*There*s an App for that* - An investigation into the Effect of Context

advertisement
1
A real-time examination of context effects on alcohol cognitions
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
Rebecca L. Monk and Derek Heim
of Edge Hill University, UK
Author Note
11
Rebecca Louise Monk and Derek Heim, Department of Psychology, Edge Hill
12
University, St. Helens Road, Ormskirk, Lancashire, L39 4QP, UK. Email:
13
monkre@edgehill.ac.uk; derek.heim@edgehill.ac.uk
14
15
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rebecca Monk,
16
Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, St. Helens Road, Ormskirk,
17
Lancashire, L39 4QP, UK. Email: monkre@edgehill.ac.uk. Tel: +44 (0)1695 65 0940
18
Word count: 3695
19
Running Head: Context effects on alcohol-related expectancies
20
21
1
22
“A real-time examination of context effects on alcohol cognitions”
23
24
Background: This research used context aware experiential sampling to investigate
25
the effect of contexts on in vivo alcohol-related outcome expectancies. Method: A
26
time-stratified random sampling strategy was adopted in order to assess 72 students
27
and young professionals at 5-daily intervals over the course of a week using a
28
specifically designed smart-phone application. This application recorded
29
respondents' present situational and social contexts, alcohol consumption and
30
alcohol-related cognitions in real-time. Results: In-vivo social and environmental
31
contexts and current alcohol consumption accounted for a significant proportion of
32
variance in outcome expectancies. For instance, prompts which occurred whilst
33
participants were situated in a pub, bar or club and in a social group of friends were
34
associated with heightened outcome expectancies in comparison with other settings.
35
Conclusion: Alcohol-related expectancies do not appear to be static but instead
36
demonstrate variation across social and environmental contexts. Modern technology
37
can be usefully employed to provide a more ecologically valid means of measuring
38
such beliefs.
Key Words: Alcohol, Social cognition, Social cognition models, Context,
Expectancies, Smartphone technology, Real-time sampling
39
40
41
2
42
Despite longstanding awareness that people's immediate environments mediate
43
behaviour (Bourdieu, 1977; Nyaronga, Greenfield, & McDaniel, 2009; Lott, 1996;
44
Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989), most psychological theories of behaviour and cognitions
45
are formulated upon data which are obtained without sufficient consideration of
46
contextual influences (Biglan & Hayes, 1996; Biglan, 2001; Hayes, 2004). When
47
using social cognition models to explain alcohol consumption this negligence might
48
constitute a critical oversight in view of long-documented contextual influences on
49
alcohol behaviours (MacAndrew & Edgerton, 1969).
50
51
Research indicates that alcohol-related beliefs predict consumption and, resultantly,
52
interventions have been designed to target these beliefs to reduce drinking (c.f. Jones
53
et al., 2001). Specifically, outcome expectancies – people’s beliefs about the likely
54
consequences of drinking have been found to impact both the quantity and frequency
55
of alcohol consumption (c.f. Ham & Hope, 2003; Oei & Morawska, 2004; Reich,
56
Below, & Goldman, 2010). Specifically, high positive outcome expectancies appear
57
to be associated with recurrent drinking in greater quantities (c.f. for example
58
Andersson et al., 2012), whilst higher negative expectancies seem to be associated
59
with reduced consumption (c.f. for example Stacy, Widaman, & Marlatt, 1990). While
60
it has also been noted for some time that outcome expectancies may vary across
61
different contexts (Wall, Mckee, & Hinson, 2000), this body of research has tended
62
to rely on single occasion testing and on retrospective self-reports obtained within
63
laboratory settings or non-alcohol-related environments (e.g. lecture theatres) without
64
adequate consideration of possible contextual influences (Monk & Heim 2013a; in
65
press). Accordingly, studies have begun to address these limitations by utilising more
66
ecologically aware testing environments such as simulated bars (e.g., Larsen, Engels,
3
67
Wiers, Granic, & Spijkerman, 2012) or wine tasting events (e.g., Kuendig &
68
Kuntsche, 2012), and recent findings suggest that social contexts and alcohol-related
69
environments are associated with increases in positive expectancies (Monk & Heim,
70
2013b; 2013c). While pointing to the importance of social and environmental contexts
71
in shaping alcohol-related beliefs, these studies have tended to test participants in
72
environments which, to a greater or lesser extent, are removed from real world
73
drinking contexts. The current study addresses this by using an experience sampling
74
method.
75
76
The increasing accessibility of advanced mobile devices (Katz & Aakus, 2002) has
77
facilitated the regular, day-to-day assessment of individuals in naturally diverging
78
contexts and has opened the field for Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) or
79
Experience Sampling (Collins, Lapp, Emmons, & Isaac, 1990; Collins et al., 1998;
80
Courvoisier, Eid, Lischetzke, & Schreiber, 2010; Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010;
81
Kuntsche & Robert, 2009). The present research used smartphone technology to
82
enable participants to provide real-time in vivo reports with a particular focus on
83
alcohol-related expectancies. In line with previous research (Monk & Heim, 2013a;
84
2013b; Wall et al., 2000; 2001; Wiers et al., 2003), it was predicted that there would
85
be an increase in alcohol-related expectancies when assessment occurred within
86
alcohol-related environments and in the presence of a social group (in comparison
87
with assessments that take place in alcohol neutral environments and in solitary
88
contexts).
4
89
Method
90
Design
91
A within participant design was utilised to investigate the effect of environmental and
92
social contexts on participant real-time responses to alcohol expectancy questions.
93
94
Participants
95
72 participants comprising students (n = 43) and young professionals (n = 29) who
96
were aged 18-34 years (M = 21.73, S.D = 3.64) were recruited for this study from
97
universities and businesses in the UK (North West). The majority of the sample were
98
White British (88.9%) and 69% of this sample were female. Baseline average AUDIT
99
scores were 9.02 (2.07) in the student sample and 8.72 (1.28) in the business sample.
100
101
Measures
102
Demographic information and reports regarding personal alcohol consumption
103
(AUDIT-C) were recorded at participants’ initial briefings. These were anonymously
104
combined with participants’ individual responses using a unique numeric identifier.
105
The
106
work/lecture, bar/pub/club, restaurant, sporting event, party or other) and social
107
contexts (alone, with one friend, with two or more fiends, with family, work
108
colleagues or other), whether they were drinking or had had a drink (yes or no), and if
109
so what they had been drinking (quantity). Furthermore, all participants answered a
110
random selection of items taken from the 34-item Alcohol Outcomes Expectancy
111
Questionnaire (Leigh & Stacy, 1993) which covers a range of outcomes, including
112
social, sexual and emotional outcomes. However, pilot studies (n = 42) which trialled
113
the administration of full and abridged versions of this questionnaire revealed that
smart-phone
application
ascertained
participants’
environment
(home,
5
114
participants were less likely to respond when all items were included. Furthermore, if
115
all of the 34 items had been available for random allocation, analyses would be
116
limited as any variation observed between contexts may have been the result of
117
variation in the expectancy measure presented (e.g. social vs. sexual expectancy
118
items). Resultantly, it was only the six social items that were part of the question pool
119
(three positive and three negative). In each response session, two positive and two
120
negative expectancy items were randomly selected from the question pool and
121
separate average scores for positive and negative expectancies were subsequently
122
calculated, giving a standardised maximum and minimum score of 1-6.
123
124
Equipment
125
A web based smart-phone application designed specifically for this research enabled
126
participants to respond to questioning via the use of their own mobile phone – when
127
prompted by automated SMS messages. The application was a website built using
128
HTML and JavaScript (JavaScript's jQuery mobile library) and answers were tracked
129
and stored using Google Analytics. The survey was designed to work on mobile
130
phones and native mobile browsers and was web-standards compliant. Each response
131
session was individually tracked and involved a personally interactive user experience
132
using tree based logic. For example, only those who responded that they consumed
133
alcohol were asked about what they had consumed. Participants’ response
134
mechanisms were also interactive, determined by the users’ smart-phone - for
135
example, Iphone or Android users could indicate their response by pressing or
136
‘dragging’ the onscreen response items whilst those without touch screen technology
137
responded in a fashion compatible with their phone (e.g., ‘scroll and click’). The
138
questions were randomly selected from the database of questions using a computer-
6
139
generated randomisation code. The application was designed to make the user
140
interface as
141
recommendations (c.f. Palmblad & Tiplady, 2004), there no default answers set..
intuitive/user friendly as
possible and,
in
accordance with
142
143
Procedure
144
Following ethical approval, participants were recruited and given a demonstration of
145
the response mechanism on their personal mobile phone. In accordance with similar
146
EMA procedures (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1992; Wichers et al., 2007) and
147
recommendations by Larson and Delespaul (1992), a time-stratified random sampling
148
strategy was adopted (c.f. Moberly & Watkins, 2008). Pilot questionnaire data
149
examining perceptions of online vs. real-time assessments (Response N = 108)
150
indicated that respondents preferred SMS reminders and that five daily prompts were
151
deemed the most acceptable number of daily participation requests. Therefore, the
152
volunteers received five randomly allocated SMS participation prompts every day for
153
one week. No two prompts could occur within 15 minutes (ibid) or outside 0800 -
154
2300 hours. Each day of participation was divided into five equal three hour periods
155
and one prompt was randomly sent within each period (e.g., once between 0800 and
156
1100, once between 11 and 1400 and so on). The exact time a participant was
157
prompted at was determined using a random number generator - each 3 hour section
158
was split into 15 minute blocks and the generator selected the time that the prompt
159
would be sent, making response sessions unpredictable Upon receiving the prompts,
160
participants activated the Application by clicking on a link provided in the SMS. The
161
questions provided were randomly selected from the question database in order to
162
prevent the order effects (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1992).
7
163
Average completion time was recorded at 2 minutes 27 seconds and the overall study
164
retention rate was 84.7%. Only relatively few participants completely stopped
165
responding and dropped out (n = 8). Furthermore, respondents were removed from
166
the sample (n = 3) where the response rate was below 40 percent, based on previous
167
research which indicates that low response rates on substance-use-related assessments
168
have low reliability (Shiffman, 2009).
169
Over the course of the week, there was the potential for participants to respond to 35
170
prompted sessions (5 per day for a week). There was no substantial increase in the
171
number of missed response sessions as interaction with the application increased,
172
suggesting that order effects were limited by the use of this technology. The average
173
percentage of failed responses (sessions which were not completed following a
174
prompt) was 20% per participant, with the 0800-1100 time-slot eliciting the highest
175
number of late or failed responses. The average percentage of late responses (> 15
176
minutes post prompt) was 5% per participant and these late responses were excluded
177
from subsequent analyses in order to ensure that the results could reasonably be
178
asserted to be a representative account of the specific time as opposed to a
179
retrospective report (Delespaul, 1995). The study therefore had an average overall
180
valid response rate of 75% per participant (26 out of a total possible 35 prompts
181
responded to).
182
Analytic Strategy
183
Multilevel modelling (MLM) is a method of statistical analyses which is capable of
184
advanced portioning of variance (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). MLM was used as this
185
technique can incorporate the natural complex (and related) nature of the data (Heck,
186
Thomas, & Tabata, 2010) and look for explained and unexplained variance both
187
between and within groups (see Goldstein, 2011). MLM is also able to deal with
8
188
missing data which are to be expected in experiential sampling (Tabachnick & Fidell,
189
2001). In the present study variances in outcome expectancies (the dependent
190
variable) were modelled at a number of levels: Prompts were nested within days
191
which were nested within participants. However, given that data were not recorded at
192
the day level (e.g. day, weather etc), it was decided that this level did not warrant
193
inclusion within the statistical modelling. Indeed, the day of the week in which
194
participants began the research was not consistent in this study (participants chose
195
their most personally convenient starting point). This meant that no specific predictors
196
required modelling at this level and the lack of information at this level may have
197
unduly reduced the overall explanatory power of the model. A series of 2 level
198
random intercept multilevel models (prompts within participants) were therefore fitted
199
– one for each of the alcohol-related cognitions (positive and negative outcome
200
expectancies). MLM therefore allowed analysis of variance at the prompt level
201
(context factors) and the person level (individual differences). The resultant
202
hierarchical random intercept multilevel model was fitted with predictor variables
203
which were justified by correlational analyses (see Table 1). Preliminary analyses
204
revealed no evidence of multicollinearity, residuals were normally distributed and
205
scatterplots indicated that the assumption of linearity and homoscedasticity were met.
206
The MLM was designed to portion variance in outcome expectancies and the
207
predicted variance from the null and fitted models were compared in each case. Table
208
1 outlines the correlational analyses and the findings of these analyses were used to
209
inform the subsequent MLMs. Any variable which significantly correlated with at
210
least one of the dependent variables was included.
9
211
Results
212
Full random intercept MLMs were calculated, one with positive expectancies as the
213
dependent variable and another for negative expectancies. Predictor variables were
214
imputed at both levels (as specified in Table 1): Prompt level variables (j social
215
context, environmental context, alcohol consumption - yes or no, and number of
216
drinks), and individual level predictors (ij age, gender, ethnicity, student/professional
217
status and raw (as opposed to therapeutic categories) AUDIT scores were used for
218
analyses. In all analyses, binary variables (Gender, 1 = female; Student/Professional
219
status, 1 = student ; Ethnicity, 1 = white British; Alcohol Consumption, 1 = yes) were
220
dummy coded (for a more easily interpretable outcome), the two categorical
221
predictors (environmental and social context) were dummy coded using Home and
222
Alone conditions as the respective reference categories (k-1), and the remaining
223
variable were left as continuous variables (Positive expectancies, Negative
224
expectancies, Age, AUDIT, Number of drinks).
225
INSERT TABLE 1 HERE
226
How much variance in positive and negative outcome expectancies is found and can
227
be subsequently explained at the individual level (variance between participants) and
228
the group level (prompt level, variance between prompts/within participants)?
229
Empty models (also known as the variance component models - models without
230
imputed predictor variables) indicated that there was a significant proportion of
231
variance (ICC = 95.55%) to be explained at the prompt (μ0j = 3.68, p < .001) and the
232
individual level (ICC = 4.41%, μ0ij = .17, p < .01). The same was also true of
233
negative expectancies, with 46.36% (μ0j = .61, p < .001) and 19.74% (μ0ij = .15, p <
234
.01) of unexplained variability being identified at the prompt and the individual levels
235
respectively. 2* log likelihood statistics (using chi square) and ICC calculations
10
236
revealed that the full positive expectancy model resulted in a significant reduction of
237
unexplained variance (χ² (30, n = 61) = 978.06, p < .001), explaining 36.7%.and
238
35.3% of the identified variability in positive expectancies at the prompt and
239
individual levels respectively. The negative expectancy model also produced
240
significant reduction in the amount of unexplained variance (χ² = (9, n = 61) = 575.88,
241
p < .001), with 22.95% and 15.38% of variance in negative expectancies being
242
explained at the prompt and individual levels respectively.
243
Which predictors are significant predictors of variance in expectancies?
244
No single individual level predictor was significant within the MLM model of
245
negative expectancies. However, for positive expectancies, the only individual level
246
predictor that was significant was student/professional status (β0ij = -.23, p < .01),
247
such that being a young professional was associated with reduced positive
248
expectancies to a significant degree, whilst being a university student was associated
249
with an increase in positive expectancies. At the prompt level, having consumed
250
alcohol within the last hour of prompting was a significant predictor of both increased
251
positive (β0j = -.82, p < .001) and negative expectancies (β0j = -.51, p < .001) whilst
252
number of drinks was not a significant predictor of positive expectancies but it did
253
negatively predict variance in negative expectancies (β0j = -.09, p < .001). This
254
suggests that any level of alcohol consumption may increase both positive and
255
negative expectancies. Nonetheless,, whilst the number of drinks did not appear to
256
alter positive beliefs (they remained heightened during consumption), negative beliefs
257
began to decrease as alcohol consumption increased.
258
Both prompt level categorical predictor variables (social and environmental context)
259
were also significant predictors of positive and negative outcome expectancies.
11
260
Specifically, responses whilst situated within alcohol-related contexts including bars
261
(β0j = -.52, p < .05), parties (β0j = -.91, p < .01) and sporting events (β0j = - .79, p <
262
.05) were associated with increased positive expectancies. Similarly, negative
263
expectancies were significantly predicted by being in a bar/pub/club (β0j = -.25, p <
264
.01), although sporting and party venues did not account for significant variance.
265
Being at a friend or family member’s house was also a significant predictor of
266
increased positive (β0j = -1.10, p < .001) and negative expectancies (β0j = -.67, p <
267
.001). Being at work was also a significant predictor of positive (β0j = .61, p < .01)
268
and negative expectancies (β0j = -.28, p < .05). Here, being outside of work was
269
associated with an increase in positive expectancies, and a decrease in negative
270
expectancies. Being at home during responses was the reference category for both
271
expectancy types and this context also appeared to be associated with decreased
272
positive and negative expectancies..
273
The social context sub-categories also varied to a statistically significant degree.
274
Prompts that occurred whilst participants were with 1 friend (β0j = -1.78, p < .001:
275
β0j = -.74, p < .001), 2 or more friends (β0j = -1.75, p < .001: β0j = -.84, p < .001) or
276
family members (β0j = -1.10, p < .001: β0j = -.79, p < .001) were significant
277
predictors associated with increases in positive and negative expectancies
278
respectively. However, being with work colleagues predicted significant decreases in
279
positive expectancies (β0j = .72, p < .05) and increases in negative expectancies (β0j
280
= -.43, p < .001). Being alone during responses was the reference category for both
281
expectancies categories, meaning that this context also appears to be associated with
282
decreased expectancies. The ‘other’ response for social context was also a significant
283
predictor of positive expectancies (β0j = 2.44, p < .01) but the large standard error
284
here (.92) suggests a high degree of variability in participants’ responses in this
12
285
category, perhaps due to the diversity of contexts captured by this response. Any
286
attempt to interpret this finding without any further contextual information would
287
therefore be unwise.
288
Discussion
289
With the aim of conducting an ecologically valid assessment of the impact social and
290
environmental contexts have on outcome expectancies, this study used smart-phone
291
technology to conduct context aware experiential sampling. Social and environmental
292
contexts, specifically, being in a pub, bar or club, were significant predictors of both
293
increased positive and negative outcome expectancies. The same pattern was observed
294
for social contexts including being with a friend, with two or more friends and with
295
family members. Being at work or at home, and being with work colleagues or alone
296
was associated with a reverse pattern of results, whereby these contexts were
297
associated with decreased expectancies. In accordance with previous lab (e.g., Wall et
298
al., 2000; 2001) and field research (e.g., LaBrie et al., 2011), these findings provide
299
real-time support for the assertion that alcohol-related environmental contexts are
300
associated with changes in cognition – specifically, changes in the anticipated
301
consequences of alcohol consumption. It was particularly interesting to note that,
302
against expectations, negative as well as positive expectancies increased in alcohol-
303
related environments and in social group contexts. In studies of problem and non-
304
problem drinkers, alcohol-related cues (their favourite alcoholic drink) have been
305
shown to create both positive and negative expectations and physiological arousal
306
(Cooney, Gillespie, Baker, & Kaplan, 1987). These results suggest that in vivo
307
contextual cues can trigger both positive and negative beliefs (c.f. Wall et al., 2000;
308
Wiers et al., 2003) and underline the current findings that both positive and negative
309
expectancies increased when participants were in social groups and alcohol-related
13
310
environments. The importance of the relationship between social and environmental
311
contexts and the decision to drink or exercise restraint is also affirmed by the current
312
findings (Andersson et al., 2013; Lau-Barraco & Linden, 2014). It has been suggested
313
that interventions need to be able to target the context-dependent nature of substance
314
use and associated beliefs in order to be successful (Biglan & Hayes, 1996; Davies,
315
1997). The current research may therefore offer insights towards the improvement of
316
therapeutic practice, by increasing our ability to target the contextual-varying beliefs
317
which are associated with alcohol consumption. Any level of alcohol consumption
318
alcohol within the last hour was also associated with increases in both positive and
319
negative expectancies respectively. However, number of drinks was only a significant
320
predicator of decreased negative expectancies. Therefore, whilst positive expectancies
321
appear to remain heightened regardless of the level of alcohol consumed, greater
322
levels of consumption may be associated with subsequent decreases in negative
323
beliefs. This suggests that real-time alcohol consumption is associated with a
324
reduction of the invivo cognitions which are related to restraint (c.f. Baldwin, Oei, &
325
Young, 1993). Conversely, consumption appears to increase the positive beliefs which
326
are associated with drinking (c.f. Reich et al., 2010).
327
Whilst AUDIT scores did correlate with positive expectancies, being a university
328
student was the only demographic variable which, on its own, was a significant
329
predictor of increased positive outcome expectancies. Therefore, whilst the majority
330
of expectancy research relies on student samples, using a non-student sample with a
331
comparable age may produce different results (lower average expectancy scores).
332
Indeed, age was a not a significant predictor in the study which may suggest that there
333
are aspects of the student experience which create deviations in expectancies in
334
comparison to those of the same age who are not students. This pattern of results is in
14
335
line with suggestions that there is a ‘culture of drinking’ at University which
336
moderates students’’ expectancies (Borsari & Carey, 2001). Future research may
337
therefore benefit from greater inclusion of non-student participants.
338
339
As responses that did not occur within 15 minutes of the participation prompt were
340
discarded, the current findings can be reasonably believed to be representative of real-
341
time cognitions. This removes the problems noted in previous EMA research (c.f.
342
Kuntesche & Labhart, 2012) where a lack of signal or power may delay prompts, thus
343
increasing the reliance on the participant’s memory and potentially limiting response
344
reliability. Nevertheless, it remains possible that a lack of signal or power of
345
respondents’ mobiles may have resulted in some data loss in the current research,
346
although the high response rate for this study suggests that this is likely to have been
347
minimal. It must also be noted that whilst the participation window of 0800-2300 was
348
selected in order to maximise responses, future research may be improved by
349
exploring the feasibility of responses beyond 2300. This would allow assessments of
350
late night/early morning drinking practices and may further elucidate complex
351
cognitive processes. Furthermore, it should be noted that participants’ intoxication
352
levels may have impaired/hindered responses (cf. Fromme, Katz, & D’Amico, 1997;
353
Hindmarch, Kerr, & Sherwood, 1991 LaBrie et al., 2011). While such effects may
354
mirror real-life situations, a degree of caution is nonetheless advised when
355
considering the current findings.
356
357
In conclusion the present research confirms concerns about the abundant previous
358
research which is conducted with participants who are assessed alone, in non alcohol-
359
related environments and are sober during the completion of their questionnaires. In
15
360
particular, the results of the current investigation indicate that responses which were
361
recorded in solitary contexts and when in alcohol-neutral environments (such as at
362
work or at home) were associated with lower expectancies. As specified, alcohol
363
consumption was also associated with changes in responses. These results therefore
364
suggest that previous research in this field may have captured responses which do not
365
necessarily equate to cognitions in real-life situations. Here, the use of smart-phone
366
technology to conduct real-time, context aware experiential sampling appears to offer
367
a viable solution to this issue. Findings from this research may also provide a
368
promising avenue to pursue for the development of context-sensitive interventions.
369
16
370
References
371
Andersson, C., Sundh, V., Waern, M., Jakobsson, A., Lissner, L., & Spak, F. (2012).
372
Drinking context and problematic alcohol consumption in young Swedish
373
women. Addiction Research & Theory, 21, 457-468.
374
375
Baldwin, A.R., Oei, T.P.S., & Young, R.D. (1993). To drink or not to drink: The
376
differential role of alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self efficacy in
377
quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption. Cognitive Therapy and
378
Research, 17, 511-529.
379
380
Biglan, A. (2001). Contextualism and the development of effective prevention
practices. Prevention Science, 5, 15-21.
381
Biglan, A., & Hayes, S.C. (1996). Should the behavioral sciences become more
382
pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on human
383
behaviour. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 5, 47-57.
384
385
386
387
388
Borsari, B., & Carey, K.B. (2001). Peer influences of college drinking: A review of
the research. Journal of Substance Abuse, 13, 391-424.
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge, London: Cambridge
University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1992). Validity and reliability of the experience
389
sampling method. In: M. deVries (Ed.) The Experience of Psychology:
390
Investigating mental disorders in their natural settings (pp. 43-57). Cambridge:
391
Cambridge University Press.
17
392
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
393
Collins, R.L., Lapp, W.M., Emmons, K.M., & Isaac, L.M. (1990). Endorsement and
394
strength of alcohol expectancies. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 336-342.
395
Collins, R.L., Morsheimer, E.T., Shiffman, S., Paty, J.A., Gnys, M., & Papandonatos,
396
G.D. (1998). Ecological momentary assessment in a behavioral drinking
397
moderation training program. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,
398
6, 306-315.
399
Cooney, N. L., Gillespie, R. A., Baker, L. H., & Kaplan, R. F. (1987). Cognitive
400
changes after alcohol cue exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
401
Psychology, 55, 150-155.
402
Courvoisier, D.S., Eid, M., Lischetzke, T., & Schreiber, W.H. (2010). Psychometric
403
properties of a computerized mobile phone method for assessing mood in daily
404
life. Emotion, 10, 115-124.
405
406
407
Delespaul, P. (1995). Assessing Schizophrenia in Daily Life. Maastricht University
Press: Maastricht.
Fromme, K., Katz, E., & D’Amico, E. (1997). Effects of alcohol intoxication on the
408
perceived consequences of risk taking. Experimental and Clinical
409
Psychopharmacology, 5, 14–23.
410
Goldstein, H. (2011). Multilevel statistical models. Chichester: Wiley.
411
Ham, L. S., & Hope, D. A. (2003) College students and problematic drinking: A
412
review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 719-759.
18
413
Hayes, S.C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory,
414
and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35,
415
639-665.
416
417
Heck, R.H., Thomas, S.L., & Tabata, L.N. (2010). Multilevel and Longitudinal
Modeling with IBM SPSS. London: Routledge.
418
Hindmarch, I., Kerr, J. S., & Sherwood, N. (1991). The effects of alcohol and other
419
drugs on psychomotor performance and cognitive function. Alcohol and
420
Alcoholism, 26, 71–79.
421
422
423
Jones, B.T., Corbin, W., & Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and
alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96, 57-72.
Katz, J.E., & Aakus, M. (2002). Framing the issues. In: J.E. Katz & M. Aakhus (Eds).
424
Perpetual Contact: Mobile communication, private talk, public performance.
425
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
426
427
Killingsworth, M.A. & Gilbert, D.T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind.
Science, 330, 932-940.
428
Kuendig, H., & Kuntsche, E. (2012). Solitary versus vocial drinking: An experimental
429
study on effects of social exposures on in situ alcohol consumption. Alcoholism:
430
Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 732-738.
431
Kuntsche, E., & Labhart, F. (2012). ICAT: development of an Internet-based data
432
collection method for ecological momentary assessment using personal cell
433
phones. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 1-9.
434
435
Kuntsche, E., & Robert, B. (2009). Short message service (SMS) technology in
alcohol research–a feasibility study. Alcohol, 44, 423-428.
19
436
LaBrie, J. W., Grant, S., & Hummer, J. F. (2011). “This would be better drunk”:
437
Alcohol expectancies become more positive while drinking in the college social
438
environment. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 890–893.
439
Larsen, H., Engels, R.C., Wiers, R.W., Granic, I., & Spijkerman, R. (2012). Implicit
440
and explicit alcohol cognitions and observed alcohol consumption: three studies
441
in (semi) naturalistic drinking settings. Addiction, 107, 1420-1428.
442
Larson, R., & Delespaul, P. (1992). Analyzing experience sampling data: A
443
guidebook for the perplexed. In: M. deVries (Ed.) The Experience of
444
Psychology: Investigating mental disorders in their natural settings (pp. 58-78).
445
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
446
447
Lau-Barraco, C., & Linden, A. N. (2014). Drinking buddies: Who are they and when
do they matter? Addiction Research & Theory, 22, 57-67
448
Leigh, B.C., & Stacy (1993). Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and
449
predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological
450
Assessment, 5, 216-229.
451
452
453
454
Lott, B. (1996). Politics or science? The question of gender sameness/ difference.
American Psychologist, 51, 155-156.
MacAndrew, C., & Edgerton, R. (1969). Drunken Comportment: A Social
Explanation. Aldine, Chicago.
455
Moberly, N.J., & Watkins, E.R. (2008). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An
456
experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 314-323.
20
457
Monk, R.L., & Heim, D. (in press). A systematic review of the Alcohol Norms
458
literature: A focus on context. Drugs: Education, Prevention & Policy.
459
Monk, R.L., & Heim, D. (2013a). Environmental context effects on alcohol-related
460
outcome expectancies, efficacy and norms: A field study. Psychology of
461
Addictive Behaviors, 27, 814-818.
462
463
Monk, R.L., & Heim, D. (2013b). A critical systematic review of alcohol-related
outcome expectancies. Substance Use and Misuse, 48, 539-557.
464
Monk, R.L., & Heim, D. (2013c). Panoramic projection: Affording a wider view on
465
contextual influences on alcohol-related cognitions. Experimental and Clinical
466
Psychopharmacology, 21, 1-7.
467
Nyaronga, D., Greenfield, T.K., & McDaniel, P.A. (2009). Drinking context and
468
drinking problems among black, white, and hispanic men and women in the
469
1984, 1995, and 2005 U.S. national alcohol surveys. Journal of Studies on
470
Alcohol and Drugs, 70, 16-26.
471
Oei, T.P.S., & Morawska, A. (2004). A cognitive model of binge drinking: The
472
influence of alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self-efficacy. Addictive
473
Behaviors, 29, 159-174.
474
Palmblad, M. & Tiplady, B. (2004). Electronic diaries and questionnaires: Designing
475
user interfaces that are easy for all patients to use. Quality of Life Research, 13,
476
1199-1207.
21
477
Reich, R.R., Below, M.C., & Goldman, M.S. (2010). Explicit and implicit measures
478
of expectancy and related alcohol cognitions: A meta-analytic comparison.
479
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24, 13-25.
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
Rosnow, R.L., & Rosenthal, R. (1989). Statistical procedures and the justification of
knowledge in psychological science. American Psychologist, 44, 1276-1284.
Shiffman, S. (2009). Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) in Studies of
Substance Use. Psychological Assessment, 21, 486-497.
Stacy, A.W, Widaman, K.F., & Marlatt, G.A. (1990). Expectancy models of alcohol
use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 918-928.
Tabachnik, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. NewYork:
Harper & Row.
488
Wall, A.M., Hinson, R.E., McKee, S.A., & Goldstein, A. (2001). Examining alcohol
489
outcome expectancies in laboratory and naturalistic bar settings: A within-
490
subjects experimental analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 219-226.
491
Wall, A.M., Mckee, S.A., & Hinson, R.E. (2000). Assessing variation in alcohol
492
outcome expectancies across environmental context: An examination of the
493
situational-specificity hypothesis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14, 367-
494
375.
495
Wiers, R.W., Wood, M.D., Darkes, J., Corbin, W.R., Jones, B.T., & Sher, K.J. (2003).
496
Changing expectancies: Cognitive mechanisms and context effects. Alcoholism-
497
Clinical and Experimental Research, 27, 186-197.
22
498
499
500
Table 1
Bivariate correlations between mean alcohol-related cognitions and all predicator
variables.^
1. Positive Expect.
2. Negative Expect.
3. Environmental
Context
4. Social Context
5. Student/ Young
Professional (YP = 0)
6.Ethnic (Non White British
=0)
7.Gender (Male = 0)
8. Age
9. AUDIT
10.Consumed Alcohol (No=
0)
11. Number drinks
consumed
1
.71**
.67**
2
3
4
5
.48**
-
.59**
-.09**
.50**
-.10**
.02
.59**
.09**
.17*
-
-.04*
.04*
.05*
.10**
-
.09**
-.04
.00
.50**
.04
.08**
.04*
.26**
.01
.05*
.05*
.66**
.01
.14**
-.02
.31**
-.07**
.70**
.00
.09
.50**
.28**
.63**
.63**
.30**
501
** p < .01 * p < .05
502
503
504
505
^
6
7
8
9
10
.49**
.27**
.12**
.37**
-.22**
-.04
.22**
-.23**
.06**
.01
-
.07**
.04
.03
.04
.04
It may be noted that a number of these correlations are significant but are not
sufficient to be deemed strong (r = .07). However, these weak effects may be an issue
of sample size, whereby the ability to detect effects is only improved when sample
sizes are increased (Cohen, 1992).
506
507
508
509
23
11
-
Download