1 True navigation in birds: from quantum physics to global migration 2 Richard A. Holland 3 School of Biological Sciences, Queen’s University of Belfast, 97 Lisburn Road, BT9 7BL, 4 UK. 5 Short title: Bird navigation 6 Abstract 7 Birds are capable of true navigation, the ability to return to a known goal from place they have 8 never visited before. This is demonstrated most spectacularly during the vast migratory journeys 9 made by these animals year after year often between continents and occasionally global in 10 nature. However, it remains one of the great unanswered questions in science, despite more than 11 50 years of research in this field. Nevertheless, the study of true navigation in birds has made 12 significant advances in the previous 20 years, in part thanks to the integration of many 13 disciplines outside its root in behavioural biology, to address questions of neurobiology, 14 molecular aspects and the physics of sensory systems and environmental cues involved in bird 15 navigation, often involving quantum physics. However, true navigation remains a controversial 16 field, with many conflicting and confusing results making interpretation difficult, particularly for 17 those outside or new to the field. Unlike many general texts on migration, which avoid 18 discussion of these issues, this review will present these conflicting findings and assess the state 19 of the field of true navigation during bird migration. 20 Keywords: Navigation, Migration, Orientation, Bird, Magnetoreception, Olfaction, Map 21 22 Introduction 23 The apparent ability of migratory birds to make journeys of thousands of miles, crossing deserts, 24 oceans and mountain ranges, sometimes even circumnavigating the globe has long fascinated 25 both scientists and laymen alike. Fifty years of intensive research on the mechanisms and 26 sensory cues required have revealed much about the way birds can achieve this feat of navigation 27 with such precision but also leaves many open questions and the field is one that is seen as beset 28 with controversy over conflicting results (Alerstam, 2006). Recently, this problem was described 29 as a “chronic disease” (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012), suggesting that the field is unhealthy, in a 30 scientific sense, and data should not be trusted. The “mystery” of how birds navigate continues 31 to be alluded to both in popular and professional media (Baker, 1984, HollandThorup and 32 Wikelski, 2007), and remains one of the great unanswered questions in science (Kennedy and 33 Norman, 2005) but in the last 20 years animal navigation has taken huge strides forwards by 34 becoming a truly interdisciplinary field. Researchers from physical, chemical, histological, 35 neuropysiological and electrophysiological disciplines all contribute to our understanding of bird 36 navigation and a researcher working in this field must now cast their literature search far wider 37 than the traditional behaviour focused journals that the early work was published in. This 38 combination of a difficulty in interpreting conflicting results and the diverse fields which 39 contribute to our understanding of bird navigation may make a daunting prospect for those new 40 to the subject. It is thus the aim of this review to assess these conflicting results and integrate the 41 new information from other disciplines from the perspective of a behavioural biologist working 42 at the level of the organism, in order to make the field more accessible to new scientists entering 43 the field from this area, and while remaining critical, present a positive outlook for the field of 44 bird navigation. Finally, it will identify the key questions that remain in true navigation in birds 45 that must be tackled if the subject is to be resolved. 46 Migratory true navigation 47 What is true navigation? 48 Donald Griffin was the first to conceptualise bird navigation (Griffin, 1952) and he recognised a 49 specific form of navigational challenge, which he defined “type III”, in which the bird was able 50 to return to a goal after being displaced (even artificially) to an unknown area. Subsequently the 51 term “true navigation” was adopted to describe this by Keeton (Keeton, 1974), although Keeton 52 used it as a term to describe all forms of orientation and navigation from unfamiliar area that 53 were not explained by other processes. This was problematic as true navigation was defined by 54 that which could not be explained by other means, rather than as a testable hypothesis (Wiltschko 55 and Wiltschko, 2003). However, over time a workable hypothesis for true navigation emerged as 56 a number of consistent definitions acknowledged true navigation to be the ability to return to a 57 known goal using only cues detected locally, not by cues detected during the displacement e.g. 58 (Able, 2001, Phillips, 1996, Papi, 1992, PhillipsSchmidt-Koenig and Muheim, 2006). The most 59 current definition of true navigation is “the ability of an animal to return to its original location 60 after displacement to a site in unfamiliar territory, without access to familiar landmarks, goal 61 emanating cues, or information about the displacement route” (Phillips et al., 2006). This 62 definition does not specifically recognise migratory navigation however, in which the displaced 63 animal may not be navigating to its original location prior to displacement (i.e. homing) but a 64 final breeding or wintering area that it did not set out from. Hereafter this is defined as migratory 65 true navigation: the ability of an animal to navigate to a specific breeding or wintering area (that 66 it has not just set out from) following displacement. 67 Evidence for migratory true navigation 68 Initially, a series of displacement experiments on migrating birds using mark/recapture 69 techniques gathered evidence for true navigation (reviewed in (Thorup and Holland, 2009)). The 70 clearest example (Perdeck, 1958) demonstrated that adult but not juvenile birds are capable of 71 migratory true navigation. More recent studies have shown that adult, but not juvenile white 72 crowned sparrows are able to head towards their winter area within the first 100km of departure 73 from the site of displacement of 3700km from their normal route during autumn migration 74 (Thorup et al., 2007), and that reed warblers can correct for displacements of 1000km during 75 their first return migrating to their previous natal area (ChernetsovKishkinev and Mouritsen, 76 2008). Migratory true navigation is thus experience based, i.e. an ability to correct and return to a 77 known goal from an unfamiliar place is a consequence of information learned on a previous 78 journey to, or from that goal (figure 1). The test of true navigation is thus being able to correct 79 after displacement to a novel location. A few studies suggest that juvenile birds may in some 80 circumstances appear to make corrections for displacements (Thorup et al., 2011, Thorup and 81 Rabøl, 2007, Thorup and Rabøl, 2001, Åkesson et al., 2005), but it is not clear whether this is the 82 result of homing to a known goal along the migratory route (e.g. the last known stopover site or 83 the natal area) or part of an inherited programme that allows them to compensate for 84 displacements. Such a mechanism has been described in sea turtles (Putman et al., 2011), but it 85 remains to be seen whether either of these mechanisms exist, or the more common viewpoint of 86 an inherited compass direction is the only mechanism juveniles possess. 87 What is less often cited are the failures of displaced birds to correct for a displacement. 88 For instance, a repeat of Perdeck’s study in which adult birds were displaced to Spain did not 89 indicate that the birds could correct their orientation and return to the species winter area 90 (Perdeck, 1967). White and Golden crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii and Z. 91 altricapila) that were translocated from the USA to Korea from their winter grounds did not 92 appear to return to the USA (MewaldtCowley and Won, 1973). The fact that some birds make 93 vast migrations that are global in nature is often used to argue that true navigation ability must 94 also be global (Bingman and Cheng, 2006), but these studies suggest that there may be limits to 95 the extent of a migratory true navigation ability at least in the animals studied. Whether 96 migratory true navigation ability varies with migration distance, or has a general limit in all birds 97 is not yet known, but current evidence does suggest variation, with the results of Thorup et al. 98 (2007) indicating at least a 3700km range, while it appears shorter in Starlings, possibly in the 99 region of 2000km (Perdeck, 1967). 100 The theory of migratory true navigation. 101 As the previous paragraph demonstrates, displacement experiments provide evidence for true 102 navigation ability, but not for how they achieve it. Although not developed to describe migratory 103 true navigation specifically, the “map and compass” hypothesis was developed by Kramer to 104 explain navigation behaviour by a process that comprises 2 steps, determining position with 105 respect to the goal, “the map step” and determining direction to a goal, “the compass step” 106 (Kramer, 1953). The map and compass theory has remained the most robust explanation for 107 animal true navigation since its inception and no significant challenge to the idea that animal 108 navigation is a two-step process has been made. True navigation ability refers specifically to the 109 “map” step, the ability to locate position with respect to a goal. Experienced birds are presumed 110 to possess a “navigational map” that allows them to locate their position with respect to a final 111 goal and navigate towards it using their compass sense. One theory proposed that the map might 112 work in a way akin to our Cartesian coordinate system, with animals able to refer to 113 environmental gradients that vary predictably with latitude and longitude (figure 2). For these 114 gradients to be usable, the animal would have to learn that they vary predictably in intensity with 115 space (and possibly time) within their home range and extrapolate this beyond the learned area 116 (Wallraff, 1991, Wallraff, 1974). Thus when displaced to an unfamiliar area the animal could 117 recognise a value in the gradients that was, for example, higher than the home range and 118 recognise its displacement relative to it. For a migratory bird, the presumption is that this process 119 of learning these values occurs before departing on the first migration for the breeding area, and 120 during the first winter for the winter area. Thus, migratory birds are presumed to learn the value 121 of gradients at 2 goals. This gradient map tends to be thought of as a 2 cue system, often 122 presuming that a different environmental cue provides the longitude and latitude equivalents. 123 However, it has occasionally been suggested that different aspects of the same environmental cue 124 could form those two gradients (e.g. sun’s arc and sunrise time (Matthews, 1953), intensity and 125 slope or inclination of the magnetic field (Walker, 1998, BoströmÅkesson and Alerstam, 2012)). 126 Thus, we know that migratory birds can perform true navigation, and we have a theoretical 127 construct for how they could achieve this, but how do we study the nature of the environmental 128 cues and sensory systems required to achieve true navigation? 129 Studying migratory true navigation 130 The study of true navigation requires either displacement of the animal outside its familiar area, 131 or a simulated displacement where an environmental cue is manipulated to represent a different 132 location than the one currently experienced. The former requires the ability to study the response 133 to the displacement in the field, and the latter requires that the animal shows behaviour in the 134 laboratory that correlates with orientation decisions in the wild. First, a laboratory based 135 correlate of migratory orientation exists in the form of migratory restlessness (Emlen and Emlen, 136 1966). This has been used with great success to investigate the nature of the magnetic compass 137 sense in migrants (Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1972, Ritz et al., 2004, Wiltschko et al., 1993, 138 Zapka et al., 2009). Orientation cages provide the potential for greater control as they can be 139 performed indoors. Surprisingly, orientation cage studies have not been used as extensively to 140 investigate the cues used in the navigational map as they have the compass, despite the fact that 141 testing orientation after displacement has been shown to be possible (Thorup and Rabøl, 2007, 142 Chernetsov et al., 2008) as are simulated displacement experiments (Fransson et al., 2001, 143 Henshaw et al., 2010, DeutchlanderPhillips and Munro, 2012). Second, recently there have been 144 calls for a return to field based study of true navigation in migratory birds (Guilford et al., 2011, 145 Thorup et al., 2010, Wikelski et al., 2007). This stems from concerns that migratory restlessness 146 does not fully represent the behaviour of animals in the wild (Wikelski et al., 2007), and that we 147 do not understand the full extent of the challenges that animals face during migration (Holland et 148 al., 2007). Animal movements in the wild can now be tracked using remote monitoring devices 149 that provide the precision that was lacking in mark recapture techniques. In some cases GPS 150 precision is available and remote download from a satellite can be achieved (see (Bridge et al., 151 2011) for review of currently available technology for tracking migratory birds). However, 152 tracking devices that can follow a migratory journey with sufficient precision to test navigational 153 decisions are still too large for the small songbirds that remain the focus of much of true 154 navigation in migration. As tracking of migratory birds becomes more widespread, our 155 understanding of the navigational challenges faced by both adults and juveniles will increase 156 which will undoubtedly aid in adapting the theories of true navigation (Guilford et al., 2011). 157 However, field based study of wild birds faces the same inherent weaknesses as field based study 158 in the other model systems, in that control of access to cues is difficult. Field based study of 159 migration faces the added difficulty of predicting both the timing of departure and goal of the 160 animals. The former may cause problems in predicting the effect of treatments of sensory 161 systems particularly when they are transitory and the latter may increase the scatter in 162 experimental groups, meaning an increase in the number of animals needed. Given that tracking 163 technology remains relatively expensive and studies are often restricted by the number of devices 164 available, this may lead to inconsistencies in results through lack of statistical power. Such 165 studies are thus relatively rare, with no study of migratory true navigation using GPS telemetry 166 having yet been published. The field thus relies on two imperfect systems, a laboratory correlate 167 that provides precision and control, but which has limits in its relevance to natural behaviour, 168 and a field based system that is logistically difficult and lacks sufficient power at present. 169 The role of environmental cues in true navigation 170 The sensory basis of the true navigation map contributes significantly to bird navigation’s 171 reputation as a controversial field. Many general reviews of migration that include a chapter on 172 navigation avoid discussion of this sub-topic altogether (e.g. (Newton, 2007, Dingle, 1996)). 173 Repeatability continues to dog the field and certainly, interpreting findings where no effect of a 174 treatment is obtained is problematic. However, simply ignoring the large amount of research that 175 has attempted to elucidate the sensory basis of true navigation does a disservice to the field. 176 Without an understanding of research that has attempted to understand this, advances cannot be 177 made. The remainder of this review will thus assess the experimental evidence for sensory cues 178 in migratory bird navigation, in the hope that understanding what has been tried, what has failed 179 and what is incomplete will aid in moving towards a resolution for this field. 180 Celestial cues 181 It has been proposed that animals could use celestial cues for navigation (Matthews, 1953, 182 Matthews, 1951, Pennycuick, 1960). Both the sun and stars can provide a cue to north-south 183 position because the zenith varies with latitude. Longitudinal displacement could potentially be 184 detected if they were able to recognise that sun or star rise time was different from that at the 185 goal site. What is more, these provide a global reference frame and so in theory the animal’s 186 position could be located anywhere on the Earth so long as a view of the cue was available. 187 However, both sun and star navigation are generally rejected based on two factors. First, tests on 188 homing pigeons have demonstrated that they have a time compensated sun compass that can be 189 manipulated by shifting their internal clock (Schmidt-KoenigGanzhorn and Ranvaud, 1991, 190 Schmidt-Koenig, 1960). This rejects sun navigation on two counts. First, it suggests that the 191 birds (or at least homing pigeons) do not note the altitude of the sun, or they would not be fooled 192 by the shifts in their internal clock and thus do not use it as a cue to latitude. Second, a 6 hour 193 forward shift in the internal clock leads to a deflection of approximately 90° counter clockwise 194 (i.e. to the west), matching the rate of movement of the sun across the sky. This is not consistent 195 with the use of the sun as a cue to longitude, which would be perceived as a displacement of 196 approximately 5000km to the west (i.e. the bird would need to fly east to return home). It has 197 been argued that such displacements are unrealistic to a homing pigeon, and so a six hour shift is 198 an unrealistic test of the sun navigation hypothesis (Pennycuick, 1961). However, subsequent 199 tests involving much smaller shifts were also consistent with sun compass but not sun navigation 200 (Walcott and Michener, 1971). On this basis sun navigation has been rejected (Baker, 1984). 201 Clock shift has also been demonstrated in migratory birds in orientation cages, which might 202 suggest it should be rejected for migratory true navigation (Able and Cherry, 1986, Able and 203 Dillon, 1977, Muheim and Akesson, 2002), and one study did not support a significant role for 204 either the sun compass or sun navigation in a migrant songbird (Munro and Wiltschko, 1993). 205 The original experiments of Emlen, which established stars as a compass cue , actually 206 provided some suggestion of time compensation, although only with three birds (Emlen, 1967). 207 However, subsequent investigation provided no evidence of time compensation (Mouritsen and 208 Larsen, 2001), without which longitude is not discernible. Additionally, there is no evidence for 209 a clock mechanism playing a role in detecting displacements per se, which would preclude both 210 star and sun navigation as a mechanism for longitude (KishkinevChernetsov and Mouritsen, 211 2010). However, a meta-analysis of displacement experiments of juvenile migratory birds in 212 orientation cages suggests that they are more likely to correct under starry skies than overcast 213 skies, suggesting a role for celestial cues in this behaviour (Thorup and Rabøl, 2007). Indeed, 214 many studies of the role of sun and stars in migratory navigation test only juvenile birds (e.g. 215 Mouritsen and Larsen 2001, Muheim and Akesson 2002), or the age is not reported (e.g. Able 216 and Dillon 1977, Able and Cherry 1986). Rejection of celestial navigation thus relies to some 217 extent on the assumption that the cues used by homing pigeons and migratory birds are the same. 218 It is however difficult to reconcile the global availability of celestial navigation with the apparent 219 limits on true navigation in some migrating songbirds (see above). 220 Infrasound 221 Sounds in the range of 0.1-10 Hz are known to spread over hundreds if not thousands of miles. If 222 stable, these have the potential to act as a gradient for navigation. Evidence has been presented 223 that pigeon homing performance is disrupted by infrasound disturbance, such as disturbance of 224 pigeon races by sonic booms of aircraft (Hagstrum, 2000, Hagstrum, 2001), or fluctuations in 225 orientation performance that correlate with atmospheric fluctuations (Hagstrum, 2013). The data, 226 while in many cases compelling, are correlational however, making it difficult to currently assess 227 whether this is a result of disruption of infrasound navigation cues, co-correlation with other 228 factors propagated by atmospheric means, or disturbance in motivation to home. An experiment 229 which removed the cochlea of homing pigeons did not produce any deficits in homing 230 performance (Wallraff, 1972), which, although not precluding that infrasound is part of a 231 multifactorial map, does not support the argument made by (Hagstrum, 2013) that infrasonic 232 cues are the sole solution to the navigational map question in pigeons. No experiment has yet 233 demonstrated any effects of infrasound on bird migration. Nevertheless, it remains a viable cue 234 which should be investigated further and the range over which it could operate makes it a 235 possibility for the distances seen in migratory displacements. 236 Olfactory cues 237 No aspect of bird navigation contributes to its reputation as a controversial field more than that 238 of the role of olfactory cues in the true navigation map. By far the majority of work has involved 239 homing pigeons and a large number of experiments, possibly more than in any other aspect of 240 bird true navigation, have been performed. A comprehensive review of these experiments is 241 available in (Wallraff, 2005) and a detailed treatment of all of these is beyond the scope of this 242 review given that the focus is on navigation in migratory birds,. However, olfactory navigation is 243 the most extensively tested hypothesis in true navigation and as such its potential role in true 244 navigation of migrants should be considered. 245 Key findings in olfactory navigation 246 Olfactory deprivation removes the ability of homing pigeons to return to the home loft, and this 247 is most clearly demonstrated by sectioning the olfactory nerve (Gagliardo et al., 2006, Gagliardo 248 et al., 2008, Gagliardo et al., 2009, Benvenuti et al., 1973). Further key findings in which 249 orientation is altered rather than impaired have been argued to suggest that the olfactory cues 250 provide navigational information to homing pigeons. A ‘false release site’ experiments in which 251 birds were transported to a releases site in one direction, allowed to sample air from this site, and 252 then transported to a release site in the opposite direction without further access to environmental 253 odours found that birds flew in the direction expected if they were trying to home from the 254 original release site (Benvenuti and Wallraff, 1985). An experiment in which artificial odours 255 (benzaldehyde) were presented to pigeons at the loft from the north west by fans found that when 256 displaced with benzaldehyde on their beaks, the birds oriented in the direction consistent with a 257 north west displacement, rather than with the actual home direction (IoaleNozzolini and Papi, 258 1990). Further experiments in which lofts are shielded or winds are manipulated argued that 259 pigeons learn to associate odours brought by different wind directions with different directions 260 (Baldaccini et al., 1975, Ioale et al., 1978, FoaBagnoli and Giongo, 1986, Gagliardo et al., 2001). 261 In theory this does not require sampling of gradients as suggested by the bi-coordinate map, but 262 merely association between an odour and a direction. 263 Criticisms of olfactory navigation 264 Olfactory navigation has been criticised on a number of counts. First, lack of repeatability of the 265 effects of olfactory deprivation argues that olfaction is neither the only, nor an essential cue 266 (Wiltschko, 1996). However, it is not clear whether this lack of repeatability comes from 267 redundancy in navigation cues or from variations caused by difficulties in control of the field 268 based system of experimentation, or in the experiments themselves. If homing performance of 269 birds treated with zinc sulphate is considered, olfactory deprivation has been demonstrated in a 270 number of countries and on four continents (Wallraff, 2005). A number of the key findings have 271 also been challenged. The deflector loft effect is shown in some cases to be a consequence of 272 deflection of polarized light, involved in compass calibration, as anosmic birds still deflect after 273 exposure (Phillips and Waldvogel, 1982, Waldvogel and Phillips, 1991, WaldvogelPhillips and 274 Brown, 1988). However, the similar findings of experiments in which winds are reversed or 275 shielded are not challenged by this discovery. The question of whether olfactory inputs are 276 navigational or related to motivational factors has always been a concern in interpretation 277 (Wiltschko, 1996). In support of this odours appear to ‘activate’ other navigational processes in 278 young pigeons navigating by route reversal (JorgeMarques and Phillips, 2009). Jorge et al. found 279 that young pigeons, which navigate by route reversal, were unable to orient homeward if 280 transported in filtered air, but could if transported either with access to natural odours, or 281 artificial ‘novel’ odours. This argues that smelling ‘non-home’ odours I, triggers the bird to 282 access a navigation system based on other cues. The site simulation experiments of Benvenuti 283 and Wallraff (1985) have also been argued to be a consequence of activation of a navigational 284 map by non-navigational olfactory cues rather than navigational in themselves (JorgeMarques 285 and Phillips, 2010). Presenting non-specific odours at the false release site produced the same 286 behaviour as access to natural odours. A subsequent test of the activation hypothesis did not 287 support a role for activation however, birds transported to a release site with access to novel 288 odours were no more likely to orient homewards than those transported in filtered air (Gagliardo 289 et al., 2011). However, they used higher concentrations of novel odours than those used in the 290 previous navigation experiments, which it has been argued would make the pigeons anosmic 291 (Phillips, personal communication). However, the experiments of (Ioale et al., 1990) cannot be 292 explained by activation, as if the benzaldehyde odour was activating a non-olfactory navigational 293 map, it would result in homeward orientation, not orientation consistent with a north west 294 displacement. One striking finding of the experiments on olfactory navigation in pigeons is that 295 if olfactory navigation is correct generally, it suggests that the view of redundancy of cues is not 296 correct. Where olfactory deprevation effects have been demonstrated they lead to significant 297 impairment of homing performance of pigeons at unfamiliar release sites, i.e. the majority do not 298 return to the home loft. If olfactory cues are navigational, this argues that in their absence no 299 cues are available to take their place which goes against the widely held view that the 300 navigational map must be made up of redundant cues (Walcott, 1996, Wiltschko et al., 2010). 301 Olfactory navigation thus continues to provide debate and has not been widely accepted as an 302 explanation for true navigation in homing pigeons. 303 While the olfactory navigation hypothesis is by far the most extensively tested when 304 considering pigeon homing, it has rarely been considered when discussing true navigation in 305 migrating birds. Stable odour gradients such as would be necessary for a bi-coordinate map have 306 not been demonstrated to exist beyond approximately 200km (Wallraff and Andreae, 2000). This 307 makes it difficult to explain the majority of displacement experiments on migrants by the use of 308 olfactory navigation. Nevertheless, two experiments on homing of migratory birds in the 309 breeding season found a deficit in performance after olfactory deprivation (Wallraff et al., 1995, 310 FiaschiFarina and Ioalé, 1974). More surprisingly, a recent experiment demonstrated that adult 311 catbirds displaced 1000km east from Illinois to Princeton in the USA, subjected to olfactory 312 deprivation by zinc sulphate treatment and then radio-tracked from a light aircraft were unable to 313 correct for the displacement in the way that controls were (Holland et al., 2009). If this finding is 314 borne out by further experimental support and shown to be a deficit based on removal of 315 navigation cues, then it may require a re-analysis of the bi-coordinate map theory for true 316 navigation. It appears to be hard to explain how stable olfactory gradients could exist over the 317 1000km necessary to explain this behaviour navigationally. Homing pigeons have not been 318 shown to use olfactory cues beyond 700km, and then only if they had access to environmental air 319 during the displacement (Wallraff, 1981). 320 With regard to the use of olfactory signals by migrants, an interesting parallel finding 321 from a neurobiological study of migratory restlessness is that both visual and olfactory areas of 322 the brain become more active at night during the migratory period, while they are most active 323 during the day outside this time (Rastogi et al., 2011). This suggests that olfaction plays a 324 significant role in migratory behaviour, but it is still an open question as to what role this is. A 325 recent hypothesis proposes that in fact the primary role of olfaction across organisms (and thus 326 reason for its evolution) is navigation (Jacobs, 2012). If it does indeed turn out to be the case 327 then theories of true navigation based on a bi-coordinate map made stable environmental 328 gradients may need to be significantly reconsidered, since olfactory cues do not seem to fit easily 329 into this paradigm. 330 Magnetic cues 331 The intensity of the Earth’s magnetic field was proposed as a cue for bird navigation over a 332 century ago (Viguier, 1882). The Earth’s magnetic field is stronger at the poles than at the 333 equator and it therefore has the potential to indicate latitudinal position. However, this is only 334 functional over a relatively coarse scale (Bingman and Cheng, 2006).There are variations in the 335 strength of the magnetic field at a fine scale that mean it may be stronger at a lower latitudes in 336 some cases and varies with longitude rather than latitude in some places (Phillips et al., 2006). 337 Thus, even at a coarse scale the magnetic field may not be as consistent a cue to latitudinal 338 position as it is often portrayed. In seeming support of this, a number of experiments in which 339 magnets are attached to the heads of birds homing over long distance failed to find any deficit in 340 homing performance (BenhamouBonadonna and Jouventin, 2003, Bonadonna et al., 2005, 341 Mouritsen et al., 2003). However, since the 1960’s evidence of behavioural responses to 342 artificially changing the Earth’s magnetic field have been obtained (Merkel and Wiltschko, 1965, 343 Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1972). To date at least 24 species of bird have been shown to respond 344 to changes in the Earth’s magnetic field (Wiltschko and Wiltchko, 2007) but by far the majority 345 of studies on magnetoreception in birds involve investigating its use as a compass and it has been 346 challenging to demonstrate the use of the magnetic field for a map (Phillips et al., 2006). 347 Artificial displacement experiments, where the magnetic field is changed to indicate different 348 latitudes to birds orienting in emlen funnels, provide some support that birds recognise magnetic 349 intensity signatures as a cue to end migration (Henshaw et al., 2010, FischerMunro and Phillips, 350 2003). However, in these studies (performed on Silvereyes, Zosterops l. lateralis and Lesser 351 Whitethroats Sylvia curruca) intensity signatures indicating displacements outside of the normal 352 range and migration route of the population did not produce navigational responses, as would be 353 expected for a map cue. Instead they become disoriented. This may be a similar response to that 354 seen in juvenile migrants, in which magnetic “sign posts” indicate latitudes at which innate 355 compass directions must change for successful migration (Beck and Wiltschko, 1988) and thus 356 the birds may merely stop migrating when a certain latitude is reached. Interestingly, this is also 357 consistent with activation, as proposed for olfactory cues, with magnetic field signatures 358 activating a non-magnetic navigation system below some threshold value, but once that value is 359 reached, the navigation system is no longer activated, even if the magnetic value is far greater 360 than the threshold. A recent follow-up study has indicated that only adults are affected by such 361 magnetic displacements suggesting that it is a different behaviour than the innate signpost 362 recognition seen in juveniles (Deutchlander et al., 2012). However, the lack of orientation 363 towards the winter site when the artificial displacement was north of it remained, making it 364 difficult to conclude that the behaviour represented true navigation in the strict sense rather than 365 an age dependant response to latitudinal sign posts, or activation of other navigational cues. 366 Recall however, that when (Perdeck, 1958) displaced adult starlings outside the wintering 367 latitude, they were able to correct and return to the normal winter area. This indicates the 368 challenge of orientation experiments: it is possible that different site fidelity is present in the 369 different species tested, with starlings showing more fidelity to their winter site than Silvereyes 370 or Lesser Whitethroats, and thus these two species do not represent the ideal model for this test. 371 Contrast this to similar experiments on newts, turtles and spiny lobsters, which have been 372 demonstrated to alter their orientation in response to artificial displacements either north or south 373 of their current position (Fischer et al., 2001, Lohmann et al., 2004, Lohmann et al., 1995). 374 Experiments on the orientation performance of homing pigeons has also been shown to be 375 disrupted at magnetic anomalies (areas with stronger or weaker magnetic intensity than 376 expected), which suggests that magnetic intensity plays a role in their navigational map 377 (DennisRayner and Walker, 2007, Walcott, 1991, Wiltschko et al., 2010, Mora and Walker, 378 2009), although many of these experiments are conducted within a range where the variation in 379 magnetic intensity is thought to make the earth’s magnetic field unreliable as a cue to position 380 (Phillips et al., 2006). This may indicate a different mechanism than that proposed for true 381 navigation in migrating birds, or perhaps that magnetic intensity correlates with other factors 382 which disrupt orientation in these experiments (Wallraff, 2005). Part of the challenge in 383 demonstrating a role for magnetic intensity has been because most navigational experiments 384 involve sensory manipulation, and the way in which birds sense the magnetic field is by far the 385 most uncertain aspect of navigation research. However, within the last 20 years, significant 386 advances have been made in this area. This has involved the integration of theoretical work from 387 physics, biochemistry, neurobiology and molecular biology alongside traditional behavioural 388 experiments. As a consequence, we now have an understanding of the way birds may perceive 389 aspects of the magnetic field and how this may contribute to the map and the compass aspects of 390 true navigation. An understanding of the potential sensory pathways is thus crucial to 391 understanding the behavioural experiments that support the use of the magnetic field as a map. 392 The magnetic sense: different receptors for different tasks? 393 The behavioural evidence for magnetoreception was met with initial scepticism due to the lack of 394 an obvious sense organ. However, consideration of physical principles of the magnetic field 395 means that such sense organs need not be located at the surface in the same way as photo or 396 auditory receptors must: the Earth’s magnetic field can pervade all tissue. During the 1980’s 397 several models were proposed for magnetoreception but 2 have withstood scrutiny: a mechanism 398 based on photoreceptive molecules (the radical pair mechanism) and a mechanism based on 399 magnetic iron particles (the ferrimagnetic mechanism). 400 Radical pairs 401 Magnetically sensitive reactions involve radicals in which unpaired electrons are present in 402 different “spin” states, either antiparallel (“singlet” state) or parallel (“triplet” state) (Rogers and 403 Hore, 2009). The yield of the different states has been demonstrated to be influenced by strong 404 magnetic fields, and based on this it was hypothesised that a molecule that formed such radicals 405 in different yields depending on the magnetic field alignment could be the basis of a 406 magnetoreceptor (SchultenSwenberg and Weller, 1978) (figure 3). It was subsequently 407 discovered that magnetic compass orientation is dependent on the wavelength of light (Wiltschko 408 et al., 1993, Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2006) and so the model was modified to suggest that the 409 molecule involved in the radical pair process was photoreceptive and that a photon of light 410 would instigate this reaction (RitzAdem and Schulten, 2000). Evidence that the magnetic 411 compass was lateralized via the right eye to the left brain hemisphere suggested that the magnetic 412 field was perceived through the eyes (Wiltschko et al., 2002b), although see (Hein et al., 2011) 413 for evidence of no lateralization. A study involving ZENK, an immediate early gene which is 414 expressed in neurones indicated that an area of the brain called cluster-N, responsible for night 415 vision was active during migratory restlessness (Mouritsen et al., 2005). A subsequent study in 416 which this area of the brain was lesioned indicated that migratory robins could no longer use 417 their magnetic compass (Zapka et al., 2009). Thus, migratory songbirds appear to possess a 418 magnetoreceptor mediated by the visual system which is based on a photoreceptive molecule. 419 Evidence that this is because of a radical pair mechanism comes from an experiment based on 420 the prediction that the interaction between a radical pair and the magnetic field could be 421 disrupted by a weak electromagnetic field in the radio spectrum (1.315 MHz, the so called 422 Larmor frequency). It was indeed the case that migratory robins could no longer orient in an 423 emlen funnel when such a field was applied (Ritz et al., 2004). The molecule involved has been 424 proposed to be a cryptochrome (Ritz et al., 2000). This is a blue light receptor and appears to 425 form long lived radical pairs, which would be necessary for it to work as a magnetoreceptor 426 (Liedvogel et al., 2007). Four different cryptochromes have been found in the eyes of migratory 427 birds, Cry 1a, (Mouritsen et al., 2004, Moller et al., 2004, Niessner et al., 2011), Cry 1b (Moller 428 et al., 2004), Cry 2 (Mouritsen et al., 2004) and Cry 4 (Mouritsen et al., 2004). In terms of figure 429 3, it is thought that the radical pair comprises a flavosemiquinone radical and a terminal residue 430 of a conserved triad of tryptophan residues (a flavin-tryptophan radical pair) (Maeda et al., 2012, 431 Biskup et al., 2009). Based on our understanding of how a similar reaction occurs in plants, the 432 flavosemiquinone radical would appear to lead to the signalling state (Bouly et al., 2007). No 433 direct evidence yet exists however, to demonstrate that cryptochrome is the primary sensing 434 molecule involved in magnetoreception (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012, Liedvogel and Mouritsen, 435 2010). More detailed discussion of the issues around the radical pair compass can be found in 436 (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012, Rogers and Hore, 2009). What is crucial to this review is, does it 437 have a role in the navigational map? All the experiments described above involved disrupting the 438 magnetic compass, in no case was there an indication that the radical pair pathway is involved in 439 map navigation. It does not appear that this mechanism detects intensity, nor indeed the polarity 440 of the magnetic field, only inclination (Ritz et al., 2000). In theory, inclination could be used to 441 detect latitude, so there is no reason why the radical pair mechanism could not be involved in the 442 navigational map, but no experiment has tested this hypothesis. This may be because it could be 443 challenging to design an experiment that is able to disentangle the use of the radical pair sense 444 for a compass from its use in a map. 445 The Ferrimagnetic sense 446 Ferrimagnetic materials are those in which spontaneous magnetization occurs because the 447 magnetic moments of atoms are opposed but unequal. This is seen in iron oxides, including the 448 oldest known magnetic substance, magnetite. Ferrimagnetic material exists in a number of 449 crystalline “domains”, including multi, single and supaparamagnetic. Multi domain magnetite 450 has no magnetisation, single domain has a permanent magnetic moment whereas 451 superparamagnetic magnetite has a fluctuating magnetic moment, but it can be aligned to an 452 external magnetic field (Kirschvink and Walker, 1985). Based on the discovery that bacteria 453 containing single domain magnetite passively align to the magnetic field (Blakemore, 1975), and 454 that magnetite is a biogenic material that is present widely in the tissue of a diverse array of 455 organisms, it was proposed that such material could form the basis of a magnetic sense in multi- 456 cellular organisms (Kirschvink and Gould, 1981, Yorke, 1979). To test this, it was proposed that 457 the physical properties of the ferrimagnetic material could be used to predict the presence of 458 magnetic material in sensory cells in the same way as it had been done in bacteria (Kirschvink, 459 1982). If ferrimagnetic material was involved in a sensory receptor that detected the Earth’s 460 magnetic field then a brief strong magnetic pulse that exceeded the coercivity (the magnetic 461 force required to reduce the magnetisation of the substance to zero) would re-magnetise the 462 substance in the opposite direction if applied antiparallel to the original magnetization (Figure 4). 463 For most biogenic magnetite, the strength required to re-magnetise would be 0.1T, 5000 times 464 the strength of the Earth’s magnetic field (Kirschvink et al., 1985). If single domain magnetite 465 was present it would be re-magnetised, and if used by sensory cells, in theory, would lead to a 466 change in the information the receptor gave. Subsequently, a significant number of experiments 467 have treated birds, with a strong magnetic pulse and indeed found that their orientation is 468 affected by such a pulse. Effects have been found both on migratory birds tested in emlen 469 funnels (BeasonDussourd and Deutschlander, 1995, Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995, Wiltschko 470 et al., 1994, Wiltschko et al., 1998), in naturally migrating birds (Holland, 2010) and in homing 471 pigeons (BeasonWiltschko and Wiltschko, 1997). In all these cases a magnetic pulse leads to a 472 deflection in orientation. However, where the pulse was applied antiparallel to the direction of 473 magnetisation, the expected reorientation in the opposite direction did not occur (Wiltschko et 474 al., 2002a, Holland, 2010). This is not consistent with single domain magnetite that is free to 475 rotate in the way a bacteria cell can and does not fit with the popularized concept of a 476 ferrimagnetic sense consisting of tiny compass needles (Mouritsen, 2012). Nor is the fact that the 477 pulse effect appears to be temporary, with birds returning to normal orientation after 478 approximately 10 days (Wiltschko et al., 2007, Wiltschko et al., 1998). This does not support the 479 permanent re-magnetisation of magnetic material. One pulse experiment demonstrated that the 480 deflecting effect of the pulse was removed if the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve 481 (which innervates the beak) was anaesthetised with lidocane, a local anaesthetic (Beason and 482 Semm, 1996). This suggested that the magnetic pulse effected receptors located in the beak area 483 and the trigeminal nerve was responsible for conveying the input from these receptors to the 484 brain. 485 Two subsequent studies have confirmed the finding that the trigeminal nerve conveys 486 magnetic information. Mora and colleagues (Mora et al., 2004) conditioned homing pigeons to a 487 magnetic intensity anomaly, and found that they could no longer discriminate if the trigeminal 488 nerve was lesioned (although see (KirschvinkWinklhofer and Walker, 2010) for possible 489 weaknesses in the experimental design and (KishkinevMouritsen and Mora, 2012) for failure to 490 repeat the conditioning paradigm). This indicated that the trigeminal nerve was responsible for 491 conveying information on the magnetic field. Following this a study of ZENK expression 492 indicated activation of neurons in the trigeminal brainstem only in migratory robins orienting in a 493 magnetic field that had an intact trigeminal nerve (Heyers et al., 2010). However, homing 494 pigeons that had their trigeminal nerve lesioned were not disrupted in their homing performance 495 (Gagliardo et al., 2006, Gagliardo et al., 2008, Gagliardo et al., 2009). Until recently this made 496 the study of Beason and Semm (1996) the only study to date to indicate a role for the trigeminal 497 nerve in the process of navigation, but what aspect of navigation? Lesions of the trigeminal nerve 498 do not appear to affect magnetic compass orientation in juvenile robins (Zapka et al., 2009), and 499 the pulse deflects the orientation of birds in emlen funnels, but does not affect the magnetic 500 compass (Wiltschko et al., 2006, Munro et al., 1997b). A particular design of pulse experiment 501 however suggests a possible role for the ferrimagnetic receptor in bird navigation. Pulses only 502 appear to effect the orientation of adult migrating birds, not juveniles (Munro et al., 1997a, 503 Munro et al., 1997b, Holland and Helm, 2013), which suggests that the ferrimagnetic sense is 504 involved in an experience based mechanism possessed by adult but not juvenile birds. Since 505 adults have true navigation, this suggests the ferrimagnetic sense is involved in the true 506 navigation map. A recent study has also shown that migrating reed warblers returning to their 507 breeding grounds, are unable to correct for a displacement of 1000km eastwards if the trigeminal 508 nerve is cut, unlike intact and sham operated birds, who are able to do so (Kishkinev et al., 509 2013). This finding, on migratory birds, is in contrast to the findings on homing pigeons, where 510 no role for the trigeminal nerve in navigation is supported. 511 On this basis it is argued that migrating birds possess two magnetoreceptive pathways: a 512 radical pair mechanism in the eye, which is responsible for at least compass orientation, and a 513 ferrimagnetic sense, which is implicated in the detection of magnetic intensity and is involved in 514 the navigational map (Wiltschko and Wiltchko, 2007). However, caution is urged in accepting 515 this interpretation without question. Adult but not juvenile migratory birds have been shown to 516 respond to changes in intensity (Deutchlander et al., 2012) and adult but not juvenile migratory 517 birds have been shown to be affected by a strong magnetic pulse (Munro et al., 1997a, Holland 518 and Helm, 2013), but there is no direct causal link between the two. Similarly, the trigeminal 519 nerve has been shown to be involved in detecting the magnetic field (Mora et al., 2004), the 520 pulse effect no longer persists when this is anaesthetised, and migratory birds with trigeminal 521 nerve section can no longer correct for displacement (Kishkinev et al., 2013), but there is no 522 direct link between the pulse and magnetic intensity, or the trigeminal nerve and magnetic 523 intensity. Evidence for a ferrimagnetic sense that is responsible for detecting intensity as part of a 524 true navigational map is thus based on several indirect links. We do not know for certain that the 525 pulse affects a receptor that detects intensity, only that it changes navigation behaviour and that 526 the behaviour appears to be mediated by the trigeminal nerve. To be certain of that we would 527 need to know the nature and location of the magnetic receptor. 528 The ferrimagnetic receptor: magnetite or macrophage? 529 Initially, iron-containing cells found in the upper beak of the homing pigeons and other birds 530 were suggested as magnetoreceptors innervated via the trigeminal nerve, although no clear 531 sensory receptor was identified (Beason and Nichols, 1984, Williams and Wild, 2001). A 532 structure that has the potential to be a magnetic receptor has been described in the beak of 533 homing pigeons (Fleissner et al., 2003), chickens (Gallus domesticus), Garden Warblers (Sylvia 534 borin) and Robins (Erithacus rubecula) (Falkenberg et al., 2010). The structure appears to 535 consist of sensory dendrites in the upper beak, which contains iron rich bullets and an iron 536 containing vesicle. It is argued that these are distributed in such a way as to provide magnetic 537 field information in three axes and thus form elements of a magnetometer. Appearing to support 538 the argument that this is a magnetoreceptor, the effect of a magnetic pulse disappears when the 539 upper beak is anaesthetised with local anaesthetic (Wiltschko et al., 2009). The disrupting effect 540 of a magnetic anomaly on homing pigeon orientation also disappears when the beak is 541 anaesthetised (Wiltschko et al., 2010). Again, however, the link is indirect. It is not certain that 542 the anaesthetic is acting directly on the magnetoreceptor in these experiments, and the effects of 543 local anaesthetics have been questioned (Mouritsen and Hore, 2012). A further significant 544 cautionary note to the beak based magnetoreceptor theory has recently emerged. A thorough 545 study made on homing pigeons (Treiber et al., 2012) strongly suggested that the majority of cells 546 identified as containing iron, if not all, both in the upper beak and other parts of the body, such as 547 the skin, respiratory epithelium and feather folliculi are macrophages, cells responsible for 548 engulfing waste and pathogens in the body. Treiber et al. (2012) argue that the structures 549 described in previous work are thus not sensory cells at all. This raises the question of whether a 550 magnetoreceptor exists in the beak. However, the work of Treiber et al. (2012) should not be 551 over interpreted. While the burden of proof is on those who argue that the beak is the site of 552 magnetoreception (Mouritsen, 2012), Treiber et al. do acknowledge that there may be 553 magnetoreceptors in some as yet unidentified location in the beak. Added to this, a number of 554 behavioural studies supporting magnetoreception in the beak have been identified (Wiltschko 555 and Wiltschko, 2013). 556 A second potential site of a magnetoreceptor has also been identified, in the inner ear 557 lagena of homing pigeons, using electrophysiology recordings (Wu and Dickman, 2011, Wu and 558 Dickman, 2012). Recent evidence from electron microscopy has identified iron rich cells in the 559 inner ear (Lauwers et al., 2013), although they do not fit all the properties of a magnetoreceptor. 560 Furthermore, experiments on homing pigeons did not show any deficit in homing with the inner 561 ear removed (Wallraff, 1972), so unlike the beak based sense, behavioural evidence is lacking. 562 Future perspectives: Chronic disease or rude health? 563 As noted at the start of this review, a recent review of magnetoreception suggested that this field 564 suffered from a chronic disease in its lack of repeatability of findings (Mouritsen and Hore, 565 2012). It could be argued that this applies equally to all aspects of bird navigation, with many 566 experiments failing to repeat others, or contradictory results within and between different 567 disciplines. Does this mean that research on bird navigation is in ill health? The assertion by 568 Mouritsen and Hore that experiments must be carefully controlled and designed to avoid 569 observer bias is an important one. However, the recent work of Treiber et al. (2012) that 570 questions the structure and location of magnetoreceptors could actually be viewed as a strength 571 and sign of health: of a field that welcomes new results that may force revisions of current 572 models of understanding. While many aspects of navigation are unresolved, as this review 573 indicates, that does not mean that there is no data. While the models for studying navigation are 574 imperfect, closer links between laboratory work and field work are being established and the 575 addition of new technology for studying animals in the wild will broadened our understanding of 576 the behaviour of migrating birds and the challenges they face (Guilford et al., 2011). The 577 integration of neurobiology, physics and molecular biology into the discipline is now well 578 established and has led to a number of breakthroughs in our understanding of the magnetic sense 579 as well as the role of the olfactory sense in navigation. The integration of these disciplines has 580 led to testable predictions about the structure of sensory systems and potentially the mechanisms 581 of navigation. For the field to advance further, the link between these disciplines and behavioural 582 biology needs to strengthen further, in order to reduce the “black box” understanding of some of 583 the systems involved. For example, a better knowledge of the structure of the ferromagnetic 584 sense will allow better predictions about the effect of magnetic pulse treatments to understand 585 how receptors are changed by the treatment. Strengthening this integration of other disciplines, 586 whilst maintaining the roots as a behavioural biology discipline, will ultimately lead to the 587 solution of the “mystery” of bird navigation. . I will finish this review by highlighting some of 588 the key issues that should be resolved in order for the field of true navigation in migratory birds 589 to advance 590 Key questions in migratory true navigation 591 1) Is the true navigation map unimodal, i.e. one environmental cue provides all information 592 on location, bimodal, i.e. two separate environmental cues provide different aspects of the 593 location (e.g. latitude and longitude), or redundant, i.e. do multiple cues provide the same 594 information for different aspects of the location. Solving this will help to understand 595 some of the inconsistencies and conflicting evidence in the field, as it will establish 596 whether failure to repeat is a consequence of experimental design rather than redundancy 597 of cues. 598 2) Are there one or two magnetic sensory systems with different functions? Clearly 599 establishing whether the magnetite based system is responsible for detecting intensity 600 would establish that not just direction but also other aspects of the magnetic field could 601 be used and form part of the magnetic map. 602 3) Related to 2), can a magnetite based sensory receptor be located and described? 603 Understanding the structure of the magnetoreceptor will help to provide testable 604 predictions for how it might control birds’ behaviour, particularly in light of the pulse 605 experiments. 606 4) To what extent does migratory behaviour in the wild mirror behaviour in an orientation 607 cage? The field of navigation now involves multiple disciplines including those outside 608 biology and requires a controlled laboratory based system that allows predictions to be 609 tested by isolating cues. The orientation cage provides this. However, currently, we have 610 little understanding of how small songbirds respond to displacements in the wild with 611 current techniques being too coarse (ringing, geolocators) or lacking in range (radio 612 tracking). Understanding how songbirds respond to displacements in more detail will 613 indicate the range of their navigation system and thus the extent to which environmental 614 cues will provide reliable information on their location. 615 5) Is “activation” a significant phenomenon within true navigation? The results of some 616 experiments on both olfactory and magnetic cues are consistent with them activating 617 other navigational cues, but this would appear to violate the principle of Occam’s razor 618 by adding another step to the navigation process: If activation plays a part in true 619 navigation, then it moves from a 2 step process (what is my location, what direction to 620 reach home?) to a 3 step process (Am I at home? If no, what is my location, what 621 direction to reach home?), and the two cues providing the most evidence for navigation 622 (olfactory and magnetic) become relegated to intermediate steps towards the actual 623 navigational cues. 624 Acknowledgements 625 I thank John Phillips and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript. 626 Aspects of this review also came as a result of enjoyable discussions with the Navigation 627 special interest group at the MIGRATE NSF funded meeting in Konstanz, 2010 with 628 Susanna Åkesson, Verner Bingman, Tim Guilford, Anna Gagliardo, Henrik Mouritsen, 629 Rachel Muheim, Rosie Wiltschko and Wolfgang Wiltschko. 630 631 References 632 Able, K. P. (2001). The concepts and terminology of bird navigation. Journal of Avian Biology 633 634 32, 174-183. Able, K. P. & Cherry, J. D. (1986). Mechanisms of dusk orientation in white-throated sparrows 635 (Zonotrichia albicollis): clock-shift experiments. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 636 159, 107-113. 637 638 639 Able, K. P. & Dillon, P. M. (1977). Sun compass orientation in a nocturnal migrant white throated sparrow. Condor 79, 393-395. Åkesson, S., Morin, J., Muheim, R. & Ottosson, U. (2005). Dramatic orientation shift of white- 640 crowned sparrows displaced across longitudes in the high arctic. Current Biology 15, 641 1591-1597. 642 643 Alerstam, T. (2006). Conflicting evidence about long-distance animal navigation. Science 313, 791-794. 644 Baker, R. R. (1984). Bird navigation: the solution of a mystery?, London, Hodder Arnold. 645 Baldaccini, N. E., Benvenuti, S., Fiaschi, V. & Papi, F. (1975). Pigeon Navigation - Effects of 646 Wind Deflection at Home Cage on Homing Behavior. Journal of Comparative 647 Physiology 99, 177-186. 648 Beason, R. C., Dussourd, N. & Deutschlander, M. E. (1995). Behavioral Evidence for the Use of 649 Magnetic Material in Magnetoreception by a Migratory Bird. Journal of Experimental 650 Biology 198, 141-146. 651 652 Beason, R. C. & Nichols, J. E. (1984). Magnetic Orientation and Magnetically Sensitive Material in a Transequatorial Migratory Bird. Nature 309, 151-153. 653 654 655 656 657 Beason, R. C. & Semm, P. (1996). Does the avian ophthalmic nerve carry magnetic navigational information? Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 1241-1244. Beason, R. C., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1997). Pigeon homing: Effects of magnetic pulses on initial orientation. Auk 114, 405-415. Beck, W. & Wiltschko, W. (1988). Magnetic factors control the migratory direction of Pied 658 Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca Pallas). In: Ouellet, H. (ed.) Acta 19th Congress 659 Internationa Ornithology, vol II. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press. 660 Benhamou, S., Bonadonna, F. & Jouventin, P. (2003). Successful homing of magnet-carrying 661 white-chinned petrels released in the open sea. Animal Behaviour 65, 729-734. 662 Benvenuti, S., Fiaschi, V., Fiore, L. & Papi, F. (1973). Homing performances of inexperienced 663 and directionally trained pigeons subjected to olfactory nerve section. Journal of 664 Comparative Physiology 83, 81-92. 665 Benvenuti, S. & Wallraff, H. G. (1985). Pigeon navigation: site simulation by means of 666 atmospheric odours. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 156, 737-746. 667 Bingman, V. P. & Cheng, K. (2006). Mechanisms of animal global navigation: Comparative 668 perspectives and enduring challenges. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 17, 295-318. 669 Biskup, T., Schleicher, E., Okafuji, A., Link, G., Hitomi, K., Getzoff, E. D. & Weber, S. (2009). 670 Direct Observation of a Photoinduced Radical Pair in a Cryptochrome Blue-Light 671 Photoreceptor. Angewandte Chemie-International Edition 48, 404-407. 672 Blakemore, R. P. (1975). Magnetotactic bacteria. Science 190, 377-379. 673 Bonadonna, F., Bajzak, C., Benhamou, S., Igloi, K., Jouventin, P., Lipp, H. P. & Dell'omo, G. 674 (2005). Orientation in the wandering albatross: Interfering with magnetic perception does 675 not affect orientation performance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological 676 Sciences 272, 489-495. 677 Boström, J. E., Åkesson, S. & Alerstam, T. (2012). Where on earth can animals use a 678 geomagnetic bi-coordinate map for navigation? Ecography 35, 1039-1047. 679 Bouly, J.-P., Schleicher, E., Dionisio-Sese, M., Vandenbussche, F., Van Der Straeten, D., 680 Bakrim, N., Meier, S., Batschauer, A., Galland, P., Bittl, R. & Ahmad, M. (2007). 681 Cryptochrome blue light photoreceptors are activated through interconversion of flavin 682 redox states. Journal of Biological Chemistry 282, 9383-9391. 683 Bridge, E. S., Thorup, K., Bowlin, M. S., Chilson, P. B., Diehl, R., Fleron, R. W., Hartl, P., 684 Kays, R. W., Kelly, J. F., Robinson, W. D. & Wikelksi, M. (2011). Technology on the 685 move: recent and forthcoming innoviations for tracking migratory birds. Bioscience 61, 686 689-698. 687 Chernetsov, N., Kishkinev, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2008). A long distance avian migrant 688 compensates for longitudinal displacement during spring migration. Current Biology 18, 689 188-190. 690 Dennis, T. E., Rayner, M. J. & Walker, M. M. (2007). Evidence that pigeons orient to 691 geomagnetic intensity during homing. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological 692 Sciences 274, 1153-1158. 693 Deutchlander, M. E., Phillips, J. B. & Munro, U. (2012). Age-Dependent Orientation to 694 Magnetically-Simulated Geographic Displacements in Migratory Australian Silvereyes 695 (Zosterops l. lateralis). The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 124, 467-477. 696 Dingle, H. (1996). Migration: the biology of life on the move, London, Lightning Source. 697 698 699 700 701 Emlen, S. T. (1967). Migratory Orientation in Indigo Bunting Passerina Cyanea. Part II: Mechanisms of celestial navigation. Auk 84, 463-489. Emlen, S. T. & Emlen, J. T. (1966). A technique for recording migratory orientation of captive birds. Auk 83, 361-367. Falkenberg, G., Fleissner, G., Schuchardt, K., Kuehbacher, M., Thalau, P., Mouritsen, H., 702 Heyers, D., Wellenreuther, G. & Fleissner, G. (2010). Avian Magnetoreception: 703 Elaborate Iron Mineral Containing Dendrites in the Upper Beak Seem to Be a Common 704 Feature of Birds. PLoS ONE 5, e9231. 705 Fiaschi, V., Farina, M. & Ioalé, P. (1974). Homing experiments on swifts Apus apus (L.) 706 deprived of olfactory perception. Monitore Zoologica Italiano 8, 235-244. 707 Fischer, J., Munro, U. & Phillips, J. B. (2003). Magnetic navigation by an avian migrant? In: 708 Berthold, P., Gwinner, E. & Sonnenschein, E. (eds.) Avian migration. Heidelberg-New 709 York: Springer-Verlag,. 710 711 712 Fischer, J. H., Freake, M. J., Borland, S. C. & Phillips, J. B. (2001). Evidence for the use of magnetic map information by an amphibian. Animal Behaviour 62, 1-10. Fleissner, G., Holtkamp-Rotzler, E., Hanzlik, M., Winklhofer, M., Petersen, N. & Wiltschko, W. 713 (2003). Ultrastructural analysis of a putative magnetoreceptor in the beak of homing 714 pigeons. Journal of Comparative Neurology 458, 350-360. 715 Foa, A., Bagnoli, P. & Giongo, F. (1986). Homing pigeons subjected to section of the anterior 716 commissure can build up 2 olfactory maps in the deflector lofts. Journal of Comparative 717 Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 159, 465-472. 718 719 Fransson, T., Jakobsson, S., Johansson, P., Kullberg, C., Lind, J. & Vallin, A. (2001). Bird migration: Magnetic cues trigger extensive refuelling. Nature 414, 35-36. 720 Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Filannino, C. & Wikelski, M. (2011). Homing Pigeons Only Navigate in 721 Air with Intact Environmental Odours: A Test of the Olfactory Activation Hypothesis 722 with GPS Data Loggers. PLoS ONE 6, e22385. 723 Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Odetti, F. & Bingman, V. P. (2001). The ontogeny of the homing pigeon 724 navigational map: evidence for a sensitive learning period. Proceedings of the Royal 725 Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 268, 197-202. 726 Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2006). Having the nerve to home: trigeminal 727 magnetoreceptor versus olfactory mediation of homing in pigeons. Journal of 728 Experimental Biology 209, 2888-2892. 729 Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2008). Navigational abilities of homing 730 pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic information when 731 young. Journal of Experimental Biology 211, 2046-2051. 732 Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Savini, M. & Wild, J. M. (2009). Navigational abilities of adult and 733 experienced homing pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic 734 information. Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 3119-3124. 735 Griffin, D. R. (1952). Bird Navigation. Biological Reviews 27, 359-400. 736 Guilford, T., Akesson, S., Gagliardo, A., Holland, R. A., Mouritsen, H., Muheim, R., Wiltschko, 737 R., Wiltschko, W. & Bingman, V. P. (2011). Migratory navigation in birds: new 738 opportunities in an era of fast-developing tracking technology. Journal of Experimental 739 Biology 214, 3705-3712. 740 741 Hagstrum, J. T. (2000). Infrasound and the avian navigational map. Journal of Experimental Biology 203, 1103-1111. 742 743 Hagstrum, J. T. (2001). Infrasound and the avian navigational map. Journal of Navigation 54, 377-391. 744 Hagstrum, J. T. (2013). Atmospheric propagation modelling indicates homing pigeons use loft- 745 specific infrasonic 'map' cues. Journal of Experimental Biology , 216, 687-699. 746 747 748 Hein, C. M., Engels, S., Kishkinev, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2011). Robins have a magnetic compass in both eyes. Nature 471, E11-E12. Henshaw, I., Fransson, T., Jackobsson, S. & Kullberg, C. (2010). Geomagnetic field affects 749 spring migratory direction in a long distance migrant. Behavioral Ecology and 750 Sociobiology 64, 1317-1323. 751 Heyers, D., Zapka, M., Hoffmeister, M., Wild, J. M. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). Magnetic field 752 changes activate the trigeminal brainstem complex in a migratory bird. Proceedings of 753 the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 9394-9399. 754 Holland, R. & Helm, B. (2013). A magnetic pulse affects the precision of departure direction of 755 naturally migrating adult but not juvenile birds. Joural of the Royal Society: Interface 10, 756 20121047. 757 Holland, R. A. (2010). Differential effects of magnetic pulses on the departure directions of 758 naturally migrating birds. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 7, 1617-1625. 759 Holland, R. A., Thorup, K., Gagliardo, A., Bisson, I., Knecht, E., Mizrahi, D. & Wikelski, M. 760 (2009). Testing the role of sensory systems in the migratory heading of a songbird. 761 Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 4065-4071. 762 763 Holland, R. A., Thorup, K. & Wikelski, M. (2007). Where the wild things go. Biologist 54, 214219. 764 765 766 Ioale, P., Nozzolini, M. & Papi, F. (1990). Homing pigeons do extract directional information from olfactory stimuli. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 26, 301-305. Ioale, P., Papi, F., Fiaschi, V. & Baldaccini, N. E. (1978). Pigeon navigation: effects of homing 767 behaviour by reversing wind direction at the loft. Journal of Comparative Physiology 768 128, 285-295. 769 Jacobs, L. F. (2012). From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction. 770 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, 771 10693-10700. 772 773 774 Jorge, P. E., Marques, A. E. & Phillips, J. B. (2009). Activation rather than navigational effects of odours on homing of young pigons. Current Biology 19, 1-5. Jorge, P. E., Marques, A. E. & Phillips, J. B. (2010). Activation effects of odours on avian 775 navigation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277, 45-49. 776 Keeton, W. T. (1974). The orientational and navigational basis of homing in birds. Advances in 777 the study of behavior 5, 47–132. 778 Kennedy, D. & Norman, C. (2005). What don’t we know? Science special Issue 309, 1-204. 779 Kirschvink, J. L. (1982). Birds, Bees and Magnetism - a New Look at the Old Problem of 780 781 782 783 Magnetoreception. Trends in Neurosciences 5, 160-167. Kirschvink, J. L. & Gould, J. L. (1981). Biogenetic magnetite as a basis for magnetic field detection in animals. BioSystems 13, 181-201. Kirschvink, J. L. & Walker, M. M. (1985). Particle-size considerations for magnetite based 784 magnetoreceptors. In: Kirschvink, J. L., Jones, D. S. & Macfadden, B. J. (eds.) Magnetite 785 biomineralization and magnetoreception in organisms. New York: Plenum Press. 786 Kirschvink, J. L., Walker, M. M., Chang, S. B., Dizon, A. E. & Peterson, K. A. (1985). Chains of 787 single domain magnetite particles in chinook salmon, Onchorhynchus Tshawytscha. 788 Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 157, 789 375-381. 790 Kirschvink, J. L., Winklhofer, M. & Walker, M. M. (2010). Biophysics of magnetic orientation: 791 strengthening the interface between theory and experimental design. Journal of the Royal 792 Society Interface doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0491.focus. 793 Kishkinev, D., Chernetsov, N., Heyers, D. & Mouritsen, H. (2013). Migratory Reed Warblers 794 Need Intact Trigeminal Nerves to Correct for a 1,000 km Eastward Displacement. PLoS 795 ONE 8, e65847. 796 Kishkinev, D., Chernetsov, N. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). A double clock or jetlag mechanism is 797 unlikely to be involved in detection of east-west displacements in a long distance avian 798 migrant. Auk 127, 773-780. 799 Kishkinev, D., Mouritsen, H. & Mora, C. V. (2012). An attempt to develop an operant 800 conditioning paradigm to test for magnetic discrimination behavior in a migratory 801 songbird. Journal of Ornithology 153, 1165-1177. 802 803 804 Kramer, G. (1953). Wird die Sonnehöhe bei der Heimfindeorientierung verwertet? Journal fur Ornithology 94, 201-219. Lauwers, M., Pichler, P., Edelman, N. B., Resch, G. P., Ushakova, L., Salzer, M. C., Heyers, D., 805 Saunders, M., Shaw, J. & Keays, D. A. (2013). An iron-rich organelle in the cuticular 806 plate of avian hair cells. Current biology : CB 23, 924-9. 807 Liedvogel, M., Maeda, K., Henbest, K., Schleicher, E., Simon, T., Timmel, C. R., Hore, P. J. & 808 Mouritsen, H. (2007). Chemical Magnetoreception: Bird Cryptochrome 1a Is Excited by 809 Blue Light and Forms Long-Lived Radical-Pairs. PLoS ONE 2. 810 Liedvogel, M. & Mouritsen, H. (2010). Cryptochromes-a potential magnetoreceptor: what do we 811 know and what do we want to know? Journal of the royal Society Interface 7, S147- 812 S162. 813 814 815 Lohmann, K. J., Lohmann, C. M. F., Ehrhart, L. M., Bagley, D. A. & Swing, T. (2004). Geomagnetic map used in sea turtle navigation. Nature 428, 909-910. Lohmann, K. J., Pentcheff, N. D., Nevitt, G. A., Stetten, G. D. & Zimmer-Faust, R. K. (1995). 816 Magnetic orientation of spiny lobsters in the ocean: experiments with underseas coil 817 systems. Journal of Experimental Biology 198, 2041-2048. 818 Maeda, K., Robinson, A. J., Henbest, K. B., Hogben, H. J., Biskup, T., Ahmad, M., Schleicher, 819 E., Weber, S., Timmel, C. R. & Hore, P. J. (2012). Magnetically sensitive light-induced 820 reactions in cryptochrome are consistent with its proposed role as a magnetoreceptor. 821 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, 822 4774-4779. 823 824 825 826 827 828 Matthews, G. V. T. (1951). The experimental investigation of navigation in homing pigeons. Journal of Experimental Biology , 28, 508-536. Matthews, G. V. T. (1953). Sun navigation in homing pigeons. Journal of Experimental Biology 30, 243-267. Merkel, F. W. & Wiltschko, W. (1965). Magnetismus und Richtungsfinden zugunruhiger Rotkehlchen (Erithacus rubecula). Vogelwarte 23, 71–77. 829 830 831 Mewaldt, L. R., Cowley, L. T. & Won, P. O. (1973). Californian sparrows fail to return from displacement to Korea. Auk 90, 857–861. Moller, A., Sagasser, S., Wiltschko, W. & Schierwater, B. (2004). Retinal cryptochrome in a 832 migratory passerine bird: a possible transducer for the avian magnetic compass. 833 Naturwissenschaften 91, 585-588. 834 835 836 Mora, C. V., Davison, M., Wild, J. M. & Walker, M. M. (2004). Magnetoreception and its trigeminal mediation in the homing pigeon. Nature 432, 508-511. Mora, C. V. & Walker, M. M. (2009). Do release-site biases reflect response to the Earth's 837 magnetic field during position determination by homing pigeons? Proceedings of the 838 Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276, 3295-3302. 839 840 Mouritsen, H. (2012). SENSORY BIOLOGY Search for the compass needles. Nature 484, 320321. 841 Mouritsen, H., Feenders, G., Liedvogel, M., Wada, K. & Jarvis, E. D. (2005). A night vision 842 brain area in migratory songbirds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 8339-8344. 843 844 Mouritsen, H. & Hore, P. J. (2012). The magnetic retina: light-dependent and trigeminal magnetoreception in migratory birds. Current opinion in neurobiology 22, 343-352. 845 Mouritsen, H., Huyvaert, K. P., Frost, B. J. & Anderson, D. J. (2003). Waved albatrosses can 846 navigate with strong magnets attached to their head. Journal of Experimental Biology 847 206, 4155-4166. 848 Mouritsen, H., Janssen-Bienhold, U., Liedvogel, M., Feenders, G., Stalleicken, J., Dirks, P. & 849 Weiler, R. (2004). Cryptochromes and neuronal-activity markers colocalize in the retina 850 of migratory birds during magnetic orientation. Proceedings of the National Academy of 851 Sciences of the United States of America 101, 14294-14299. 852 Mouritsen, H. & Larsen, O. N. (2001). Migrating songbirds tested in computer-controlled Emlen 853 funnels use stellar cues for a time-independent compass. Journal of Experimental Biology 854 204, 3855-3865. 855 Muheim, R. & Akesson, S. (2002). Clock-shift experiments with Savannah sparrows, 856 Passerculus sandwichensis, at high northern latitudes. Behavioral Ecology and 857 Sociobiology 51, 394-401. 858 859 860 Munro, U., Munro, J. A., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (1997a). Evidence for a magnetite-based navigational ‘map’ in birds. Naturwissenschaften 84, 26-28. Munro, U., Munro, J. A., Phillips, J. B. & Wiltschko, W. (1997b). Effect of wavelength of light 861 and pulse magnetisation on different magnetoreception systems in a migratory bird. 862 Australian Journal of Zoology 45, 189-198. 863 Munro, U. & Wiltschko, R. (1993). Clock shift experiments with migratory yellow faced 864 honeyeaters, Lichenostomus crysops (Meliphagidae), and australian day migrating bird. 865 Journal of Experimental Biology 181, 233-244. 866 Newton, I. (2007). The migration ecology of birds, London, Academic Press. 867 Niessner, C., Denzau, S., Gross, J. C., Peichl, L., Bischof, H.-J., Fleissner, G., Wiltschko, W. & 868 Wiltschko, R. (2011). Avian Ultraviolet/Violet Cones Identified as Probable 869 Magnetoreceptors. PLoS ONE 6, e20091. 870 Papi, F. (1992). General Aspects. In: Papi, F. (ed.) Animal Homing. London: Chapman Hall. 871 Pennycuick, C. J. (1960). The physical basis of astronavigation in birds: theoretical 872 873 considerations. Journal of Experimental Biology , 37, 573-593. Pennycuick, C. J. (1961). Sun navigation in birds? Nature 190, 1127-1128. 874 875 Perdeck, A. C. (1958). Two types of orientation in migrating Sturnus vulgaris and Fringilla coelebs. Ardea 46. 876 Perdeck, A. C. (1967). Orientation of starlings after displacement to Spain. Ardea 55, 194-202. 877 Phillips, J. B. (1996). Magnetic navigation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 180, 309-319. 878 Phillips, J. B., Schmidt-Koenig, K. & Muheim, R. (2006). True navigation: Sensory bases of 879 gradient maps. In: Brown, M. F. & Cook, R. G. (eds.) Animal Spatial Cognition: 880 Comparative, Neural, and Computational Approaches. [On-line]: Available: 881 www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/asc/phillips/. 882 Phillips, J. B. & Waldvogel, J. A. (1982). Reflected light cues generate the short-term deflector 883 loft effect. In: Papi, F. & Wallraff, H. G. (eds.) Avian navigation. Berlin: Springer. 884 Putman, N. F., Endres, C. S., Lohmann, C. M. F. & Lohmann, K. J. (2011). Longitude perception 885 886 and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Current Biology 21, 463-466. Rastogi, A., Kumari, Y., Rani, S. & Kumar, V. (2011). Phase inversion of neural activity in the 887 olfactory and visual systems of a night-migratory bird during migration. European 888 Journal of Neuroscience 34, 99-109. 889 890 891 892 893 Ritz, T., Adem, S. & Schulten, K. (2000). A model for vision based magnetoreception in birds. Biophysics Journal 78, 707-718. Ritz, T., Thalau, P., Phillips, J. B., Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2004). Resonance effects indicate radical pair mechanism for avian magnetic compass. Nature 429, 177-180. Rogers, C. T. & Hore, P. J. (2009). Chemical magnetoreception in birds: the radical pair 894 mechanism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of 895 America 106, 353-360. 896 897 898 899 900 Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1960). The sun azimuth compass: one factor in the orientation of homing pigeons. Science (New York, N.Y.) 131, 826-7. Schmidt-Koenig, K., Ganzhorn, J. U. & Ranvaud, R. (1991). The sun compass. Experientia Supplementum (Basel) 60, 1-15. Schulten, K., Swenberg, C. E. & Weller, A. (1978). A biomagnetic sensory mechanism based on 901 magnetic field modulated coherent electron spin motion. Zeitschrift fur Physical 902 Chemistry NF111, 1–5. 903 Thorup, K., Bisson, I., Bowlin, M., Holland, R. A., Ramenofsky, M., Wingfield, J. & Wikelski, 904 M. (2007). Migration routes of adult and juvenile white-crowned sparrows differ after 905 continent-wide displacement during migration. PNAS 104, 18115-18119. 906 907 Thorup, K. & Holland, R. (2009). The Bird GPS. Journal of Experimental Biology 212, 35973604. 908 Thorup, K., Holland, R. A., Tottrup, A. P. & Wikelski, M. (2010). Understanding the Migratory 909 Orientation Program of Birds: Extending Laboratory Studies to Study Free-Flying 910 Migrants in a Natural Setting. Integrative and Comparative Biology 50, 315-322. 911 Thorup, K. & Rabøl, J. (2001). The orientation system and migration pattern of long-distance 912 migrants: conflict between model predictions and observed patterns. Journal of Avian 913 Biology 32, 111-119. 914 Thorup, K. & Rabøl, J. (2007). Compensatory behaviour following displacement in migratory 915 birds. A meta-analysis of cage-experiments. . Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 61, 916 825-841. 917 Thorup, K. T., Ortvad, T. E., Rabøl, J., Holland, R. A., Tøttrup, A. P. & Wikelski, M. (2011). 918 Juvenile songbirds compensate for displacement of Oceanic Islands during autumn 919 migration. PLoS One e6, 17903. 920 Treiber, C. D., Salzer, M. C., Riegler, J., Edelman, N., Sugar, C., Breuss, M., Pichler, P., Cadiou, 921 H., Saunders, M., Lythgoe, M., Shaw, J. & Keays, D. A. (2012). Clusters of iron-rich 922 cells in the upper beak of pigeons are macrophages not magnetosensitive neurons. Nature 923 484, 367-70. 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 Viguier, C. (1882). Le sens de l’orientation et ses organes chez les animaux et chez l’homme. Review Philisosophique Frances Etranger 14:, 1–36. Walcott, C. (1991). Magnetic maps in pigeons. In: Berthold, P. (ed.) Orientation in birds. Basel: Birkhauser-Verlag. Walcott, C. (1996). Pigeon homing: observations, experiments and confusions. Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 21-27. Walcott, C. & Michener, M. C. (1971). Sun navigation in homing pigeons-attempts to shift the sun's coordinates. Journal of Experimental Biology , 54, 291-316. Waldvogel, J. A. & Phillips, J. B. (1991). Olfactory cues perceived at the home loft are not 933 essential for the formation of a navigational map in pigeons. Journal of Experimental 934 Biology 155, 643-660. 935 936 937 938 Waldvogel, J. A., Phillips, J. B. & Brown, A. I. (1988). changes in the short term deflector loft effect are linked to the sun compass of homing pigeons. Animal Behaviour 36, 150-158. Walker, M. M. (1998). On a wing and a vector: a model for magnetic navigation in birds. Journal of Theoretical Biology 192, 341-349. 939 940 Wallraff, H. G. (1972). Homing of Pigeons after Extirpation of their Cochleae and Lagenae. Nature New Biology 236, 223-224. 941 Wallraff, H. G. (1974). Das Navigationssystem der Vögel, Munich, Oldenburg. 942 Wallraff, H. G. (1981). The olfactory component of pigeon navigation: steps and analysis. 943 944 945 Journal of comparative Physiology 143, 411-422. Wallraff, H. G. (1991). Conceptual approaches to avian navigation. In: Berthold, P. (ed.) Orientation in Birds. Basel: Birkhauser. 946 Wallraff, H. G. (2005). Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm, Berlin, Springer. 947 Wallraff, H. G. & Andreae, M. O. (2000). Spatial gradients in ratios of atmospheric trace gasses: 948 a study stimulated by experiments on bird navigation. Tellus 52B, 1138-1157. 949 Wallraff, H. G., Kiepenheuer, J., Neumann, M. F. & Streng, A. (1995). Homing experiments 950 951 with starlings deprived of the sense of smell. Condor 97, 20-26. Wikelski, M., Kays, R. W., Kasdin, N. J., Thorup, K., Smith, J. A. & Swenson, G. W. (2007). 952 Going wild: what a global small-animal tracking system could do for experimental 953 biologists. Journal of Experimental Biology 210, 181-186. 954 955 956 Williams, N. M. & Wild, J. M. (2001). Trigeminally innervated iron-containing structures in the beak of homing pigeons, and other birds. Brain Res. 889, 243-246. Wiltschko, R. (1996). The function of olfactory input in pigeon orientation: Does it provide 957 navigational information or play another role? Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 113- 958 119. 959 960 Wiltschko, R., Schiffer, I., Fuhrmann, P. & Wiltschko, W. (2010). The role of magnetite based receptors in the beak in pigeon homing. Current Biology 20, 1-5. 961 962 Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts. Animal Behaviour 65, 257-272. 963 Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2006). Magnetoreception. Bioessays 28, 157-168. 964 Wiltschko, R. & Wiltschko, W. (2013). The magnetite-based receptors in the beak of birds and 965 their role in avian navigation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, 966 Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 199, 89-99. 967 Wiltschko, W., Ford, H., Munro, U., Winklhofer, M. & Wiltschko, R. (2007). Magnetite-based 968 magnetoreception: the effect of repeated pulsing on the orientation of migratory birds. 969 Journal of Comparative Physiology a-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral 970 Physiology 193, 515-522. 971 Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Beason, R. C., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1994). A magnetic pulse 972 leads to a temporary deflection in the orientation of migratory birds. Experientia 50, 697- 973 700. 974 975 976 Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1993). Red light disrupts magnetic orientation of migratory birds. Nature 364, 525-527. Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (1998). Effect of a magnetic pulse on the 977 orientation of Silvereyes Zosterops l. lateralis, during spring migration. Journal of 978 Experimental Biology 201, 3257-3261. 979 Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2006). Bird Navigation: What type of 980 information does the magnetite-based receptor provide. Proceedings of the Royal Society 981 B-Biological Sciences 273, 2815-2820. 982 Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2009). Avian orientation: the pulse effect 983 is mediated by the magnetite receptors in the upper beak. Proceedings of the Royal 984 Society B-Biological Sciences 276, 2227-2232. 985 Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Wiltschko, R. & Kirschvink, J. L. (2002a). Magnetite-based 986 magnetoreception in birds: the effect of a biasing field and a pulse on migratory behavior. 987 Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 3031-3037. 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 Wiltschko, W., Traudt, J., Gunturkun, O., Prior, H. & Wiltschko, R. (2002b). Lateralisation of magnetic compass orientation in a migratory bird. Nature 419, 467-470. Wiltschko, W. & Wiltchko, R. (2007). Magnetoreception in birds: two receptors for two different tasks. Journal for Ornithology 148, S61-S67. Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1972). Magnetic compass of European Robins. Science 176, 6264. Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (1995). Migratory orientation of European robins is affected by 995 the wavelength of light as well as by a magnetic pulse. Journal of comparative 996 Physiology A 177, 363–369. 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 Wu, L.-Q. & Dickman, J. D. (2011). Magnetoreception in an Avian Brain in Part Mediated by Inner Ear Lagena. Current Biology 21, 418-423. Wu, L.-Q. & Dickman, J. D. (2012). Neural Correlates of a Magnetic Sense. Science 336, 10541057. Yorke, E. D. (1979). A possible magnetic transducer in birds. Journal of Theoretical Biology 77, 101–105. 1003 Zapka, M., Heyers, D., Hein, C. M., Engels, S., Schneider, N.-L., Hans, J., Weiler, S., Dreyer, 1004 D., Kishkinev, D., Wild, J. M. & Mouritsen, H. (2009). Visual but not trigeminal 1005 mediation of magnetic compass information in a migratory bird. Nature 461, 1274-1277. 1006 1007 Figure legends 1008 Figure 1. 1009 Migratory true navigation (A1, A2) is distinct from homing (B) in that the goal is dependent 1010 upon the season. If displaced during the breeding season the animal homes to the same location. 1011 If displaced from the breeding ground during or just prior to migration, the animal will navigate 1012 to the winter ground (A1), or if displaced from the winter ground, to the breeding ground (A2). 1013 Whether this requires different mechanisms or cues that homing has not yet been established. 1014 Figure 2. 1015 In a bi-coordinate map, two gradients (represented by the broad intersecting arrows A and B) are 1016 learned by exploration in the home breeding area. As long as the gradients continue to increase 1017 or decrease predictably, then if a bird is displaced, it can compare the values at the displaced 1018 location with those at the desired goal to calculate the direction of displacement. Only by 1019 learning the values of the winter area can the bird navigate to it, and so migratory true navigation 1020 can only be achieved by adult birds that have made a previous journey. 1021 1022 1023 Figure 3. 1024 A simplified schematic of the radical pair reaction. A photoreceptive molecule forms radical 1025 pairs in the presence of specific wavelengths of light. An applied magnetic field alters the yield 1026 of conversion between singlet and triplet states (yield A vs. yield B in the diagram), leading to 1027 different expression of the signalling state of the molecule (after (Rogers and Hore, 2009)). It has 1028 been hypothesised that this signalling state may be expressed as patterns on the retina, thus 1029 allowing to bird to see the magnetic field, but this has not yet been confirmed (Ritz et al., 2000) . 1030 Figure 4. 1031 Chains of single domain magnetite align with a biasing magnetic field in the direction of 1032 magnetisation (indicated by the red end). Application of a strong magnetic pulse antiparallel to 1033 the direction of magnetisation will re-magnetise in the opposite direction. If such chains are 1034 present in sensory cells and were free to rotate they would give different information about the 1035 magnetic field after such a treatment. In practice however, the effect of pulse treatments on birds 1036 do not clearly indicate that such structures exist.