Online Supplement B CT Comparison Sample. To compare the results obtained in our sample of patients in combined treatment to those in CT alone, we utilized data reported by Strunk et al. (2010a). That sample was composed of 60 patients with a primary Axis I diagnosis of Major Depressive Disorder who were randomly assigned to the CT condition of a two-site (University of Pennsylvania and Vanderbilt University) clinical trial of CT, ADM, and placebo for moderate to severe depression (see DeRubeis et al., 2005). To be eligible for that trial, patients had to meet criteria for a current episode of depression (based on the SCID; First et al., 2001) and score 20 or higher on the modified 17-item version of the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Hamilton, 1960). The exclusionary factors were the same as those reported for our primary sample. Six therapists provided CT (for more details, see DeRubeis et al., 2005). Among the 60 CT patients, 58% were women and ages ranged from 19 to 68 years (M = 40, SD = 12). Most patients were Caucasian (78%); 12% were African American, and 10% were of other ethnicities. Therapist Competence. To facilitate exploratory analyses involving therapist competence, a principal investigator of both trials (Steven D. Hollon) provided global ratings of competence for all cognitive therapists across both trials. These ratings were made based on his knowledge of the therapists over the course of the trials. Cognitive therapists were rated using the overall competence item from the Cognitive Therapy Scale (Young and Beck, 1988), with possible responses ranging from a score of 0 (poor) to 6 (excellent). Ratings in the primary combined treatment sample ranged from 0 to 5 (M = 3.7; SD = 1.4). Ratings for the CT alone sample ranged from 2 to 5 (M = 4.3; SD = 1.2). The overall mean was 4.0 (SD = 1.4), which corresponds to a rating of “good.” Comparison of Process-Outcome Relations in Combined Treatment vs. CT alone To compare the process-outcome relations across trials, we pooled the datasets and tested interactions of each process measure with treatment (i.e., combined treatment vs. CT alone) as predictors of subsequent session-to-session change. Recall that Strunk and colleagues reported strong positive relations with greater symptom change for both the Cognitive Methods (r = -.44) and Negotiating / Structuring (r = -.38) scales (but not Behavioral Methods / Homework, r = .04). In our combined treatment sample, Behavioral Methods / Homework emerged as the most robust predictor of session-to-session improvement (r = -.24), with Cognitive Methods and Negotiating / Structuring failing to emerge as significant predictors (rs of .11 and .07, respectively). To test whether each of the process-outcome relations obtained differed across trials, we tested interactions between treatment (i.e., study) and each process variable. In the pooled dataset, there was a significant interaction between Cognitive Methods and treatment (r =-.21, t(212) = -3.14, p = .002) and between Behavioral Methods / Homework and treatment (r = .17, t(212) = 2.49, p = .01). There was a non-significant trend for the interaction between Negotiating / Structuring and treatment (r = .12 t(212) = -1.78, p = .08) and no evidence of an interaction of alliance and treatment (r = -.00, t(212) = -.03, p = .98). To address concerns about differences in inclusion criteria between the two clinical trials, we conducted additional analyses restricting the sample to patients who either had chronic depression or a prior depressive episode (inclusion criteria for the study of combined treatment) and to patients with intake HRSD scores of 20 or greater (inclusion criteria for the study of CT alone). In this restricted sample of the combined condition (n = 112) and CT alone (n = 52), we examined the two significant interactions identified in the full sample. The interaction between Cognitive Methods and treatment remained significant (r = .23, t(154) = -3.00, p = .003) while the interaction between Behavioral Methods / Homework and treatment was reduced to the level of a non-significant trend (r = .15, t(154) = 1.89, p = .06). Exploratory Analysis of Therapist Competence We were surprised that the strong association of Cognitive Methods and subsequent symptom change identified by Strunk and colleagues (2010a) in CT alone was not evident in our combined treatment sample. While the presence or absence of ADM might account for this difference, we also suspected that variability in therapist competence might help to explain the result. To explore this possibility, we examined the interaction of Cognitive Methods and ratings of therapist competence in predicting subsequent session-to-session symptom change using data from both the CT alone and the combined treatment samples. This interaction was significant (r = -.16, t(212) = -2.39, p = .02). To explore the nature of this interaction, we examined the relation of Cognitive Methods and subsequent symptom change separately for the 12 therapists rated at or above the median competence rating of “good” and the eight therapists rated below this level. Among the therapists rated as more competent, Cognitive Methods predicted significantly greater symptom change (r = -.19, t(134) = -2.25, p = .03); among the therapists rated as less competent, Cognitive Methods was associated with non-significantly less robust symptom change (r = .15, t(78) = 1.31, p = .20). To determine whether the interaction of competence and Cognitive Methods accounted for the interaction of Cognitive Methods and treatment (CT alone vs. combined) noted earlier, we then tested a model with both interactions. In this model, the interaction of Cognitive Methods and treatment remained significant (r = -.17, t(211) = -2.57, p = .01). Thus, while we did find evidence for the interaction of competence and Cognitive Methods, this interaction did not appear to account for the difference in the relation of Cognitive Methods and subsequent symptom change observed across the CT alone and combined treatment samples. There was no evidence of an interaction between therapist competence ratings and Behavioral Methods/ Homework (r = .10, t(212) = 1.40, p =.16). Conclusions Across the CT alone and combined samples, we found significant differences in the relation of both Cognitive Methods and Behavioral Methods / Homework to session-to-session symptom change. A similar pattern emerged in a sample restricted to those patients who would have been eligible for both trials, though the association between Behavioral Methods / Homework and symptom change was reduced to a non-significant trend. Our analyses of competence identified a significant interaction with Cognitive Methods. These analyses suggested that Cognitive Methods were more predictive of symptom change among those therapists judged to be more competent, but not predictive of improvement among therapists judged as less competent. However, because the competence by Cognitive Methods interaction did not fully explain the treatment by Cognitive Methods interaction, future research utilizing more fine-grained assessments of competence may be important to shed further light on this issue. Two important limitations bear consideration. First, it is important to recognize that the ratings of competence used in these analyses were made by an expert who was not blind to patient outcomes. Nonetheless, because we failed to find a relation between competence ratings and session-to-session symptom change (r = .05, t(216) = .80, p = .4), it is unlikely that any interaction of competence and other process measures would be accounted for by bias in competence ratings related to knowledge of patients’ outcomes. Secondly, the process ratings provided for the two studies utilized in our pooled sample were generated by different groups of raters. As such, we cannot rule out the possibility that rater-related measurement error may have contributed to differences between process-outcome relations across samples. Nevertheless, we have difficulty imagining a reason why such error would result in the pattern of results obtained. References (Not Included in Manuscript) Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurological Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 23, 56-62. Young, J. E., & Beck , A. T. (1988). Revision of the Cognitive Therapy Scale. Unpublished manuscript. University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.