Regulating violence: response inhibition is differentially related to instrumental and reactive aggression Kevin Kemkes, 3542998 August 24, 2014 Writing assignment Neuroscience and Cognition Master Program, Utrecht University Conducted at: Helmholtz Institute Department of Experimental Psychology Utrecht University, The Netherlands Word count: 6033 References: 47 Supervisor/primary assessor: David Terburg, PhD. Assistant Professor Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University Secondary assessor: Prof. Jack van Honk Professor of Social Neuroscience Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University Honorary Professor, University of Cape Town Abstract Response inhibition is the termination or prevention of impulsive motor responses. Impaired response inhibition has been primarily related to disorders with pronounced impulsivity. The current review proposes that response inhibition performance is also central to the symptomatology of aggressive disorders. Importantly, it is proposed that response inhibition performance is related to increased instrumental aggression (or goal-directed aggression), but decreased reactive aggression (or aggression in response to frustration/threat). This is because response inhibition is implicated in emotional and behavioral regulation. Reactive aggression is related to impaired emotional and behavioral regulation, while instrumental aggression requires strong emotional and behavioral regulation. The purpose of this review is to explore this hypothesis by focusing on the relationship of response inhibition with disorders related to of impulsivity and aggression. Strong evidence in favor of the hypothesized relationship will be presented by focusing on studies showing that aggressive disorders hallmarked by instrumental aggression are related to improved response inhibition, while disorders with pronounced reactive aggression are related to impaired response inhibition. Furthermore, the current review aims to explore possible neurobiological mechanisms underlying this relationship. Recently the dual-hormone serotonergic (DHS) hypothesis of aggression has been proposed. This states that high testosterone/cortisol ratios predispose to increased general aggression, after which 5-HT (or serotonin) levels affect the balance between instrumental and reactive aggression by mediating impulsivity. The current review will propose that response inhibition mediates the relationship of 5-HT with instrumental and reactive aggression. 2 1. Introduction Aggressive behavior is a big concern for society because of the devastating consequences for both the aggressor and the victim. In addition, aggressive behaviors form a serious economic burden. In The Netherlands, the annual costs of intimate partner violence alone is already estimated around 142.2 million US dollars (World Health Organization, 2004). This burden is larger at the extremities of violent behavior such as aggressive disorders. For example, the overall prevalence of psychopathic personality traits associated with increased violent behavior are commonly estimated around 1% of the general population (for example Neumann & Hare, 2008), while the prevalence of psychopathic traits in the Dutch prison population has been estimated around 21.7% (Hildebrand, de Ruiter, & Nijman, 2008). It is therefore important to study how the nervous system is implicated in aggressive behaviors since this could lead to new strategies to prevent aggressive behaviors from occurring. 1.1 Aggression and impulsivity Aggression disorders are often accompanied by increased impulsivity, and disorders with pronounced impulsiveness are often accompanied by increased aggression (Hart & Dempster, 1997; Retz & Rösler, 2010; Wallcott, Mangione & Landau, 2004). This suggests that a relationship exists between levels of impulsivity and aggression. However, aggression and impulsiveness are very heterogeneous concepts, and it is currently unclear how different forms of aggression are related to the neurobiological and cognitive mechanisms underlying impulsivity. Aggression is an overt behavior that is intended to inflict physical damage on another individual (Nelson & Trainor, 2007). Researchers have found two well-validated dimensions of aggressive behavior. Reactive aggression is performed in response to frustration/threat, whereas instrumental aggression is performed to achieve goals (Blair, 2001). Impulsivity has been defined as a personality trait associated with risk-taking, lack of planning, and making up one’s mind quickly (Arce & Santisteban, 2006). Experimental psychologists distinguish between motor impulsiveness and cognitive impulsiveness (Bechara, Damasio & Damasio, 2000). The former is similar to (and hereafter will be referred to as) response inhibition, whereas the latter is related to the capacity to delay gratification. Response inhibition can be described as the termination or prevention of impulsive motor responses. In line with this distinction of cognitive and motor impulsivity, lesion and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies show that response inhibition is more related to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), whereas cognitive impulsiveness is related to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; Bechara, Damasio & Damasio, 2000). 3 The motor tasks most often used to test response inhibition performance are the stop-signal task and the Go/NoGo task (Swick, Ashley, & Turken, 2011). The stop signal task involves a simple choice-reaction time task, in which the primary task is followed by a stop signal during a proportion of trials. The delay between primary task and stop signal (or the stop signal delay; SSD) is altered to find the latency at which the participant can inhibit responses in 50% of the stop trials. The Go/NoGo task involves a discrimination task in which the participants should respond to certain, but not to other stimuli. Difficulty is altered by the proportion of NoGo stimuli. The former task involves terminating an ongoing response process, whereas the latter involves the prevention of a response process. The current review will focus on the relationship between response inhibition, but not cognitive impulsiveness, with instrumental and reactive aggressive behavior for the following reasons. First, Dougherty, Bjork, Harper, el al. (2003) found that computer tasks designed to assess response inhibition are more sensitive for discrimination between groups with and without disruptive behaviors then those that asses cognitive impulsivity. This suggests that response inhibition task performance reflects regulatory deficits in aggressive populations more uniquely then cognitive inhibition tasks. Second, Feilhauer, Cima, Korebrits, and Kunert (2012) demonstrated in a sample of subjects with conduct disorder that response inhibition performance is positively correlated to self-reported reactive aggression, while being negatively correlated to instrumental aggression. This shows that response inhibition performance can discriminate between the two dimensions of aggressive behavior. Reactive aggression, with its uncontrolled and explosive nature, is related to impaired response inhibition. Instrumental aggression, with its callous and goal-directed nature, is related to improved response inhibition. Last, response inhibition is a mechanism implicated in emotion-regulation. For example, a study by Walcott, Mangione, and Landau (2004) showed that emotion regulation during a frustrating peer competition is related to response inhibition performance. In this study, an ADHD and healthy group were instructed to either regulate or not to regulate their emotions during the competition. Response inhibition was impaired after the competition when healthy participants were not required to regulate emotions. The ADHD group showed an overall lack of emotional regulation and repsonse inhibition. Response inhibition is a mechanism used n emotion-regulation important to delay impulsive behavioral responses. This allows a person consider social consequences of intended behaviors. For these reasons, the current review will explore the hypothesis that response inhibition performance is related to increased instrumental and decreased reactive aggression. In the conceptualization of this review the neural mechanisms of response inhibition are required to 4 regulate emotion and behavior. Instrumental aggression requires strong regulation of emotion and behavior, whereas a tendency to reactive aggression is caused by impaired regulation. If it is the case that response inhibition can differentiate between instrumental and reactive aggression, then response inhibition performance could potentially be measured to personalize medicine and predict treatment outcome. Another important question that will be addressed is which neurobiological mechanisms could underlie the relation of response inhibition with reactive and instrumental aggression. Since much is known about the neurobiology of response inhibition and aggressive behavior, relating response inhibition to instrumental and reactive aggressive behavior could lead to new insights. First, the relationship of response inhibition with reactive and instrumental aggression in mental disorders will be discussed to explore the hypothesis on a behavioral level. This will be followed by a review of the neurobiology underlying response inhibition, reactive aggression and instrumental aggression. There will be a special focus on the Dual-Hormone Serotonergic (DHS) hypothesis that has been proposed by Montoya, Terburg, Bos, and van Honk (2012). 2. Disorders of aggression and impulsivity Traditionally, researchers interested in response inhibition have focused on inhibitory deficits in ADHD. However, response inhibition also seems to be a central component in disorders with pronounced instrumental and reactive aggression. The hypothesis stated above predicts that disorders with clear distortions in impulsivity are related to increased reactive aggression and reduced instrumental aggression. It also predicts that aggression disorders hallmarked by instrumental aggression are related to improved response inhibition, and those disorders related to reactive aggression are related to impaired response inhibition. 2.1 Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder ADHD is characterized by deficits in executive functions resulting in increased impulsive behavior (Walcott, Mangione & Landau, 2004). ADHD is strongly associated with impaired response inhibition. A study by Retz & Rösler (2010) investigated the prevalence of reactive and instrumental violent offenses in a forensic sample of adult men. In this sample ADHD and childhood ADHD traits were diagnosed by trained psychiatrists. Participants were placed either in a no-ADHD, ADHD (partially remitted) or ADHD group. Childhood ADHD symptoms were scored by use of a self-report questionnaire. In addition, another group of researchers rated the criminal offenses conducted by the offenders on reactive and instrumental aggressive components. 5 The results by Retz & Rösler (2010) show that the offenses committed by the ADHD group scored higher on reactive aggression then the ADHD (partially remitted) group, which in turn scored higher than the no-ADHD group. In addition, the offenses by the no-ADHD group showed increased instrumental aggression compared to the other groups. The self-report data showed that childhood ADHD symptoms had a positive relationship with reactive aggression, but a negative relationship with instrumental aggression. 2.2 conduct disorder Conduct disorder (CD) is a childhood disorder that features a “repetitive and persistent pattern of behavior in which the basic rights of others and major age-appropriate societal norms or rules are violated” (Loeber, Burke, Lahey, Winters, & Zera, 2000, p. 1469) and could be considered as a child form of antisocial disorder (Blair, 2001). Conduct disorder is often comorbid with ADHD, but is also independently diagnosed. Compared to psychopathy it is hallmarked by increased reactive aggression and decreased instrumental aggression (Feilhauer, Cima, Korebrits, & Kunert, 2012), although both behaviors can be present. The study by Loeber et al. (2002) investigated response inhibition in children with ADHD, ADHD with conduct disorder, conduct disorder, no disorder and anxiety disorders (the latter will not be discussed). They aimed to investigate if response inhibition was a core feature of conduct disorder, or if this was due to its comorbidity with ADHD. Loeber et al. (2002). found that the ADHD, CD and combined group showed response inhibition impairments compared to the control group. Furthermore, no distinction could be made between the three disorder groups based on response inhibition performance. Thus, this disorder that is often associated with increased reactive aggression is related to impaired response inhibition. Conduct disorder, however, is a very heterogeneous concept, and a subset of patients with this disorder shows increased instrumental aggression. The study by Loeber et al. (2002) did not account for this heterogeneity. A study by Bennett, Pitale, Vora, & Rheingold (2004) sheds more light on the relationship between childhood antisocial behaviors and the dimensions of aggression. Note however that this study measured antisocial traits in a non-clinical sample. Bennett et al. (2004) divided antisocial traits in reactive and proactive antisocial dimensions. Reactive and instrumental aggression were included in the former and latter concepts, respectively. The study by Bennett et al. (2004) showed that reactive antisocial traits are more related to measurements of ADHD traits then proactive antisocial traits. However, proactive antisocial traits also correlated with ADHD traits in adolescents and higher age groups (in a weaker but positive relationship, in contrary to the hypothesis of the current review). As the authors pointed out, this is 6 in line with the earlier onset of reactive versus proactive traits in the development of antisocial behavior. This study is relevant for several reasons. First, response inhibition deficits are central to ADHD traits. Second, reactive aggression is a core feature of reactive antisocial traits and instrumental aggression is a core feature of proactive antisocial traits. Thus, the study by Bennett et al. (2004) shows that increased impulsive traits are more strongly related to antisocial behavior that includes reactive aggression then to antisocial behavior that includes instrumental aggression. However, it is important to note that levels of aggressive behavior were not assessed directly. 2.3 antisocial personality disorder A diagnosis for an antisocial personality disorder has similar behavioral criteria as conduct disorder, except that it is diagnosed in adults (Loeber et al, 2000). A study by Dolan & Park (2002) investigated whether patients with a diagnosis for an antisocial personality disorder show deficits in neuropsychological tasks related to the VMPFC and DLPFC. As mentioned, emotion regulation has been related to both of these areas. These VMPFC-related tasks included a Go/NoGo task to measure response inhibition. The study by Dolan & Park (2002) was inspired by inconsistencies in the literature about whether antisocial personality disorder is related to VMPFC damage or also to DLPFC damage. Patients showed impaired performance on VMPFC-related and DLPFC-related tasks. The authors therefore suggested that antisocial personality disorder is associated with dysfunction of both the VMPFC and DLPFC. This data is in line with the above mentioned study by Loeber et al. (2002). Those authors reported essentially the same general impairment of response inhibition in CD patients. Thus, both conduct disorder and antisocial disorder are in general associated with decreased response inhibition. Both CD and antisocial personality disorder are more associated with reactive aggression, and less associated with instrumental aggression, then psychopathy. Similar to Loeber et al. (2012), an important limitation of the study by Dolan & Park (2002) is that the authors did not differentiate between those patients expressing instrumental and reactive aggression. 2.4 Psychopathy Psychopathy is a developmental disorder hallmarked by affective and behavioral components like a lack of empathy and increased violent and criminal behavior (Blair, 2010b; Feilhauer, et al., 2012). Psychopathic behavior can be described by two dimensions, the affective-interpersonal and the antisocial dimension. The former is traditionally characterized by manipulation and instrumental aggression, whereas the latter is characterized by impulsivity and reactive aggression (Feilhauer et al., 2012). The antisocial dimension also highly overlaps with antisocial disorders. The primary 7 distinction between psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder is often in the severity of affective-interpersonal traits. The study by Feilhauer et al. (2012) investigated the validity of this dual model for psychopathy by assessing psychopathic traits in both healthy participants and those diagnosed with conduct disorder. They related self-reported instrumental and reactive aggression to response inhibition. In addition Feilhauer et al. (2012) also related psychopathic traits in these samples to reactive and instrumental aggression. Importantly, their study clearly demonstrates that response inhibition performance is positively related to self-reported instrumental aggression but negatively related to self-reported reactive aggression. Their results also show that the antisocial psychopathic dimension is associated with increased reactive aggression. No relationship was found for the affective-interpersonal dimension and response inhibition. The study by Feilhauer et al. (2012) is the most convincing evidence that shows the existence of the hypothesized relation of response inhibition performance with reactive and instrumental aggression to date. 3. The prefrontal cortex The previous section showed that response inhibition performance is negatively related to reactive aggression but positively related to instrumental aggression in impulsive and aggressive disorders. In order to understand the link between response inhibition and aggression one needs to understand the underlying neurobiology. Response inhibition and emotional regulation are both related to the prefrontal cortex (Nelson & Trainor, 2007; Aron, Fletcher, Bullmore, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2003). It thus seems logical to expect an interplay between response inhibition and aggression regulation in this region of the brain. 3.1 Role of prefrontal areas in aggression and response inhibition Especially the DLPFC and VMPFC play a key role in emotion regulation (Blair, 2010a; Perach-Barzilay, Tauber, Klein, et al. 2013). Frontal areas regulate aggressive behavior with their inhibitory input into subcortical structures that are involved in aggression (Nelson & Trainor, 2007). In line with this, low baseline activity in the prefrontal cortex has been associated with increased reactive aggression (Volkow, Tancredi, Grant, Gillespie, Valentine, Mullani, Wang, & Hollister, 1995). Several studies suggest that prefrontal regulation of aggression depends on 5-HT input (for a review see Montoya et al. 2012). Since these receptors receive input from the 5-HT raphe system that originates in the brainstem (Lesch & Merschdorf, 2000), this shows that the relationship between cortical and subcortical areas is not simply characterized by top-down control, but that the cortex also receives bottom-up connections. Increased instrumental aggression has been related to increased activation 8 of the DLPFC during emotion-processing tasks (Glenn & Raine, 2009). This suggests that instrumental aggression relates to enhanced cognitive control in order to guide emotional processing. The DLPFC also seems to play an important role in response inhibition (Aron et al., 2003). The DLPFC, as a functional area, is located on the inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG). The study by Aron et al. (2003) found that rIFG lesions often result in the impairment of response inhibition. Moreover, they found that the lesion size is correlated to response inhibition impairment. This relationship was not found for the left IFG or other brain areas. These studies provide strong evidence for a rightlateralized response inhibition network of the human brain. This finding has been replicated by using transcranial magnetic stimulation, in which inhibiting rIFG activity resulted in impaired response inhibition performance (Chambers, Bellgrove, Stokes et al., 2006). FMRI studies confirm the right-lateralized inhibition network for response inhibition (Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2013). These studies show that the DLPFC is a key regulator, supplemented by the frontalparietal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (for an extensive overview see Garavan, Hester, Murphy, Fassbender, & Kelly, 2006; Aron, 2003). After receiving information about the presence of a stop signal, these areas work in concert to suppress activity in the motor areas via suppression of basal ganglia output. This last finding is very important because the proposed function of the DLPFC in response inhibition and emotion regulation shows important overlap. During response inhibition, the DLPFC is thought to inhibit basal ganglia output (Aron, 2011), whereas in emotion regulation it inhibits subcortical areas related to anger and aggression (Blair, 2010a; Perach-Barzilay, et al. 2013). This overlap is consistent with the view that response inhibition mechanisms are required for the regulation of aggressive behaviors. 4. The role of subcortical structures in aggression and response inhibition Aggressive responses rely on an evolutionary old system composed of subcortical structures that drive aggressive behavior (Panksepp, 1998, p. 196). In contrast to aggression, much less is known about the influence of subcortical structures on response inhibition performance. This section will describe how the amygdala and hypothalamus are related to aggressive behavior and will mention some interesting findings on their relation to response inhibition. 4.1. Role of the amygdala in aggression and response inhibition The amygdala receives sensory input and is implicated in both dimensions of aggressive behavior (Blair, 2010a). It functions to detect emotionally salient stimuli. Based on this information it mediates activity in other subcortical structures including the hypothalamus (Panksepp, 1998, p.196). The 9 amygdala is also interconnected with the prefrontal cortex (Banks, Eddy, Angstadt, Nathan, & Phan; 2007). The coupling of the amygdala with the prefrontal cortex is of major importance for emotion regulation (Banks et al. 2007). That is, stronger covariation of neural activity between the amygdala and prefrontal cortex is related to improved regulation of negative affect. These prefrontal areas include the DLPFC that is also related to response inhibition. In subjects demonstrating high reactive aggression the amygdala response caused by threat stimuli is increased (Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald, & Phan, 2007). This probably results in overinterpretation of ambiguous and threat stimuli, thereby increasing reactive aggressive behavior. Psychopathic traits like instrumental aggression have been associated with reduced amygdala responses to a variety of tasks that involve emotional learning and emotional processing (Blair, 2010b). This is in line with impairments in recognizing distress in other individuals. Thus amygdala reactivity seems to be suppressed in patients with pronounced instrumental aggression but enhanced in patients with pronounced reactive aggression. There are not many studies regarding the role of the amygdala in response inhibition. Increased amygdala activity has been related to improved response inhibition in an emotional Go/NoGo task (Pawliczek, Derntl, Kellermann, Kohn, Gur, & Habel, 2013). In this task, participants with high trait aggression showed reduced response inhibition. Also, participants showed relative improvement of response inhibition when the stop stimulus was an angry facial expression compared to a neutral expression. In addition to the cortical response inhibition network, the angry stop stimulus was related to increased right amygdala activity (amongst others). According to Pawliczek et al. (2013) the amygdala, due to its extensive connections with the cortex, enhances the processing of the stop signal when it contains a threat stimulus. This results in improved response inhibition performance. One argument in favor of this view is that Pawliczek et al. (2013) also found increased activity in the occipital lobe found during angry stop signals compared to neutral stop trials. 4.2 Role of hypothalamus in aggression and response inhibition The hypothalamus receives major input from the amygdala (Panksepp, 1998, p.196). In addition, the hypothalamus is part of, and receives information from, the endocrine system (discussed below). This is where physiological “irritations” that dependent on the endocrine system, like hunger, can influence aggressive behavior. The hypothalamus regulates the endocrine and autonomic nervous system. Animal studies show that reactive aggression is mainly associated with the medial hypothalamus, whereas the lateral hypothalamus is implicated in predatory aggression (Siegel & Victoroff, 2009). Predatory aggression is often used as a model for human instrumental aggression. It 10 shows some superficial characteristics similar to instrumental aggression such as a goal-directed and callous nature. Predatory aggression, however, is not homologous to human instrumental aggression and one should be careful with generalizations. The hypothalamus relays signals to the periaqueductal grey (PAG), which has extensive reciprocal connections with the frontal cortex and other areas of the brain (Panksepp, 1998, p. 197). Together, the amygdala, hypothalamus and PAG together form a well-known reactive aggression network that causally induces reactive aggressive behavior in animals when stimulated. To the authors best knowledge no study has directly associated the hypothalamus activity or lesions with response inhibition. However, the hypothalamus is a key regulator of the endocrine system. Later, some interesting evidence for hormonal influence on response inhibition will be discussed. 5. The role of 5-HT, cortisol and testosterone in aggression and response inhibition Studying role of hormones and 5-HT in aggression and response inhibition is very important since they are potential targets of pharmacological treatments. 5-HT is a major modulator of aggressive behavior (Lesch, 2000, Nelson & Trainor, 2007). Furthermore, 5-HT is explicitly related to impulsivity (Clark, Roiser, Cools, Rubinsztein, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2005), which makes it a likely candidate for mediating the hypothesized relation of response inhibition with increased instrumental and decreased reactive aggression. Testosterone has been known to regulate social aggression in animals (for example Wingfield et al. 1990) including humans (Bos, Panksepp, Bluthé, & van Honk. 2012). The effect of testosterone and 5-HT on aggression might depend on low levels of cortisol (Montaya et al. 2012). This section will explore how these hormones and 5-HT are related to reactive aggression, instrumental aggression and response inhibition. 5.1 The role of testosterone/cortisol ratios in aggression Testosterone is an androgen steroid hormone that is regulated by the hypothalamic-pituitarygonadal (HPG) axis. Testosterone promotes aggression by increasing reactivity on social challenges (referred to as the “challenge hypothesis”, see Wingfield et al., 1990; Bos et al., 2012). It should be noted, however, that dealing with social challenges can be achieved by other means then aggression. Some challenges will result in aggressive behavior, and therefore the total number of aggressive behaviors increase with high levels of testosterone. In line with this view, research clearly shows a relationship between testosterone levels and aggressive behavior (Pavlov, Chistiakov & Chekhonin, 2012; Montoya et al., 2012; Bos et al. 2012). For example, testosterone increases hearth rate responses specifically after seeing angry facial expressions (van Honk, Tuiten, Hermans et al., 2001) This could be interpreted as an enhanced 11 disposition to defend social status. In addition, testosterone increases amygdala, hypothalamus and rIFG activation in response to emotional faces compared to neutral faces, especially for angry facial expressions (Hermans, Ramsey, & van Honk, 2008). Thus, testosterone improves the responsiveness of structures related to aggression and aggression regulation. This is in line with the idea that testosterone enhances the readiness to fight by increased threat perception (Bos et al., 2012). The relationship of testosterone with aggression depends strongly on the interaction of testosterone with cortisol. Cortisol is a steroid hormone released by the adrenal gland and regulated by the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Montoya et al. 2012). It is the primary stress hormone. The effect of testosterone on aggression is more pronounced under low basal levels of cortisol (for a review see Montoya et al., 2012). Low basal levels of cortisol are related to low fear and therefore can promote aggressive behavior. High testosterone/cortisol ratios decrease the communication between cortical and subcortical areas (Terburg, Morgan & van Honk, 2009). Due to enhanced subcortical activity in the amygdala high testosterone/cortisol ratios promote reactive aggressive behaviors. Likewise, this decoupling results in “emotionally cold” instrumental aggression since the cortex does not receive input from limbic structures. These findings resulted in the hypothesis that high testosterone/cortisol ratios are related to both increased reactive and instrumental aggressive behavior. These relationships have been confirmed in several clinical populations including conduct disorder and psychopathy (see Montoya et al. 2012). There is a clear lack of studies regarding the effect of testosterone and cortisol on response inhibition. There is a single but interesting study that has been done in relation to anti-androgen therapy in men, which is a common treatment for prostate cancer (Chao, Uchio, Zhang, Hu, Benarski, Luo, Rose, Concato, & Li, 2012). Note that an anti-androgen treatment does not only target testosterone. However, antagonizing receptors for testosterone on cancer cells to reduce stimulation of cell division is a primary treatment goal. By using fMRI, the study by Chao et al. (2012) found that androgen depletion decreased the BOLD response in the MPFC, right insula, and importantly the middle/inferior frontal cortex during a stop signal task. These frontal areas are implicated in emotion-regulation. This study shows that response inhibition networks can be modulated by hormone levels. Future studies should aim to investigate the effect of testosterone, cortisol and testosterone/cortisol ratios on response inhibition. 5.2 Role of 5-HT in aggression and impulsivity The 5-HT system specifically mediates reactive aggression. The effects of 5-HT on aggression are mainly mediated by the 5-HT raphe system in the brain stem (Lesch & Merschdorf, 2000; Siegel & 12 Victoroff, 2009). This system projects from the raphe nucleus to a large amount of brain areas including the amygdala, hypothalamus and the areas in the frontal cortex Low baseline levels of 5-HT are associated increased reactive aggressive behaviors in men and rodents (Nelson & Trainor, 2007; Montoya, 2012). This is supported by human studies, in which reactive aggressive behavior was inversely related to 5-HT1A receptor binding potential (Parsey, Oquendo, Simpson, Ogden, van Heertum, Arango, & Mann (2002). The relationship between 5-HT and instrumental aggression is unclear for humans. In addition, animal studies on the relation of 5-HT and predatory behavior show mixed results. Some animal studies regarding predatory aggression in rodents show that administering 5-HT affects reactive aggression but not predatory aggression (Ferrari, Palanza, Rodgers, Mainardi, & Parmigiani, 1996). However, a review by Nikulina (1991) showed that psychopharmacological treatments that increase 5-HT level inhibits predatory behavior in several kinds of animals. Thus, the relationship of 5-HT with predatory aggression in animals and instrumental aggression in humans remains unclear. Increasing 5-HT results in increased glucose metabolism in the orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate cortex and VMPFC in healthy subjects (Siever, Buchsbaum, New, et al., 1999). However, this effect was not found for reactive aggressive patients. This finding is in line with the view that 5-HT increases activity in frontal areas related to emotion-regulation and that reactive aggressive patients lack cortical regulation of aggression. The findings on testosterone/cortisol ratios and 5-HT have led to the DHS hypothesis (Montoya et al. 2012). This hypothesis states that high basal testosterone/cortisol ratios predispose towards general aggression, while 5-HT levels affect the balance between instrumental and reactive aggression. Low prefrontal 5-HT predisposes to reactive aggression by increasing impulsivity, while high prefrontal 5-HT levels predispose to instrumental aggression by reducing impulsivity. The notion that 5-HT influences the balance between instrumental and reactive aggression due its relation to impulsivity is very important. This is because response inhibition is also strongly related to impulsivity (Arce & Santisteban, 2006). Because of the hypothesized relation of 5-HT with reactive and instrumental aggression, the hypothesis outlined at the start of this review would predict a positive relation between 5-HT and response inhibition. There is evidence that neural motor inhibition networks are effected by 5-HT levels. A study by Rubia, Lee, Cleare, Tunstall, Fu, Brammer & McGuire (2005) showed a reduction of activity in the rIFG after tryptophan depletion during motor inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Tryptophan is a precursor of 5-HT, and tryptophan depletion thus results in low 5-HT levels in the brain. This reduction in rIFG activation shows that low 5-HT levels impair activity in areas that are strongly implicated in response inhibition. In another study, Passamonti, Fera, Magariello, et al. (2006) found a similar pattern. They compared carriers of a low-activity versus high-activity allele of monoamine oxidase-A (MAOA) gene. 13 High expression of MAOA genes is associated with lower synaptic serotonin levels. In this study the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex showed increased activity during response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task for subjects with high expression of this gene. This area has been associated with impulsiveness. Because of the association of response inhibition with impulsivity and emotion-regulation, response inhibition might be a mechanism that mediates the relationship of 5-HT with instrumental and reactive aggression that is proposed by the DHS hypothesis. The findings by Rubia et al. (2005) and Passamonti et al. (2006) show that lower 5-HT levels are related to lower activity in areas that have been related to response inhibition and impulsivity. As mentioned above, response inhibition performance is related to increased instrumental and decreased reactive aggression in disorders of aggression and impulsivity. These relationships of response inhibition with 5-HT and aggression are in congruent DHS hypothesis. 6. Conclusion and future directions The current review investigated the hypothesis that response inhibition is related to increased instrumental and decreased reactive aggression. This was investigated on a behavioral level by looking at the relationship of response inhibition with reactive and instrumental aggression in mental disorders. Further, the relationship of neural mechanisms of response inhibition with reactive aggression and instrumental aggression have been compared. This led to an extension of the DHS hypothesis proposed by Montoya et al. (2012). 6.1 conclusions Studies towards ADHD and aggressive disorders convincingly demonstrate the hypothesized relation of response inhibition with reactive and instrumental aggression on a behavioral level. Research towards psychopathic traits in conduct disorder showed that response inhibition is related to increased instrumental but decreased reactive aggressive behavior. Furthermore, aggressive disorders that are associated with pronounced reactive aggression are related to impaired response inhibition. Studies regarding ADHD, a disorder hallmarked by impaired response inhibition, clearly demonstrate increased reactive and decreased instrumental aggression compared to healthy subjects. The VMPFC and DLPFC are important areas for the regulation of aggression. In addition, the VMPFC is related to cognitive inhibition, whereas the DLPFC is important for response inhibition. The DLPFC has inhibitory inputs into subcortical structures involved in aggressive behavior and the basal ganglia involved in motor activity. Instrumental aggression requires strong prefrontal regulation of 14 emotion and behavior as it is callous and goal-directed. Reactive aggression can be the result of impaired prefrontal regulation of emotion and behavior, resulting in increased aggressive drive. In line with this, individuals with pronounced instrumental aggression show reduced amygdala activity but increased DLPFC activity during emotional processing tasks. Furthermore, reactive aggression is related to increased amygdala activity in response to threat stimuli but reduced baseline prefrontal activity. The DHS hypothesis states that the balance between instrumental and reactive aggression is mediated by 5-HT under high testosterone/cortisol ratios. 5-HT is strongly associated with impulsivity, which in turn is associated with impaired response inhibition. The 5-HT raphe nucleus projects to major cortical and subcortical areas implicated in emotional and behavioral regulation. It has been shown that low 5-HT levels are related to increased reactive aggression. Furthermore, increasing 5-HT levels results in increased activity of prefrontal areas related to emotional and behavioral regulation. In addition, reducing 5-HT by tryptophan depletion reduces activity in the rIFG during response inhibition tasks. This area is of major importance for response inhibition. These findings demonstrate that the 5-HT raphe system is a neurobiological mechanism that affects the neural response inhibition networks. Based on these findings, the current review extends the DHS hypothesis by proposing that the effect of 5-HT on instrumental and reactive aggression under high testosterone/cortisol ratios is mediated by response inhibition mechanisms (see figure 1). Low 5-HT levels, strongly related to impulsivity, result in decreased activity in the IFG (which overlaps with the DLPFC). This explains the negative association between impulsivity and response inhibition. Response inhibition is implicated in emotion-regulation. The current model predicts that It is positively related to instrumental aggression, but negatively related to reactive aggression. This is because the DLPFC regulates activity in subcortical areas related to aggression, such as the amygdala. Decreased regulation results in increased subcortical aggressive drive, resulting in increased reactive aggression. On the other hand, strong cortical regulation results in increased callous and goal-directed instrumental aggression. 15 Figure 1. Proposed model for the relations between trait impulsivity, response inhibition, reactive aggression and instrumental aggression under high testosterone/cortisol ratios. Behavioral phenomena are depicted in bold, neural mechanisms in italic. Positive associations are depicted by continuous lines while negative associations are depicted by dashed lines. The grey lines show relations predicted by the DHS hypothesis. The black lines represent the additional associations proposed in this review. In addition to the DHS hypothesis it is proposed that high impulsivity and low 5-HT activity are related to reduced response inhibition performance and DLPFC activity during response inhibition. Response inhibition performance is positively related to instrumental aggression and negatively related to reactive aggression. Instrumental aggression, because of its goal-directed nature, requires strong cortical control for emotional and behavioral regulation. Reduced prefrontal regulation results in enhanced aggressive drive to threats and provocations, increasing reactive aggression 6.2 Future directions Future studies should elucidate on the neural mechanisms underlying the relationship of response inhibition with reactive and instrumental aggression. Most importantly, they should aim to study the influence of hormones on response inhibition. In order to validate the proposed model it is of primary importance to investigate if response inhibition is more positively related to instrumental aggression and more negatively to reactive aggression in individuals with high testosterone/cortisol ratios compared to low testosterone/cortisol ratios. This would be in line with the view that response inhibition affects the dimension of aggression more strongly in individuals predisposed towards high general aggression. Elevated testosterone/cortisol ratios seem to occur in a diversity of aggressive disorders such as conduct disorder and psychopathy (Montoya et al. 2012). The current review clearly demonstrated a positive relationship between response inhibition performance and instrumental aggression. However, the relationship of 5-HT with instrumental 16 aggression remains unclear. Although the current review mentioned several studies suggesting a negative relationship, there is a lack of direct experimental evidence. Reduced 5-HT levels are related to lower activation in neural response inhibition networks (Rubia et al. 2005; Passamonti et al. 2006) and increased impulsivity (Clark et al., 2005). Taking these findings into account, the positive association of instrumental aggression with improved response inhibition suggests a positive relationship between 5-HT and instrumental aggression. Future studies should directly test this hypothesis in a single experiment. The notion that response inhibition is related to increased instrumental and decreased reactive aggression is highly relevant for clinical treatment and research. Studying response inhibition in aggressive disorders is especially relevant in the case of psychopathy, since instrumental and reactive aggression is of primary interest in the distinction between the affective-interpersonal and the antisocial dimension. The relation of response inhibition with instrumental and aggressive behavior could be utilized as a predictor of treatment success in aggressive disorders. The current hypothesis predicts that pharmacological treatments that reduce reactive or instrumental aggression should be accompanied by an improvement or reduction in response inhibition performance, respectively. Studying the relationship of response inhibition with instrumental and reactive aggression could therefore significantly contribute to the well-being of patients and reduce the large psychological and economical damage caused by aggressive behavior. References Arce, E., & Santisteban, C. (2006). Impulsivity: a review. Psycothema, 18(2), 213-220. Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e58-e68. Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 115116. Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 62, 1575-1581. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295-307. Bennett, D. S., Pitale, M., Vora, V., & Rheingold, A. A. (2004). Reactive vs. Proactive antisocial behavior: differential correlates of child ADHD symptoms? Journal of Attention Disorders, 7(4), 197-204. 17 Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Avances in Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71, 727-731. Blair, R. J. R. (2010a). Psychopathy, frustration and reactive aggression: the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 383-399. Blair, R. J. R. (2010b). Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted review. Current Psychiatry Report, 12, 76-82. Bos, P. A., Panksepp, J., Bluthé, R., & van Honk, J. (2012). Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: a review of single administration studies. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 33, 17-35. Chambers, C. D., Bellgrove, M. A., Stokes, M. G., Henderson, T. R., Garavan, H., Robertson, I. H., Morris, A. P. & Mattingley, J. B. (2006). Executive “break failure” following deactivation of human frontal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 444-455. Chao, H. H., Uchio, E., Zhang, S., Hu, S., Bednarski, S. R., Lu X., Rose, M., Concato, J., & Li, C. S. (2012). BMC Cancer, 12, 371. Clark, L., Roiser, J. P., Cools, R., Rubinsztein, D. C., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Stop signal response inhibition is not modulated by tryptophan depletion or the serotonin transporter polymorphism in healthy volunteers: implications for the 5-HT theory of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 182(4). 570-578. Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 62(2), 168-178. Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Evidence of successful modulation of brain activation and subjective experience during reappraisal of negative emotion in unmedicated depression. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 212(2). 99-107. Dolan, M., & Park, I. 2002. The neuropsychology of antisocial personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 417-427. Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Harper, R. A., Marsh, D. M., Moeller, F. G., Mathias, C. W., & Swann, A. C. (2003). Behavioral impulsivity paradigms: a comparison in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Diciplines, 44(8), 1145-1157 Feilhauer, J., Cima, M., Korebrits, A., & Kunert, H. J. (2012). Differential associations between psychopath dimensions, types of aggression and response inhibition. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 77-88. 18 Ferrari, P. F., Palanza, P., Rodgers, R. J., Mainardi, M., Parmigiani, S. (1996). Comparing different forms of male and female aggression in wild and laboratory mice: an ethopharmacological study. Physiology & Behavior, 60(2), 549-53. Garavan, H., Hester, R., Murpy, K., Fassbender, C., & Kelly, C. (2006). Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control. Brain Research, 1105, 130-142. Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2009). Psychopathy and instrumental aggression: evolutionary, neurobiological and legal perspectives. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(4). 253-258. Hart, S. D., & Dempster, R. J. (1997). Impulsivity and Psychopathy. In C. D. Webster, & M. A. Jackson, Impulsivity: theory, assessment and treatment. (pp. 212-231). New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press. Hermans, E. J., Ramsey, N. F., & van Honk, J. (2008). Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 263-270. Hildebrand, M., de Ruiter, C., & Nijman, H. (2008). PCL-R psychopathy predicts disruptive behavior among male offenders in a Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19(1), 13-29. Lesch, K. P. & Merschdorf, U. (2000). Impulsivity, aggression and serotonin: a molecular psychobiological perspective. Behavioral Sciences in the Law, 18, 581-604. Loeber, R., Burke, J. D., Lahey, B. B., Winters, A., & Zera, M. (2000). Oppositional defiant and conduct disorder: a review of the past then years, part I. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(12), 1468-1484. Montoya, E. R., Terburg, D., Bos, P. A., & van Honk, J. (2012). Testosterone, cortisol and serotonin as key regulators of social aggression: a review and theoretical perspective. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 65-73. Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 536-546 Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2008). Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: links to violence, alcohol use and intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 76(5). 893-899. Nikliuna, E. M. (1991). Neural control of predatory aggression in wild and domesticated animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 15, 545-547. Panksepp, J. (1998). Basic emotional and motivational processes. In Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press. 19 Parsey, R. V., Oguendo, M. A., Simpson, N. R., Ogden, R. T., van Heertum, R., Arango, V., & Mann, J. J. (2002). Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-100635. Brain Research, 954(2), 173182. Passamonti, L., Fera, F., Magariello, A., Cerasa, A., Gioia, M. C., Muglia, M., Nicoletti, G., Gallo, O., Provinciali, L., & Quattrone, A. (2006). Monoamine oxidase-A variations influence brain activity associated with inhibitory control: new insights into the neural correlates of impulsivity. Biological Psychiatry, 59(4), 334-340. Pavlov, K. A., Chistiakov, D. A., Chekhonin, Chekhonin, V. P. (2012). Genetic determinants of aggression and impulsivity in humans. Journal of Applied Genetics, 53(1), 61-82. Pawliczek, C. M., Derntl, B., Kellermann, T., Kohn, N., Gur, R. C., & Habel, U. (2013). Inhibitory control and trait aggression: neural and behavioral insights using the emotional stop signal task. Neuroimage, 79, 264-274. Perach-Barzilay, N., Tauber, A., Klein, E., Chistyakov, A., Ne’eman, R., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013). Asymmetry in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and aggressive behavior: a continuous thetaburst magnetic stimulation study. Social Neuroscience, 8(2). 178-188. Retz, W., & Rösler, M. (2010). Association of ADHD with reactive and proactive violent behavior in a forensic population. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 2(4), 195-202. Rubia, K., Lee, F., Cleare, A. J., Tunstall, N., Fu, C. H. Y, Brammer, M., & McGuire, P. (2005). Tryptophan depletion reduces right inferior prefrontal activation during response inhibition in fast event-related fMRI. Psychopharmacology, 4, 791-803. Siegel, A., Victoroff, J. (2009). Understanding human aggression: new insights from neuroscience. International journal of Law and Psychiatry. 209-215. Siever, L. J., Buchsbaum, M. S., New, A. S., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Wei, T., Hazlett, E. A., Sevin, B. S., Nunn, M. & Mitropoulo, V. (1999). D,l-fenfluramine resonse in impulsive personality disorder assessed with [18F]fluordeoxyglucose positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 413-423. Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, U. (2011). Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage, 56(3). 1655-1665. Terburg, D., Morgan, B., & van Honk, J. (2009). The testosterone-cortisol ratio: a hormonal marker for proneness to social aggression. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(4). 216-223. Van Honk, J., Tuiten, A., Hermans, E., Putman, P., Koppeschaar, H., Thijsen, J., Verbaten, R., & van Doornen, L. (2001). A single administration of testosterone induces cardiac accelerative responses to angry faces in healthy young women. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 238-242. 20 Volkow, N. D., Tancredi, L. R., Grant, C., Gillespie, H., Valentine, A., MUllani, N., Wang, G. J., & Hollister, L. (1995). Brain glucose metabolism in violent psychiatric patients: a preliminary study. Psychiatric Research, 61(4), 243-253. Wallcott, C. M., Mangione, C., & Landau, S. (2004). The relation between disinhibition and emotion regulation in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical and Child Psychology, 33(4), 772-782. Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty, A. M., & Ball, G. F. (2014). The ‘challenge hypothesis’: theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems and breeding strategies. The American Naturalist, 136(6), 829-846. World Health Organisation (2004). The Economic Dimensions of Interpersonal Violence. Retrieved: http://whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2004/9241591609.pdf. 21 Layman’s abstract Response inhibition is the capacity to inhibit impulsive responses in reaction-time tasks. It is generally accepted as a measurement of behavioral control and is inversely related to the personality trait impulsivity. Response inhibition mechanisms are also important for emotion regulation. Traditionally response inhibition impairments have been associated with ADHD. The current review proposes that response inhibition is also central to the symptomatology of aggressive disorders. Specifically, this reviews shows that response inhibition is differentially related to two major categories of aggressive behavior. These two categories are reactive and instrumental aggression. Reactive aggression is aggressive behavior is response to provocation or threat. It has very explosive nature. Instrumental aggression is performed to achieve goals. It has a goal-directed and callous nature. This review shows that improved response inhibition is related to increased instrumental aggression but decreased reactive aggression in several mental disorders. This is because instrumental aggression requires strong emotional and behavioral regulation while reactive aggression occurs due to a lack of regulation. In addition, this review compares the neurobiology of instrumental and reactive aggression with the neurobiology of response inhibition. A previous model suggested that 5-HT levels, a chemical used for neural communication, affects the balance between reactive and instrumental aggression under high hormonal testosterone-to-cortisol ratios. This review shows that low 5-HT levels also impair the functioning of brain areas important for response inhibition. A model is proposed in which response inhibition mechanisms mediate the relationship of 5-HT with reactive and instrumental aggression. 22