Peer Review Paper 1 2 3 4 Citation: Schrank B, Riches S, Coggins T, Tylee A, Slade M (2013) From objectivity to subjectivity: conceptualisation and measurement of well-being in mental health, Neuropsychiatry, 3, 525-534. 5 Revision submitted to Neuropsychiatry on 18th July 2013. 6 From objectivity to subjectivity: conceptualisation and measurement of well-being in 7 mental health 8 9 10 11 12 Beate Schrank, Simon Riches, Tony Coggins, Andre Tylee, Mike Slade 13 Practice points 14 15 Incorporating research on well-being into clinical practice has the potential to improve mental health care Using a combination of approaches to measure components of well-being and mental 16 health together with or in addition to scales capturing multidimensional concepts of well- 17 being, is recommended for clinical practise 18 The Complete State Model of Mental Health, which captures both mental health and 19 mental ill-health and allows the consideration of well-being despite the presence of 20 mental illness, is recommended as a framework for clinical practice 21 Elements of the ‘Good Life’ approach to well-being have the potential to be included in 22 clinical practice as they conform with the transcendent principle of personhood and allow 23 for varied and everyday approaches to increase well-being 24 25 26 A range of promising interventions exist that target components of well-being, such as self-worth, meaning or hope Positive psychology interventions combining a range of resource oriented exercises 27 appear to be promising to improve well-being. Their application to people with mental 28 illness, particularly severe, will need to be established in further research. 29 30 Abstract/Summary 31 The concept of well-being has not been well defined or reliably measured in academic 32 research. This article identifies four academic strands of well-being conceptualisation and 33 measurement (economic, medical, psychological, and integrative) and shows how well-being 34 has shifted from being conceived as a collectivist concept with objective measures, to being Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 35 conceived in individualistic terms, with subjective measures. Given its clinical relevance the 36 main emphasis is on subjective well-being. While well-being has become a key concept in 37 mental health, the article also discusses some limitations to its use in practice and proposes 38 considerations for future research. Key issues are a consensus definition of well-being in 39 people with mental illness, and empirical studies on the measurement of well-being and its 40 determinants. Future research might be based on the ‘Good Life’ approach, the Complete 41 State Model of Mental Health, or the academic field of Positive Psychology. 42 43 Introduction 44 45 Well-being has been a topical policy focus in recent years and has attracted research interest 46 across health conditions. An explicit description of the concept of well-being is often absent 47 in research [1]. This article provides an overview on the various concepts of well-being in the 48 literature and their related measurement tools, with a specific reference to mental health. In 49 examining the definitions of well-being, we identify four academic strands of well-being 50 research: 51 52 Economic Strand: grounded in economic research, well-being is framed in terms of 53 national wealth, social determinants, development and general quality of life. 54 55 Medical Strand: grounded in medical research, well-being is framed in relation to 56 disorder and illness. 57 58 Psychological Strand: Grounded in psychological research, well-being is framed in 59 terms of subjective and mental concepts. 60 61 Integrative Strand: informed by economic, medical, and psychological phases, with a 62 distinct focus on positive psychology and recovery research. 63 64 The paper discusses each phase, with particular focus on the birth of the psychological 65 conception and its evolution into the contemporary, integrative phase. The primary focus is to 66 show how well-being has shifted from being conceived as a collectivist concept with objective 67 measures, to being conceived as an individualistic concept with subjective measures. This 68 transition was instrumental in well-being becoming a key concept in mental health. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 69 70 71 From the economic phase to the medical phase 72 In the economic strand, well-being was initially conceived of primarily in collectivist terms. 73 Measuring and comparing the well-being of populations (rather than individuals) was first 74 undertaken by economists in the early 20th century. Initially, financial indicators of well-being 75 such as Gross National Product (GNP) were used to measure and compare. As these failed 76 to discriminate between countries of similar developmental status, alternative economic 77 indices were proposed to estimate societal functioning [2]. These composite measures 78 further increased the validity of well-being estimates. Today, they are known as “Quality of 79 Life” measures. First, they included purely objective measures, such as mortality, nutrition, 80 literacy, clean water supply, or education [3]. Later ‘subjective’ indicators, such as affect, 81 well-being, or life satisfaction were added to capture how people actually feel about their 82 lives [4]. Composite measures of population well-being are still developed today. For 83 example, the UK Office for National Statistics has developed a new assessment of 84 population well-being, including subjective domains such as spirituality, personal and cultural 85 activities, political participation, or life satisfaction in addition to environmental and 86 sustainability issues and UK economic performance [5]. 87 88 Inclusion of population level health indicators, e.g. mortality, into composite measures 89 evolving from economic research signifies the emergence of the medical strand to well-being 90 research. The addition of subjective measures, e.g. life satisfaction, signifies the emergence 91 of the psychological strand to well-being research. Medical research also marks a shift in that 92 it emphasises individual health status in understanding well-being. Health research is 93 another major application of the concept of quality of life, in this context called “Health 94 Related Quality of Life” (HRQoL). 95 96 HRQoL has attracted substantial research since its introduction, as well as criticism for its 97 lack of uniformity and clarity. The terms ‘health related quality of life’, ‘quality of life’ and ‘well- 98 being’ are often used interchangeably, and few articles claiming to measure HRQoL provide 99 a definition or identify constituent domains [6]. Conceptualisations and measures of HRQoL 100 can be described according to a number of defining features, e.g. generic versus disease 101 specific, or objective versus subjective. Individual measurement tools often cover widely 102 different dimensions, including access to resources and opportunities, environmental factors, 103 social relationships, employment, leisure activities, sex life, mobility, or satisfaction with 104 social domains. The unifying feature of HRQoL concepts is their focus on illness symptoms Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 105 and functioning based on the assumption that illness and disability inhibits full well-being [7]. 106 107 While physical health symptoms and functioning are major domains within HRQoL, 108 measurement tools also often use the terms well-being and mental health (i.e. the absence 109 of mental illness symptoms) interchangeably [8]. Examples of generic HRQoL measures with 110 a mental health or well-being sub-scale include the World Health Organization Quality of Life 111 (WHOQOL) questionnaires with their domain on ‘psychological health’ [9], the European 112 Quality of Life-5 Dimensions questionnaire (EQ-5D) with its ‘anxiety and depression’ domain 113 [10], or the Short Form (SF) measures, with their ‘emotional well-being’ domain assessing 114 feeling happy, sad, depressed, or anxious [11]. Other scales use a more elaborate 115 conceptual foundation grounded in and overlapping with psychological conceptions of well- 116 being. For example, the Lancashire Quality of Life Profile [12] and the Manchester Short 117 Assessment of Quality of Life [13] base their well-being domain on concepts of affect 118 balance, life satisfaction and happiness. 119 120 One issue in measuring HRQoL in people with mental disorders is the potential distortion of 121 subjective assessments due to ’psychopathological fallacies‘, most prominently the ‘affective 122 fallacy’ which indicates that the momentary affective state can influence people’s judgement 123 about their overall life [14]. This is most problematic in case HRQoL measures which contain 124 ‘emotional’ items relating to feelings of depression and anxiety, as is the case e.g. in the 125 Quality of Life in Depression Scale [15]. Quantitative results support the ‘affective fallacy’ as 126 depressive symptoms have been shown to have an independent and significantly negative 127 effect on subjective ratings of HRQoL [16]. 128 129 A second issue is concerns about the reliability of subjective assessment in people with 130 psychiatric disorders. This concern led to the inclusion of supposedly objective assessment 131 methods, derived from clinicians or family members [14]. However, subjective assessment 132 has become more accepted as people with severe mental illness were shown to reliably and 133 consistently complete self-rating questionnaires [17]. Moreover, the views of clinicians and 134 family members may be biased, and service users’ subjective position is argued to be no 135 less true in case it diverges from an outsider. In fact, ‘insider’ and ‘outsider’ perspectives 136 have been shown to differ due to differing values placed on contextual factors and a 137 tendency towards a negative bias from the outsider perspective [18]. This supports the 138 meaningfulness of the subjective assessment of well-being. While in HRQoL subjective and 139 objective measures still coexist, the psychological approach has completely shifted to 140 subjective assessment [19,20,21]. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 141 142 143 The emergence of the psychological phase 144 Psychological research has created specific conceptualisations and measures to capture 145 well-being in its own right without embedding it within other constructs such as national 146 development or HRQoL. 147 148 As with HRQoL, a review on psychological concepts of well-being criticised frequently 149 missing or ambiguous definitions and the interchangeable use of similar terms [22]. 150 Distinctive features of the psychological approach include its focus on subjective experience 151 and personal feelings, and on positive mental health and functioning, e.g. positive affect, life 152 satisfaction, autonomy, competence or personal growth [19,20,21]. Moreover, specific well- 153 being concepts allow for peak experiences (e.g. peak positive affect) or for the temporary 154 cessation of affective experience during flow [23]. Such specific states of well-being may 155 bring symptomatic relief which will not be captured in measures of HRQoL. 156 157 Differences between measures of HRQoL and psychological well-being are empirically 158 confirmed by their only moderate statistical correlations. Factor analysis including twelve 159 scales assessing HRQoL and seven psychological well-being scales showed the two 160 concepts to generally load on separate factors. Correlations between these scales ranged 161 between 0.05 and 0.63, but mostly around 0.2 to 0.4 [24]. 162 163 Despite broad agreement that well-being is a subjective condition in which positive feelings 164 dominate, there is disagreement over more detailed ingredients of well-being in the 165 psychological approach. Diverging views are often grouped under two broad perspectives: 166 hedonic and eudaimonic. In this context hedonism usually refers to maximising pleasure and 167 happiness while reducing pain. The eudaimonic perspective maintains that not all desires 168 yield well-being when achieved, even though they might produce pleasure. Instead well- 169 being means to live in accordance with one’s true self and derives from personal growth and 170 self-actualisation, from actively contributing, holding virtue and doing what is right. Happiness 171 may be a pleasant result of this way of living but not its core [19,25]. 172 173 Another organising principle for psychological well-being concepts is the distinction between 174 the absence of mental illness symptoms and the presence of positive mental health, e.g. 175 framed as positive emotion. This is reflected in an increasing focus on positively framed 176 items in well-being questionnaires. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 177 178 One rationale for including positively framed variables (e.g. “I have felt cheerful and in good 179 spirits”, “I woke up feeling fresh and rested”) into measurement tools was the observation 180 that a substantial proportion of people in the general population rarely or never report 181 occurrences of psychological distress symptoms [26]. For example, the USA national 182 comorbidity survey found a proportion of 14.9% of people from 26 samples worldwide to 183 qualify for the diagnosis of a mental disorder [27]. This introduces a ceiling effect [8], i.e. a 184 zero score on a depression scale indicates the absence of depression but not the presence 185 of happiness [28]. Including characteristics of psychological well-being (e.g. zest, interest in 186 and enjoyment of life) increases measurement precision and distinguishes among persons 187 who receive perfect scores on measures of psychological distress [26]. An example of a 188 scale covering both ends of a continuum is the Depression-Happiness-Scale [28,29]. The 189 WHO-5 goes one step further. It contains only positive items and assesses emotional well- 190 being. However, although framed positively the WHO-5 still covers core dimensions of 191 depression and proved to be a sensitive tool for screening for depression and suicide risk 192 [30,31]. This may indicate that simply rewording negative to positive items may not be 193 sufficient to meaningfully capture well-being. 194 195 The complex interrelations between the mental health domain in HRQoL and assessment 196 tools designated to specifically measure mental health or well-being are exemplified by the 197 RAND health survey. The original survey included the 38-item Mental Health Inventory (MHI- 198 38) to assess mental health [26]. The MHI-38 had assimilated 15 questions from Dupuy’s 199 General Well-being Index [19,32]. Later, the MHI-38 was condensed into the 5-item MHI-5, 200 which was in turn used as the mental health sub-scale in the ‘Short Form’ HRQoL measures 201 (e.g. SF-36 and SF-20) [33, 34]. In addition, from the same item pool items were adapted 202 into the WHO-5, which aims to assess positive mental well-being [8,19]. These measurement 203 overlaps show the gradual development from purely deficit-focused HRQoL measures to 204 measures of positive well-being. 205 206 Measuring subjectivity in the psychological strand 207 Despite their contrasts, the hedonic and eudaimonic views are not independent but overlap 208 conceptually and correlate with each other with varying magnitude [19,25]. The same issue 209 of contrasting but overlapping dimensions also applies to a view of well-being as lack of 210 mental illness versus positive mental well-being, or positive versus negative affect [35]. 211 Hence, measurement tools for well-being may be better described according to their focus on 212 affective, cognitive and multidimensional components. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 213 214 Affective measures 215 Assessment tools that conceptualise well-being in terms of affect usually use the term affect 216 to refer to mood states or feelings, without acknowledging the psychological distinctions 217 between affect, emotion and feelings. These scales assume that well-being is the degree to 218 which positive feelings outweigh negative feelings. They measure positive and negative 219 feelings either as two distinct dimensions resulting in separate scores or in one overall 220 ‘balance score’. Prominent examples are the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) 221 [36], Bradburn’s Affect Balance Scale (ABS) [25], the Affectometer [37] or the Everyday 222 Feeling Questionnaire [38]. The evident overlap between affective well-being measures and 223 measures of psychiatric symptoms is empirically supported by high correlations especially 224 with depression [29]. 225 226 Cognitive measures 227 Affective and cognitive measurement strategies overlap. An example for the intersection 228 between emotional and cognitive understandings of well-being is Diener’s concept of 229 ‘subjective wellbeing’ and the corresponding Satisfaction With Life Scale [39] which assesses 230 positive affect and subjective life satisfaction. Similarly, the Oxford Happiness Inventory 231 defines happiness as a combination of positive affect or joy, satisfaction, and the absence of 232 distress and negative feeling [40]. 233 234 Purely cognitive measures of well-being include single item questionnaires asking one 235 question such as “How satisfied are you with your life overall?” or “Taking everything into 236 consideration, how would you say you are today?”[19]. This requires respondents to reflect 237 on their overall state of life including all individually relevant domains [41]. As with HRQoL, 238 the ‘affective fallacy’ caveat applies to global cognitive ratings, as they may be influenced by 239 momentary affective states [42]. At the same time global ratings are thought to better reflect 240 subjective valuation since different areas of life may be valued differently by individuals 241 [43,44]. 242 243 Global cognitive measures of well-being have been widely applied and inspired the theory of 244 ‘subjective well-being homeostasis’. This theory is based on the finding that mean population 245 values of overall life satisfaction typically vary only within a narrow range. On a scale from 0 246 to 100, people in Western countries answer the question for overall life satisfaction roughly 247 around 75, in non-Western countries the average lies around 70 [35]. The theory of 248 subjective wellbeing homeostasis is comparable to the homeostatic regulation of body Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 249 functions. It proposes that subjective well-being is maintained within narrow margins (a ‘set 250 point’) by a set of psychological devices, e.g. cognitive bias, personality factors, and 251 adaptation [46,47,48]. Hence, people maintaining a normally functioning homeostatic system 252 show little fluctuations in well-being as a consequence of normal variations in their living 253 conditions. Only highly unusual events cause the level of global subjective well-being to 254 change temporarily, but it will return to its previous level over time, e.g. the subjective 255 happiness of both lottery winners and paralyzed accident victims soon returns to previous 256 levels [47]. The conclusions that may be drawn for the measurement of well-being in general 257 are that the classic “life as a whole” question is useful as an estimate of the personal set- 258 point of well-being, and is unlikely to change as a result of a therapeutic intervention [49]. 259 260 Multidimensional measures 261 Multidimensional well-being concepts and their corresponding measures include widely 262 varying psychological dimensions apart from affective and cognitive aspects. Even though 263 multidimensional well-being concepts are most comprehensive, none of them covers all 264 potential well-being dimensions. No single concept can be regarded generally superior to 265 others as their utility strongly depends on the target group to which they are applied. 266 267 Ryff’s (1989) multidimensional concept of ‘psychological well-being’ and corresponding Scale 268 of Psychological Well-being (SPWB) captures positive functioning with a focus on life span 269 development, individuation, maturity and self-actualisation [44,50]. The scale’s unstable 270 factor structure has attracted criticism but the particularly comprehensive concept is still used 271 today and informs other conceptualisations of well-being. Likewise, the Warwick-Edinburgh 272 Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS) covers a comprehensive understanding of well-being 273 including affective-emotional aspects, cognitive-evaluative dimensions and psychological 274 functioning [51]. It builds on previous measures including the Affectometer, Psychological 275 Well-being Scale, PANAS, Depression-Happiness Scale, and WHO-5 [52]. Other 276 multidimensional well-being scales include the “l’Echelle de Bien-Etre” (EBE), which aims to 277 capture 278 circumstances [53]; the Mental, Physical and Spiritual Well-being Scale (MPSWB) which 279 defines well-being as the balanced nourishment of the mind, body, and spirit [54]; the Life 280 Satisfaction Index (LSI) which focuses on morale and adjustment in the elderly [19]; or the 281 Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale (PGCM) which equals morale with ‘generalized 282 well-being’ [55]. Table 1 organises these measure of multidimensional well-being into 283 categories, to illustrate the variable coverage of diverse dimensions with relatively little 284 overlap between the individual measures. variations in well-being following changing environmental and personal Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 285 286 Insert Table 1 here 287 288 289 Integrative phase 290 The World Health Organisation (WHO) defines health as “a state of complete physical, 291 mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” and mental 292 health as “a state of well-being in which the individual realises his or her own abilities, can 293 cope with the normal stresses of life, can work productively and fruitfully, and is able to make 294 a contribution to his or her community" [56]. These highly inclusive definitions of well-being 295 and mental health span both poles of the spectrum, i.e. illness or the absence thereof as well 296 as optimum or peak states. 297 298 Researchers, mainly in psychology, have devised equally comprehensive and overarching 299 definitions of well-being and health, i.e. of a good life, by including components of all 300 previously outlined approaches. While the medical and psychological approaches are usually 301 covered, the economic approach is represented to a lesser degree. This varying emphasis is 302 partly supported by empirical research showing that the goodness of life is commonly judged 303 by people according to happiness, meaning, and – to a lesser extent – wealth [57]. 304 305 Table 2 maps some examples of good life conceptualisations. While their constituent 306 components widely overlap some differences also remain, e.g. physical health is only 307 included in one definition [58,59]. Differences between good life concepts may arise from 308 their target population, e.g. Lawton’s (1983) concept is specifically developed for older 309 people [60], or from their cultural background, e.g. a concept developed in Asia includes 310 conformity with conventions [61] while one developed in the United States includes ‘a life 311 dedicated to achieving for the sake of achievement’ in its dimensions of the good life, or 312 “authentic happiness” [62]. These overarching definitions of the good life have no 313 corresponding measurement tools but come with recommendations for combinations of 314 varying published or unpublished scales for their assessment. 315 316 Insert Table 2 here 317 318 In the integrative approach, positive mental health receives explicit attention. This added 319 focus on the positive instead of just on deficits corresponds with the introduction of a 320 recovery orientation in mental health research and practise as well as with the emergence of Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 321 the academic discipline of positive psychology. Both developments provide an important 322 context for the good life approach in a mental health context [63]. 323 324 However, the concept of positive mental health is also far from uniform. One proposed 325 organisation identifies six models of positive mental health from the literature [64]: (i) ‘above 326 normal’, i.e. supremely functioning, empathic, socially competent, resilient, self-actualised, 327 future oriented, autonomous and self-aware; (ii) ‘positive psychology’, i.e. character strength, 328 talents and enablers; (iii) ‘maturity’, i.e. reflecting theories of life time development with 329 greater maturation reflecting better mental health, (iv) ‘social-emotional intelligence’, (v) ‘well- 330 being’, i.e. happiness depending on genetic, environmental and personality factors; and (vi) 331 ‘resilience’. This suggested classification again stresses how diverse and entangled the 332 concepts of well-being and mental health are and how subjective their classification. 333 334 The Complete State Model of Mental Health is another classification proposed to provide 335 clarity for research and practise. This conceptual framework includes both poles of mental 336 health and well-being. Mental illness lies on a spectrum from absent to present. Well-being 337 lies on a spectrum from low to high. This notion of a dual continuum may be particularly 338 relevant in a mental health context, because it highlights that people can experience well- 339 being at the same time as experiencing mental illness. Well-being in this model is 340 conceptualised to include all aspects summarised in the psychological approach i.e. positive 341 affect, satisfaction, psychological well-being according to Ryff’s (1995) comprehensive 342 definition, and social well-being [44,65,66]. 343 344 Conclusion and Future Perspective 345 346 There is substantial variation in the definition and measurement of well-being. Probably an 347 integrative approach to the concept best reflects its complexity and may hence be most 348 suitable for use in mental health research and practise. 349 350 The emergent understanding of well-being as a multi-dimensional construct mirrors 351 systematic review evidence on the recovery process [67]. Empirical research into these 352 complex constructs is possible [68,69]. A recent clinically relevant advance in well-being 353 research was the development of a dynamic framework of well-being applicable to people 354 with severe mental illness. [70] This consensus definition of well-being was based on a 355 systematic literature review and qualitative research, and found that improved well-being was 356 equivalent to an enhanced sense of self. This definition links well-being to identity, and Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 357 provides an empirically defensible basis for understanding well-being in terms of 358 determinants, influences and indicators. The influences are targets for interventions to 359 improve well-being, and the indicators are outcome domains to assess the effectiveness of 360 interventions to support well-being. In practice, the convergence of well-being and recovery 361 may suggest a re-orientation of mental health services towards mainstream sources of well- 362 being. From a recovery perspective, it has been argued that mainstream solutions should be 363 preferred over specialist solutions to mainstream problems [63]. The same may be true for 364 well-being, in that factors such as employment, friendship, exercise, sex, or prayer, are as 365 important for people with severe mental illness as for any other group in society. This is 366 consistent with: the Transcendent Principle of Personhood which states that ‘people with 367 severe mental illness are people’ [71]; the Good Life approach which allows a wide range of 368 factors to be focussed on in potential interventions to improve well-being; and the Complete 369 State Model of Mental Health which acknowledges the possibility of well-being co-existing 370 with mental illness. 371 372 One future research focus will need to be on developing reliable and valid measurement 373 tools to capture outcomes consistent with these models. At present, using a combination of 374 established measures is proposed to capture diagnoses of mental illness, functioning, social 375 disability, as well as indicators of well-being such as hope, empowerment, self-esteem, social 376 connection, self-worth or meaning, together with or in addition to broad multidimensional 377 well-being scales. 378 379 Specific interventions to target well-being and its associated dimensions, such as self-worth, 380 meaning, or hope [72] will need to be further developed and evaluated. A promising resource 381 to draw on is the academic discipline of Positive Psychology [73]. The promotion of well- 382 being is a focus of this discipline, which has shifted away from ameliorating deficits (e.g. 383 symptoms) towards building positive emotions, character strengths and meaning [74]. There 384 is good evidence that interventions using positive psychology strategies increase well-being 385 and decrease depression in its recipients [75]. One specific intervention based on this body 386 of knowledge is Positive Psychotherapy (PPT). This intervention draws on a range of 387 resource oriented exercises to improve well-being in its recipient [76]. Initial evidence 388 suggests its usefulness for people with common mental disorders, such as depression 389 [74,76,77], and it may also be promising for people with severe mental illness, such as 390 psychosis [78]. Future research will have to establish the usefulness of positive psychology 391 intervention approaches, including PPT, across a spectrum of mental illnesses. 392 Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 393 394 Disclosure 395 The study is funded by Guy’s & St Thomas’ Charity (Ref G101016). Mike Slade and Andre 396 Tylee are funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research 397 Centre for Mental Health at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and 398 [Institute of Psychiatry] King's College London. The views expressed are those of the 399 author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. 400 401 References 402 403 1. Schrank B, Bird V, Tylee A, et al. Conceptualising and measuring the well-being of people 404 with psychosis: systematic review and narrative synthesis. Soc. Sci. Med. 92, 9–21 (2013). 405 2. Cummins RA, Eckersley R, Pallant J et al. Developing a National Index of Subjective 406 Wellbeing: The Australian Unity Wellbeing Index. Soc. Ind. Res. 64(2), 159-190 (2003). 407 3. Hicks N, Streeten P. Indicators of development: The search for a basic needs yardstick. 408 World Develop. 7(6), 567-580 (1979). 409 4. Glock C.Y. The Sense of Well-Being: Developing Measures. Science, 194, 52-54 (1976). 410 5. ONS. Measuring National Well-being: Summary of proposed domains and measures. 411 Office for National Statistics, London, UK (2012). 412 6. Gill TM, Feinstein AR. A Critical Appraisal of the Quality of Quality-of-Life Measurements. 413 JAMA. 272(8), 619-626 (1994). 414 7. Kaplan RM, bush JW, Berry CC. Health Status: types of validity for an index of well-being. 415 Health Serv. Res. 11(4), 478-507 (1976). 416 8. Bech P, Olsen LR, Kjoller M et al. Measuring well-being rather than the absence of 417 distress symptoms: a comparison of the SF-36 Mental Health subscale and the WHO-Five 418 Well-Being Scale. Internat. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 12(2), 85-91 (2003). 419 9. Skevington SM, Mccrate FM.Expecting a good quality of life in health: assessing people 420 with diverse diseases and conditions using the WHOQOL-BREF. Health Expect. 15(1), 49-62 421 (2011) 422 10. The EuroQol Group. EuroQol - a new facility for the measurement of health-related 423 quality of life. Health Policy. 16(3), 199-208 (1990). 424 11. Hays RD, Morales LS.The RAND-36 measure of health-related quality of life. Ann. Med., 425 33(5), 350-357 (2001). 426 12. Van Nieuwenhuizen C, Schene AH, Koeter MW et al. The Lancashire Quality of Life 427 Profile: modification and psychometric evaluation. Soc. Psychiatry. Psychiatr. Epidemiol. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 428 36(1), 36-44 (2001). 429 13. Priebe S, Huxley P, Knight S et al. Application and Results of the Manchester Short 430 Assessment of Quality of Life (Mansa). Internat. J.Soc. Psychiatry. 45(1), 7-12 (1999). 431 14. Katschnig H. How useful is the concept of quality of life in psychiatry? In: Quality of Life in 432 Mental Disorders. Katschnig H, Freeman H, Sartorius N. (Eds.), John Wiley & Sons, 433 Chichester, UK (2005) 434 15. Hunt SM, Mckenna SP. The QLDS: A scale for the measurement of quality of life in 435 depression. Health Policy, 22(3), 307-319 (1992). 436 16. Mechanic D, Mcalpine D, Rosenfield S et al. Effects of illness attribution and depression 437 on the quality of life among persons with serious mental illness. Soc. Sci. Med. 39(2), 155- 438 164 (1994). 439 17. Vothknecht S, Schoevers RA, De Haan L. Subjective well-being in schizophrenia as 440 measured with the Subjective Well-Being under Neuroleptic Treatment scale: a review. Aust. 441 N Z J Psychiatry. 45(3), 182-192 (2011). 442 18. Wright BA, Lopez SJ. Widening the Diagnostic Focus. A Case for Including Human 443 Strenghts and Environmental Resources. In: Handbook of Positive Psychology. Snyder CR, 444 Sloan JA. (Eds.), Oxford University Press, New York, US (2005). 445 19. McDowell I. Measures of self-perceived well-being. J. Psychosom Res. 69(1), 69-79 446 (2010). 447 20. Böckerman P, Johansson E, Saarni SI. Do established health-related quality-of-life 448 measures adequately capture the impact of chronic conditions on subjective well-being? 449 Health Policy. 100(1), 91-95 (2011). 450 21. Bettazzoni M, Zipursky RB, Friedland J et al. Illness Intrusiveness and Subjective Well- 451 Being in Schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 196(11), 798-805 (2008). 452 22. Kiefer RA. An Integrative Review of the Concept of Well-Being. Hol. Nurs. Practice. 22, 453 253-254 (2008). 454 23. Magyar-Moe JL. Therapist's Guide to Positive Psychological Interventions, Elsevier 455 Science & Technology, New York, US (2009). 456 24. Spiro AI, Bosse R. Relations between Health-Related Quality of Life and Well-Being: The 457 Gerontologist's New Clothes? Int. J.Aging Hum. Dev. 50(4), 297-318 (2000). 458 25. Ryan RM, Deci EL. On Happiness and Human Potentials: A Review of Research on 459 Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-Being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52(2), 141-166 (2001). 460 26. Veit CT, Ware JE. The structure of psychological distress and well-being in general 461 populations. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 51(5), 730-742 (1983). 462 27. Bruffaerts R, Vilagut G, Demyttenaere K, et al. Role of common mental and physical 463 disorders in partial disability around the world. Br. J. Psychiatry. 200(6), 454-61 (2012). Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 464 28. Joseph S, McCollam P. A bipolar happiness and depression scale. J. Genet. Psychol. 465 154(1), 127-9 (1993). 466 29. Lewis CAM, Ccollam P, Joseph S. Convergent validity of the Depression-Happiness 467 Scale with the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Soc. Behav.Per. 28(6), 579-584 (2000). 468 30. Sisask M, Värnik A, Kõlves K et al. Subjective psychological well-being (WHO-5) in 469 assessment of the severity of suicide attempt. Nord. J. Psychiatry. 62(6), 431-435 (2008). 470 31. Schneider CB, Pilhatsch M, Rifati M et al. Utility of the WHO-five well-being index as a 471 screening tool for depression in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 25(6), 777-783 (2010). 472 32. Dupuy H. The Psychological general Well-Being (PGWB) Index. In: Wenger N, Mattson 473 M, Furberg C, Elinson J. (eds.) Assessment of Quality of Life in clinical trials of 474 cardiovascular therapies. Le Jacq Publishing (1984). 475 33. Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. 476 Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30, 473-481 (1992). 477 34. BULLINGER M. Generic quality of life assessment in psychiatry. Potentials and 478 limitations. European Psychiatry, 12, 203-209 (1997) 479 35. McDowell I, Praught E. On the measurement of happiness. An examination of the 480 Bradburn scale in the Canada Health Survey. Am. J.Epidemiol. 116(6), 949-958 (1982). 481 36. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and Validation of Brief Measures of 482 Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. J.Pers. Soc.Psychol. 54(6), 1063-1070 483 (1988). 484 37. Tennant R, Joseph S, Stewart-Brown S. The Affectometer 2: a measure of positive 485 mental health in UK populations. Qual.Life Res. 16(4), 687-695 (2007). 486 38. Uher R, Goodman R. The Everyday Feeling Questionnaire: the structure and validation 487 of a measure of general psychological well-being and distress. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. 488 Epidemiol. 45(3), 413-423 (2010). 489 39. DIENER E, ROBERT EA, RANDY LJ et al. 1985. The Satisfaction With Life Scale 490 Journal of Personality Assessment 49, 71 - 75. 491 40. Hills P, Argyle M. The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: a compact scale for the 492 measurement of psychological well-being. Pers. Individ. Diff. 33(7), 1073-1082 (2002). 493 41. Cummins RA. The Second Approximation to an International Standard for Life 494 Satisfaction. Soc. Ind. Res. 43(3), 307-334 (1998). 495 42. Schwarz N, Clore GL. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and 496 directive functions of affective states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 513-523 (1983). 497 43. Diener E, Eunkook M, Suh EM et al. Subjective Well-Being: Three Decades of Progress. 498 Psychol. Bull. 125(2), 276-302 (1999). 499 *44. Ryff CD, Keyes CLM. The Structure of Psychological Well-Being Revisited. J.Pers. Soc. Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 500 Psychol. 69(4), 719–727. 501 Introduces the so far most comprehensive and widely used multidimensional model of 502 well-being from the psychological strand of well-being research. 503 45. Cummins RA. Normative Life Satisfaction: Measurement Issues and a Homeostatic 504 Model. Soc. Ind. Res. 64(2), 225-256 (2003). 505 46. Cummins RA, Nistico H. Maintaining Life Satisfaction: The Role of Positive Cognitive 506 Bias. J Happiness Stud. 3(3), 37-69 (2002). 507 47. Brickman P, Coates D, Janoff-Bulman R. Lottery winners and accident victims: Is 508 happiness relative? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36(8), 917-927 (1978). 509 48. Cloninger CR, Zohar AH. Personality and the perception of health and happiness. J. 510 Affect. Dis. 128, 24-32 (2011). 511 49. Oswald AJ, Wu S. Objective Confirmation of Subjective Measures of Human Well-Being: 512 Evidence from the U.S.A. Science. 327(5965), 576-579 (2010). 513 50. Ryff CD. Happiness Is Everything, or Is It? Explorations on the Meaning of Psychological 514 Well-Being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069-1081 (1989). 515 51. Stewart-Brown S, Tennant A, Tennant R et al. Internal construct validity of the Warwick- 516 Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): a Rasch analysis using data from the 517 Scottish Health Education Population Survey. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 7, 15 (2009). 518 *52. Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale 519 (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual. Life Outcomes. 5, 63 (2007). 520 Introduces the WEMWBS as a scale that brings together a range of pre-existing 521 assessments for well-being. 522 53. Badoux A, Mendelsohn GA. Subjective well-being in French and American samples: 523 Scale development and comparative data. Qual. Life Res. 3(6), 395-401 (1994). 524 54. Vella-Brodrick DA, Allen F. Development and psychometric validation of the Mental, 525 Physical, and Spiritual Well-being Scale. Psychol. Rep. 77(2), 659-674 (1995). 526 55. Lawton PM. The dimensions of morale In: Research planning and action for the elderly: 527 the power and potential of social science. Kent DP. (Ed.) Behavioral Publications, New York, 528 US (1972). 529 56. World Health Organization. Promoting Mental Health. Concepts, Emerging Evidence, 530 Practice., Geneva (2004). 531 57. King LA, Napa CK. What Makes a Life Good? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 156-165 (1998). 532 58. Ventegodt S, Merrick J, Andersen NJ. Quality of Life Theory I. The IQOL Theory: An 533 Integrative Theory of the Global Quality of Life Concept. ScientificWorldJournal. 13(3), 1030- 534 1040 (2003). 535 59. Ventegodt S, Merrick J, Andersen NJ. Measurement of Quality of Life II. From the Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 536 Philosophy of Life to Science. ScientificWorldJournal. 13(3), 962-971 (2003). 537 60. Lawton MP. Environment and Other Determinants of Weil-Being in Older People. 538 Gerontologist. 23(4), 349-357 (1983). 539 61. Cheung C-K. Toward a Theoretically Based Measurement Model of the Good Life J. 540 Genet. Psychol. 158(2), 200 – 215 (1997). 541 *62. Seligman M. Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize Your 542 Potential for Lasting Fulfillment, Free Press, New York,US (2002). 543 Introduces the concept of authentic happiness and positive psychology. 544 63. Slade M. Everyday Solutions for Everyday Problems: How Mental Health Systems Can 545 Support Recovery. Psychiatr. Serv. 63(7), 702–704 (2012) 546 64. Vaillant GE. Mental Health. Am. J. Psychiatry, 160, 1373-1384 92003). 547 65. Keyes CL. Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete 548 state model of health. J.Consult. Clin.Psychol. 73(3), 539-48 (2005). 549 550 **66. Keyes CL, Dhingra SS, Simoes EJ. Change in level of positive mental health as a 551 predictor of future risk of mental illness. Am J Public Health, 100(12), 2366-71 (2010). 552 This article describes and scientifically validates the complete state model of health. 553 ** 67. Leamy M, Bird V, Le Boutillier C et al. Conceptual framework for personal recovery in 554 mental health: systematic review and narrative synthesis. Br J Psychiatry. 199(6), 445-452 555 (2011). 556 This systematic literature review provides a consensus definition of recovery and the 557 processes involved in recovery from severe mental illness. 558 68. Slade M, Hayward M. Recovery, psychosis and psychiatry: research is better than 559 rhetoric. Acta. Psychiatr. Scand. 116(2), 81–83 (2007). 560 69. Slade M, Bird V, Le Boutillier C, Williams J et al. REFOCUS Trial: protocol for a cluster 561 randomised controlled trial of a pro-recovery intervention within community based mental 562 health teams. BMC Psychiatry 11: 185 (2011). 563 *70. Schrank B, Riches S, Bird V, Murray J, Tylee A, Slade M. A conceptual framework for 564 improving well-being in people with a diagnosis of psychosis. Epidem. Psych. Sci. in press 565 (2013) 566 This qualitative research identifies a definition of well-being in people with severe 567 mental illness, with a particular focus on the processes involved in improving well- 568 being. 569 71. Anthony W.A. The principle of personhood: the field's transcendent principle. Psychiatr. 570 Rehabil. J. 27, 205 (2004). Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 571 72. Schrank B, Stanghellini G, Slade M. Hope in psychiatry: a review of the Literature. Acta 572 Psychiatr. Scand. 118(6), 421–433 (2008) 573 73. Slade, M. Mental illness and well-being: the central importance of positive psychology 574 and recovery approaches. BMC Health Serv. Res. 10: 26 (2010). 575 74. Seligman MEP, Steen, TA, Peterson C. Positive psychology progress: empirical 576 validation of interventions. Am Psychologist 60(5), 410-421 (2005). 577 **75. Sin NL, Lyubomirsky S. Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms 578 with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis." J Clin Psychol, 579 65(5), 467-487 (2009). 580 This systematic literature review provides an overview on randomised controlled trials 581 investigating various positive psychology interventions in different client groups. It 582 also investigates determinants for the effectiveness of the included therapeutic 583 interventions. 584 76. Rashid T. Positive Psychotherapy. . In: Positive psychology: Exploring the best in people. 585 Lopez SJ (Ed.) Greenwood Publishing Company, Westport CT, US (2008). 586 76. Seligman ME P, Rashid T, Parks AC. Positive psychotherapy. Am Psychologist, 61(8), 587 774-788 (2006). 588 77. Parks-Sheiner AC. Positive psychotherapy: Building a model of empirically supported 589 self-help, University of Pennsylvania (2009). 590 78. Meyer PS, Johnson DP, Parks A, Iwanski, C, Penn DL. Positive living: A pilot study of 591 group positive psychotherapy for people with schizophrenia." J. Pos. Psychol. 7(3), 239-248 592 (2012). 593 Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper 594 Table 1: Dimensions of multidimensional well-being scales captured well-being dimensions Social Scales SPWB WEMWBS EBE satisfying social interpersonal connectedness relationships competence Psychological selfacceptance personal development Physical Spiritual clear thinking energy LSI PGCM positive lonely relations with dissatisfaction others environmental mastery autonomy resolution, fortitude selfpositive acceptance selfconcept personal growth purpose in life attitudes to own aging autonomy Mental MPSWB mental activities zest positive affect mood tone Health perception physical symptoms spiritual activities positive and negative affect agitation 595 596 597 Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance. Peer Review Paper Table 2: Components of the ‘good life’ 598 599 Components General Satisfaction Happiness Ventegodt 2003 [58,59] well-being (i.e. overall feeling about one’s being) satisfaction with life happiness (i.e. existential feeling of cheerfulness and harmony) meaning in life Meaning biological balance of the body, reflecting overall physical health realising life potential Balance Realise potential Authors [reference] Cheung 1997 Lawton 1983 [61] [60] psychological well-being life satisfaction positive and negative affect fulfilment of needs objective factors (e.g. income or marital status) Fitting in engaged life (i.e. optimal use of character strengths) achieving life (i.e. achieving for the sake of achievement) Achievement Connectednes s behavioural competence domain specific perceived quality of life objective environment Meet needs Observable pleasant life (i.e. hedonistic orientation to positive emotions and pleasures) meaningful life (i.e. belonging to and serving something larger than oneself) religious commitment autonomy, selfactualising Seligman 2002 [62] mutual interpersonal concerns (e.g. marital satisfaction, understanding others) conformity with conventions 600 601 Please return your comments for the attention of the Assistant Commissioning Editor at d.chamberlain@futuremedicine.com Many thanks in advance for your kind assistance.