1 Running Head: RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION For ruminators, the emotional future is bound to the emotional past: Heightened ruminative disposition is characterised by increased emotional extrapolation Edward Watkinsa,b, Ben Graftonb, Stacey Megan Weinsteinb, Colin MacLeodb,c Study of Maladaptive to Adaptive Repetitive Thought (SMART) Lab, School of Psychology, University of Exetera Centre for the Advancement of Research on Emotion, School of Psychology, University of Western Australiab School of Psychology, Babes-Bolyai University c Contact details for corresponding author: Edward Watkins, Sir Henry Wellcome Building for Mood Disorders Research, University of Exeter, UK, EX4 4QG e-mail: e.r.watkins@exeter.ac.uk Keywords: rumination, text comprehension, implicational, dysphoria, extrapolation, abstract 2 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Abstract Processing mode theory (Watkins, 2008) proposes that rumination is characterized by abstract processing that involves increased thinking about the implications of emotional events, which derives the prediction that heightened ruminative disposition will be associated with elevated emotional extrapolation from current events when formulating future expectancies. To test this, we used a novel Emotional Extrapolation Assessment Task (EEAT) that measured individual differences in the degree to which the emotional tone of initial events influence relative expectancy for subsequent events that are emotionally consistent or inconsistent with this initial event. In Experiment 1, ruminative disposition was associated with increased self-reported expectancy for negative subsequent events relative to positive subsequent events. As predicted, in Experiment 2, heightened ruminative disposition was associated with increased emotional extrapolation, assessed using a comprehension latency performance-based measure. Keywords: rumination, text comprehension, implicational, dysphoria, extrapolation, abstract 3 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Rumination, defined as “passively and repetitively focusing on one’s symptoms of distress and the circumstances surrounding these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema, McBride, & Larson, 1997, p.855), has been implicated as a major process in the onset and maintenance of depression (NolenHoeksema, Wisco & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Watkins, 2008). People differ in the degree to which they tend to experience rumination, with this variation in ruminative disposition most commonly assessed using the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). Prospective longitudinal studies have found that elevated dispositional rumination predicts the likelihood, severity, and duration of major depression (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Spasojevic & Alloy, 2001; see reviews in Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008, Watkins, 2008). Moreover, it appear that rumination plays a causal role in the development of depression-relevant symptoms, as the experimental induction of rumination exacerbates negative mood and thought content (NolenHoeksema et al., 2008; Watkins, 2008). Watkins and colleagues recently proposed processing mode theory to explain the cognitive basis of heightened ruminative disposition (Watkins, 2008; Watkins & Moulds, 2005; Watkins, Moberly, & Moulds, 2008). According to this account, the heightened tendency to engage in maladaptive rumination is characterised by an “abstract” mode of processing, which focuses on causes, meanings, implications, significance, and consequences of feelings and events, consistent with the phenomenology of depressive rumination, as opposed to a “concrete” mode of processing focused on direct, detailed, concrete experience and the mechanics of how such events occur. In turn, these distinct modes of processing are proposed to have distinct functional consequences, with the abstract processing mode implicated in causing the negative consequences of rumination, such as increased emotional reactivity and poor problem-solving, relative to the concrete processing mode, when applied to negative situations, in experimental studies (Watkins & Mould, 2005; Watkins et al., 2008). Because the abstract style of processing engenders mental representations that 4 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION summarize the gist, meanings, and implications of events without contextual detail, and, thereby, involves heightened implicational processing (Trope & Liberman, 2003; Vallacher & Wegner, 1987; Watkins, 2004; Watkins & Teasdale, 2001), it leads people to anticipate the likely impact of present events and experiences on future events and experiences, whereas the concrete style of processing leads people to focus only on current events and experiences within their particular context, without considering their implications for the future. Within processing mode theory, the implicational thinking associated with the abstract processing mode is considered to have negative repercussions when applied to difficulties and negative events, and there is evidence to support this. For example, inducing such abstract implicational thinking increases global negative selfevaluations (Rimes & Watkins, 2005), increases recall of overgeneral autobiographical memories that predict poor outcome in depression (Watkins & Teasdale, 2001, 2004), and increases the generalization gradient for responses to conditioned negative stimuli (Van Lier, Vervliet, Vanbrabrant, Lenaert, & Raes, in press). Thus, the processing mode theory (Watkins, 2008) hypothesises that dysregulation of processing mode results in overly abstract implicational processing in those individuals prone to rumination, without necessarily hypothesizing this deficit to be specific to negative information, but, rather, proposing that it is particularly unconstructive when it occurs in the context of negative information. Because it involves heightened implicational thinking, the abstract processing mode will lead people to more strongly infer that present events are likely to have consequences for future events, compared to the concrete processing mode. Thus, processing mode theory predicts that heightened ruminative disposition will be characterised by an increased tendency to extrapolate from the gist of current events, to anticipate the likely gist of future events. An important aspect of gist is the emotional tone of events, for example, whether the event had a positive or negative outcome, and emotional extrapolation occurs when individuals base their expectancies concerning 5 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION the emotional tone of future events upon the emotional tone of prior events. Such emotional extrapolation is a manifestation of implicational thinking, in that the emotional tone of the initial emotional event is taken to carry implications for the likely emotional tone of future events, regardless of whether the emotional tone is positive or negative. Processing mode theory therefore predicts that heightened ruminative disposition, because it involves an abstract mode of processing that favors implicational thinking, will be characterized by increased emotional extrapolation (without predicting that this increase will be specific to negative emotional extrapolation). To date, our capacity to empirically evaluate processing mode theory, by assessing the degree to which people who vary in ruminative disposition engage in a processing mode that favors implicational thinking, has been constrained by reliance on self-report measures of processing style. There is compelling evidence that self-report measures of cognitive processes commonly lack validity (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). However, it would be possible to assess engagement in implicational thinking indirectly, by examining the degree to which people display emotional extrapolation when judging the likelihood of events. More specifically, individual differences in the degree to which people show evidence that they expect the emotional tone of a subsequent event to be consistent with, rather than inconsistent with, that of a prior event (i.e. the degree to which they demonstrate emotional extrapolation), will reveal individual differences in implicational thinking. Thus, a central hypothesis of processing mode theory that heightened rumination is characterized by adoption of an abstract processing mode that favors implicational thinking, can be evaluated by testing the veracity of the resulting prediction that people with higher scores on the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) will be associated with greater evidence of emotional extrapolation on such an assessment task. In the present program of research, we developed a new assessment approach to measure the degree to which participants demonstrate emotional extrapolation and reveal individual differences 6 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION in implicational thinking - the Emotional Extrapolation Assessment Task (EEAT). The basic format of this task involves presenting participants with textual scenarios, and having participants rate their expectancies for emotional events described towards the end of each scenario. While this approach has previously been used to assess emotional expectancies (e.g., Klar, Gabai, & Baron, 1997), the novel feature of the present EEAT approach is that each scenario now also described an earlier emotional event, occurring within a situation, which preceded the final emotional event that participants rated in terms of expectancy. Thus, by enabling assessment of the degree to which expectancy for differentially emotional subsequent events is influenced by the emotional tone of previous events in a prior situation (counterbalanced across positive and negative outcomes), the EEAT permits assessment of emotional extrapolation. We used this task in two experiments to test the prediction, generated by processing mode theory, that elevated ruminative disposition will be characterised by heightened emotional extrapolation. Experiment 1 examined whether heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by increased emotional extrapolation using a variant of the EEAT in which participants explicitly rated their expectancies for the final emotional events in each scenario. In contrast, Experiment 2 examined whether heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by increased emotional extrapolation using an EEAT variant in which participants’ relative expectancies for the final emotional events in each scenario were inferred from their comprehension latencies for these event descriptions. Experiment 1 Method Participants. Participants were 64 (18 male) undergraduate psychology students from the University of Western Australia and the University of Exeter. The pool was sampled in a manner that ensured a wide range of ruminative disposition scores and depression scores, with Ruminative 7 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Response Scale (RRS) scores of participants ranging from 23 to 77 (M = 41.45, SD = 12.65) and Beck Depression Inventory-II scores ranging from 0 to 46 (M = 13.14, SD =10.14). Participants ranged in age from 17 to 55 years (M = 20.30, SD = 6.57). Materials and Tasks. Questionnaire Measures. Ruminative Response Scale. Ruminative disposition was assessed using the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991), which requires participants to rate their tendency to experience each of 22 particular ruminative responses when in a negative mood. The responses are focused on self, symptoms, and possible consequences and causes of their mood (e.g., “Think about how you don’t seem to feel anything anymore”). Previous studies have reported acceptable convergent and predictive validity for the RRS (e.g., Butler & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991). Beck Depression Inventory – II. Depression was assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), which requires participants to respond to 21 statements describing various depressive symptoms, on a scale ranging from 0 – 3. The BDI-II has been shown to have both good reliability (Wiebe & Penley, 2005) and validity (Storch, Roberti, & Roth, 2004). Experimental Stimulus materials. The experimental stimulus set comprised 80 passages, each describing a situation that participants were required to imagine themselves in. Each passage consisted of a title and 7 sentences. The first three sentences described the initial setting of the scenario (e.g. “You have invited some friends over for dinner. You have prepared beef Wellington. Over dessert, the conversation turns to politics”). The 4th sentence described an Initial Emotional Event, and by altering a single word this sentence could be delivered in either the Initial Event Positive condition (e.g. “Your friend Andy agrees with your opinion) or in the Initial Event 8 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Negative condition (e.g. “Your friend Andy disagrees with your opinion”). The 5th sentence transitioned to a later situation by introducing an explicit temporal shift and describing this subsequent setting (e.g., “Two weeks later you are sitting at the table drinking a cup of coffee”). The 6th sentence then described a specific situation within this subsequent circumstance (e.g. “You remember to ask your friend Andy if you can borrow his drill to complete some DIY”), which led to a Subsequent Emotional Event described in the 7th sentence. By altering a single word, this sentence could be delivered in either the Subsequent Event Positive condition (e.g. “When you ask to borrow the drill, Andy agrees”), or in the Subsequent Event Negative condition (e.g. “When you ask to borrow the drill, Andy disagrees”). Our interest was in participants’ relative expectancies for the Subsequent Emotional Event in each of these two emotional conditions, as a function of both Initial Event Valence and ruminative disposition. Experimental Hardware. A Logitech Express PC with an LG Flatron E2210 computer screen was used to deliver the task. The space-bar key on a Logitech keyboard was used to deliver the sentences at a self-paced rate. Emotional Extrapolation Assessment Task (EEAT). The Emotional Extrapolation Assessment Task presented each of these 80 passages in a random order. For every passage, participants received each sentence individually, in white text, upon pressing the space bar. They were required to silently read each sentence for meaning as it appeared on the screen, and to push the space-bar when they had understood it, to receive the next statement. After the first six sentences had been read, when the space bar was pressed the 7th sentence describing the Subsequent Emotional Event appeared in yellow text, together with an on-screen expectancy rating Likert scale. Participants were required to rate the degree to which they would expect this event to occur, in the situation described by the scenario, using this -30 (highly unlikely) to +30 (highly likely) scale. Higher scores indicated increased self-reported expectancies for this described emotional event. 9 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Once this rating had been made, then the screen was cleared, and the next passage title appeared 1000 ms later, commencing the next trial. For each participant, 40 of the 80 passages appeared in the Initial Event Positive condition, and 40 appeared in the Initial Event Negative condition. In each case, half of these 40 trials were presented in the Subsequent Event Positive condition and half were presented in the Subsequent Event Negative condition. Therefore 20 of the 80 experimental passages were delivered in each of the four possible conditions resulting from the nested combination of these two experimental factors. Across every four participants, each passage appeared once in each of these unique conditions. The dependent variable was the self-reported expectancy for the Subsequent Emotional Event, as described in sentence 7. To the extent that participants exhibit emotional extrapolation, the relative expectancy for negative subsequent events compared to positive subsequent events will be inflated in the Initial Event Negative condition compared to the Initial Event Positive condition. This interaction effect would be exaggerated in participants with higher RRS scores, if heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by increased emotional extrapolation, as hypothesized. Procedure. Participants were tested individually. Each participant was seated approximately 60 cm from the computer screen, and given instructions for the EEAT. These instructions emphasised that the participant should imagine themselves in the scenarios described in each passage, and to read each sentence at their own pace, before making the expectancy judgement concerning the event described in the final sentence of each passage. Participants completed the EEAT, followed by the BDI-II and RRS, before being thanked and debriefed. Results 10 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Mean expectancy ratings for the emotional events described in the final sentences of passages present in each experimental condition are shown in Table 1. These expectancy rating scores were subjected to a repeated measures ANCOVA that considered two within-subject factors, Initial Event Valence (Positive Initial Event vs. Negative Initial Event), and Subsequent Event Valence (Positive Subsequent Event vs. Negative Subsequent Event), with RRS rumination scores entered as a continuous variable. This analysis revealed a significant interaction of Subsequent Event Valence by RRS scores, F(1, 62) = 10.95, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.15, reflecting an increase in the relative expectancy for negative subsequent events, compared to positive subsequent events, as RRS scores increased, r (62) = .39, p = .002). There was also a significant two way interaction of Initial Event Valence and Subsequent Event Valence, F(1, 62) = 4.10, p = .047, η2 = 0.06, the nature of which confirmed that participants engaged in emotional extrapolation when performing this task. Specifically, participants gave higher expectancy ratings for negative subsequent events when the initial event valence was negative rather than positive (M = 3.96, SD =6.10 vs M= 1.76, SD =7.23), and gave higher expectancy ratings for positive subsequent events when the initial event valence was positive rather than negative (M = 9.45, SD = 5.91 vs M =-1.47, SD = 6.17). Of crucial importance to the hypothesis under test, however, there was no evidence that RRS score moderated such emotional extrapolation, as the three way interaction involving Initial Event Valence, Subsequent Event Valence, and RRS score did not approach significance F(1, 62) = 0.30, p = .58, η2 = .005. Heightened ruminative disposition is often accompanied by heightened depression and indeed, in our present sample, RRS scores were positively correlated with BDI-II scores, r (62) = .67, p < .001. Hence the observed expectancy bias, reflecting a relative increased expectancy for negative compared to positive subsequent events in participants with higher RRS scores, may have been a specific function of depression, rather than ruminative disposition. To determine if this was 11 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION the case, we entered RRS scores and BDI-II scores simultaneously into a multiple regression in which the DV was negative expectancy bias index score (computed by subtracting the expectancy rating for positive subsequent events from the expectancy ratings for negative subsequent events). This analysis showed that the regression model was significant, F (2, 61) = 6.37, p = .003, adjusted R2 = 0.15, although neither the RRS rumination score, t (62) = 1.61, p =.11, β = .252, nor the BDI-II depression scores predicted independent variance in the expectancy bias scores, t (62) = 1.30, p =.20, β = .203. Discussion These results indicate that heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by increased negative expectancy bias, reflecting an inflated self-reported expectancy for negative events relative to positive events. The combination of ruminative disposition and depressive symptoms together explained significant variance in the negative expectancy bias, without either being an independent predictor, suggesting that the shared variance across both is related to negative expectancy. There was also evidence that participants engaged in emotional extrapolation, reporting a heightened expectancy for subsequent events consistent with the emotional valence of initial events, relative to subsequent events inconsistent with the emotional valence of subsequent events. However, there was no support for the emotional extrapolation hypotheses derived from processing mode theory (Watkins, 2008), as there not a significant relationship between ruminative disposition and the degree to which participants exhibited such emotional extrapolation of inferences from the outcome of the initial event to the outcome of the subsequent event. While the findings from this study do not support the extrapolation hypothesis, we should perhaps be cautious about rejecting this hypothesis on the basis of findings that concern only selfreport measures of expectancy. Individuals often have a limited ability to accurately report on their cognitive processes, especially if these processes are subtle or implicit, and commonly show an 12 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION inability to make fine-grain distinctions concerning cognitive processes (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Moreover, self-report studies are often highly susceptible to response bias (e.g., MacLeod & Mathews, 1991; Mathews & MacLeod, 1994; Mogg, Bradbury, & Bradley, 2006), such as an emotionally-linked bias that increases the tendency to emit or endorse negative responses, especially when in a negative mood. Such a response bias effect is likely to be highly pertinent for the observation of the negative expectancy bias. For example, individuals with an elevated ruminative disposition may exhibit a negative response bias, whereby they explicitly report a preference for negative expectancies even under conditions where true expectancy for such negative expectancies may be attenuated. Self-report studies also are susceptible to demand effects whereby participants may be vulnerable to responding in a way that they think is expected of them (e.g., MacLeod & Cohen, 1993; MacLeod & Mathews, 1991a), and again this could lead participants with higher ruminative disposition scores to over-report negative expectancies. Such effects plausibly could result in the underestimation of emotional extrapolation in participants with heightened ruminative disposition, when self-report measures of expectancy are employed. To overcome these methodological limitations, researchers have developed non self-report performance-based approaches to examine putative cognitive biases, for example, the use of the attentional probe task to investigate attentional biases (Mathews & MacLeod, 1994). Replication of the present study, using a performance-based measure capable of indexing relative expectancy for the positive and negative subsequent events in these experimental scenarios without reliance on self-report, may provide a more compelling test of the extrapolation hypothesis. This was the aim of Experiment 2. Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, we adapted the EEAT into a performance-based measure of expectancy, in order to test the hypothesis that individuals with a heightened ruminative disposition display 13 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION elevated extrapolation for emotional events. Specifically, we used relative comprehension latencies for the sentences communicating the alternative types of subsequent emotional events, to index relative expectancies for the differing categories of events. This text comprehension approach has a long tradition within cognitive-experimental psychology, where it has been clearly shown that comprehension latencies are longer for textual information that is unexpected (see Anderson, Garrod, & Sanford, 1983; Garrod & Sanford, 1981 for evidence). Using this approach for the passages employed in Experiment 1, negative expectancy bias can be inferred from the degree to which comprehension latencies are relatively speeded for the sentences communicating the subsequent events when these events are negative rather than positive in emotional tone. Of most relevance to the hypothesis under scrutiny, rumination-linked differences in emotional extrapolation will be revealed by the existence of an association between RRS scores and the degree to which the comprehension latencies for negative subsequent events compared to positive subsequent events are speeded in the Initial Event Negative condition compared to the Initial Event Positive condition. To our knowledge, this text comprehension EEAT task provides the first performance-based assessment of emotional extrapolation in participants who vary in ruminative disposition. Method Design. To test the experimental hypotheses under investigation, individuals ranging in ruminative tendency (as inferred by current RRS) were given the Text Comprehension EEAT. The experimental design employed two within-subject factors. The dependent variable was comprehension latency for sentence 7 in our test passages, which communicated the Subsequent Emotional Event. Experimental factors were Initial Event Valence condition (Positive Initial Event vs. Negative Initial Event), and Subsequent Event Valence condition (Positive Subsequent Event vs. Negative Subsequent Event). Ruminative disposition was examined as a continuous covariate. 14 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Participants. Participants were 67 (31 male) undergraduate psychology students from the University of Western Australia. The pool was sampled in a manner that ensured a wide range of ruminative tendency scores and depression scores, with Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) scores of participants at testing ranging from 25 to 78 (M = 44.22, SD = 14.30) and BDI-II scores of participants at testing ranging from 0 to 46 (M = 11.06, SD = 10.79). Participants ranged in age from 17 to 40 years (M = 19.09, SD = 3.56). Materials and Tasks. The materials replicated Experiment 1, with the following amendments for the experimental stimulus materials and EEAT: (i) An 8th neutral sentence added to the end of each passage, in order that we could assess comprehension latency for sentence 7, by measuring how long participants took to process this sentence before they pressed the space bar to receive the subsequent sentence. (ii) A comprehension question was constructed for each passage, which did not pertain to the subsequent emotional event described in sentence 7 but did pertain to one of the other sentences, and to which the answer was “Yes” or “No” (e.g. in the example passage given in the previous Method section this was “Did you talk about sport at dinner?”). Participants were required to answer this question after reading each passage, to ensure that they read for meaning, as instructed. (iii) A set of 80 filler passages was constructed, each comprising 8 sentences and a question, but without the consistent internal structure that characterized the experimental stimulus set. We added these filler passages to obscure the structural consistency within the experimental passages, by presenting them mixed together with the filler passages in the test session. (iv) For the text comprehension variant of EEAT, the Subsequent Emotional Event sentence did not now appear in yellow text, accompanied by an expectancy rating scale. Rather it appeared in white text, just like the preceding sentences, and participants were given no special instruction 15 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION concerning this particular sentence. Rather, they read it, and pressed the space bar to receive the next sentence when they had understood it. The dependent variable was comprehension latency for this Subsequent Emotional Event sentence, operationalized as the interval between the space-bar press that exposed this sentence and the subsequent space-bar press which exposed the next sentence. Procedure. Participants were tested individually. Each participant was seated approximately 60 cm from the computer screen, and given instructions for the text comprehension task. These instructions emphasised that the participant should imagine themselves in the scenarios described in each passage, and to ensure accurate responses to the final comprehension questions, whilst not taking any more time than necessary to read each sentence and to answer the question. A short practice that employed only neutral stimuli was given. Then the participant completed the Text Comprehension EEAT, before then completing the BDI-II and RRS, before being thanked and debriefed. Results Mean comprehension latencies for the Subsequent Emotional Event sentences, under each passage condition, are shown in Table 2. To test the extrapolation hypothesis, these comprehension latencies were subjected to a repeated measures ANCOVA that considered two within-subject factors, Initial Event Valence (Positive Initial Event vs. Negative Initial Event), and Subsequent Event Valence (Positive Subsequent Event vs. Negative Subsequent Event). The RRS rumination scores were entered as a continuous variable. This analysis revealed a significant two way interaction of Initial Event Valence x Subsequent Event Valence, F (1, 65) = 5.09, p = .03, η2 = .07, that was further modified by RRS score within a higher order interaction involving all three factors, F(1, 65) = 5.63, p = .02, η2 = 0.08. No other effects were significant (all p’s > .26, all η2 <.02). 16 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION The pattern of this three way interaction was examined, to determine whether its nature was consistent with that predicted by the extrapolation hypothesis. Specifically, we first computed an emotional extrapolation index, expressing the degree to which participants were speeded on this subsequent emotional event sentence, when its valence was consistent with, compared to inconsistent with, the valence of the initial emotional event sentence. A high score on this index, reflecting a heightened tendency to display speeding to comprehend later events consistent in emotional tone with earlier events, indicates greater emotional extrapolation. We then examined the association between this emotional extrapolation index and RRS score, revealing a significant positive correlation, r (65) = .28, p = .02. Hence the three way interaction reflects the fact that higher RRS scores were characterized by a heightened tendency to engage in emotional extrapolation. It is interesting to note that the strength of this observed effect, reflecting increased emotional extrapolation as a function of ruminative disposition, did not differ as a function of the emotional tone of the initial event. Higher RRS scores were positively associated with the degree to which participants displayed speeding to comprehend later events consistent with rather than inconsistent with the emotional tone of initial events, regardless of whether initial events were emotionally negative, r (65) =.21, p = .09, or were emotional positive, r (65) = .26, p = .03, and there was no significance difference in the magnitude of these two correlations, z = .30, p = .76. Because RRS scores were highly correlated with BDI-II scores, r(65) = .86, p < .001, it is not surprising that their relative effects on emotional extrapolation could not be separated: when RRS scores and BDI-II scores were entered simultaneously into a multiple regression in which emotional extrapolation index was the dependent variable, the overall regression model was significant, F (2, 64) = 3.33, p =.04, adjusted R2 = 0.07, but neither ruminative disposition, t (64) = .317, p =.75, β = .075 nor depression score, t (64) = 1.01, p = .31, β = .24, individually predicted the emotional extrapolation index. 17 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Discussion The relationship found between ruminative disposition and the performance-based index of expectancy provided by the text comprehension EEAT is consistent with the predictions generated by the emotional extrapolation hypothesis under test. These findings represent objective evidence that heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by an elevated tendency to generate expectations on the basis of emotional extrapolation. Specifically, people who have an elevated tendency to exhibit rumination are disproportionately inclined to display inflated relative expectancy for events that are consistent with the emotional tone of recent past events. To our knowledge, this is the first study to have demonstrated this rumination-linked anomaly in expectancy generation, and revealing this with a performance-based measure of expectancy, which negates reliance on self-report, increases confidence in the veracity of the effect. Interestingly, Experiment 2 did not replicate the finding from Experiment 1 suggesting that ruminative disposition may be associated with an overall negative expectancy bias. Because the key difference between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 is the use of a self-report measure of expectancy in the former study, and a performance-based measure in the latter study, this discrepancy invites speculation that this effect may have resulted from a response bias in the previous experiment. As we noted earlier, such a response bias could potentially compromise detection of rumination-linked differences in emotional extrapolation, when self-report measures of expectancy are employed. Consistent with this possibility, when the limitations of self-report were eliminated by the use of the comprehension measure in the present study, then not only does the relationship between ruminative disposition and negative expectancy bias disappear, but a relationship between ruminative disposition and emotional extrapolation appears. This pattern of findings not only lends weight to the possibility elevated ruminative disposition is associated with a negative response bias when self-report measures of expectancy are employed, but also 18 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION demonstrates that performance-based measures can reveal processes underpinning the generation of expectancies that are not accurately indexed by self-report (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). General Discussion The purpose of the present experiments was to determine whether heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by an elevated tendency to make emotional extrapolations, evidenced by the degree to which the emotional tone of an initial event influences relative expectancies for negative and positive subsequent events. When we had participants self-report their expectancies for subsequent events, in Experiment 1, there was no evidence to support this prediction. Rather participants with higher ruminative disposition self-reported only a generally heightened expectancy for negative relative to positive events. However, when expectancies for subsequent events instead were indirectly assessed, using the performance measure provided by comprehension latencies, then the pattern of findings was quite different. Now there was no evidence that heightened ruminative disposition was associated with a general negative expectancy bias. Rather it was associated with the predicted increase in emotional extrapolation. This pattern of findings, obtained using our performance-based measure of emotional extrapolation, is consistent with the prediction generated by processing mode theory (Watkins, 2008). According to this theory, heightened ruminative disposition is characterized by increased engagement in an abstract mode of processing that favors implicational thinking. Such implicational thinking is evidenced in the present studies by increased emotional extrapolation. Therefore the observation in Experiment 2 that heightened ruminative disposition was characterized by increased emotional extrapolation supports the validity of processing mode theory. 19 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION What conclusions should be drawn from the observed discrepancy between the findings obtained in Experiment 1 (using a self-report expectancy measure) and in Experiment 2 (using a performance-based expectancy measure)? It is our own view that when self-report measures and performance-based measures of a cognitive process disagree, then it is prudent to have greater faith in the latter. Hence, we believe that this present demonstration that self-report and performancebased measures can invite discrepant conclusions underscores the importance of investigating cognitive processes through the use of objective behavioural indices, to circumvent the wellestablished limitations of reliance on self-report measures, which include but are not restricted to the possibility of contamination by response biases and demand effects (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). We note, however, that an alternative explanation is a simple failure to replicate prior findings. These findings obtained in Experiment 2 have a number of theoretical and clinical implications. As already noted, they provide direct support for processing mode theory, which proposes that the ruminative disposition depends on the degree to which individuals adopt the abstract rather than the concrete processing mode when processing self-relevant emotional events (Watkins, 2008). We note that the increased emotional extrapolation associated with heighted ruminative disposition will not always lead to inflated expectancies for negative events. Rather, when currently experiencing positive events, this same disposition would lead to the increased expectancy that future events also are likely to be positive. This tendency towards abstract implicational processing when applied to positive events, such as receiving a compliment, can lead to positive consequences such elevated positive affect (Marigold, Holmes, & Ross, 2007). Thus, elevated ruminative disposition may not always be maladaptive. Instead, this disposition may perhaps be more accurately conceptualized as a mechanism that inflates the emotional impact of current events, by increasing the probability that people will anticipate that future events will likely be of a similar emotional valence. This disposition may thus confer vulnerability to depression 20 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION because when highly negative life events are experienced, the resulting disproportionate elevation in expectancy that future events will also be highly negative produces the sense of hopelessness and pessimism commonly experienced in depression. Because heightened ruminative disposition is also accompanied by patterns of cognitive bias, which increase the probability that current events will be construed in emotionally negative terms, this may further amplify the risk that the accompanying increase in emotional extrapolation will elicit such pessimism. Such cognitive biases include increased selective attention to negative elements of the current environment, and an increased tendency to impose negative interpretations on ambiguous aspects of this environment, both of which have been shown to be associated with elevated ruminative disposition (Donaldson, Lam, & Mathews, 2007; Hertel, Mor, Ferrari, Hunt, & Agrawal, 2014; Joormann, Dkane, & Gotlib, 2006; Mor, Hertel, Ngo, Shachar, & Redak, 2014). An increased tendency to engage in emotional extrapolation, coupled with processing biases that increase the perceived negativity of current events and situations, could be a potent combination of risk factors in determining vulnerability to depression. At a clinical level, these findings have several potential applied implications. First, they suggest the possibility that the text comprehension EEAT may provide a clinically useful assessment of vulnerability to depression, which might improve on the predictive capacity of questionnaire measures of ruminative disposition. Examining the relative extent to which RRS score and the index of emotional extrapolation provided by the EEAT predict the development of depression in response to negative life events will test such clinical utility. Second, and relatedly, the emotional extrapolation measure provide by the EEAT may be a useful marker of clinical change, revealing the degree to which clinical interventions for depression have attenuated this vulnerability factor, without the contamination influence of demand effects that compromise selfreport measures of therapeutic change. 21 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Third, the present findings lend weight to the idea that implicational thinking in general, and perhaps emotional extrapolation in particular, may be an appropriate target for direct therapeutic intervention. Watkins et al. (2008) recently developed an instruction-based training procedure intended to attenuate engagement in abstract implicational thinking. However, the capacity of this training to genuinely reduce reliance on this processing mode was evaluated using only self-report measures of abstract vs concrete thinking, which are vulnerable to demand effects and reporting bias. Research can employ the newly developed EEAT procedure to verify this instruction-based training does genuinely reduce abstract implicational thinking, as objectively indexed by RT measure of emotional extrapolation, thereby bypassing the limitations of self-report. Extensions of such future work then can investigate whether the therapeutic benefits of this instructional training procedure are mediated by the intended change in abstract implicational thinking. For example, the processing mode theory predicts that the concreteness training form of cognitive bias modification, derived from these training manipulations (Watkins et al., 2009, 2012), will reduce emotional extrapolation on the EEAT, and that this reduction in implicational thinking will mediate the observed benefits of concreteness training on depression and ruminative disposition. There are of course some limitations associated with the current studies. First, we did not recruit participants characterised by an excessively high ruminative disposition or patients with depression, and so it remains to be seen whether such participants exhibit the same patterns of emotional extrapolation as those in the present research, and whether these findings generalize to clinical populations. It was not possible to discriminate independent effects of ruminative disposition and depressive symptoms on negative expectancy bias and abstract extrapolation, respectively. Because our novel text comprehension latency measure of inferred expectancies found an emotional extrapolation effect, it would be valuable to see if this pattern can be found in patients with depression. 22 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION A second limitation is that we only assessed extrapolation from one situation to another situation in terms of the emotionality of events. Thus, whilst our findings are consistent with ruminative disposition being characterized by increased emotional extrapolation, it remains to be seen whether this reflects only emotional extrapolation or a more general heightened tendency to extrapolate from present events when forming future expectancies that implicates non-emotional aspects of events also. For example, heightened rumination may also be associated with an increased tendency to extrapolate that if present events are of any given nature (e.g., involve particular settings, particular people, and/or particular activities) then future events are disproportionately likely to have similar qualities. Once again, it will take further research to determine whether the increased extrapolation associated with heightened ruminative disposition is specific to emotional dimensions of events and experience. A critical next step for this field is to establish the causal direction of the relationship between rumination and abstract-implicational thinking. Whilst the current studies indicate there is such a relationship, a key unresolved question is whether the tendency towards implicational thinking that underpins the observed effects on emotional extrapolation is a cause or a consequence of ruminative disposition. An important set of studies would therefore adapt the cognitive bias modification approach such as used to study attentional and interpretative processes, to the examination of extrapolation across events. To measure bias in the EEAT, negative and positive outcomes appear with equal 50% likelihood for the subsequent event, no matter the outcome of the primary event. Modifying the tendency to emotionally extrapolate involves introducing contingencies such that, for example, the valence of the subsequent event is congruent with the valence of the initial event on the majority of trials (e.g., 90% likelihood) to train towards emotional extrapolation, or the valence of the subsequent event is incongruent with the valence of the initial event on the majority of trials (e.g. 90% likelihood) to train away from emotional extrapolation. The 23 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION desired response is rewarded by easier and more fluent performance on the reading task and via overt feedback on comprehension questions (e.g. “Correct”). For example, studies manipulating emotional extrapolation could then test whether this process causally influences ruminative disposition, such as assessed in response to a failure experience. Conversely, the causal effect of rumination on implicational thinking should be tested, for example, by comparing the effects of the standard rumination versus distraction manipulations (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995) on tasks such as the EEAT. A related critical question for the study of rumination, in part highlighted by this study, is the need to better delineate what patterns of selective information-processing maintain rumination, with particular reference to clarifying the roles and potential interactions of valence-specific cognitive biases (such as attentional bias towards negative information; Donaldson et al., 2006; Joormann et al., 2006 or bias towards negative interpretations of ambiguous information, Hertel et al., 2014; Mor et al., 2014) versus non-valence-specific information processing (such as abstract processing, Watkins, 2008). First, resolving the role and relationship between valenced and nonvalenced processing in rumination would test and refine existing theoretical models. For example, in the current studies, both a negative expectancy bias and emotional extrapolation were associated with ruminative disposition. The processing mode theory (Watkins, 2008) hypothesises that a deficit in regulating processing mode results in overly abstract implicational processing in those individuals prone to rumination, without hypothesizing this deficit to be specific to only negative information, but, rather, proposing that it is particularly unconstructive in the context of negative information. Likewise, the impaired disengagement theory (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De Raedt, 2011) hypothesises that rumination is engendered by difficulties in disengaging processing from negative self-related information, but to date it is unresolved to what extent difficulties in disengagement might reflect a general deficit in cognitive control (e.g., inhibition of previously 24 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION relevant information in working memory) and/or a selective processing bias towards negative information. Second, to date, these distinct biases (e.g., processing mode, cognitive control, attentional bias, interpretative bias) have been examined separately, so it has not been possible to determine if they multiply contribute to rumination, interact, or are related processes. An important line of enquiry is therefore the concurrent study of multiple cognitive biases with respect to ruminative disposition, both cross-sectionally and through experimental manipulation. 25 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION References Anderson, A., Garrod, S. C., & Sanford, A. J. (1983) 'The accessibility of pronominal antecedents as a function of episode shifts in narrative text', The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 35: 3, 427-440. doi: 10.1080/14640748308402480 Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory Manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Butler, L. D., & Nolen- Hoeksema, S. (1994). Gender differences in response to a depressed mood in a college sample. Sex Roles, 30, 331-346. Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 45, 2664-2678. Garrod, S.C., & Sanford, A. J. (1981). Bridging inferences and the extended domain of reference. In Long, J. and Baddeley, A. (Eds), Attention and Performance IX, pp. 311- 346, Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hertel, P., Mor, N., Ferrari, C., Hunt, O., & Agrawal, N. (2014). Looking on the dark side: Rumination and cognitive bias modification. Clinical Psychological Science, published online 16 April 2014. doi: 10.1177/2167702614529111 Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269-281. Klar, Y., Gabai, T., & Baron, Y. (1997). Depression and generalization about the future: Who overgeneralizes what? Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 575–584. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00186-9 Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement 26 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 138-145. Lyubomirsky, S. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of Self-Focused Rumination on Negative Thinking and Interpersonal Problem-Solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176-190. MacLeod, C., & Cohen, I. L. (1993). Anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguity: A text comprehension study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 238-247. doi: 0021-843X/93 MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1991). Mood disorders and cognition. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of behaviour therapy and psychological science: An integrative approach. Oxford.: Pergamon. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1991a). Cognitive-experimental approaches to the emotional disorders. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of Behaviour Therapy and Psychological Science: An Integrative Approach (pp. 116-150). New York: Wiley. Marigold, D. C., Holmes, J. G., & Ross, M. (2007). More than words: Reframing compliments from romantic partners fosters security in low self-esteem individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 232-248. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology., 45, 25-50. Mogg, K., Bradbury, K. E., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Interpretation of ambiguous information in clinical depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1411-1419. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.008 Mor, N., Hertel, P., Ngo, T. A., Shachar, T., & Redak, S. (2014). Interpretation bias characterizes trait rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 4, 67–73. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259. 27 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569-582. Nolen- Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504-511. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997). Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 855-862. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115-121. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400-424. Rimes, K. A. & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative selfjudgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673-1681. Spasojević, J. & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion, 1, 25-37. Storch, E., Roberti, J., & Roth, D. (2004). Factor structure, concurrent validity, and internal consistency of the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition in a sample of college students. Depression and Anxiety, 19(3), 187-189. Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403-421. Vallacher, R. R. & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15. Van Lier, J., Vervliet, B., Vanbrabant, K., Lenaert, B., & Raes, F. (in press). Abstract thinking about negative events in dysphoric students leads to negative generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 28 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Watkins, E. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1037-1052. Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163-206. Watkins, E. R., Baeyens, C. B., & Read, R. (2009). Concreteness training reduces dysphoria: Proofof-principle for repeated cognitive bias modification in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 55-64. Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8, 364-378. Watkins, E.R. & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319-328. Watkins, E.R., Mullan, E.G., Wingrove, J., Rimes, K., Steiner, H., Bathurst, N., Eastman, E., & Scott, J. (2011). Rumination-focused cognitive behaviour therapy for residual depression: phase II randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 317- 322. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.090282. Watkins, E.R., Taylor, R.S., Byng, R., Baeyens, C.B., Read, R., Pearson, K., & Watson, L. (2012). Guided self-help concreteness training as an intervention for major depression in primary care: a Phase II randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1359-1373. doi:10.1017/S0033291711002480. Watkins, E. & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 353-357. Watkins, E. & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 1-8. 29 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Wiebe, J., & Penley, J. (2005). A psychometric comparison of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in English and Spanish. Psychological Assessment, 17(4), 481-485. 30 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Table 1 Mean Expectancy Ratings (-30 to +30) across Conditions (SD in Parentheses) for Experiment 1 Initial Event Valence Subsequent Event Valence Negative Positive Negative 3.96 (6.10) 1.76 (7.23) Positive -1.47 (6.17) 9.45 (5.91) 31 RUMINATION AND EMOTIONAL EXTRAPOLATION Table 2 Mean Comprehension Latency times for Subsequent Event Valence sentences in Milliseconds (SD in parentheses) across Conditions in Experiment 2. Initial Event Valence Subsequent Event Valence Negative Positive Negative 2054.99 (680.33) 1988.80 (639.63) Positive 2012.33 (662.66) 1945.15 (691.56)