Symptom Overlap Between Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder (Neurobiological and Genetic insights) Abstract The debate of symptom overlap in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia is not a new one. However the irony is that despite advancements in biological sciences of mental illness, diagnosis and classification systems and treatments, the question remains alive as ever. This limitation opens up possibilities of some overlap of symptoms between several psychiatric disorders, more specifically between schizophrenia and mood disorders. The overlap of symptoms has given rise to a thought in retrospect whether some of syndromes or domains or the illnesses itself arise from a common origin. The present paper examines the categorical diagnostic position of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder based upon current evidence. We argue that both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder lie of same spectrum of psychopathology and therefore have significant overlap of symptoms at least in few of the domains of the illnesses. We then discuss the common territories of over lap in symptomatology and psychopathology. Further we propose that the two illnesses or at least a subgroup of each, are not distinctly different but share a common origin and manifestation. We conclude that both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder share significant feature which are indistinguishable from each other suggesting possibility of both these illnesses as continuous and not dichotomous. Key words : bipolar disorder, symptoms, schizophrenia, psychopathology Introduction The debate of symptom overlap in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia is not a new one. However the irony is that despite advancements in biological sciences of mental illness, diagnosis and classification systems and treatments, the question remains alive as ever. Further increasing evidence, showing biological overlap between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder challenges the concept of two distinct clinical identities. Symptoms and symptom clusters are the main criteria for diagnosis in nosological as well as in systems of classifications. Though there have been revolutionary advancements in the field of biological basis of mental disorders, no definite markers have emerged for any particular disorder. In the psychosocial field, no definite factor or group of factors, have been helpful in defining any ‘pathognomonic’ symptom for making a definitive diagnosis. In fact so far neither biological nor psychosocial etiopathological factors form basis or criteria for diagnosis of mental disorder. Consequently we are still relying of characteristic symptomatology for diagnosis. This limitation opens up possibilities of some overlap of symptoms between several psychiatric disorders, more specifically between schizophrenia and mood disorders. The overlap of symptoms has given rise to a thought in retrospect whether some of syndromes or domains or the illnesses itself arise from a common origin. More precisely questioning that schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are two different disorders. The present paper examines the categorical diagnostic position of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder based upon current evidence. We argue that both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder lie of same spectrum of psychopathology and therefore have significant overlap of symptoms at least in few of the domains of the illnesses. It is likely that overlapping symptoms form manifestation of ‘common endophenotype’, which may need to be assessed and treated differently. In this paper we first examine the current state of diagnostic criteria, diagnostic stability, psychopathological and biological evidence in support of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. We then discuss the common territories of over lap in symptomatology and psychopathology. Further we propose that the two illnesses or at least a subgroup of each, are not distinctly different but share a common origin and manifestation. We conclude that both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder share significant feature which are indistinguishable from each other suggesting possibility of both these illnesses as continuous and not dichotomous. Historical evolution of a psychopathologic terminology The Kraepelinian dichotomy between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, otherwise classically referred to dementia praecox and manic-depressive insanity respectively, has sustained a recent and vigorous onslaught.1 This debate echoes Kraepelin’s own prescient observation that no one will deny that there is n overlap in large number of cases in which it seems impossible, in spite of the most careful observation, to make a firm diagnosis.2 Historical aspects of the dichotomy between manic-depressive disorders and schizophrenia raise the question of a continuum between the two entities. Although Kraepelin’s traditional dichotomy is still a common base for clinicians every day: diagnosis, prognosis and treatment of psychotic disorders, recent epidemiological and neurobiological data are congruent with a dimensional aspect of psychosis. Epidemiological data are consistent with the existence of an individual and a familial overlap between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.2 Attempts to define patient-experienced symptoms, was not made until the 19 century and psychiatrists at that time were embedded in an intellectual system that described the functions of the mind on the basis of the “pure reasoning” of philosophers such as Kant4 and Locke5. The descriptions of the mind focused on the senses, and mental representations. This emphasis led to a fairly straightforward development of clinical terms for dysfunctions of the ‘thought’ process. A more difficult challenge for psychiatrists in the 1800s was the creation of concepts such as ‘mood’ and ‘affect’ and the conceptualization of affect as a discrete faculty of the mind.6 Further, nineteenthcentury psychiatrists soon found that there were many possible combinations of psychotic disorders i.e. disorders marked by symptoms such as delusions, hallucinations, and disintegration of linear thought, and affective disorder. These were defined by the evolving concept of affective syndromes such as depression and mania. Discussions soon developed on the means of determining when mania or depression might be secondary or primary to psychotic disorders7-8 and whether mood disorders with comorbid psychosis should be considered as separate from mood disorders without psychosis.9 th 3 In 1863 Kahlbaum10 described volition as a third mental faculty and defined a category of psychiatric disturbances that could be described as dysfunctional conditions of volition, a separate from disturbances of the intellect and the emotions. This division gained predominance. In 20th-century it took a unique proposition which described Kraepelin’s ‘dementia praecox’ and ‘manic-depressive illness’ as two separate disease processes. Bleuler7 and Schneider11 took Kraepelin’s basic notion of a chronic psychotic disease, which was renamed as schizophrenia. They developed descriptions of particular symptoms or primary traits, i.e. symptoms that were always present and were believed to be pathognomonic of the disease. Bleuler recognized disorders of affect as one of the primary or fundamental symptoms of schizophrenia and described that other types of mood disorder, such as depression and mania, are often present among persons who have schizophrenia. It is important to note that he considered depression and mania as two of many possible secondary or ‘accessory’ syndromes of schizophrenia. Concurrently with Bleuler’s and Schneider’s attempts to define the schizophrenias, English clinicians defined particular symptom clusters and syndromal variants that could be used to define the manic-depressive illnesses, including nonpsychotic versions of these illnesses.12 In 1933 a third diagnostic entity called ‘schizoaffective psychosis’ was introduced by Kasanin13 to describe psychiatric illness marked by prominent affective and psychotic features that appear in a cyclical course. The 1970s saw the development of tentative, or experimental, specific diagnostic categories by Feighner and associates14 and Spitzer and associates.15 These categories were intended to provide criteria to be used by clinicians who wanted to define major depressive, manic, and schizoaffective syndromes as well as the diseases. These authors derived their criteria by asking a number of colleagues what they considered to be the essential symptoms or signs of these syndromes or illnesses. The concepts for making differential diagnoses were derived principally from the work of Feighner and associates14, who emphasized course of illness in contrast with the cross-sectional diagnoses. The American Psychiatric Association16, in successive versions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, adopted the research criteria developed by Feighner and Spitzer and further clarified the notions of syndromes, episodes, and disorders. The nomenclature that has developed allows identical clusters of symptoms to be considered as an episode, for example, major depressive episode and manic episode or a syndrome for example, major depressive syndrome and manic syndrome on the basis of whether the cluster of symptoms occurs in the context of bipolar disorder or major depressive disorder or in the context of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, respectively. Kendler and colleagues17 applied a latent class analysis to 343 patients who had been treated for psychotic and affective disorders and, using a 21-item rating scale, determined six underlying disease syndromes. These syndromes were validated on the basis of distinct demographic, familial, clinical, and course data that were not used in the latent class analysis. Their study used modern computer and statistical methods to define a nosology in much the same way that Kraepelin had once used empirical observation and grouping of patients to derive his binary psychosis model. The six types of illness proposed by Kendler and colleagues suggest that the spectrum of psychotic and affective disorders is more complex than Kraepelin suggested. The types of illness proposed by Kendler and colleagues are classic schizophrenia, bipolar schizomania, major depression, schizo-depression, schizophreniform disorder, and hebephrenia. At last, the question of schizophrenia and mania as a distinct disorder remains, suggesting what is more clearer today than ever is the significance and evidence of overlap of symptoms in both the disorders. Overlapping symptoms (what do we mean by overlapping ) While categories at the extreme end of the psychotic spectrum meaningfully differed across a number of the illness-related variables, no substantial discontinuity was apparent between adjacent categories of psychotic disorders. Risk factors, premorbid adjustment, clinical features and impairment appeared to be present in a mostly monotonic continuous fashion from non affective psychoses to mood disorders with psychotic features. The overall association pattern of illness-related variables with mood and psychotic syndromes was largely independent of specific diagnostic categories, and the dimensional approach was neatly superior to the traditional diagnostic approach in explaining the characteristics of the illness.18 However the extent to which bipolar disorder is considered separately from schizophrenia and other psychoses varies. For example, schizophrenia usually includes psychotic symptoms such as hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorder, as well as “negative” symptoms such as flatness of affect, poverty of speech, or loss of motivation. The diagnosis of schizophrenia excludes significant mood disorder. In contrast bipolar disorder is characterized by prominent mood symptoms and may or may not involve psychosis.19 Diagnostic dilemma Cross-sectional diagnoses, while improved with the advent of welldefined diagnostic criteria, still remain a blunt instrument. Kraepelin emphasized the importance of the course of illness in arriving at a diagnosis. Subsequent work has shown that initial diagnoses do not always remain stable over time, though the great majority of those with an initial SCZ or mood disorder diagnosis do receive the same diagnosis on reassessment.20-23 For example, one study showed that between six months from initial contact, and 24 months, 5% of those initially diagnosed with SCZ switched to mood disorder or schizoaffective disorder, while 9% of those initially diagnosed with mood disorder switched to SCZ or schizoaffective disorder.23 In another study, 15 of the 16 patients whose diagnosis changed at later follow-up from affective to non-affective psychosis had mood-incongruent features initially.24 Diagnostic stability has been particularly poor for schizoaffective disorder. One study showed that only 36% of those initially diagnosed with the disorder received the same diagnosis at a later time point.25 In addition to the problems with diagnostic stability over time in schizoaffective disorder, investigators have also found a lack of cross-sectional diagnostic reliability for this disorder.26-27 The evidence for the overlap Biological parameters (neurochemistry, neurophysiology and neuroimaging) Finally, as neuroscience has developed, more detailed hypotheses have emerged to describe the neural substrate from which all psychiatric disorders ultimately derive. Anatomical studies, functional imaging studies, and more detailed cognitive studies have begun to piece together the neural mechanisms of emotion.28 As described by Derryberry and Tucker28, psychiatry’s historical tendencies to search for biological systems that underlie a given syndrome, such as depression or mania, ultimately fail to appreciate the interdependence of the multiple layers of the neural hierarchy. The hippocampal system, for example, connects putative behavioral inhibitory and excitatory systems with other neural mechanisms in the paralimbic cortex and neocortex, which process complex mental representations of the self and the social environment. Medications provide yet another useful tool for separating distinct pathologic processes that, from an observational standpoint, might appear identical. Pharmaceutical treatments may reflect the pathological mechanism of a disease. A few same classes of pharmaceutical treatments are arguably considered to treat these two disorders. The mechanisms of actions of these treatments may shed some insights into the molecular basis for these two disorders. Atypical antipsychotics that target both the dopamine 2 (D2) and serotonin 5-HT2A receptors can be used to treat schizophrenia (SCZ). Recently, anti-psychotic agents have been increasingly prescribed to bipolar mood disorder (BMD) patients. The effects of these pharmaceutical compounds on SCZ and BMD suggest that dopaminergic and serotonergic pathways are both involved in the pathogenesis of SCZ and BMD.29 It is of note that these antipsychotics may have varying affinities for these receptors. The efficacy of these different anti-psychotics may also vary by diagnosis. Possibly, the pathogenesis of these two disorders may be influenced by heterogeneous mechanisms underlying dopaminergic and serotonergic pathways. Genetic pathways The most compelling line of support for a common biological pathogenesis shared by SCZ and BMD is provided by genetic studies suggesting that some of the same genes influence risk for both disorders. For example, one study has recently reported altered expressions of oligodendroglia-related genes in multiple brain regions to be associated with both SCZ and BMD.30 Linkage studies have provided another line of support. In genome-wide linkage analyses of these disorders, at least 5 distinct genomic regions have been implicated as being linked to susceptibility for both SCZ and BMD.31 Among the chromosomal regions identified as possibly harboring putative risk genes for both SCZ and BMD are 4p32, 6q, 18p, 13q, and 22q.33-34 Candidate gene-based association studies have also implicated several risk genes that may contribute to susceptibility to both SCZ and BMD. Among these implicated genes that may influence susceptibility to both disorders are dysbindin (DTNBP1), G72 (DAOA), disrupted in schizophrenia (DISC1), catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT), and brainderived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and others, as reviewed elsewhere.35 These findings have provided potentially useful leads for efforts to disentangle the shared liability for SCZ and BMD. In the next section, we describe some of the epidemiological and statistical approaches for such efforts. Craddock and colleagues35 postulated a formulation to conceptualize a spectrum of clinical phenotypes associated with SCZ, schizoaffective disorder, and mood disorders. In this putative spectrum of symptoms, psychotic symptoms, mixed psychoticaffective features, and mood symptoms (particularly mania) are modulated by 3 clusters of susceptibility genes; these 3 clusters of genes are partially overlapped with each other. Owen and others36 pointed out that these 3 subsets of genes are represented by the DTNBP1, DISC1, and DAOA genes, respectively. Based on this model, one may infer that different genes may influence the risk of SCZ or BMD to the different extents. Familial Co-Aggregation of SCZ and BMD and genetics Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are 2 of the most severe mental disorders that still are associated with insufficient clinical response, a chronic relapsing course, and functional disability in a substantial number of patients. Although treatments during the first episode of psychosis and mania have yielded encouraging results, follow-up studies37 have been somewhat disappointing, reconfirming that despite high initial response rates, illness relapse38 and lack of functional recovery39-40 are relatively frequent. Over the past decade, the schizophrenia field has responded to this situation with a push toward early recognition and intervention during the pre-psychotic (i.e., prodromal) phase of the illness.40-41 However, there is a lack of studies that have focused on the identification of patients who are identified based on emerging clinical symptoms that would be indicative of future bipolar I disorder, rather than on the presence of genetic risk or a preceding diagnosis, such as unipolar depression, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, or a bipolar spectrum disorder. This dearth of clinical high-risk studies in bipolar disorder can be explained by the fact that the presence and nature of a clinical prodrome is not well established in this condition.42 determinants 43 However, both conditions are intimately related, with shared genetic and common polygenic variants, as confirmed by the International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC) in a genome-wide association study of 3,322 Europeans.44 Thus, epidemiological characteristics, family studies, and overlapping genetic linkages together support shared genetic risk factors in bipolar and schizophrenia33 and there is additional new evidence showing similar changes in gene expression in both conditions.45 Bipolar disorder shares many of the same brain regions as schizophrenia. However, relative to neurotypical controls, lower gray matter volume in schizophrenia is more extensive and includes the amygdala. Common biological mechanisms may explain the neuroanatomical overlap between these major disorders, but explaining why brain differences are more extensive in schizophrenia remains challenging. There is a substantial overlap in clinical and neuropathological findings between these disorders. Moreover, recent studies have demonstrated that the genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression is shared.46-49 Consequently there is also an increasing debate about the current classification of psychotic disorders. Indeed, the identification of groups of patients with a particular vulnerability to underlying neuropathological processes could provide an alternative classification of psychotic disorders of greater utility to psychiatric genetic research as compared to current classifications. A common pathological mechanism for two diseases may be reflected by comorbidity in the same individual. However, the current hierarchical diagnostic systems for psychiatric diseases do not allow dual diagnoses for SCZ and BMD in the same individual (with BMD-Not Otherwise Specified as an exception) and thus pose a challenge for assessing shared etiology for SCZ and BMD at the individual level. As an alternative, familial co-aggregation, which reflects excessive occurrence of two disorders within the same family, can provide evidence for common genetic pathways for SCZ and BMD. Familial co-aggregation50 and co-segregation51 differ in that the former indicates that the clustering of two diseases within families, which does not necessarily result in the occurrence of two diseases in the same individual; the latter can lead to the occurrence of two diseases in the same individual. One common approach for testing for the presence of familial co-aggregation is to determine if the risk for one disease (e.g., SCZ) is elevated in relatives of an individual affected with a second disease (e.g., BMD). Excess familial risk can be assessed either by contrasting disease prevalence (e.g., of SCZ) in relatives of case (e.g., BMD) probands with disease prevalence in either the relatives of control probands or with overall population prevalence rates. In fact, evidence for familial co-segregation of SCZ and BMD has been provided by Valles52 who reported that first-degree relatives of BMD patients had a 4-fold higher risk of SCZ compared with relatives of healthy individuals. In familial cosegregation studies, various statistical approaches can be used for the comparisons to take into account such issues as the ages of the family members, other disease risk factors, and the correlations in measurements due to the family members being related to each other.53 One caveat of co-aggregation studies is that they may provide spurious evidence for familial co-aggregation if the 2 diseases being studied are easily misdiagnosed or can be confused with each other due to resemblances of clinical features of these two disorders. Gene versus environment factors The clustering of a disease within families alone does not permit one to distinguish between the effects of genetic factors and environmental factors in the etiological pathway of disease because relatives who share genes in common are also more likely to share similar lifestyles and/or environmental risk factors. In the same way, the presence of familial co-aggregation of two diseases within the same family alone cannot distinguish between the role of shared genetic factors and environmental factors in a shared etiological pathway.54 One conventional approach used to clarify the relative impact of genetic variants versus environmental factors on a single disorder is to parse out the variance in trait susceptibility to that attributable to genes and that attributable to non-genetic (or environmental) risk factors using statistical approaches akin to analysis of variance. In such approaches, the variation in the trait due to genetic factors is modeled as a function of trait similarity among related individuals, and the heritability of the trait is defined as the proportion of the total trait variance due to genetic effects.55 The standard variance decomposition procedures can be extended for the joint study of two diseases to tease apart genetic and environmental influences of two disorders using a bivariate extension of the variance component approach. This method partitions the joint variation in the two traits into their trait-specific genetic components, trait-specific environmental components, shared genetic effects, and shared environmental effects. The shared genetic effects represent effectively the “coheritability” of the two traits. One can use bivariate variance component method to study the genetic relationship between 2 continuous traits.56 One application of this approach is described by researchers57 who reported strong genetic correlations between serum concentrations of insulin and body mass index and between insulin and plasma levels of high-density lipoprotein-cholesterol, suggesting that one or more genes influences joint variation in these sets of traits. This bivariate analysis method has been extended for analysis of binary phenotypes using variance component models or generalized linear mixed models.58-60 Twin studies The analysis of twin studies represents a subtype of family analysis that can be used to differentiate between genetic and environmental contributions to familial aggregation. In principle, one can evaluate whether genes play an important role in susceptibility to disease by comparing disease prevalence in the monozygotic (MZ) twin siblings of affected probands to disease prevalence in the dizygotic (DZ) twin siblings of affected probands. Higher disease prevalence in the MZ twin pairs is generally interpreted to indicate a genetic basis for disease if one assumes that environmental risk factors are shared equally among DZ twin pairs as among MZ twin pairs (an assumption that can be challenged in some situations).61 By extending the framework of twin studies from one disorder to two disorders, one can further test whether the MZ twin siblings of SZ probands have higher risk of BP compared with DZ twin siblings of SZ probands (or vice versa) to provide insights into the relative impact of genes on familial co-aggregation of these two disorders.62 Cardno and colleagues examined genetic correlations between SCZ, schizoaffective disorder, and BMD in 77 monozygotic and 89 same-sex dizygotic twin pairs using relaxed diagnostic criteria. They found evidence for both common and syndrome-specific genetic contributions to the variance in liability to SCZ and manic syndromes, but the genetic liability to the schizoaffective syndrome was entirely shared in common with the other two syndromes. In contrast, environmental liability to the schizoaffective syndrome was not shared with the other syndromes.63 Identifying the Shared Risk Genes Conventional approaches used to identify risk alleles for single disorders include linkage and association studies. Linkage analysis is based on using recombination frequencies to infer physical distance between a genetic marker and target risk locus64, while association studies directly measure the correlation between the genetic polymorphism at a locus and the disease endpoint.65 Association analyses are more powerful to detect causal variants, provided there is linkage disequilibrium (i.e., correlation between a paired of genetic loci) between the genetic marker and disease loci; however, linkage analyses are more powerful in the absence of such disequilibrium. 66 Excellent reviews of both approaches have been published elsewhere.67 The introduction of high-output, low-cost genotyping technologies has recently generated great enthusiasm in the field of complex disease genetics by making possible the conduct of large-scale genetic association studies that use 500,000 or more single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) scattered throughout the genome.68 Such studies have rapidly gained popularity and complement traditional candidate gene studies that are based on measurement and analysis of only a single SNP or set of SNPs within a single gene. Identification of disease susceptibility genes using the genome-wide association approach has proven remarkably successful, with novel genes already reported for complex traits such as cardiovascular diseases70-71 and diabetes.72-73 These studies have employed either single- or multistage designs to generate evidence for association in an initial original sample and have then replicated these associations in other populations. An important lesson learned from the GWAS scans of diabetes and cardiovascular disease is that the associations detected have tended to be in novel genes rather than in previously studied candidate genes. It is possible that approaches such as genome-wide association analysis may identify single SNPs that will turn out to be associated with both SCZ and BMD or may even reveal different SNPs in the same gene to be associated with each disorder. Other studies have explored the genetic underpinnings for disorders characterized by a mix of mood and psychotic features, such as schizoaffective disorder.73 The pathological processes in schizoaffective disorder are thought to be correlated with those in SCZ and BMD, although some investigators have questioned the validity of the independent diagnostic entity of schizoaffective disorder.74 It thus remains to be seen whether susceptibility genes for schizoaffective disorder will turn out to be, at least in part, involved in the shared genetic liability of SCZ and BMD. Common Endophenotypes for SCZ and BMD According to Gottesman and Gould75, endophenotypes are neurophysiological, biochemical, endocrinological, neuroanatomical, cognitive, or neuropsychological components associated with the target disorder. From a genetic perspective, endophenotype can be very attractive targets for study if they are easily and reliably measured, co-aggregate with the target disorder within families, and are also present in unaffected relatives. A desirable endophenotype is also one that is more proximal to a causative gene than the end-stage disease state and thus may be more amenable to genetic study than the downstream disease. Many candidate endophenotypes in SCZ and BMD are neurophysiological markers. Other endophenotype that should be explored extensively include drug response and metabolism, RNA expression, and protein levels.76 Studies of other neurocognitive functions related to information processing also reveal the biological resemblances of SCZ and BMD. For instance, impaired performance in span of apprehension has been shown in both SCZ and BMD.77 Other abnormalities in information processing associated with these 2 disorders include P300-evoked response latency78 and amplitude79, P50 auditory-evoked response suppression80-81, prepulse inhibition82, facial scan path patterns83 and a mismatch negativity paradigm.84 Additionally, other cognitive function impairments, such as executive deficits, can be demonstrated in psychotic and bipolar disorder.85 These biomarkers related to neurocognitive functions may hence serve as common endophenotype upstream to pathological pathways to SCZ and BMD. If an endophenotype is influenced more directly by genetic factors, one may expect to observe a higher heritability of an endophenotype compared with its endpoint disease (although a high heritability may not necessarily result from a smaller number of genes involved in the pathological mechanism). Take smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM) as an example. SPEM refers to the movement of the eyes as they track a slowly moving target, a process that is initiated by visual processing of motion signals (i.e., extra-retinal motion). One of the major SPEM sub measurements, predictive pursuit gain, is highly heritable (heritability estimate = 0.90)86, indicating that this trait is under substantial genetic control. Additionally, both schizophrenic patients and their unaffected relatives are more likely than healthy individuals to have deficits in SPEM, suggesting that this trait co-segregates with SCZ and that deficits in SPEM are not secondary sequelae occurring as a result of SCZ. Moreover, individuals affected with BMD and their relatives are also more likely to have deficits in SPEM compared with healthy individuals.87 Genetic analysis of SPEM-related phenotypes has provided further insights into shared genetic influences that might cut across different psychiatric diagnoses, including SCZ and BMD. For example, 2 studies have reported evidence for linkage of SPEM phenotype to 6p23-21, suggesting that this chromosomal region may harbor one or more genes influencing variation in SPEM.88-89 Interestingly, the same region also harbors 2 genes previous associated with risk of schizophrenia, ATXN1 (SCA1) and NOTCH4.90 Other candidate genes associated with SPEM include dopamine D3 receptor gene (DRD3)91, DISC192 and COMT.92 All these genes have also been hypothesized to play a role in the pathogenesis of SCZ and BMD. Taken together, these findings suggest that the study of common endophenotypes for SCZ and BMD, such as SPEM, may reveal insights into alleged etiologic factors linking these two disorders. Studying common endophenotypes may circumvent the limitation of hierarchical diagnostic system posed on SCZ and BMD. Meanwhile, the conceptualization of endophynotypes does not contradict the putative hierarchical pathological relationship between SCZ and BMD. Furthermore, endophenotypes can allow the investigator to examine the genotype-phenotype relationship in the same population. Conventional studies focusing on SCZ and BMD in different populations separately may produce findings that cannot be transferred to each other. Therefore, deciphering the genetics of common endophenotypes may serve as an alternative and effective approach to untangling the mechanism of shared genetic liability for these 2 disorders. The success of endophenotype-based approaches hinges on the assumption that endophynotypes are modulated by less complex genetic factors than the disease syndrome itself. Hence, the identification of genetic variants that yield a larger effect on endophynotypes than the end-point disease will benefit from such an approach. Goldman et al.94 discovered a number of loci with a greater impact on endophynotypes compared with related psychiatric disorders, such as BMD and alcoholism. However, one recent study compared the effects of genetic variants on several endophenotypes and end-point diseases using the meta-analysis technique and did not produce supportive evidence for this assumption. The investigators examined 7 different endophenotypes, such as “circadian rhythm” and prefrontal cognitive function, etc., as the endophynotypes for BP, and “spatial and verbal working memory” and “ventricular enlargement,” etc., as endophynotypes for SCZ. Their findings suggest that genetic contributions of the COMT gene Val/Met polymorphism to endophenotypes were not significantly different from those effects on SCZ or BMD.95 Therefore, one needs to carefully evaluate the locusspecific genetic effect size of the endophenotype in order to unravel the joint genetic determinants for SCZ and BMD. Alternatively, investigators can use an endophenotype to select a more clinically homogeneous subgroup of subjects for genetic studies. SCZ and BMD characterized by a shared endophenotypic feature may be regarded as subtypes of SCZ and BMD, respectively. Such an endophenotype-based approach may not only overcome the problem of genetic heterogeneity in each individual disorder but also enhance clinical resemblances for these 2 disorders and hence help identify the shared genetic variant of a possibly larger effect. This approach may allow investigators to avoid the concern that an endophenotype is not modulated by less complex genetic factors than those associated with the risk of SCZ or BMD. Conclusion To summarize, the conventional nosological distinction between SCZ and BMD has been challenged by research showing a phenomenological and biological overlap of these two disorders. Genetic research suggesting that common genes may be involved in both SCZ and BMD has lent additional support for the presence of shared etiological pathways between these two disorders, although specific genes associated with SCZ and BMD jointly have yet to be identified. Just as the long-standing “Kraepelin dichotomy” has become subject to re-evaluation, the diagnostic systems for other disorders cantered on psychotic symptoms, such as schizoaffective disorder, may also need to be re-examined. The hierarchical diagnostic system for SCZ and BMD precludes the usual approaches for assessing their being associated with each other because the two diagnoses usually cannot be assigned to the same individual. However, assessment of familial co-aggregation may provide very useful insights into whether these two disorders share common etiologies. Although previous evidence has suggested a number of susceptibility genes shared by SCZ and BMD, most of these studies have focused on one disorder at a time in independent populations. Alternatively, mapping genes for schizoaffective disorder, which shares symptoms related to both SCZ and BMD, may help unravel shared genetic mechanisms for these two disorders. Finally, identifying the genes modulating common endophenotypes, such as SPEM, provided that they are influenced more directly by genetic factors, may unveil the shared genetic pathways for SCZ and BMD. References 1. Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, Javitt DC, Benes FM, Heckers S, Grace AA. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci 2008; 31(5): 234-242. 2. Angst J. Bipolar disorder : methodological problems and future perspectives. Dialog Clin Neurosci 2008; 10(2): 129-139. 3. Berrios GE. The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology since the Nineteenth Century. New York, Cambridge University Press; 1996. 4. Kant E. Critique of Practical Reason. London, Longman ; 1909. 5. Locke J. An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. New York, Dover ; 1959. 6. Albrecht FM. A re-appraisal of faculty psychology. J Hist Behav Sci 1970; 6: 3640. 7. Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York, International Universities Press ; 1950 8. Royal College of Physicians. The Nomenclature of Diseases. (4th ed). London, His Majesty's Stationary Office ; 1906. 9. Bucknill J, Tuke DH. A Manual of Psychological Medicine. London, John Churchill ; 1858. 10. Kahlbaum K. Die Gruppirung der psychischen Krakheiten un die Eintheilung der Seelenstörunge. Danzig, AW Kafemann ; 1863. 11. Schneider K. Clinical Psychopathology. New York, Grune and Stratton ; 1959. 12. Buzzard EF. Discussion on the diagnosis and treatment of the milder forms of the manic depressive psychosis. Proc Royal Soc Med 1930; 23: 881-895. 13. Kasanin J. The acute schizoaffective psychoses. Am J Psychiatry 1933; 90: 97126. 14. Feighner JP, Robins E, Guze SB. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972; 26: 57-63. 15. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E. Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research ; 1981. 16. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for the classification of psychiatric disorders – 3rd edition (DSM-III). American Psychiatric Press ; 1980. 17. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Walsh D. The structure of psychosis: latent class analysis of probands from the Roscommon Family Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55: 492-509. 18. Kay SR, Sevy S. Pyramidical model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1990; 16: 653-662. 19. Marengo J, Harrow M, Herbener ES. A prospective longitudinal 10-year study of schizophrenia's three major factors and depression. Psychiatr Res 2000; 97: 61-77. 20. McGlashan TH, Carpenter WT. Postpsychotic depression in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33: 231-239. 21. Jorgensen P, Mortensen PB. Admission pattern and diagnostic stability of patients with functional psychoses in Denmark during a two-year observation period. Acta Psych Scand 1988; 78: 361–365. 22. Marneros A, Deister A, Rohde A. Stability of diagnoses in affective, schizoaffective and schizophrenic disorders. Cross-sectional versus longitudinal diagnosis. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci 1991; 241: 187–192 23. Schwartz JE, Fennig S, Tanenberg-Karant M, Carlson G, Craig T, Galambos N. Congruence of diagnoses 2 years after a first-admission diagnosis of psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57: 593–600. 24. Fennig S, Bromet EJ, Karant MT, Ram R, Jandorf L. Mood-congruent versus mood-incongruent psychotic symptoms in first-admission patients with affective disorder. J Affect Disord 1996; 37: 23–29. 25. Tsuang MT, Dempsey GM. Long-term outcome of major psychoses II : schizoaffective disorder compared with schizophrenia, affective disorders, and a surgical control group. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36: 1302-1304. 26. Faraone SV, Matise T, Svrakic D, Pepple J, Malaspina DB, Suarez, B. Genome scan of European-American schizophrenia pedigrees: Results of the NIMH Genetics initiative and Millennium Consortium. Am J Med Genetics 1998; 81: 290–295. 27. Maj M, Pirozzi R, Formicola AM, Bartoli L, Bucci P. Reliability and validity of the DSM-IV diagnostic category of schizoaffective disorder: Preliminary data. J Affect Disorders 2000; 57: 95–98. 28. Derryberry D, Tucker DM. Neural mechanisms of emotion. J Consult Clin Psychology 1992; 60: 329-338. 29. Coryell W, Leon AC, Turvey C, Akiskal HS, Mueller T, Endicott J. The significance of psychotic features in manic episodes: a report from the NIMH collaborative study. J Affect Disord 2001; 67: 79–88. 30. Keck PE Jr, McElroy SL, Havens JR. Psychosis in bipolar disorder: phenomenology and impact on morbidity and course of illness. Compr Psychiatry 2003; 44: 263–269. 31. Berrettini W. Bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: convergent molecular data. Mol Med 2004; 5: 109–117. 32. Maziade M, Roy MA, Rouillard E. A search for specific and common susceptibility loci for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a linkage study in 13 target chromosomes. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6: 684–693. 33. Berrettini W. Evidence for shared susceptibility in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 2003; 123: 59–64. 34. Badner JA, Gershon ES. Meta-analysis of whole-genome linkage scans of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2002; 7: 405–411. 35. Craddock N, O'Donovan MC, Owen MJ. Genes for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder? Implications for psychiatric nosology. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32: 9–16. 36. Owen MJ, Craddock N, Jablensky A. The genetic deconstruction of psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33: 905–911. 37. Hafner H, Maurer K, Trendler G, Van der Heiden W, Schmidt M, Konnecke R. Schizophrenia and depression: challenging the paradigm of two separate diseases – a controlled study of schizophrenia, depression and healthy controls. Schizophr Res 2005; 77: 11–24 38. Weiden P, Olfson M. The cost of relapse in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1995; 21(3): 419-429. 39. Harvey PD, Bellack AS. Toward a terminology for functional recovery in schizophrenia : is functional remission a viable concept. Schizophr Bull 2009; 35(2): 300-306. 40. Lencz T, Smith CW, Auther AM, Correll CU, Cornblatt BA. The assessment of prodromal schizophrenia : unresolved issues and future directions. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29(4): 717-728. 41. Yung AD, McGorry PD. The prodromal phase of first episode schizophrenia : past and current conceptualizations. Schizophr Bull 1996; 22(2): 353-370. 42. Colom F, Lam D. Psychoeducation : improving outcomes in bipolar disorder. Eur Psychiatry 2005; 20(5-6): 359-364. 43. Lake CR, Hurwitz N. Schizoaffective disorders are psychotic mood disorders; there are no schizoaffective disorders. Psychiatry Res 2006; 143: 255–287. 44. International Schizophrenia Consortium. Common polygenic variation contributes to the risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 2009; 460: 748-752. 45. Forrester A, Owens DG, Johnstone EC. Diagnostic stability in subjects with multiple admissions for psychotic illness. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 151–158. 46. Cardno AG, Rijsdijk FV, Sham PC, Murray RM, McGuffin P. A twin study of genetic relationships between psychotic symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159: 539–545. 47. Kempf L, Hussain N, Potash JB. Mood disorder with psychotic features, schizoaffective disorder, and schizophrenia with mood features: trouble at the borders. Int Rev Psychiatry 2005; 17: 9–19. 48. Sokolov BP. Oligodendroglial abnormalities in schizophrenia, mood disorders and substance abuse. Comorbidity, shared traits, or molecular phenocopies? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2007; 10: 547–555. 49. Sullivan PF. The Psychiatric GWAS Consortium : big science comes to psychiatry. Neuron 2010; 68(2): 182-186. 50. Bramon E, Sham PC. The common genetic liability between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Curr Psychiatr Rep 2001; 3: 332-337. 51. Klar AJS. Genetic models for handedness, brain lateralization, schizophrenia, and manic-depression. Schizophr Res 1999; 39(3): 207-218. 52. Valles V, Van Os J, Guillamat R. Increased morbid risk for schizophrenia in families of in-patients with bipolar illness. Schizophr Res 2000; 42: 83–90. 53. Hudson JI, Laird NM, Betensky RA. Multivariate logistic regression for familial aggregation of two disorders. I. Development of models and methods. Am J Epidemiol 2001; 153: 500–505. 54. Moffitt TE. The New Look of Behavioral Genetics in Developmental Psychopathology : Gene–Environment Interplay in Antisocial Behaviors. Psychol Bull 2005; 131: 533-554. 55. Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Gene–environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006; 7: 583-590. 56. Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene–environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol Psychiatry 2007; 12: 432-442. 57. Mitchell BD, Kammerer CM, Mahaney MC. Genetic analysis of the IRS. Pleiotropic effects of genes influencing insulin levels on lipoprotein and obesity measures. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 1996; 16: 281–288. 58. Burton PR, Tiller KJ, Gurrin LC. Genetic variance components analysis for binary phenotypes using generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) and Gibbs sampling. Genet Epidemiol 1999; 17: 118–140. 59. Yip BH, Bjork C, Lichtenstein P. Covariance component models for multivariate binary traits in family data analysis. Stat Med 2008; 20: 1086–1095. 60. Noh M, Yip B, Lee Y. Multicomponent variance estimation for binary traits in family-based studies. Genet Epidemiol 2006; 30: 37–47. 61. Owen MJ, Cardno AG, O’Donovan MC. Psychiatric genetics : back to the future. Mol Psychiatry 2000; 5: 22-31. 62. Elston RC. Linkage and association to genetic markers. Exp Clin Immunogenet 1995; 12: 129–140. 63. Maniatis N. Linkage disequilibrium maps and disease-association mapping. Methods Mol Biol 2007; 376: 109–121. 64. Borecki IB, Suarez BK. Linkage and association: basic concepts. Adv Genet 2001; 42: 45–66. 65. March RE. Gene mapping by linkage and association analysis. Mol Biotechnol 1999; 13: 113–122. 66. Sham PC. Statistical methods in psychiatric genetics. Stat Meth Med Res 1998; 7: 279-300. 67. De Lisi LE. A critical overview into the recent investigations in the genetics of schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry 1999; 12: 29-39. 68. McGuffin P, Owen MJ. Molecular genetic studies in schizophrenia. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1996; 61: 815-822. 69. Helgadottir A, Thorleifsson G, Manolescu A. A common variant on chromosome 9p21 affects the risk of myocardial infarction. Science 2007; 316: 1491–1493. 70. McPherson R, Pertsemlidis A, Kavaslar N. A common allele on chromosome 9 associated with coronary heart disease. Science 2007; 316: 1488–1491. 71. Salonen JT, Uimari P, Aalto JM. Type 2 diabetes whole-genome association study in four populations: the DiaGen consortium. Am J Hum Genet 2007; 81: 338–345. 72. Scuteri A, Sanna S, Chen WM. Genome-wide association scan shows genetic variants in the FTO gene are associated with obesity-related traits. PLoS Genet 2007; 3: e115. 73. Murphy KC, Cardno AG, McGuffin P. The molecular genetics of schizophrenia. J Mol Neurosci 1996; 7: 147-157. 74. Lake CR, Hurwitz N. Schizoaffective disorder merges schizophrenia and bipolar disorders as one disease—there is no schizoaffective disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2007; 20: 365–379. 75. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 636–645. 76. Carlson CS, Eberle MA, Kruglyak L. Mapping complex disease loci in wholegenome association studies. Nature 2004; 429: 446–452. 77. Strauss ME, Prescott CA, Gutterman DF. Span of apprehension deficits in schizophrenia and mania. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 699–704. 78. Souza VB, Muir WJ, Walker MT. Auditory P300 event-related potentials and neuropsychological performance in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1995; 37: 300–310. 79. Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. P300 topography differs in schizophrenia and manic psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45: 98–106. 80. Franks RD, Adler LE, Waldo MC. Neurophysiological studies of sensory gating in mania: comparison with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1983; 18: 989–1005. 81. Adler LE, Gerhardt GA, Franks R. Sensory physiology and catecholamines in schizophrenia and mania. Psychiatry Res 1990; 31: 297–309. 82. Perry W, Minassian A, Feifel D. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50: 418–424. 83. Loughland CM, Williams LM, Gordon E. Schizophrenia and affective disorder show different visual scanning behavior for faces: a trait versus state-based distinction? Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52: 338–348. 84. Catts SV, Shelley AM, Ward PB. Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 213–219. 85. Szoke A, Meary A, Trandafir A. Executive deficits in psychotic and bipolar disorders—implications for our understanding of schizoaffective disorder. Eur Psychiatry 2008; 23: 20–25. 86. Hong LE, Mitchell BD, Avila MT. Familial aggregation of eye-tracking endophenotypes in families of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 259–264. 87. Kathmann N, Hochrein A, Uwer R. Deficits in gain of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia and affective disorder patients and their unaffected relatives. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 696–702. 88. Arolt V, Lencer R, Nolte A. Eye tracking dysfunction is a putative phenotypic susceptibility marker of schizophrenia and maps to a locus on chromosome 6p in families with multiple occurrence of the disease. Am J Med Genet 1996; 67: 564– 579. 89. Matthysse S, Holzman PS, Gusella JF. Linkage of eye movement dysfunction to chromosome 6p in schizophrenia: additional evidence. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004; 128: 30–36. 90. Waterworth DM, Bassett AS, Brzustowicz LM. Recent advances in the genetics of schizophrenia. Cell Mol Life Sci 2002; 59: 331–348. 91. Rybakowski JK, Borkowska A, Czerski PM. Dopamine D3 receptor (DRD3) gene polymorphism is associated with the intensity of eye movement disturbances in schizophrenic patients and healthy subjects. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6: 718–724. 92. Hennah W, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Paunio T. A haplotype within the DISC1 gene is associated with visual memory functions in families with a high density of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10: 1097–1103. 93. Rybakowski JK, Borkowska A, Czerski PM. Eye movement disturbances in schizophrenia and a polymorphism of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene. Psychiatry Res 2002; 113: 49–57. 94. Goldman D, Ducci F. Deconstruction of vulnerability to complex diseases: enhanced effect sizes and power of intermediate phenotypes. Scientific World Journal 2007; 7: 124–130. 95. Flint J, Munafo MR. The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychol Med 2007; 37: 163–180. 1) Tsuang, M. T., Woolson, R. F., Winokur, G., & Crowe, R. R. (1981). Stability of psychiatric diagnosis. Schizophrenia and affective disorders followed up over a 30- to 40-year period. Archives in General Psychiatry, 38, 535–539 2) Dr B Green, Schizophrenia, 2007 3) Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435 4) Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neursurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56-62