Nirel Marofsky Animal Behaviour Coursera / U of Melbourne 7 July 2014 I see you, I smell you, I love you: zebrafish kin recognition An evolutionary trait seen in many animal species, cooperative behavior requires some degree of restriction in order to be an evolutionarily stable strategy. When it comes to cooperation between an individual and its kin, the ability to distinguish its kin from nonkin is an important one. This ensures that the individual is not wasting resources on conspecifics (members of the same species) that don’t share its genes. Phenotype matching is a social-recognition mechanism by which animals learn early in life which phenotypes are exhibited by their kin. They create an olfactory, visual or acoustic template that helps them later in life discriminate between kin and nonkin. Think of it as a blueprint: Individuals may recognize familiar traits in kin because the kin hold similar traits as those in their mental blueprint -- indicating that they are, by some degree, related. Conversely, they may find certain traits unfamiliar and categorize conspecifics with that trait as unrelated. Very little is known about how animals create a phenotype-matching template to begin with, but new research is beginning to investigate this process. Studies using zebrafish have found that larvae can recognize kin by olfactory cues (using a phenotype-matching process) and later in life prefer to associate with kin, based on those cues. One 2013 study published in Animal Behavior examined whether additional visual cues (as well as olfactory cues) are involved in the imprinting, or blueprint-forming, stage in zebrafish. At days 5-7 post fertilization of zebrafish eggs, researchers introduced olfactory cues to the eggs by replacing some tank water with water from either their kin group or nonkin group. Then, they introduced visual cues to about half the fish that had received water from the kin group tank. They did this by placing beakers containing the fish in glass bowls in which other conspecific larvae were present. Another variable researchers were interested in testing was whether visual imprinting occurs in the same time window as olfactory imprinting. In order to test this, the fish were exposed to visual cues over the course of a few days (some earlier, some later). Next came the test: Would the fish that had been exposed to kin olfactory cues recognize and prefer kin to nonkin? Would those that had been exposed to nonkin cues prefer nonkin, or would some underlying genetic preference cause them to still prefer kin? Results showed that larvae (now at 8-12 weeks post fertilization) able to choose between water from unfamiliar kin and water from unfamiliar nonkin preferred the water from kin. However, the larvae only recognized kin when they had, earlier in life, experienced a combination of olfactory and visual cues of the kin. Mere exposure to one or the other was found to be insufficient. Researchers also wondered if there is a critical period for visual imprinting – a period of time during which larvae must be exposed to visual cues in order for visual imprinting to occur. As it turns out, one does exist. Only larvae that had been visually exposed to kin on day 5 post fertilization showed a significant preference for kin odor. Finally, it turns out that even when exposed to olfactory and visual cues during the critical period, larvae still do not imprint on nonkin. This suggests that some genetic predisposition to recognize kin may come into play. Two questions arise as a follow-up to this study: 1. Why are visual cues required for imprinting when later in life kin can be recognized by olfaction alone? This need for the combination of both visual and olfactory cues probably keeps larvae from imprinting on the wrong cues. 2. If a predisposition to attend to visual and olfactory cues from kin already exists, why is imprinting necessary? The genetic component to kin recognition likely prevents larvae from imprinting on conspecific nonkin, while larvae may learn phenotypes associated with kin during visual imprinting that are necessary to identifying kin during later olfactory imprinting. …So what? Why are the findings of this study important in the greater context of understanding animal behavior? In short, we now know that zebrafish have evolved sophisticated strategies to distinguish even unfamiliar kin from non-kin. This changes the way individuals cooperate with one-another, making cooperation with kin a worthwhile one because it ensures that doing so will eventually lead to the propagation of their genes. The study demonstrates another fascinating example of the way animals learn and interact.