True navigation in birds: from quantum physics to global

advertisement
1
True navigation in birds: from quantum physics to global migration
2
Richard A. Holland
3
School of Biological Sciences, Queen’s University of Belfast, 97 Lisburn Road, BT9 7BL,
4
UK.
5
Short title: Bird navigation
6
Abstract
7
Birds are capable of true navigation, the ability to return to a known goal from place they have
8
never visited before. This is demonstrated most spectacularly during the vast migratory journeys
9
made by these animals year after year often between continents and occasionally global in
10
nature. However, it remains one of the great unanswered questions in science, despite more than
11
50 years of research in this field. Nevertheless, the study of true navigation in birds has made
12
significant advances in the previous 20 years, in part thanks to the integration of many
13
disciplines outside its root in behavioural biology, to address questions of neurobiology,
14
molecular aspects and the physics of sensory systems and environmental cues involved in bird
15
navigation, often involving quantum physics. However, true navigation remains a controversial
16
field, with many conflicting and confusing results making interpretation difficult, particularly for
17
those outside or new to the field. Unlike many general texts on migration, which avoid
18
discussion of these issues, this review will present these conflicting findings and assess the state
19
of the field of true navigation during bird migration.
20
Keywords: Navigation, Migration, Orientation, Bird, Magnetoreception, Olfaction, Map
21
22
Introduction
23
The apparent ability of migratory birds to make journeys of thousands of miles, crossing deserts,
24
oceans and mountain ranges, sometimes even circumnavigating the globe has long fascinated
25
both scientists and laymen alike. Fifty years of intensive research on the mechanisms and
26
sensory cues required have revealed much about the way birds can achieve this feat of navigation
27
with such precision but also leaves many open questions and the field is one that is seen as beset
28
with controversy over conflicting results (Alerstam, 2006). Recently, this problem was described
29
as a “chronic disease” (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012), suggesting that the field is unhealthy, in a
30
scientific sense, and data should not be trusted. The “mystery” of how birds navigate continues
31
to be alluded to both in popular and professional media (Baker, 1984, HollandThorup and
32
Wikelski, 2007), and remains one of the great unanswered questions in science (Kennedy and
33
Norman, 2005) but in the last 20 years animal navigation has taken huge strides forwards by
34
becoming a truly interdisciplinary field. Researchers from physical, chemical, histological,
35
neuropysiological and electrophysiological disciplines all contribute to our understanding of bird
36
navigation and a researcher working in this field must now cast their literature search far wider
37
than the traditional behaviour focused journals that the early work was published in. This
38
combination of a difficulty in interpreting conflicting results and the diverse fields which
39
contribute to our understanding of bird navigation may make a daunting prospect for those new
40
to the subject. It is thus the aim of this review to assess these conflicting results and integrate the
41
new information from other disciplines from the perspective of a behavioural biologist working
42
at the level of the organism, in order to make the field more accessible to new scientists entering
43
the field from this area, and while remaining critical, present a positive outlook for the field of
44
bird navigation. Finally, it will identify the key questions that remain in true navigation in birds
45
that must be tackled if the subject is to be resolved.
46
Migratory true navigation
47
What is true navigation?
48
Donald Griffin was the first to conceptualise bird navigation (Griffin, 1952) and he recognised a
49
specific form of navigational challenge, which he defined “type III”, in which the bird was able
50
to return to a goal after being displaced (even artificially) to an unknown area. Subsequently the
51
term “true navigation” was adopted to describe this by Keeton (Keeton, 1974), although Keeton
52
used it as a term to describe all forms of orientation and navigation from unfamiliar area that
53
were not explained by other processes. This was problematic as true navigation was defined by
54
that which could not be explained by other means, rather than as a testable hypothesis (Wiltschko
55
and Wiltschko, 2003). However, over time a workable hypothesis for true navigation emerged as
56
a number of consistent definitions acknowledged true navigation to be the ability to return to a
57
known goal using only cues detected locally, not by cues detected during the displacement e.g.
58
(Able, 2001, Phillips, 1996, Papi, 1992, PhillipsSchmidt-Koenig and Muheim, 2006). The most
59
current definition of true navigation is “the ability of an animal to return to its original location
60
after displacement to a site in unfamiliar territory, without access to familiar landmarks, goal
61
emanating cues, or information about the displacement route” (Phillips et al., 2006). This
62
definition does not specifically recognise migratory navigation however, in which the displaced
63
animal may not be navigating to its original location prior to displacement (i.e. homing) but a
64
final breeding or wintering area that it did not set out from. Hereafter this is defined as migratory
65
true navigation: the ability of an animal to navigate to a specific breeding or wintering area (that
66
it has not just set out from) following displacement.
67
Evidence for migratory true navigation
68
Initially, a series of displacement experiments on migrating birds using mark/recapture
69
techniques gathered evidence for true navigation (reviewed in (Thorup and Holland, 2009)). The
70
clearest example (Perdeck, 1958) demonstrated that adult but not juvenile birds are capable of
71
migratory true navigation. More recent studies have shown that adult, but not juvenile white
72
crowned sparrows are able to head towards their winter area within the first 100km of departure
73
from the site of displacement of 3700km from their normal route during autumn migration
74
(Thorup et al., 2007), and that reed warblers can correct for displacements of 1000km during
75
their first return migrating to their previous natal area (ChernetsovKishkinev and Mouritsen,
76
2008). Migratory true navigation is thus experience based, i.e. an ability to correct and return to a
77
known goal from an unfamiliar place is a consequence of information learned on a previous
78
journey to, or from that goal (figure 1). The test of true navigation is thus being able to correct
79
after displacement to a novel location. A few studies suggest that juvenile birds may in some
80
circumstances appear to make corrections for displacements (Thorup et al., 2011, Thorup and
81
Rabøl, 2007, Thorup and Rabøl, 2001, Åkesson et al., 2005), but it is not clear whether this is the
82
result of homing to a known goal along the migratory route (e.g. the last known stopover site or
83
the natal area) or part of an inherited programme that allows them to compensate for
84
displacements. Such a mechanism has been described in sea turtles (Putman et al., 2011), but it
85
remains to be seen whether either of these mechanisms exist, or the more common viewpoint of
86
an inherited compass direction is the only mechanism juveniles possess.
87
What is less often cited are the failures of displaced birds to correct for a displacement.
88
For instance, a repeat of Perdeck’s study in which adult birds were displaced to Spain did not
89
indicate that the birds could correct their orientation and return to the species winter area
90
(Perdeck, 1967). White and Golden crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii and Z.
91
altricapila) that were translocated from the USA to Korea from their winter grounds did not
92
appear to return to the USA (MewaldtCowley and Won, 1973). The fact that some birds make
93
vast migrations that are global in nature is often used to argue that true navigation ability must
94
also be global (Bingman and Cheng, 2006), but these studies suggest that there may be limits to
95
the extent of a migratory true navigation ability at least in the animals studied. Whether
96
migratory true navigation ability varies with migration distance, or has a general limit in all birds
97
is not yet known, but current evidence does suggest variation, with the results of Thorup et al.
98
(2007) indicating at least a 3700km range, while it appears shorter in Starlings, possibly in the
99
region of 2000km (Perdeck, 1967).
100
The theory of migratory true navigation.
101
As the previous paragraph demonstrates, displacement experiments provide evidence for true
102
navigation ability, but not for how they achieve it. Although not developed to describe migratory
103
true navigation specifically, the “map and compass” hypothesis was developed by Kramer to
104
explain navigation behaviour by a process that comprises 2 steps, determining position with
105
respect to the goal, “the map step” and determining direction to a goal, “the compass step”
106
(Kramer, 1953). The map and compass theory has remained the most robust explanation for
107
animal true navigation since its inception and no significant challenge to the idea that animal
108
navigation is a two-step process has been made. True navigation ability refers specifically to the
109
“map” step, the ability to locate position with respect to a goal. Experienced birds are presumed
110
to possess a “navigational map” that allows them to locate their position with respect to a final
111
goal and navigate towards it using their compass sense. One theory proposed that the map might
112
work in a way akin to our Cartesian coordinate system, with animals able to refer to
113
environmental gradients that vary predictably with latitude and longitude (figure 2). For these
114
gradients to be usable, the animal would have to learn that they vary predictably in intensity with
115
space (and possibly time) within their home range and extrapolate this beyond the learned area
116
(Wallraff, 1991, Wallraff, 1974). Thus when displaced to an unfamiliar area the animal could
117
recognise a value in the gradients that was, for example, higher than the home range and
118
recognise its displacement relative to it. For a migratory bird, the presumption is that this process
119
of learning these values occurs before departing on the first migration for the breeding area, and
120
during the first winter for the winter area. Thus, migratory birds are presumed to learn the value
121
of gradients at 2 goals. This gradient map tends to be thought of as a 2 cue system, often
122
presuming that a different environmental cue provides the longitude and latitude equivalents.
123
However, it has occasionally been suggested that different aspects of the same environmental cue
124
could form those two gradients (e.g. sun’s arc and sunrise time (Matthews, 1953), intensity and
125
slope or inclination of the magnetic field (Walker, 1998, BoströmÅkesson and Alerstam, 2012)).
126
Thus, we know that migratory birds can perform true navigation, and we have a theoretical
127
construct for how they could achieve this, but how do we study the nature of the environmental
128
cues and sensory systems required to achieve true navigation?
129
Studying migratory true navigation
130
The study of true navigation requires either displacement of the animal outside its familiar area,
131
or a simulated displacement where an environmental cue is manipulated to represent a different
132
location than the one currently experienced. The former requires the ability to study the response
133
to the displacement in the field, and the latter requires that the animal shows behaviour in the
134
laboratory that correlates with orientation decisions in the wild. First, a laboratory based
135
correlate of migratory orientation exists in the form of migratory restlessness (Emlen and Emlen,
136
1966). This has been used with great success to investigate the nature of the magnetic compass
137
sense in migrants (Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1972, Ritz et al., 2004, Wiltschko et al., 1993,
138
Zapka et al., 2009). Orientation cages provide the potential for greater control as they can be
139
performed indoors. Surprisingly, orientation cage studies have not been used as extensively to
140
investigate the cues used in the navigational map as they have the compass, despite the fact that
141
testing orientation after displacement has been shown to be possible (Thorup and Rabøl, 2007,
142
Chernetsov et al., 2008) as are simulated displacement experiments (Fransson et al., 2001,
143
Henshaw et al., 2010, DeutchlanderPhillips and Munro, 2012). Second, recently there have been
144
calls for a return to field based study of true navigation in migratory birds (Guilford et al., 2011,
145
Thorup et al., 2010, Wikelski et al., 2007). This stems from concerns that migratory restlessness
146
does not fully represent the behaviour of animals in the wild (Wikelski et al., 2007), and that we
147
do not understand the full extent of the challenges that animals face during migration (Holland et
148
al., 2007). Animal movements in the wild can now be tracked using remote monitoring devices
149
that provide the precision that was lacking in mark recapture techniques. In some cases GPS
150
precision is available and remote download from a satellite can be achieved (see (Bridge et al.,
151
2011) for review of currently available technology for tracking migratory birds). However,
152
tracking devices that can follow a migratory journey with sufficient precision to test navigational
153
decisions are still too large for the small songbirds that remain the focus of much of true
154
navigation in migration. As tracking of migratory birds becomes more widespread, our
155
understanding of the navigational challenges faced by both adults and juveniles will increase
156
which will undoubtedly aid in adapting the theories of true navigation (Guilford et al., 2011).
157
However, field based study of wild birds faces the same inherent weaknesses as field based study
158
in the other model systems, in that control of access to cues is difficult. Field based study of
159
migration faces the added difficulty of predicting both the timing of departure and goal of the
160
animals. The former may cause problems in predicting the effect of treatments of sensory
161
systems particularly when they are transitory and the latter may increase the scatter in
162
experimental groups, meaning an increase in the number of animals needed. Given that tracking
163
technology remains relatively expensive and studies are often restricted by the number of devices
164
available, this may lead to inconsistencies in results through lack of statistical power. Such
165
studies are thus relatively rare, with no study of migratory true navigation using GPS telemetry
166
having yet been published. The field thus relies on two imperfect systems, a laboratory correlate
167
that provides precision and control, but which has limits in its relevance to natural behaviour,
168
and a field based system that is logistically difficult and lacks sufficient power at present.
169
The role of environmental cues in true navigation
170
The sensory basis of the true navigation map contributes significantly to bird navigation’s
171
reputation as a controversial field. Many general reviews of migration that include a chapter on
172
navigation avoid discussion of this sub-topic altogether (e.g. (Newton, 2007, Dingle, 1996)).
173
Repeatability continues to dog the field and certainly, interpreting findings where no effect of a
174
treatment is obtained is problematic. However, simply ignoring the large amount of research that
175
has attempted to elucidate the sensory basis of true navigation does a disservice to the field.
176
Without an understanding of research that has attempted to understand this, advances cannot be
177
made. The remainder of this review will thus assess the experimental evidence for sensory cues
178
in migratory bird navigation, in the hope that understanding what has been tried, what has failed
179
and what is incomplete will aid in moving towards a resolution for this field.
180
Celestial cues
181
It has been proposed that animals could use celestial cues for navigation (Matthews, 1953,
182
Matthews, 1951, Pennycuick, 1960). Both the sun and stars can provide a cue to north-south
183
position because the zenith varies with latitude. Longitudinal displacement could potentially be
184
detected if they were able to recognise that sun or star rise time was different from that at the
185
goal site. What is more, these provide a global reference frame and so in theory the animal’s
186
position could be located anywhere on the Earth so long as a view of the cue was available.
187
However, both sun and star navigation are generally rejected based on two factors. First, tests on
188
homing pigeons have demonstrated that they have a time compensated sun compass that can be
189
manipulated by shifting their internal clock (Schmidt-KoenigGanzhorn and Ranvaud, 1991,
190
Schmidt-Koenig, 1960). This rejects sun navigation on two counts. First, it suggests that the
191
birds (or at least homing pigeons) do not note the altitude of the sun, or they would not be fooled
192
by the shifts in their internal clock and thus do not use it as a cue to latitude. Second, a 6 hour
193
forward shift in the internal clock leads to a deflection of approximately 90° counter clockwise
194
(i.e. to the west), matching the rate of movement of the sun across the sky. This is not consistent
195
with the use of the sun as a cue to longitude, which would be perceived as a displacement of
196
approximately 5000km to the west (i.e. the bird would need to fly east to return home). It has
197
been argued that such displacements are unrealistic to a homing pigeon, and so a six hour shift is
198
an unrealistic test of the sun navigation hypothesis (Pennycuick, 1961). However, subsequent
199
tests involving much smaller shifts were also consistent with sun compass but not sun navigation
200
(Walcott and Michener, 1971). On this basis sun navigation has been rejected (Baker, 1984).
201
Clock shift has also been demonstrated in migratory birds in orientation cages, which might
202
suggest it should be rejected for migratory true navigation (Able and Cherry, 1986, Able and
203
Dillon, 1977, Muheim and Akesson, 2002), and one study did not support a significant role for
204
either the sun compass or sun navigation in a migrant songbird (Munro and Wiltschko, 1993).
205
The original experiments of Emlen, which established stars as a compass cue , actually
206
provided some suggestion of time compensation, although only with three birds (Emlen, 1967).
207
However, subsequent investigation provided no evidence of time compensation (Mouritsen and
208
Larsen, 2001), without which longitude is not discernible. Additionally, there is no evidence for
209
a clock mechanism playing a role in detecting displacements per se, which would preclude both
210
star and sun navigation as a mechanism for longitude (KishkinevChernetsov and Mouritsen,
211
2010). However, a meta-analysis of displacement experiments of juvenile migratory birds in
212
orientation cages suggests that they are more likely to correct under starry skies than overcast
213
skies, suggesting a role for celestial cues in this behaviour (Thorup and Rabøl, 2007). Indeed,
214
many studies of the role of sun and stars in migratory navigation test only juvenile birds (e.g.
215
Mouritsen and Larsen 2001, Muheim and Akesson 2002), or the age is not reported (e.g. Able
216
and Dillon 1977, Able and Cherry 1986). Rejection of celestial navigation thus relies to some
217
extent on the assumption that the cues used by homing pigeons and migratory birds are the same.
218
It is however difficult to reconcile the global availability of celestial navigation with the apparent
219
limits on true navigation in some migrating songbirds (see above).
220
Infrasound
221
Sounds in the range of 0.1-10 Hz are known to spread over hundreds if not thousands of miles. If
222
stable, these have the potential to act as a gradient for navigation. Evidence has been presented
223
that pigeon homing performance is disrupted by infrasound disturbance, such as disturbance of
224
pigeon races by sonic booms of aircraft (Hagstrum, 2000, Hagstrum, 2001), or fluctuations in
225
orientation performance that correlate with atmospheric fluctuations (Hagstrum, 2013). The data,
226
while in many cases compelling, are correlational however, making it difficult to currently assess
227
whether this is a result of disruption of infrasound navigation cues, co-correlation with other
228
factors propagated by atmospheric means, or disturbance in motivation to home. An experiment
229
which removed the cochlea of homing pigeons did not produce any deficits in homing
230
performance (Wallraff, 1972), which, although not precluding that infrasound is part of a
231
multifactorial map, does not support the argument made by (Hagstrum, 2013) that infrasonic
232
cues are the sole solution to the navigational map question in pigeons. No experiment has yet
233
demonstrated any effects of infrasound on bird migration. Nevertheless, it remains a viable cue
234
which should be investigated further and the range over which it could operate makes it a
235
possibility for the distances seen in migratory displacements.
236
Olfactory cues
237
No aspect of bird navigation contributes to its reputation as a controversial field more than that
238
of the role of olfactory cues in the true navigation map. By far the majority of work has involved
239
homing pigeons and a large number of experiments, possibly more than in any other aspect of
240
bird true navigation, have been performed. A comprehensive review of these experiments is
241
available in (Wallraff, 2005) and a detailed treatment of all of these is beyond the scope of this
242
review given that the focus is on navigation in migratory birds,. However, olfactory navigation is
243
the most extensively tested hypothesis in true navigation and as such its potential role in true
244
navigation of migrants should be considered.
245
Key findings in olfactory navigation
246
Olfactory deprivation removes the ability of homing pigeons to return to the home loft, and this
247
is most clearly demonstrated by sectioning the olfactory nerve (Gagliardo et al., 2006, Gagliardo
248
et al., 2008, Gagliardo et al., 2009, Benvenuti et al., 1973). Further key findings in which
249
orientation is altered rather than impaired have been argued to suggest that the olfactory cues
250
provide navigational information to homing pigeons. A ‘false release site’ experiments in which
251
birds were transported to a releases site in one direction, allowed to sample air from this site, and
252
then transported to a release site in the opposite direction without further access to environmental
253
odours found that birds flew in the direction expected if they were trying to home from the
254
original release site (Benvenuti and Wallraff, 1985). An experiment in which artificial odours
255
(benzaldehyde) were presented to pigeons at the loft from the north west by fans found that when
256
displaced with benzaldehyde on their beaks, the birds oriented in the direction consistent with a
257
north west displacement, rather than with the actual home direction (IoaleNozzolini and Papi,
258
1990). Further experiments in which lofts are shielded or winds are manipulated argued that
259
pigeons learn to associate odours brought by different wind directions with different directions
260
(Baldaccini et al., 1975, Ioale et al., 1978, FoaBagnoli and Giongo, 1986, Gagliardo et al., 2001).
261
In theory this does not require sampling of gradients as suggested by the bi-coordinate map, but
262
merely association between an odour and a direction.
263
Criticisms of olfactory navigation
264
Olfactory navigation has been criticised on a number of counts. First, lack of repeatability of the
265
effects of olfactory deprivation argues that olfaction is neither the only, nor an essential cue
266
(Wiltschko, 1996). However, it is not clear whether this lack of repeatability comes from
267
redundancy in navigation cues or from variations caused by difficulties in control of the field
268
based system of experimentation, or in the experiments themselves. If homing performance of
269
birds treated with zinc sulphate is considered, olfactory deprivation has been demonstrated in a
270
number of countries and on four continents (Wallraff, 2005). A number of the key findings have
271
also been challenged. The deflector loft effect is shown in some cases to be a consequence of
272
deflection of polarized light, involved in compass calibration, as anosmic birds still deflect after
273
exposure (Phillips and Waldvogel, 1982, Waldvogel and Phillips, 1991, WaldvogelPhillips and
274
Brown, 1988). However, the similar findings of experiments in which winds are reversed or
275
shielded are not challenged by this discovery. The question of whether olfactory inputs are
276
navigational or related to motivational factors has always been a concern in interpretation
277
(Wiltschko, 1996). In support of this odours appear to ‘activate’ other navigational processes in
278
young pigeons navigating by route reversal (JorgeMarques and Phillips, 2009). Jorge et al. found
279
that young pigeons, which navigate by route reversal, were unable to orient homeward if
280
transported in filtered air, but could if transported either with access to natural odours, or
281
artificial ‘novel’ odours. This argues that smelling ‘non-home’ odours I, triggers the bird to
282
access a navigation system based on other cues. The site simulation experiments of Benvenuti
283
and Wallraff (1985) have also been argued to be a consequence of activation of a navigational
284
map by non-navigational olfactory cues rather than navigational in themselves (JorgeMarques
285
and Phillips, 2010). Presenting non-specific odours at the false release site produced the same
286
behaviour as access to natural odours. A subsequent test of the activation hypothesis did not
287
support a role for activation however, birds transported to a release site with access to novel
288
odours were no more likely to orient homewards than those transported in filtered air (Gagliardo
289
et al., 2011). However, they used higher concentrations of novel odours than those used in the
290
previous navigation experiments, which it has been argued would make the pigeons anosmic
291
(Phillips, personal communication). However, the experiments of (Ioale et al., 1990) cannot be
292
explained by activation, as if the benzaldehyde odour was activating a non-olfactory navigational
293
map, it would result in homeward orientation, not orientation consistent with a north west
294
displacement. One striking finding of the experiments on olfactory navigation in pigeons is that
295
if olfactory navigation is correct generally, it suggests that the view of redundancy of cues is not
296
correct. Where olfactory deprevation effects have been demonstrated they lead to significant
297
impairment of homing performance of pigeons at unfamiliar release sites, i.e. the majority do not
298
return to the home loft. If olfactory cues are navigational, this argues that in their absence no
299
cues are available to take their place which goes against the widely held view that the
300
navigational map must be made up of redundant cues (Walcott, 1996, Wiltschko et al., 2010).
301
Olfactory navigation thus continues to provide debate and has not been widely accepted as an
302
explanation for true navigation in homing pigeons.
303
While the olfactory navigation hypothesis is by far the most extensively tested when
304
considering pigeon homing, it has rarely been considered when discussing true navigation in
305
migrating birds. Stable odour gradients such as would be necessary for a bi-coordinate map have
306
not been demonstrated to exist beyond approximately 200km (Wallraff and Andreae, 2000). This
307
makes it difficult to explain the majority of displacement experiments on migrants by the use of
308
olfactory navigation. Nevertheless, two experiments on homing of migratory birds in the
309
breeding season found a deficit in performance after olfactory deprivation (Wallraff et al., 1995,
310
FiaschiFarina and Ioalé, 1974). More surprisingly, a recent experiment demonstrated that adult
311
catbirds displaced 1000km east from Illinois to Princeton in the USA, subjected to olfactory
312
deprivation by zinc sulphate treatment and then radio-tracked from a light aircraft were unable to
313
correct for the displacement in the way that controls were (Holland et al., 2009). If this finding is
314
borne out by further experimental support and shown to be a deficit based on removal of
315
navigation cues, then it may require a re-analysis of the bi-coordinate map theory for true
316
navigation. It appears to be hard to explain how stable olfactory gradients could exist over the
317
1000km necessary to explain this behaviour navigationally. Homing pigeons have not been
318
shown to use olfactory cues beyond 700km, and then only if they had access to environmental air
319
during the displacement (Wallraff, 1981).
320
With regard to the use of olfactory signals by migrants, an interesting parallel finding
321
from a neurobiological study of migratory restlessness is that both visual and olfactory areas of
322
the brain become more active at night during the migratory period, while they are most active
323
during the day outside this time (Rastogi et al., 2011). This suggests that olfaction plays a
324
significant role in migratory behaviour, but it is still an open question as to what role this is. A
325
recent hypothesis proposes that in fact the primary role of olfaction across organisms (and thus
326
reason for its evolution) is navigation (Jacobs, 2012). If it does indeed turn out to be the case
327
then theories of true navigation based on a bi-coordinate map made stable environmental
328
gradients may need to be significantly reconsidered, since olfactory cues do not seem to fit easily
329
into this paradigm.
330
Magnetic cues
331
The intensity of the Earth’s magnetic field was proposed as a cue for bird navigation over a
332
century ago (Viguier, 1882). The Earth’s magnetic field is stronger at the poles than at the
333
equator and it therefore has the potential to indicate latitudinal position. However, this is only
334
functional over a relatively coarse scale (Bingman and Cheng, 2006).There are variations in the
335
strength of the magnetic field at a fine scale that mean it may be stronger at a lower latitudes in
336
some cases and varies with longitude rather than latitude in some places (Phillips et al., 2006).
337
Thus, even at a coarse scale the magnetic field may not be as consistent a cue to latitudinal
338
position as it is often portrayed. In seeming support of this, a number of experiments in which
339
magnets are attached to the heads of birds homing over long distance failed to find any deficit in
340
homing performance (BenhamouBonadonna and Jouventin, 2003, Bonadonna et al., 2005,
341
Mouritsen et al., 2003). However, since the 1960’s evidence of behavioural responses to
342
artificially changing the Earth’s magnetic field have been obtained (Merkel and Wiltschko, 1965,
343
Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1972). To date at least 24 species of bird have been shown to respond
344
to changes in the Earth’s magnetic field (Wiltschko and Wiltchko, 2007) but by far the majority
345
of studies on magnetoreception in birds involve investigating its use as a compass and it has been
346
challenging to demonstrate the use of the magnetic field for a map (Phillips et al., 2006).
347
Artificial displacement experiments, where the magnetic field is changed to indicate different
348
latitudes to birds orienting in emlen funnels, provide some support that birds recognise magnetic
349
intensity signatures as a cue to end migration (Henshaw et al., 2010, FischerMunro and Phillips,
350
2003). However, in these studies (performed on Silvereyes, Zosterops l. lateralis and Lesser
351
Whitethroats Sylvia curruca) intensity signatures indicating displacements outside of the normal
352
range and migration route of the population did not produce navigational responses, as would be
353
expected for a map cue. Instead they become disoriented. This may be a similar response to that
354
seen in juvenile migrants, in which magnetic “sign posts” indicate latitudes at which innate
355
compass directions must change for successful migration (Beck and Wiltschko, 1988) and thus
356
the birds may merely stop migrating when a certain latitude is reached. Interestingly, this is also
357
consistent with activation, as proposed for olfactory cues, with magnetic field signatures
358
activating a non-magnetic navigation system below some threshold value, but once that value is
359
reached, the navigation system is no longer activated, even if the magnetic value is far greater
360
than the threshold. A recent follow-up study has indicated that only adults are affected by such
361
magnetic displacements suggesting that it is a different behaviour than the innate signpost
362
recognition seen in juveniles (Deutchlander et al., 2012). However, the lack of orientation
363
towards the winter site when the artificial displacement was north of it remained, making it
364
difficult to conclude that the behaviour represented true navigation in the strict sense rather than
365
an age dependant response to latitudinal sign posts, or activation of other navigational cues.
366
Recall however, that when (Perdeck, 1958) displaced adult starlings outside the wintering
367
latitude, they were able to correct and return to the normal winter area. This indicates the
368
challenge of orientation experiments: it is possible that different site fidelity is present in the
369
different species tested, with starlings showing more fidelity to their winter site than Silvereyes
370
or Lesser Whitethroats, and thus these two species do not represent the ideal model for this test.
371
Contrast this to similar experiments on newts, turtles and spiny lobsters, which have been
372
demonstrated to alter their orientation in response to artificial displacements either north or south
373
of their current position (Fischer et al., 2001, Lohmann et al., 2004, Lohmann et al., 1995).
374
Experiments on the orientation performance of homing pigeons has also been shown to be
375
disrupted at magnetic anomalies (areas with stronger or weaker magnetic intensity than
376
expected), which suggests that magnetic intensity plays a role in their navigational map
377
(DennisRayner and Walker, 2007, Walcott, 1991, Wiltschko et al., 2010, Mora and Walker,
378
2009), although many of these experiments are conducted within a range where the variation in
379
magnetic intensity is thought to make the earth’s magnetic field unreliable as a cue to position
380
(Phillips et al., 2006). This may indicate a different mechanism than that proposed for true
381
navigation in migrating birds, or perhaps that magnetic intensity correlates with other factors
382
which disrupt orientation in these experiments (Wallraff, 2005). Part of the challenge in
383
demonstrating a role for magnetic intensity has been because most navigational experiments
384
involve sensory manipulation, and the way in which birds sense the magnetic field is by far the
385
most uncertain aspect of navigation research. However, within the last 20 years, significant
386
advances have been made in this area. This has involved the integration of theoretical work from
387
physics, biochemistry, neurobiology and molecular biology alongside traditional behavioural
388
experiments. As a consequence, we now have an understanding of the way birds may perceive
389
aspects of the magnetic field and how this may contribute to the map and the compass aspects of
390
true navigation. An understanding of the potential sensory pathways is thus crucial to
391
understanding the behavioural experiments that support the use of the magnetic field as a map.
392
The magnetic sense: different receptors for different tasks?
393
The behavioural evidence for magnetoreception was met with initial scepticism due to the lack of
394
an obvious sense organ. However, consideration of physical principles of the magnetic field
395
means that such sense organs need not be located at the surface in the same way as photo or
396
auditory receptors must: the Earth’s magnetic field can pervade all tissue. During the 1980’s
397
several models were proposed for magnetoreception but 2 have withstood scrutiny: a mechanism
398
based on photoreceptive molecules (the radical pair mechanism) and a mechanism based on
399
magnetic iron particles (the ferrimagnetic mechanism).
400
Radical pairs
401
Magnetically sensitive reactions involve radicals in which unpaired electrons are present in
402
different “spin” states, either antiparallel (“singlet” state) or parallel (“triplet” state) (Rogers and
403
Hore, 2009). The yield of the different states has been demonstrated to be influenced by strong
404
magnetic fields, and based on this it was hypothesised that a molecule that formed such radicals
405
in different yields depending on the magnetic field alignment could be the basis of a
406
magnetoreceptor (SchultenSwenberg and Weller, 1978) (figure 3). It was subsequently
407
discovered that magnetic compass orientation is dependent on the wavelength of light (Wiltschko
408
et al., 1993, Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2006) and so the model was modified to suggest that the
409
molecule involved in the radical pair process was photoreceptive and that a photon of light
410
would instigate this reaction (RitzAdem and Schulten, 2000). Evidence that the magnetic
411
compass was lateralized via the right eye to the left brain hemisphere suggested that the magnetic
412
field was perceived through the eyes (Wiltschko et al., 2002b), although see (Hein et al., 2011)
413
for evidence of no lateralization. A study involving ZENK, an immediate early gene which is
414
expressed in neurones indicated that an area of the brain called cluster-N, responsible for night
415
vision was active during migratory restlessness (Mouritsen et al., 2005). A subsequent study in
416
which this area of the brain was lesioned indicated that migratory robins could no longer use
417
their magnetic compass (Zapka et al., 2009). Thus, migratory songbirds appear to possess a
418
magnetoreceptor mediated by the visual system which is based on a photoreceptive molecule.
419
Evidence that this is because of a radical pair mechanism comes from an experiment based on
420
the prediction that the interaction between a radical pair and the magnetic field could be
421
disrupted by a weak electromagnetic field in the radio spectrum (1.315 MHz, the so called
422
Larmor frequency). It was indeed the case that migratory robins could no longer orient in an
423
emlen funnel when such a field was applied (Ritz et al., 2004). The molecule involved has been
424
proposed to be a cryptochrome (Ritz et al., 2000). This is a blue light receptor and appears to
425
form long lived radical pairs, which would be necessary for it to work as a magnetoreceptor
426
(Liedvogel et al., 2007). Four different cryptochromes have been found in the eyes of migratory
427
birds, Cry 1a, (Mouritsen et al., 2004, Moller et al., 2004, Niessner et al., 2011), Cry 1b (Moller
428
et al., 2004), Cry 2 (Mouritsen et al., 2004) and Cry 4 (Mouritsen et al., 2004). In terms of figure
429
3, it is thought that the radical pair comprises a flavosemiquinone radical and a terminal residue
430
of a conserved triad of tryptophan residues (a flavin-tryptophan radical pair) (Maeda et al., 2012,
431
Biskup et al., 2009). Based on our understanding of how a similar reaction occurs in plants, the
432
flavosemiquinone radical would appear to lead to the signalling state (Bouly et al., 2007). No
433
direct evidence yet exists however, to demonstrate that cryptochrome is the primary sensing
434
molecule involved in magnetoreception (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012, Liedvogel and Mouritsen,
435
2010). More detailed discussion of the issues around the radical pair compass can be found in
436
(Mouritsen and Hore, 2012, Rogers and Hore, 2009). What is crucial to this review is, does it
437
have a role in the navigational map? All the experiments described above involved disrupting the
438
magnetic compass, in no case was there an indication that the radical pair pathway is involved in
439
map navigation. It does not appear that this mechanism detects intensity, nor indeed the polarity
440
of the magnetic field, only inclination (Ritz et al., 2000). In theory, inclination could be used to
441
detect latitude, so there is no reason why the radical pair mechanism could not be involved in the
442
navigational map, but no experiment has tested this hypothesis. This may be because it could be
443
challenging to design an experiment that is able to disentangle the use of the radical pair sense
444
for a compass from its use in a map.
445
The Ferrimagnetic sense
446
Ferrimagnetic materials are those in which spontaneous magnetization occurs because the
447
magnetic moments of atoms are opposed but unequal. This is seen in iron oxides, including the
448
oldest known magnetic substance, magnetite. Ferrimagnetic material exists in a number of
449
crystalline “domains”, including multi, single and supaparamagnetic. Multi domain magnetite
450
has no magnetisation, single domain has a permanent magnetic moment whereas
451
superparamagnetic magnetite has a fluctuating magnetic moment, but it can be aligned to an
452
external magnetic field (Kirschvink and Walker, 1985). Based on the discovery that bacteria
453
containing single domain magnetite passively align to the magnetic field (Blakemore, 1975), and
454
that magnetite is a biogenic material that is present widely in the tissue of a diverse array of
455
organisms, it was proposed that such material could form the basis of a magnetic sense in multi-
456
cellular organisms (Kirschvink and Gould, 1981, Yorke, 1979). To test this, it was proposed that
457
the physical properties of the ferrimagnetic material could be used to predict the presence of
458
magnetic material in sensory cells in the same way as it had been done in bacteria (Kirschvink,
459
1982). If ferrimagnetic material was involved in a sensory receptor that detected the Earth’s
460
magnetic field then a brief strong magnetic pulse that exceeded the coercivity (the magnetic
461
force required to reduce the magnetisation of the substance to zero) would re-magnetise the
462
substance in the opposite direction if applied antiparallel to the original magnetization (Figure 4).
463
For most biogenic magnetite, the strength required to re-magnetise would be 0.1T, 5000 times
464
the strength of the Earth’s magnetic field (Kirschvink et al., 1985). If single domain magnetite
465
was present it would be re-magnetised, and if used by sensory cells, in theory, would lead to a
466
change in the information the receptor gave. Subsequently, a significant number of experiments
467
have treated birds, with a strong magnetic pulse and indeed found that their orientation is
468
affected by such a pulse. Effects have been found both on migratory birds tested in emlen
469
funnels (BeasonDussourd and Deutschlander, 1995, Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995, Wiltschko
470
et al., 1994, Wiltschko et al., 1998), in naturally migrating birds (Holland, 2010) and in homing
471
pigeons (BeasonWiltschko and Wiltschko, 1997). In all these cases a magnetic pulse leads to a
472
deflection in orientation. However, where the pulse was applied antiparallel to the direction of
473
magnetisation, the expected reorientation in the opposite direction did not occur (Wiltschko et
474
al., 2002a, Holland, 2010). This is not consistent with single domain magnetite that is free to
475
rotate in the way a bacteria cell can and does not fit with the popularized concept of a
476
ferrimagnetic sense consisting of tiny compass needles (Mouritsen, 2012). Nor is the fact that the
477
pulse effect appears to be temporary, with birds returning to normal orientation after
478
approximately 10 days (Wiltschko et al., 2007, Wiltschko et al., 1998). This does not support the
479
permanent re-magnetisation of magnetic material. One pulse experiment demonstrated that the
480
deflecting effect of the pulse was removed if the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve
481
(which innervates the beak) was anaesthetised with lidocane, a local anaesthetic (Beason and
482
Semm, 1996). This suggested that the magnetic pulse effected receptors located in the beak area
483
and the trigeminal nerve was responsible for conveying the input from these receptors to the
484
brain.
485
Two subsequent studies have confirmed the finding that the trigeminal nerve conveys
486
magnetic information. Mora and colleagues (Mora et al., 2004) conditioned homing pigeons to a
487
magnetic intensity anomaly, and found that they could no longer discriminate if the trigeminal
488
nerve was lesioned (although see (KirschvinkWinklhofer and Walker, 2010) for possible
489
weaknesses in the experimental design and (KishkinevMouritsen and Mora, 2012) for failure to
490
repeat the conditioning paradigm). This indicated that the trigeminal nerve was responsible for
491
conveying information on the magnetic field. Following this a study of ZENK expression
492
indicated activation of neurons in the trigeminal brainstem only in migratory robins orienting in a
493
magnetic field that had an intact trigeminal nerve (Heyers et al., 2010). However, homing
494
pigeons that had their trigeminal nerve lesioned were not disrupted in their homing performance
495
(Gagliardo et al., 2006, Gagliardo et al., 2008, Gagliardo et al., 2009). Until recently this made
496
the study of Beason and Semm (1996) the only study to date to indicate a role for the trigeminal
497
nerve in the process of navigation, but what aspect of navigation? Lesions of the trigeminal nerve
498
do not appear to affect magnetic compass orientation in juvenile robins (Zapka et al., 2009), and
499
the pulse deflects the orientation of birds in emlen funnels, but does not affect the magnetic
500
compass (Wiltschko et al., 2006, Munro et al., 1997b). A particular design of pulse experiment
501
however suggests a possible role for the ferrimagnetic receptor in bird navigation. Pulses only
502
appear to effect the orientation of adult migrating birds, not juveniles (Munro et al., 1997a,
503
Munro et al., 1997b, Holland and Helm, 2013), which suggests that the ferrimagnetic sense is
504
involved in an experience based mechanism possessed by adult but not juvenile birds. Since
505
adults have true navigation, this suggests the ferrimagnetic sense is involved in the true
506
navigation map. A recent study has also shown that migrating reed warblers returning to their
507
breeding grounds, are unable to correct for a displacement of 1000km eastwards if the trigeminal
508
nerve is cut, unlike intact and sham operated birds, who are able to do so (Kishkinev et al.,
509
2013). This finding, on migratory birds, is in contrast to the findings on homing pigeons, where
510
no role for the trigeminal nerve in navigation is supported.
511
On this basis it is argued that migrating birds possess two magnetoreceptive pathways: a
512
radical pair mechanism in the eye, which is responsible for at least compass orientation, and a
513
ferrimagnetic sense, which is implicated in the detection of magnetic intensity and is involved in
514
the navigational map (Wiltschko and Wiltchko, 2007). However, caution is urged in accepting
515
this interpretation without question. Adult but not juvenile migratory birds have been shown to
516
respond to changes in intensity (Deutchlander et al., 2012) and adult but not juvenile migratory
517
birds have been shown to be affected by a strong magnetic pulse (Munro et al., 1997a, Holland
518
and Helm, 2013), but there is no direct causal link between the two. Similarly, the trigeminal
519
nerve has been shown to be involved in detecting the magnetic field (Mora et al., 2004), the
520
pulse effect no longer persists when this is anaesthetised, and migratory birds with trigeminal
521
nerve section can no longer correct for displacement (Kishkinev et al., 2013), but there is no
522
direct link between the pulse and magnetic intensity, or the trigeminal nerve and magnetic
523
intensity. Evidence for a ferrimagnetic sense that is responsible for detecting intensity as part of a
524
true navigational map is thus based on several indirect links. We do not know for certain that the
525
pulse affects a receptor that detects intensity, only that it changes navigation behaviour and that
526
the behaviour appears to be mediated by the trigeminal nerve. To be certain of that we would
527
need to know the nature and location of the magnetic receptor.
528
The ferrimagnetic receptor: magnetite or macrophage?
529
Initially, iron-containing cells found in the upper beak of the homing pigeons and other birds
530
were suggested as magnetoreceptors innervated via the trigeminal nerve, although no clear
531
sensory receptor was identified (Beason and Nichols, 1984, Williams and Wild, 2001). A
532
structure that has the potential to be a magnetic receptor has been described in the beak of
533
homing pigeons (Fleissner et al., 2003), chickens (Gallus domesticus), Garden Warblers (Sylvia
534
borin) and Robins (Erithacus rubecula) (Falkenberg et al., 2010). The structure appears to
535
consist of sensory dendrites in the upper beak, which contains iron rich bullets and an iron
536
containing vesicle. It is argued that these are distributed in such a way as to provide magnetic
537
field information in three axes and thus form elements of a magnetometer. Appearing to support
538
the argument that this is a magnetoreceptor, the effect of a magnetic pulse disappears when the
539
upper beak is anaesthetised with local anaesthetic (Wiltschko et al., 2009). The disrupting effect
540
of a magnetic anomaly on homing pigeon orientation also disappears when the beak is
541
anaesthetised (Wiltschko et al., 2010). Again, however, the link is indirect. It is not certain that
542
the anaesthetic is acting directly on the magnetoreceptor in these experiments, and the effects of
543
local anaesthetics have been questioned (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012). A further significant
544
cautionary note to the beak based magnetoreceptor theory has recently emerged. A thorough
545
study made on homing pigeons (Treiber et al., 2012) strongly suggested that the majority of cells
546
identified as containing iron, if not all, both in the upper beak and other parts of the body, such as
547
the skin, respiratory epithelium and feather folliculi are macrophages, cells responsible for
548
engulfing waste and pathogens in the body. Treiber et al. (2012) argue that the structures
549
described in previous work are thus not sensory cells at all. This raises the question of whether a
550
magnetoreceptor exists in the beak. However, the work of Treiber et al. (2012) should not be
551
over interpreted. While the burden of proof is on those who argue that the beak is the site of
552
magnetoreception (Mouritsen, 2012), Treiber et al. do acknowledge that there may be
553
magnetoreceptors in some as yet unidentified location in the beak. Added to this, a number of
554
behavioural studies supporting magnetoreception in the beak have been identified (Wiltschko
555
and Wiltschko, 2013).
556
A second potential site of a magnetoreceptor has also been identified, in the inner ear
557
lagena of homing pigeons, using electrophysiology recordings (Wu and Dickman, 2011, Wu and
558
Dickman, 2012). Recent evidence from electron microscopy has identified iron rich cells in the
559
inner ear (Lauwers et al., 2013), although they do not fit all the properties of a magnetoreceptor.
560
Furthermore, experiments on homing pigeons did not show any deficit in homing with the inner
561
ear removed (Wallraff, 1972), so unlike the beak based sense, behavioural evidence is lacking.
562
Future perspectives: Chronic disease or rude health?
563
As noted at the start of this review, a recent review of magnetoreception suggested that this field
564
suffered from a chronic disease in its lack of repeatability of findings (Mouritsen and Hore,
565
2012). It could be argued that this applies equally to all aspects of bird navigation, with many
566
experiments failing to repeat others, or contradictory results within and between different
567
disciplines. Does this mean that research on bird navigation is in ill health? The assertion by
568
Mouritsen and Hore that experiments must be carefully controlled and designed to avoid
569
observer bias is an important one. However, the recent work of Treiber et al. (2012) that
570
questions the structure and location of magnetoreceptors could actually be viewed as a strength
571
and sign of health: of a field that welcomes new results that may force revisions of current
572
models of understanding. While many aspects of navigation are unresolved, as this review
573
indicates, that does not mean that there is no data. While the models for studying navigation are
574
imperfect, closer links between laboratory work and field work are being established and the
575
addition of new technology for studying animals in the wild will broadened our understanding of
576
the behaviour of migrating birds and the challenges they face (Guilford et al., 2011). The
577
integration of neurobiology, physics and molecular biology into the discipline is now well
578
established and has led to a number of breakthroughs in our understanding of the magnetic sense
579
as well as the role of the olfactory sense in navigation. The integration of these disciplines has
580
led to testable predictions about the structure of sensory systems and potentially the mechanisms
581
of navigation. For the field to advance further, the link between these disciplines and behavioural
582
biology needs to strengthen further, in order to reduce the “black box” understanding of some of
583
the systems involved. For example, a better knowledge of the structure of the ferromagnetic
584
sense will allow better predictions about the effect of magnetic pulse treatments to understand
585
how receptors are changed by the treatment. Strengthening this integration of other disciplines,
586
whilst maintaining the roots as a behavioural biology discipline, will ultimately lead to the
587
solution of the “mystery” of bird navigation. . I will finish this review by highlighting some of
588
the key issues that should be resolved in order for the field of true navigation in migratory birds
589
to advance
590
Key questions in migratory true navigation
591
1) Is the true navigation map unimodal, i.e. one environmental cue provides all information
592
on location, bimodal, i.e. two separate environmental cues provide different aspects of the
593
location (e.g. latitude and longitude), or redundant, i.e. do multiple cues provide the same
594
information for different aspects of the location. Solving this will help to understand
595
some of the inconsistencies and conflicting evidence in the field, as it will establish
596
whether failure to repeat is a consequence of experimental design rather than redundancy
597
of cues.
598
2) Are there one or two magnetic sensory systems with different functions? Clearly
599
establishing whether the magnetite based system is responsible for detecting intensity
600
would establish that not just direction but also other aspects of the magnetic field could
601
be used and form part of the magnetic map.
602
3) Related to 2), can a magnetite based sensory receptor be located and described?
603
Understanding the structure of the magnetoreceptor will help to provide testable
604
predictions for how it might control birds’ behaviour, particularly in light of the pulse
605
experiments.
606
4) To what extent does migratory behaviour in the wild mirror behaviour in an orientation
607
cage? The field of navigation now involves multiple disciplines including those outside
608
biology and requires a controlled laboratory based system that allows predictions to be
609
tested by isolating cues. The orientation cage provides this. However, currently, we have
610
little understanding of how small songbirds respond to displacements in the wild with
611
current techniques being too coarse (ringing, geolocators) or lacking in range (radio
612
tracking). Understanding how songbirds respond to displacements in more detail will
613
indicate the range of their navigation system and thus the extent to which environmental
614
cues will provide reliable information on their location.
615
5) Is “activation” a significant phenomenon within true navigation? The results of some
616
experiments on both olfactory and magnetic cues are consistent with them activating
617
other navigational cues, but this would appear to violate the principle of Occam’s razor
618
by adding another step to the navigation process: If activation plays a part in true
619
navigation, then it moves from a 2 step process (what is my location, what direction to
620
reach home?) to a 3 step process (Am I at home? If no, what is my location, what
621
direction to reach home?), and the two cues providing the most evidence for navigation
622
(olfactory and magnetic) become relegated to intermediate steps towards the actual
623
navigational cues.
624
Acknowledgements
625
I thank John Phillips and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript.
626
Aspects of this review also came as a result of enjoyable discussions with the Navigation
627
special interest group at the MIGRATE NSF funded meeting in Konstanz, 2010 with
628
Susanna Åkesson, Verner Bingman, Tim Guilford, Anna Gagliardo, Henrik Mouritsen,
629
Rachel Muheim, Rosie Wiltschko and Wolfgang Wiltschko.
630
631
References
632
Able, K. P. (2001). The concepts and terminology of bird navigation. Journal of Avian Biology
633
634
32, 174-183.
Able, K. P. & Cherry, J. D. (1986). Mechanisms of dusk orientation in white-throated sparrows
635
(Zonotrichia albicollis): clock-shift experiments. Journal of Comparative Physiology A
636
159, 107-113.
637
638
639
Able, K. P. & Dillon, P. M. (1977). Sun compass orientation in a nocturnal migrant white
throated sparrow. Condor 79, 393-395.
Åkesson, S., Morin, J., Muheim, R. & Ottosson, U. (2005). Dramatic orientation shift of white-
640
crowned sparrows displaced across longitudes in the high arctic. Current Biology 15,
641
1591-1597.
642
643
Alerstam, T. (2006). Conflicting evidence about long-distance animal navigation. Science 313,
791-794.
644
Baker, R. R. (1984). Bird navigation: the solution of a mystery?, London, Hodder Arnold.
645
Baldaccini, N. E., Benvenuti, S., Fiaschi, V. & Papi, F. (1975). Pigeon Navigation - Effects of
646
Wind Deflection at Home Cage on Homing Behavior. Journal of Comparative
647
Physiology 99, 177-186.
648
Beason, R. C., Dussourd, N. & Deutschlander, M. E. (1995). Behavioral Evidence for the Use of
649
Magnetic Material in Magnetoreception by a Migratory Bird. Journal of Experimental
650
Biology 198, 141-146.
651
652
Beason, R. C. & Nichols, J. E. (1984). Magnetic Orientation and Magnetically Sensitive Material
in a Transequatorial Migratory Bird. Nature 309, 151-153.
653
654
655
656
657
Beason, R. C. & Semm, P. (1996). Does the avian ophthalmic nerve carry magnetic navigational
information? Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 1241-1244.
Beason, R. C., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1997). Pigeon homing: Effects of magnetic
pulses on initial orientation. Auk 114, 405-415.
Beck, W. & Wiltschko, W. (1988). Magnetic factors control the migratory direction of Pied
658
Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca Pallas). In: Ouellet, H. (ed.) Acta 19th Congress
659
Internationa Ornithology, vol II. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.
660
Benhamou, S., Bonadonna, F. & Jouventin, P. (2003). Successful homing of magnet-carrying
661
white-chinned petrels released in the open sea. Animal Behaviour 65, 729-734.
662
Benvenuti, S., Fiaschi, V., Fiore, L. & Papi, F. (1973). Homing performances of inexperienced
663
and directionally trained pigeons subjected to olfactory nerve section. Journal of
664
Comparative Physiology 83, 81-92.
665
Benvenuti, S. & Wallraff, H. G. (1985). Pigeon navigation: site simulation by means of
666
atmospheric odours. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 156, 737-746.
667
Bingman, V. P. & Cheng, K. (2006). Mechanisms of animal global navigation: Comparative
668
perspectives and enduring challenges. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 17, 295-318.
669
Biskup, T., Schleicher, E., Okafuji, A., Link, G., Hitomi, K., Getzoff, E. D. & Weber, S. (2009).
670
Direct Observation of a Photoinduced Radical Pair in a Cryptochrome Blue-Light
671
Photoreceptor. Angewandte Chemie-International Edition 48, 404-407.
672
Blakemore, R. P. (1975). Magnetotactic bacteria. Science 190, 377-379.
673
Bonadonna, F., Bajzak, C., Benhamou, S., Igloi, K., Jouventin, P., Lipp, H. P. & Dell'omo, G.
674
(2005). Orientation in the wandering albatross: Interfering with magnetic perception does
675
not affect orientation performance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological
676
Sciences 272, 489-495.
677
Boström, J. E., Åkesson, S. & Alerstam, T. (2012). Where on earth can animals use a
678
geomagnetic bi-coordinate map for navigation? Ecography 35, 1039-1047.
679
Bouly, J.-P., Schleicher, E., Dionisio-Sese, M., Vandenbussche, F., Van Der Straeten, D.,
680
Bakrim, N., Meier, S., Batschauer, A., Galland, P., Bittl, R. & Ahmad, M. (2007).
681
Cryptochrome blue light photoreceptors are activated through interconversion of flavin
682
redox states. Journal of Biological Chemistry 282, 9383-9391.
683
Bridge, E. S., Thorup, K., Bowlin, M. S., Chilson, P. B., Diehl, R., Fleron, R. W., Hartl, P.,
684
Kays, R. W., Kelly, J. F., Robinson, W. D. & Wikelksi, M. (2011). Technology on the
685
move: recent and forthcoming innoviations for tracking migratory birds. Bioscience 61,
686
689-698.
687
Chernetsov, N., Kishkinev, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2008). A long distance avian migrant
688
compensates for longitudinal displacement during spring migration. Current Biology 18,
689
188-190.
690
Dennis, T. E., Rayner, M. J. & Walker, M. M. (2007). Evidence that pigeons orient to
691
geomagnetic intensity during homing. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological
692
Sciences 274, 1153-1158.
693
Deutchlander, M. E., Phillips, J. B. & Munro, U. (2012). Age-Dependent Orientation to
694
Magnetically-Simulated Geographic Displacements in Migratory Australian Silvereyes
695
(Zosterops l. lateralis). The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 124, 467-477.
696
Dingle, H. (1996). Migration: the biology of life on the move, London, Lightning Source.
697
698
699
700
701
Emlen, S. T. (1967). Migratory Orientation in Indigo Bunting Passerina Cyanea. Part II:
Mechanisms of celestial navigation. Auk 84, 463-489.
Emlen, S. T. & Emlen, J. T. (1966). A technique for recording migratory orientation of captive
birds. Auk 83, 361-367.
Falkenberg, G., Fleissner, G., Schuchardt, K., Kuehbacher, M., Thalau, P., Mouritsen, H.,
702
Heyers, D., Wellenreuther, G. & Fleissner, G. (2010). Avian Magnetoreception:
703
Elaborate Iron Mineral Containing Dendrites in the Upper Beak Seem to Be a Common
704
Feature of Birds. PLoS ONE 5, e9231.
705
Fiaschi, V., Farina, M. & Ioalé, P. (1974). Homing experiments on swifts Apus apus (L.)
706
deprived of olfactory perception. Monitore Zoologica Italiano 8, 235-244.
707
Fischer, J., Munro, U. & Phillips, J. B. (2003). Magnetic navigation by an avian migrant? In:
708
Berthold, P., Gwinner, E. & Sonnenschein, E. (eds.) Avian migration. Heidelberg-New
709
York: Springer-Verlag,.
710
711
712
Fischer, J. H., Freake, M. J., Borland, S. C. & Phillips, J. B. (2001). Evidence for the use of
magnetic map information by an amphibian. Animal Behaviour 62, 1-10.
Fleissner, G., Holtkamp-Rotzler, E., Hanzlik, M., Winklhofer, M., Petersen, N. & Wiltschko, W.
713
(2003). Ultrastructural analysis of a putative magnetoreceptor in the beak of homing
714
pigeons. Journal of Comparative Neurology 458, 350-360.
715
Foa, A., Bagnoli, P. & Giongo, F. (1986). Homing pigeons subjected to section of the anterior
716
commissure can build up 2 olfactory maps in the deflector lofts. Journal of Comparative
717
Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 159, 465-472.
718
719
Fransson, T., Jakobsson, S., Johansson, P., Kullberg, C., Lind, J. & Vallin, A. (2001). Bird
migration: Magnetic cues trigger extensive refuelling. Nature 414, 35-36.
720
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Filannino, C. & Wikelski, M. (2011). Homing Pigeons Only Navigate in
721
Air with Intact Environmental Odours: A Test of the Olfactory Activation Hypothesis
722
with GPS Data Loggers. PLoS ONE 6, e22385.
723
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Odetti, F. & Bingman, V. P. (2001). The ontogeny of the homing pigeon
724
navigational map: evidence for a sensitive learning period. Proceedings of the Royal
725
Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 268, 197-202.
726
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2006). Having the nerve to home: trigeminal
727
magnetoreceptor versus olfactory mediation of homing in pigeons. Journal of
728
Experimental Biology 209, 2888-2892.
729
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2008). Navigational abilities of homing
730
pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic information when
731
young. Journal of Experimental Biology 211, 2046-2051.
732
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2009). Navigational abilities of adult and
733
experienced homing pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic
734
information. Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 3119-3124.
735
Griffin, D. R. (1952). Bird Navigation. Biological Reviews 27, 359-400.
736
Guilford, T., Akesson, S., Gagliardo, A., Holland, R. A., Mouritsen, H., Muheim, R., Wiltschko,
737
R., Wiltschko, W. & Bingman, V. P. (2011). Migratory navigation in birds: new
738
opportunities in an era of fast-developing tracking technology. Journal of Experimental
739
Biology 214, 3705-3712.
740
741
Hagstrum, J. T. (2000). Infrasound and the avian navigational map. Journal of Experimental
Biology 203, 1103-1111.
742
743
Hagstrum, J. T. (2001). Infrasound and the avian navigational map. Journal of Navigation 54,
377-391.
744
Hagstrum, J. T. (2013). Atmospheric propagation modelling indicates homing pigeons use loft-
745
specific infrasonic 'map' cues. Journal of Experimental Biology , 216, 687-699.
746
747
748
Hein, C. M., Engels, S., Kishkinev, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2011). Robins have a magnetic compass
in both eyes. Nature 471, E11-E12.
Henshaw, I., Fransson, T., Jackobsson, S. & Kullberg, C. (2010). Geomagnetic field affects
749
spring migratory direction in a long distance migrant. Behavioral Ecology and
750
Sociobiology 64, 1317-1323.
751
Heyers, D., Zapka, M., Hoffmeister, M., Wild, J. M. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). Magnetic field
752
changes activate the trigeminal brainstem complex in a migratory bird. Proceedings of
753
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 9394-9399.
754
Holland, R. & Helm, B. (2013). A magnetic pulse affects the precision of departure direction of
755
naturally migrating adult but not juvenile birds. Joural of the Royal Society: Interface 10,
756
20121047.
757
Holland, R. A. (2010). Differential effects of magnetic pulses on the departure directions of
758
naturally migrating birds. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 7, 1617-1625.
759
Holland, R. A., Thorup, K., Gagliardo, A., Bisson, I., Knecht, E., Mizrahi, D. & Wikelski, M.
760
(2009). Testing the role of sensory systems in the migratory heading of a songbird.
761
Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 4065-4071.
762
763
Holland, R. A., Thorup, K. & Wikelski, M. (2007). Where the wild things go. Biologist 54, 214219.
764
765
766
Ioale, P., Nozzolini, M. & Papi, F. (1990). Homing pigeons do extract directional information
from olfactory stimuli. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 26, 301-305.
Ioale, P., Papi, F., Fiaschi, V. & Baldaccini, N. E. (1978). Pigeon navigation: effects of homing
767
behaviour by reversing wind direction at the loft. Journal of Comparative Physiology
768
128, 285-295.
769
Jacobs, L. F. (2012). From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction.
770
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109,
771
10693-10700.
772
773
774
Jorge, P. E., Marques, A. E. & Phillips, J. B. (2009). Activation rather than navigational effects
of odours on homing of young pigons. Current Biology 19, 1-5.
Jorge, P. E., Marques, A. E. & Phillips, J. B. (2010). Activation effects of odours on avian
775
navigation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277, 45-49.
776
Keeton, W. T. (1974). The orientational and navigational basis of homing in birds. Advances in
777
the study of behavior 5, 47–132.
778
Kennedy, D. & Norman, C. (2005). What don’t we know? Science special Issue 309, 1-204.
779
Kirschvink, J. L. (1982). Birds, Bees and Magnetism - a New Look at the Old Problem of
780
781
782
783
Magnetoreception. Trends in Neurosciences 5, 160-167.
Kirschvink, J. L. & Gould, J. L. (1981). Biogenetic magnetite as a basis for magnetic field
detection in animals. BioSystems 13, 181-201.
Kirschvink, J. L. & Walker, M. M. (1985). Particle-size considerations for magnetite based
784
magnetoreceptors. In: Kirschvink, J. L., Jones, D. S. & Macfadden, B. J. (eds.) Magnetite
785
biomineralization and magnetoreception in organisms. New York: Plenum Press.
786
Kirschvink, J. L., Walker, M. M., Chang, S. B., Dizon, A. E. & Peterson, K. A. (1985). Chains of
787
single domain magnetite particles in chinook salmon, Onchorhynchus Tshawytscha.
788
Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 157,
789
375-381.
790
Kirschvink, J. L., Winklhofer, M. & Walker, M. M. (2010). Biophysics of magnetic orientation:
791
strengthening the interface between theory and experimental design. Journal of the Royal
792
Society Interface doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0491.focus.
793
Kishkinev, D., Chernetsov, N., Heyers, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2013). Migratory Reed Warblers
794
Need Intact Trigeminal Nerves to Correct for a 1,000 km Eastward Displacement. PLoS
795
ONE 8, e65847.
796
Kishkinev, D., Chernetsov, N. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). A double clock or jetlag mechanism is
797
unlikely to be involved in detection of east-west displacements in a long distance avian
798
migrant. Auk 127, 773-780.
799
Kishkinev, D., Mouritsen, H. & Mora, C. V. (2012). An attempt to develop an operant
800
conditioning paradigm to test for magnetic discrimination behavior in a migratory
801
songbird. Journal of Ornithology 153, 1165-1177.
802
803
804
Kramer, G. (1953). Wird die Sonnehöhe bei der Heimfindeorientierung verwertet? Journal fur
Ornithology 94, 201-219.
Lauwers, M., Pichler, P., Edelman, N. B., Resch, G. P., Ushakova, L., Salzer, M. C., Heyers, D.,
805
Saunders, M., Shaw, J. & Keays, D. A. (2013). An iron-rich organelle in the cuticular
806
plate of avian hair cells. Current biology : CB 23, 924-9.
807
Liedvogel, M., Maeda, K., Henbest, K., Schleicher, E., Simon, T., Timmel, C. R., Hore, P. J. &
808
Mouritsen, H. (2007). Chemical Magnetoreception: Bird Cryptochrome 1a Is Excited by
809
Blue Light and Forms Long-Lived Radical-Pairs. PLoS ONE 2.
810
Liedvogel, M. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). Cryptochromes-a potential magnetoreceptor: what do we
811
know and what do we want to know? Journal of the royal Society Interface 7, S147-
812
S162.
813
814
815
Lohmann, K. J., Lohmann, C. M. F., Ehrhart, L. M., Bagley, D. A. & Swing, T. (2004).
Geomagnetic map used in sea turtle navigation. Nature 428, 909-910.
Lohmann, K. J., Pentcheff, N. D., Nevitt, G. A., Stetten, G. D. & Zimmer-Faust, R. K. (1995).
816
Magnetic orientation of spiny lobsters in the ocean: experiments with underseas coil
817
systems. Journal of Experimental Biology 198, 2041-2048.
818
Maeda, K., Robinson, A. J., Henbest, K. B., Hogben, H. J., Biskup, T., Ahmad, M., Schleicher,
819
E., Weber, S., Timmel, C. R. & Hore, P. J. (2012). Magnetically sensitive light-induced
820
reactions in cryptochrome are consistent with its proposed role as a magnetoreceptor.
821
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109,
822
4774-4779.
823
824
825
826
827
828
Matthews, G. V. T. (1951). The experimental investigation of navigation in homing pigeons.
Journal of Experimental Biology , 28, 508-536.
Matthews, G. V. T. (1953). Sun navigation in homing pigeons. Journal of Experimental Biology
30, 243-267.
Merkel, F. W. & Wiltschko, W. (1965). Magnetismus und Richtungsfinden zugunruhiger
Rotkehlchen (Erithacus rubecula). Vogelwarte 23, 71–77.
829
830
831
Mewaldt, L. R., Cowley, L. T. & Won, P. O. (1973). Californian sparrows fail to return from
displacement to Korea. Auk 90, 857–861.
Moller, A., Sagasser, S., Wiltschko, W. & Schierwater, B. (2004). Retinal cryptochrome in a
832
migratory passerine bird: a possible transducer for the avian magnetic compass.
833
Naturwissenschaften 91, 585-588.
834
835
836
Mora, C. V., Davison, M., Wild, J. M. & Walker, M. M. (2004). Magnetoreception and its
trigeminal mediation in the homing pigeon. Nature 432, 508-511.
Mora, C. V. & Walker, M. M. (2009). Do release-site biases reflect response to the Earth's
837
magnetic field during position determination by homing pigeons? Proceedings of the
838
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276, 3295-3302.
839
840
Mouritsen, H. (2012). SENSORY BIOLOGY Search for the compass needles. Nature 484, 320321.
841
Mouritsen, H., Feenders, G., Liedvogel, M., Wada, K. & Jarvis, E. D. (2005). A night vision
842
brain area in migratory songbirds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 8339-8344.
843
844
Mouritsen, H. & Hore, P. J. (2012). The magnetic retina: light-dependent and trigeminal
magnetoreception in migratory birds. Current opinion in neurobiology 22, 343-352.
845
Mouritsen, H., Huyvaert, K. P., Frost, B. J. & Anderson, D. J. (2003). Waved albatrosses can
846
navigate with strong magnets attached to their head. Journal of Experimental Biology
847
206, 4155-4166.
848
Mouritsen, H., Janssen-Bienhold, U., Liedvogel, M., Feenders, G., Stalleicken, J., Dirks, P. &
849
Weiler, R. (2004). Cryptochromes and neuronal-activity markers colocalize in the retina
850
of migratory birds during magnetic orientation. Proceedings of the National Academy of
851
Sciences of the United States of America 101, 14294-14299.
852
Mouritsen, H. & Larsen, O. N. (2001). Migrating songbirds tested in computer-controlled Emlen
853
funnels use stellar cues for a time-independent compass. Journal of Experimental Biology
854
204, 3855-3865.
855
Muheim, R. & Akesson, S. (2002). Clock-shift experiments with Savannah sparrows,
856
Passerculus sandwichensis, at high northern latitudes. Behavioral Ecology and
857
Sociobiology 51, 394-401.
858
859
860
Munro, U., Munro, J. A., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1997a). Evidence for a
magnetite-based navigational ‘map’ in birds. Naturwissenschaften 84, 26-28.
Munro, U., Munro, J. A., Phillips, J. B. & Wiltschko, W. (1997b). Effect of wavelength of light
861
and pulse magnetisation on different magnetoreception systems in a migratory bird.
862
Australian Journal of Zoology 45, 189-198.
863
Munro, U. & Wiltschko, R. (1993). Clock shift experiments with migratory yellow faced
864
honeyeaters, Lichenostomus crysops (Meliphagidae), and australian day migrating bird.
865
Journal of Experimental Biology 181, 233-244.
866
Newton, I. (2007). The migration ecology of birds, London, Academic Press.
867
Niessner, C., Denzau, S., Gross, J. C., Peichl, L., Bischof, H.-J., Fleissner, G., Wiltschko, W. &
868
Wiltschko, R. (2011). Avian Ultraviolet/Violet Cones Identified as Probable
869
Magnetoreceptors. PLoS ONE 6, e20091.
870
Papi, F. (1992). General Aspects. In: Papi, F. (ed.) Animal Homing. London: Chapman Hall.
871
Pennycuick, C. J. (1960). The physical basis of astronavigation in birds: theoretical
872
873
considerations. Journal of Experimental Biology , 37, 573-593.
Pennycuick, C. J. (1961). Sun navigation in birds? Nature 190, 1127-1128.
874
875
Perdeck, A. C. (1958). Two types of orientation in migrating Sturnus vulgaris and Fringilla
coelebs. Ardea 46.
876
Perdeck, A. C. (1967). Orientation of starlings after displacement to Spain. Ardea 55, 194-202.
877
Phillips, J. B. (1996). Magnetic navigation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 180, 309-319.
878
Phillips, J. B., Schmidt-Koenig, K. & Muheim, R. (2006). True navigation: Sensory bases of
879
gradient maps. In: Brown, M. F. & Cook, R. G. (eds.) Animal Spatial Cognition:
880
Comparative, Neural, and Computational Approaches. [On-line]: Available:
881
www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/asc/phillips/.
882
Phillips, J. B. & Waldvogel, J. A. (1982). Reflected light cues generate the short-term deflector
883
loft effect. In: Papi, F. & Wallraff, H. G. (eds.) Avian navigation. Berlin: Springer.
884
Putman, N. F., Endres, C. S., Lohmann, C. M. F. & Lohmann, K. J. (2011). Longitude perception
885
886
and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Current Biology 21, 463-466.
Rastogi, A., Kumari, Y., Rani, S. & Kumar, V. (2011). Phase inversion of neural activity in the
887
olfactory and visual systems of a night-migratory bird during migration. European
888
Journal of Neuroscience 34, 99-109.
889
890
891
892
893
Ritz, T., Adem, S. & Schulten, K. (2000). A model for vision based magnetoreception in birds.
Biophysics Journal 78, 707-718.
Ritz, T., Thalau, P., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2004). Resonance effects
indicate radical pair mechanism for avian magnetic compass. Nature 429, 177-180.
Rogers, C. T. & Hore, P. J. (2009). Chemical magnetoreception in birds: the radical pair
894
mechanism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
895
America 106, 353-360.
896
897
898
899
900
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1960). The sun azimuth compass: one factor in the orientation of homing
pigeons. Science (New York, N.Y.) 131, 826-7.
Schmidt-Koenig, K., Ganzhorn, J. U. & Ranvaud, R. (1991). The sun compass. Experientia
Supplementum (Basel) 60, 1-15.
Schulten, K., Swenberg, C. E. & Weller, A. (1978). A biomagnetic sensory mechanism based on
901
magnetic field modulated coherent electron spin motion. Zeitschrift fur Physical
902
Chemistry NF111, 1–5.
903
Thorup, K., Bisson, I., Bowlin, M., Holland, R. A., Ramenofsky, M., Wingfield, J. & Wikelski,
904
M. (2007). Migration routes of adult and juvenile white-crowned sparrows differ after
905
continent-wide displacement during migration. PNAS 104, 18115-18119.
906
907
Thorup, K. & Holland, R. (2009). The Bird GPS. Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 35973604.
908
Thorup, K., Holland, R. A., Tottrup, A. P. & Wikelski, M. (2010). Understanding the Migratory
909
Orientation Program of Birds: Extending Laboratory Studies to Study Free-Flying
910
Migrants in a Natural Setting. Integrative and Comparative Biology 50, 315-322.
911
Thorup, K. & Rabøl, J. (2001). The orientation system and migration pattern of long-distance
912
migrants: conflict between model predictions and observed patterns. Journal of Avian
913
Biology 32, 111-119.
914
Thorup, K. & Rabøl, J. (2007). Compensatory behaviour following displacement in migratory
915
birds. A meta-analysis of cage-experiments. . Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 61,
916
825-841.
917
Thorup, K. T., Ortvad, T. E., Rabøl, J., Holland, R. A., Tøttrup, A. P. & Wikelski, M. (2011).
918
Juvenile songbirds compensate for displacement of Oceanic Islands during autumn
919
migration. PLoS One e6, 17903.
920
Treiber, C. D., Salzer, M. C., Riegler, J., Edelman, N., Sugar, C., Breuss, M., Pichler, P., Cadiou,
921
H., Saunders, M., Lythgoe, M., Shaw, J. & Keays, D. A. (2012). Clusters of iron-rich
922
cells in the upper beak of pigeons are macrophages not magnetosensitive neurons. Nature
923
484, 367-70.
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
Viguier, C. (1882). Le sens de l’orientation et ses organes chez les animaux et chez l’homme.
Review Philisosophique Frances Etranger 14:, 1–36.
Walcott, C. (1991). Magnetic maps in pigeons. In: Berthold, P. (ed.) Orientation in birds. Basel:
Birkhauser-Verlag.
Walcott, C. (1996). Pigeon homing: observations, experiments and confusions. Journal of
Experimental Biology 199, 21-27.
Walcott, C. & Michener, M. C. (1971). Sun navigation in homing pigeons-attempts to shift the
sun's coordinates. Journal of Experimental Biology , 54, 291-316.
Waldvogel, J. A. & Phillips, J. B. (1991). Olfactory cues perceived at the home loft are not
933
essential for the formation of a navigational map in pigeons. Journal of Experimental
934
Biology 155, 643-660.
935
936
937
938
Waldvogel, J. A., Phillips, J. B. & Brown, A. I. (1988). changes in the short term deflector loft
effect are linked to the sun compass of homing pigeons. Animal Behaviour 36, 150-158.
Walker, M. M. (1998). On a wing and a vector: a model for magnetic navigation in birds.
Journal of Theoretical Biology 192, 341-349.
939
940
Wallraff, H. G. (1972). Homing of Pigeons after Extirpation of their Cochleae and Lagenae.
Nature New Biology 236, 223-224.
941
Wallraff, H. G. (1974). Das Navigationssystem der Vögel, Munich, Oldenburg.
942
Wallraff, H. G. (1981). The olfactory component of pigeon navigation: steps and analysis.
943
944
945
Journal of comparative Physiology 143, 411-422.
Wallraff, H. G. (1991). Conceptual approaches to avian navigation. In: Berthold, P. (ed.)
Orientation in Birds. Basel: Birkhauser.
946
Wallraff, H. G. (2005). Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm, Berlin, Springer.
947
Wallraff, H. G. & Andreae, M. O. (2000). Spatial gradients in ratios of atmospheric trace gasses:
948
a study stimulated by experiments on bird navigation. Tellus 52B, 1138-1157.
949
Wallraff, H. G., Kiepenheuer, J., Neumann, M. F. & Streng, A. (1995). Homing experiments
950
951
with starlings deprived of the sense of smell. Condor 97, 20-26.
Wikelski, M., Kays, R. W., Kasdin, N. J., Thorup, K., Smith, J. A. & Swenson, G. W. (2007).
952
Going wild: what a global small-animal tracking system could do for experimental
953
biologists. Journal of Experimental Biology 210, 181-186.
954
955
956
Williams, N. M. & Wild, J. M. (2001). Trigeminally innervated iron-containing structures in the
beak of homing pigeons, and other birds. Brain Res. 889, 243-246.
Wiltschko, R. (1996). The function of olfactory input in pigeon orientation: Does it provide
957
navigational information or play another role? Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 113-
958
119.
959
960
Wiltschko, R., Schiffer, I., Fuhrmann, P. & Wiltschko, W. (2010). The role of magnetite based
receptors in the beak in pigeon homing. Current Biology 20, 1-5.
961
962
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts.
Animal Behaviour 65, 257-272.
963
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2006). Magnetoreception. Bioessays 28, 157-168.
964
Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2013). The magnetite-based receptors in the beak of birds and
965
their role in avian navigation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology,
966
Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 199, 89-99.
967
Wiltschko, W., Ford, H., Munro, U., Winklhofer, M. & Wiltschko, R. (2007). Magnetite-based
968
magnetoreception: the effect of repeated pulsing on the orientation of migratory birds.
969
Journal of Comparative Physiology a-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral
970
Physiology 193, 515-522.
971
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Beason, R. C., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1994). A magnetic pulse
972
leads to a temporary deflection in the orientation of migratory birds. Experientia 50, 697-
973
700.
974
975
976
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1993). Red light disrupts magnetic
orientation of migratory birds. Nature 364, 525-527.
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1998). Effect of a magnetic pulse on the
977
orientation of Silvereyes Zosterops l. lateralis, during spring migration. Journal of
978
Experimental Biology 201, 3257-3261.
979
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2006). Bird Navigation: What type of
980
information does the magnetite-based receptor provide. Proceedings of the Royal Society
981
B-Biological Sciences 273, 2815-2820.
982
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2009). Avian orientation: the pulse effect
983
is mediated by the magnetite receptors in the upper beak. Proceedings of the Royal
984
Society B-Biological Sciences 276, 2227-2232.
985
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Wiltschko, R. & Kirschvink, J. L. (2002a). Magnetite-based
986
magnetoreception in birds: the effect of a biasing field and a pulse on migratory behavior.
987
Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 3031-3037.
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
Wiltschko, W., Traudt, J., Gunturkun, O., Prior, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2002b). Lateralisation of
magnetic compass orientation in a migratory bird. Nature 419, 467-470.
Wiltschko, W. & Wiltchko, R. (2007). Magnetoreception in birds: two receptors for two different
tasks. Journal for Ornithology 148, S61-S67.
Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1972). Magnetic compass of European Robins. Science 176, 6264.
Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1995). Migratory orientation of European robins is affected by
995
the wavelength of light as well as by a magnetic pulse. Journal of comparative
996
Physiology A 177, 363–369.
997
998
999
1000
1001
1002
Wu, L.-Q. & Dickman, J. D. (2011). Magnetoreception in an Avian Brain in Part Mediated by
Inner Ear Lagena. Current Biology 21, 418-423.
Wu, L.-Q. & Dickman, J. D. (2012). Neural Correlates of a Magnetic Sense. Science 336, 10541057.
Yorke, E. D. (1979). A possible magnetic transducer in birds. Journal of Theoretical Biology 77,
101–105.
1003
Zapka, M., Heyers, D., Hein, C. M., Engels, S., Schneider, N.-L., Hans, J., Weiler, S., Dreyer,
1004
D., Kishkinev, D., Wild, J. M. & Mouritsen, H. (2009). Visual but not trigeminal
1005
mediation of magnetic compass information in a migratory bird. Nature 461, 1274-1277.
1006
1007
Figure legends
1008
Figure 1.
1009
Migratory true navigation (A1, A2) is distinct from homing (B) in that the goal is dependent
1010
upon the season. If displaced during the breeding season the animal homes to the same location.
1011
If displaced from the breeding ground during or just prior to migration, the animal will navigate
1012
to the winter ground (A1), or if displaced from the winter ground, to the breeding ground (A2).
1013
Whether this requires different mechanisms or cues that homing has not yet been established.
1014
Figure 2.
1015
In a bi-coordinate map, two gradients (represented by the broad intersecting arrows A and B) are
1016
learned by exploration in the home breeding area. As long as the gradients continue to increase
1017
or decrease predictably, then if a bird is displaced, it can compare the values at the displaced
1018
location with those at the desired goal to calculate the direction of displacement. Only by
1019
learning the values of the winter area can the bird navigate to it, and so migratory true navigation
1020
can only be achieved by adult birds that have made a previous journey.
1021
1022
1023
Figure 3.
1024
A simplified schematic of the radical pair reaction. A photoreceptive molecule forms radical
1025
pairs in the presence of specific wavelengths of light. An applied magnetic field alters the yield
1026
of conversion between singlet and triplet states (yield A vs. yield B in the diagram), leading to
1027
different expression of the signalling state of the molecule (after (Rogers and Hore, 2009)). It has
1028
been hypothesised that this signalling state may be expressed as patterns on the retina, thus
1029
allowing to bird to see the magnetic field, but this has not yet been confirmed (Ritz et al., 2000) .
1030
Figure 4.
1031
Chains of single domain magnetite align with a biasing magnetic field in the direction of
1032
magnetisation (indicated by the red end). Application of a strong magnetic pulse antiparallel to
1033
the direction of magnetisation will re-magnetise in the opposite direction. If such chains are
1034
present in sensory cells and were free to rotate they would give different information about the
1035
magnetic field after such a treatment. In practice however, the effect of pulse treatments on birds
1036
do not clearly indicate that such structures exist.
Download