Katie Almeida APLING 623 September 16, 2012 Wardhaugh, Chapter 1: Introduction Wardhaugh seems to be orienting himself and the reader in this introductory chapter. He does an adequate job of covering the parts of sociolinguistics and the idea that power relationships are inextricably linked to how and what people say. However, he seems to stop there. While Wardhaugh agrees that linguists must consider use and variation when looking at data and developing theories, he does not acknowledge the social value in developing these theories. On page 13, Wardhaugh says, “…from time to time I will voice some skepticism about the claims of other investigators that we should pursue certain ideological ends in investigating such use (see chapters 13-15). Detachment and objectivity are essential requirements of serious scientific inquiry.” How can a sociolinguist exhibit complete detachment and objectivity? If use and variation are invaluable in a sociolinguistic theory, then the linguist cannot deny his or her own place in the spectrum of language use and variation. As the linguist studies and gathers information, he or she will inevitably learn about his or her status and position in society (and maybe even adjust his or her language as a result of these realizations). If nearly every word we utter, grammar and content words alike, carries the weight of our identity and position in society, how can we strive to be objective and detached? Linguists need to recognize their position and identity before, as well as their own use and variations and how these play into their identity, before they can attempt anything resembling detachment or objectivity. Journalists often strive for detachment and objectivity and fail miserably; we all know of liberal or conservative news outlets. So, while Wardhaugh’s claim that we should be detached and objective in order to achieve scientific accuracy may have some value, it falls flat on its face when put into practice. Furthermore, Wardhaugh goes on to say, “we must look at such issues as identity, group membership, power, and socialization,” (p.14). I agree with this statement wholeheartedly, because language does not occur in a vacuum and words do not just pop out of thin air. However, how does this statement fit with Wardhaugh’s conviction that we must be objective and detached? When studying “such issues as identity, group membership, power, and socialization,” it is inevitable that we will look at our own situation, so we cannot be detached and objective. The individual who has more information and cultural capital regarding language use and variation inevitably will have more power in more situations. So, while Wardhaugh would like to distance himself from sociolinguists who have ideological agendas, I argue that we all have ideological agendas and the closest we can come to objectivity is to be honest and straightforward with ourselves and the public about those agendas. Additionally, language itself is completely subjective because it is related to identity and power. If we need to use language to write a scientific report, then we cannot be objective in doing so. We need language to communicate our findings in journals and articles, so we cannot be completely detached. The whole research process is riddled with language, and therefore is filled with messages about power, status, and identity. We cannot separate the two, and to do so is only to fool yourself and the readers of your research.