Methods Electromyography recordings Surface EMG activity was recorded from the lateral head of the right gastrocnemius muscle, located by manual muscle testing, using bipolar Ag-AgCl electrodes. The electrodes were placed 2 cm apart over over the belly of the muscle, with the third reference electrode (ground electrode) placed over the bony prominence at the fibula head. The area of electrode placement was prepared by shaving (if required) and cleaned with 70% isopropyl alcohol. EMG signals were amplified (2 kHz-10 kHz), bandpass filtered (13 kHz-1000 kHz), digitized (2 kHz), recorded for 500 ms and analysed off-line (Nihon-Koden, Japan). M-wave and H-reflex recordings M-wave and H-reflex testing followed the methodological considerations discussed by Palmeri et al (2004). A 2 cm bipolar stimulating electrode (Nihon Koden, Japan) with the cathode and anode was placed over the posterior tibial nerve in the popliteal fossa whilst the participant remained relaxed. H-reflex/M-wave recruitment curves were completed in order to identify stimulation intensities to illicit maximal amplitude H-reflex and M-wave (Palmieri et al. 2004). Stimuli of 1 ms pulse width were delivered at a frequency of 0.1 Hz, starting at 0.2 mA. Once maximal amplitude H-reflex was identified, stimulation intensity increased until M-wave saturation. After a brief pause, 10 stimuli were delivered at the identified amplitude for maximal H-reflex, and a further 10 stimuli delivered at 20% above the stimulation intensity identified for maximal M-wave amplitude to ensure M-wave maximum (M-max) was achieved. Transcranial magnetic stimulation Maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) EMG was recorded to determine background muscle activity during the TMS protocol. The participant was instructed to dorsiflex the ball of the foot against the investigator as forcefully as possible for 3 s. The standard criteria for measurement of MVCs were fulfilled and included a period of familiarisation and verbal encouragement provided by the investigators and the rejection of the trial in the case the participant felt it was not a maximal effort (Gandevia 2001). TMS testing followed the established protocols of Byrnes et al. (1999), Pearce et al. (2000) and Wilson et al. (1993). MEPs were evoked by TMS of the contralateral motor cortical area projecting to the right gastrocnemium using a Magstim 2002 stimulator (Magstim Co, UK), with a 90 mm circular coil placed tangential to the skull in an antero-posterior direction. For reliability of coil placement participants wore a snugly fitting cap, positioned with reference to the nasion-inion and interaural lines (Wilson et al. 1993; Byrnes et al. 1999; Pearce et al. 2000; Hortobágyi et al. 2008). The cap was marked with sites at 1 cm spacing in a latitudelongitude matrix to ensure reliable coil position throughout the testing protocol and for repeated testing sessions over the period of the study. The cap was checked constantly to ensure that no changes in cap position occurred. Sites near the estimated centre of the right gastrocnemius area (2-4 cm anterior to the vertex) were explored to determine the site at which the largest MEP amplitude was observed. This site was defined as the “optimal” site. At the optimal site, active motor threshold (AMT) was determined by delivering one set of four stimuli at intensities (5% of stimulator output steps) from a level below the participant’s AMT until the MEP amplitude became saturated (i.e. until the amplitude did not increase with increased stimulation). AMT was defined as the intensity at which an MEP could be obtained in 50% of MEP sweeps during 10% of MVC EMG (Wilson et al, 1993). For main studies, 10 stimuli were delivered during a controlled, low level voluntary contraction of the gastrocnemius muscle at 10% (± 3%) of MVC rmsEMG (Pearce et al. 2000; Wilson et al. 1993). Each stimulus was delivered in random intervals every 5 s to avoid stimulus anticipation and 30 s rest was provided between each set of 5 stimuli to reduce the possibility of muscular fatigue. Data and Statistical analyses All waveforms collected (H-reflex, M-wave, and MEPs; n=10 each; see Figures 1 and 2 for examples) were displayed and averaged online for visual inspection as well as stored off-line for further analysis. H-reflex, M-wave and MEP waveforms were quantified by measuring the peak-to-trough amplitude on the EMG (Pearce et al. 2000; Palmieri et al. 2004). MEP latency and SP duration were quantified by measuring the stimulus artefact to onset of MEP and from the onset of MEP to the return of EMG respectively (Wilson et al. 1993; Pearce and Kidgell 2009). To allow for intra-participant group comparisons, data for H-reflex and MEP amplitudes were normalised by expressing the H-reflex and MEP as a percentage of the M-max amplitude. All data were first screened to ensure they were normally distributed. No variable’s z-score of skew or kurtosis was excessive. Further, Shapiro-Wilk tests suggested all variables Latency, MEP/M-max percentage, and SP duration for pre and post manipulation and control conditions were normally distributed. H-reflex/M-max percentage for pre control and post manipulation were also normally distributed, however H-reflex/M-max for post control (SW=0.84, df = 14, p = 0.02) and pre manipulation (SW=0.80, df = 14, p = 0.06) were apparently non-normal. Following visual examination of frequency histograms and detrended Q-Q plots, for these two conditions which did not show excessive skewness, it was not considered sufficient to warrant a more conservative analytic strategy. Consequently, it was decided to treat all data as essentially normal in distribution. To test the hypothesis that HVLA manipulation alters corticospinal measures, single-group two-condition paired-samples t-tests for each parameter were employed. Data is presented as means ( SD) effect size conventions were used for trivial (<0.2), small (0.21-0.5), medium (0.51-0.79) and large (>0.8) comparative effects were t-tests showed significance (Cohen 1988). Level of significants for statistical tests was set at p<0.05 Results Descriptive data are presented in Table 1. No differences were observed in MEP latency pre versus post time points for both the manipulation (p=0.41) and control (p=0.09) conditions. Similarly, no difference in M-wave latency was observed pre versus post time points for both manipulation (p=0.27) and control (p=0.52) conditions. Similarly, SP duration did not change between either condition (manipulation p=0.62; control p=0.73). MEP/M-wave percentage did not change in the control condition (p=0.39) however, MEP/M-wave percentage did significantly reduce by 2.8% (p=0.02; ES=0.25). Similarly, H-reflex/M-wave percentage showed a significant reduction of 10.5% (p=0.004; ES=0.84) following HVLA manipulation. No change in H-reflex/M-wave percentage was observed in the control condition (p=0.66). References Byrnes ML, Thickbroom GW, Phillips BA et al (1999) Physiological studies of the corticomotor projection to the hand after subcortical stroke. Clinical Neurophysiology 110: 487-498. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum. Gandevia SC (2001) Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev 81: 17251789. Hortobágyi T, Howatson G, Taylor M et al (2008) Contraction-specificity of ipsilateral motor cortical (M1) responses to transcranial magnetic brain stimulation in humans. Brain Stimulation 1: 242-243. Palmieri RM, Ingersoll CD, Hoffman MA (2004) The hoffmann reflex: methodologic considerations and applications for use in sports medicine and athletic training research. J Athl Train 39: 268-277. Pearce AJ, Kidgell DJ (2009) Corticomotor excitability during precision motor tasks. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 12: 280-283. Pearce AJ, Thickbroom GW, Byrnes ML et al (2000) The corticomotor representation of elite racquet sport athletes. Exp Brain Res 130: 238-243. Pearce AJ, Thickbroom GW, Byrnes ML et al (2000) Functional reorganisation of the corticomotor projection to the hand in skilled racquet players. Exp Brain Res 130: 238-243. Wilson SA, Lockwood RJ, Thickbroom GW et al (1993) The muscle silent period following transcranial magnetic cortical stimulation. J Neurol Sci 114: 216-222. Wilson SA, Thickbroom GW, Mastaglia FL (1993) Transcranial magnetic stimulation mapping of the motor cortex in normal subjects. The representation of two intrinsic hand muscles. J Neurol Sci 118: 134-144.