Can Visual Arts Learning Improve Geometric Reasoning? Lynn Goldsmith, Education Development Center, Inc. (presenter) Lois Hetland, Massachusetts College of Art and Design Ellen Winner, Boston College Craig Hoyle, Education Development Center, Inc. Candace Brooks, Education Development Center, Inc. Abstract below is 484 words: We represent a team of cognitive psychologists, art educators educational researcher (mathematics education, research design and methodology), and designers. We have been exploring connections between STEM thinking and artistic envisioning—the formation of images (often mental) which can then guide actions, problem solving and problemforming (Hetland et al., 2007). We propose a 60-minute discussion session for the conference, 20 minutes for describing our study of whether studying visual arts improves geometric reasoning and 40 minutes for a discussion with attendees about connections to their own work, implications for both drawing and mathematics instruction, and possible areas for future investigation. The session would require an LED projector. Our study was motivated by the desire to find new ways to keep students in the “STEM pipeline:” given the importance of visualization for STEM disciplines, strong visualizers are likely to be more successful in STEM studies and therefore more likely to continue to pursue them. We’ve reasoned that developing students’ visualization skills could help to enlarge the segment of the K–12 population with access to, and success in, higher-level mathematics and science. We tested the hypothesis that intensive study in the visual arts contributes to geometric reasoning, comparing the performance of three groups of high school freshman on measures of geometric reasoning, artistic envisioning, and spatial visualization, as well as two control measures (verbal ability and empathy) in a quasiexperimental study. The visual arts (VA) students attend an arts-based public high school; half their day is devoted to core academic studies and half to arts instruction, including a year-long drawing class. The theater students (T) attend the same school; the sports students (S) participate in an intensive after-school squash program. We tested students at three points in time: beginning of 9th grade, end of 9th grade, and end of 10th grade. Data collection has just ended; results reported here are therefore preliminary, based on a complete sample of 108 cases (41 VA, 35 T, and 32 S) and the two 9th grade data points (see Table). Analyses indicated: The VA group had a statistically higher mean score on both pre- and posttest1 geometry than did the other two (control) groups (p<.05). The VA group demonstrated greater pre- to post-1 gains on geometry (p<.05). When controlling for pre-test differences, membership in the VA group remained a significant predictor of post-test1 geometry scores (p<.05). When controlling for pretest geometry scores, scores on the art pretest were a significant predictor of post-1 geometry scores (p <.05). This study, which uses arts education as a naturalistic intervention to study a possible relationship between artistic envisioning and geometric reasoning, has yielded intriguing results and has raised as many questions as it has answered. In the future, we plan to unpack both “artistic envisioning” and the visual components of geometric reasoning in order to look for clearer connections between visual ways of thinking in the two domains. References Hetland, L., Winner, E., Veenema, S., & Sheridan, K. (2007). Studio thinking: The real benefits of visual arts education. New York: Teachers College. Walker, C.M., Winner, E., Hetland, L., Simmons, S., & Goldsmith, L. (2011). Visual thinking: Art students have an advantage in geometric reasoning. Creative Education. 2(1), 199-202.