whether or not representatives are motivated to serve their constituency is partially determined by the extent to which their values regarding (un)ethical behavior (their moral identity) are congruent with those of their constituency. However, these personal values are not strong enough to overcome the contextual pressures of the constituencies’ values on actual behavior. The Interpersonal Effects of Emotion Intensity in Customer Service: How Service Representatives' Expressions of Happiness and Sadness Shape Customer Trust and Satisfaction Arik Cheshin, The University of Haifa Adi Amit, The Open University of Israel Gerben Van Kleef, The University of Amsterdam Emotional expressions can have a pervasive impact on organizational behavior. However, surprisingly little is known about how the intensity of emotional displays modulates their effects. Using the service setting as a context, we investigate whether and how varying intensities of service representatives' emotional displays influence customer service outcomes. We chose to investigate the interpersonal effects of emotional display intensity in the context of customer service for several reasons. First, although emotions are known to play an important role in the service setting, scientific understanding of the social effects of emotions in customer service is limited by the neglect of the role of emotional intensity. This is problematic for a number of reasons, relating to both theory and practice. With respect to the former, current theorizing explicitly or implicitly assumes that particular emotional expressions have similar interpersonal effects regardless of their intensity level. As we will argue in the manuscript, this assumption is debatable. There are good reasons to suspect that the effects of emotional expressions are shaped by their intensity, and as long as this possibility is not addressed, further theory development is impeded. From a practical point of view, service workers are commonly instructed to express emotions in the workplace, but it is unclear how the intensity of such expressions influences critical outcomes such as trust and customer satisfaction. Second, service interactions require no prior relationship, allowing us to tease apart the effect of emotional displays from possible confounds such as prior knowledge or expected future relations. Third, this interaction involves a representative who displays emotions on behalf of a company, an area that has only been limitedly investigated. Fourth, the customer service setting allowed us to examine the effects of the intensity of a positive (happiness) as well as a negative (sadness) emotion in a credible way. This study thus contributes to the literature on customer service and to the literature on emotions alike. In Study 1 (laboratory experiment, N = 98), displays of high-intensity (compared to low-intensity) sadness and happiness led participants to evaluate a service provider's emotion as less authentic and appropriate, and to deem the service provider as less trustworthy. Study 2 (laboratory experiment, N = 150) replicated the interpersonal effect of emotion intensity on trust, and additionally showed that higher emotion intensity displays reduced customer evaluations of service and product quality. The effect of emotion intensity on evaluation of the service and product was mediated by perceptions of trust. Study 3 (field study, N = 162) replicated and extended the findings by demonstrating compatible effects on actual product use. These studies highlight the key role of the intensity of emotional expressions in shaping customer service outcomes. This paper presents the first systematic investigation of the ways in which positive and negative emotional expressions of various levels of intensity influence customer service outcomes. Even though it has long been acknowledged that individuals can reliably identify variations in intensity within discrete emotions, the potential differential effects of intense versus mild positive and negative emotions on customer service outcomes had not yet been examined. Moreover, customer service representatives are the ones that embody the product and/or service provided, thus representing the company and its interests. And as demonstrated in our study representatives emotional displays have critical impact on the product and service they represent. We argue that our findings could relate to many other settings where representatives are utilized. Our research constitutes a first step toward increasing understanding of the role of intensity in shaping the effects of emotional expressions. Our findings indicate that intensity matters and should be incorporated in theorizing and research on the role of emotions in social interactions. Issue specific emotionality: Beyond effects of value Simone Moran, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev Ilana Ritov, Hebrew University Negotiators, often deal with multiple issues varying in value and in emotional activation. While issue priority and emotional intensity are often positively related, there are situations in which this may not be the case. An example of an instance where value and emotionality are likely to vary independently is when negotiating on behalf of others. In such cases, the deliberate value of issues is (and should be) dictated by the constituents’ preferences, yet the extent to which the negotiating representative or agent feels strongly emotional about these issues can be self-determined autonomously, and thus may diverge from their constituents’ priorities. In other words, the agent may feel stronger emotions with regard to issues of lower priority from their constituents’ perspective. In this work, we examine how varying the issues’ emotionality, independent of their deliberate value, affects the negotiation process and outcome. Our key argument is that when negotiations involve multiple issues with different priorities, the degree of emotional intensity associated with the different issues significantly impacts the efficiency of negotiation tradeoffs and outcomes. Specifically, because efficient outcomes require both parties to concede more on their lower (vs. higher) value issues, and since negotiators are likely to be more reluctant to concede when issues are emotionally charged (Luce et al., 1999), being emotional about a lower valued issue may hinder the negotiation process. Consequently, we expect agreements to be less efficient when value and emotionality are incongruent (the lower value issue is the more emotionally charged one) compared to congruent (the higher value issue is also the more emotionally charged one). In study 1, 108 students participate in a role-play face to face two-issue negotiation wherein they are representatives of constituents who purportedly dictate the value of the issues to be negotiated. Specifically, we present the negotiator with "designated value points”, which are purportedly based on a survey that was conducted to reveal constituents’ preferences, We manipulate designated issue priorities by varying the span of the dictated points for each issue so that parties’ have reversed issue priorities. We then independently manipulate the emotionality of each issue to be either emotionally laden or neutral, in a way that creates two between subject conditions: A congruent condition (24 dyads), wherein the emotionality manipulation is compatible with relative issue values (i.e., the lower value issue is emotionally neutral, and the higher value issue is emotionally laden), vs. an incongruent condition (25 dyads), wherein the emotionality manipulation is incompatible with relative issue values (i.e., the lower value issue is emotionally laden, the higher value issue is emotionally neutral). Results demonstrate that being highly emotional about a particular issue affects the negotiation process and outcome, independent of the issue’s designated value. As predicted, negotiators are more reluctant to concede on their highly emotional issues. Consequently, they are less likely to reach efficient agreements when issues’ emotionality and designated values are incompatible compared to compatible. In study 2 (N=95), we adopt the incongruent condition used in Study 1, and rule out an alternative account for the above described results - namely, that our issue emotionality manipulation eliminated or crowded out the designated issue values. Contrary to this account, we find that participants simultaneously evaluating two multi-issue offers: a Value offer (advantageous to the participant on the higher value neutrally emotional issue, disadvantageous on the lower value highly emotional one), and an Emotionality offer (advantageous to the participant on the lower value highly emotional issue, disadvantageous on other), rate the former as more attractive than the latter. This suggests that the observed effects of emotionally charged issues cannot simply be attributed to an increase in their subjective value. To summarize, we demonstrate that the emotional intensity negotiating agents feel toward the negotiation issues, independent of the issues’ relative designated values, has a significant impact. More specifically, being emotional about a lower valued issue can hinder the efficiency of the negotiation process and outcome. (Mis)understanding Representatives: The Interactive Effect of Identity and Attributions on Trusting Politicians Adi Amit & Sharon Arieli The Open University of Israel Intergroup conflicts are often managed and resolved by representatives. We focus on the interaction between the representatives and their constituents as a crucial factor for commitment to successful intergroup conflict resolution. In the current research, we investigated constituents' interpretations of controversial behavior made by political leaders prior to the latest elections in Israel. We present supportive evidence for a newly developed model of nested social attributions, showing that constituents monitor and interpret the behavior of their representatives using nested social attributions and that these attributions (a) influence trust in the representatives and (b) depend on group identity. Attribution is the process in which people attempt to make sense and interpret the causes of their own, and others, behavior. Since first introduced (Heider, 1958), the literature on attributions focused mainly on interpretations of "what" influenced the actor, distinguishing internal/intentional from external/unintentional causes (Malle, 2011). In this research we focus on social attributions – suggesting that when interpreting behavior, individuals' ask themselves "Whose interests' did the actor wish to serve?": His own, egoistic, interests? The interests of a small, inclusive group (e.g., the political party)? Or those of a wider, superordinate group (e.g., humanity)? Note that (a) these attributions relate to the social environment benefiting from the behavior (hence social attributions); (b) the attributions are nested, with individuals nested within inclusive groups nested within super-ordinate groups. Since one of the major tensions between representatives and constituents refers to mitigating across multiple, sometimes conflicting, interests (Aaldering, Greer, Van Kleef, & De Dreu, 2013; Eisenhardt, 1989), we stress the importance of nested social attributions in the dynamics between representatives and their constituents. We offer that social attributions vary in valence, such that the larger the number of individuals the actor is perceived as wishing to benefit, the more positive the valuation. This is in line with classic research on moral judgment suggesting that moral reasoning is hierarchical, with egoistic interests (self punishment or gain) as most primitive, group interests (conformity or social-order) as intermediate, and humanistic-universalistic interests as the highest, most developed stage (Kohlberg & Hersh, 1977). In the context of representatives and constituents, we suggest that: H1: The wider the social attribution assigned to a representatives' behavior, the higher the trust expressed towards the representative, reflecting more positive judgments. Focusing on intergroup conflict, and following Hewstone's (1990) note of the importance of the intergroup context to causal attribution, we suggest that social attributions depend on group identity: H2: The behavior of an ingroup member will be interpreted as serving a wider social interest, while the same behavior by an outgroup member will be attributed a narrower interest. We tested and confirmed the nested social attributions model, the effect of group identity, and implications on trusting representatives in the context of the 2015 elections in Israel. Israelis (N=500) were presented with one of six behaviors by prominent political leaders of both sides of the political divide. The participants reported social attributions and trust in the politicians' ability to represent Israel in the ongoing negotiations with the Palestinians. As hypothesized, (1) higher trust was expressed the wider the social attribution; (2) identity moderated attributions: left-wing voters expressed a wider social attribution to the behavior of left-wing leaders and right-wing voters expressed a wider social attribution to the behavior of right-wing leaders. Implications for representatives and for conflict resolution are discussed.