Chasing the Chimera’s Tail – An analysis of contemporary and historical research in Interest in Science Abstract In recent years the concept Interest in Science and Science education overall has been given a lot of attention, both politically and in the field of research. This article aims to explore the notion, concept and statement Interest in Science in its contemporary form and four historical transformations. Interest in Science in the nineteenth century will be analysed through the discourse of Johann Friedrich Herbart (1776-1841) and the non-discursive practices surrounding Science education at the time. The transformation to a modern and contemporary form will be analysed through the discourses of Dewey, Berlyne/Strong, Gardner and Krapp with examples of how non-discursive practices shapes the formation of the various discourses imbedded in the statement of Interest in Science – most notably psychology and pedagogy. This exploration will end with an analysis of the answers that this discourse brought us in the form of OECD PISA06 survey and assessment of Interest in Science. The aim of this analysis and exploration is to show how the current contemporary discursive form of Interest in Science, the statements intersecting this discourse and its non-discursive practices frames the way we think and how it allows us to the answer the question:” Why do students lack Interest in Science?” A glimpse in the fog –Research in Interest in Science This article will explore the concept Interest in Science and how contemporary and historical research has been researching the notion. Interest in Science is a research concept, which since the PISA survey in 2000 (OECD-UNESCO-UIS, 2003) and the ‘interest’ survey in 2006(OECD 2007)has been given a lot of attention both in Denmark and nationally. The research in Interest in Science has spawned numerous projects and initiatives to improve and facilitate pupil and student Interest in Science. (ROSE)(ATTRACT)(SAUCE) My claim in this article is that through tracing the concept of Interest in Science in its genealogy and archaeology and various transformations we can in the same time shed light on the dispotif of the natural sciences (abb. Science) and the role of psychology in education and it’s discursive formation. I will argument the concept of Interest of Science as being intersected with psychology, mathematics and pedagogy through various practices exemplified in contemporary and historical projects. Through chasing the chimerical construct of Interest of Science and Psychology I will show how the Interest in Science and especially psychology and psychometrics is intrinsically linked and framed in its contemporary manifestation. I am drawing heavily on the method of genealogy and archaeology by Foucault (Foucault 1972), which has been expanded and exemplified by various others (Mouffe, Agambe, Deleuze). My analysis is a tour de force from the construction of mathematical-psychology as exemplified by Herbart, Dewey’s Interest concept, Berlyne’s behavioristic notion on Curiousity, Arousal and Interest, Gardner’s transformation of the concept of Interest in Science to a quasi-modelled construct of behaviourism, psychology and cognitive psychology (psychometrics) and finally an example of the contemporary practice of the concept in the PISA 2006 assessment survey. This will be limited to a discourse analytic approach examining the concept Interest in Science and how it intersects with other serious statements (here focusing on psychology in particular), and with only few examples of the practices and non-discursive manifestations of Interest of Science. I will be using an analogy to The Chimera as the specific hybrid of Interest in Science. The mythical Chimera is composed of a lion’s head, a goats body and a serpents tail, but those different animal traits changes place in various depictions of the chimera, sometimes the serpent is the head, the lion the body and the tail the goat and so forth. Like the chimera the concept of Interest in Science is transversing several discourses and has a different form in various historical periods. The process that leads to these transformations and reconfigurations of The Chimera I will label Chimestry, as a nomenclature of the practices leading up to a transformation, though not enacting a causality. Chimestry produces a full transformation in the sense that the earlier statement undergoes a shift or reconfiguration, so that one may see the statement and it’s concept as Interest in Science, in practice and form it is a whole new mythological beast only connected in the linguistic form. PISA06 Interest Survey – A re-enactment of The Chimera In the nineteenth-century a chimera was born and in the PISA 06 Survey a transformation of that chimera is enacted and manifested in an international assessment of Interest in Science (OECD, 2007). The survey was a culmination of research in Interest in Science and put forth by political and economical needs in the beginning of the twenty-first century; it was the first PISA Survey, which included an Interest in Science part. The findings of the survey were though not remarkable, not much had changed since the PISA03 Survey and the findings regarding Interest in Science seemed quite statistical typical, that Interest in Science had some correlation with gender and with social and economical background (Egelund, xxxx). To find the reasons for this lack of surprisingly new findings one must look at the questions and the frame in which those questions were put forth through the technology of the assessment - an examination of the discourse is needed. “Students’ support for scientific enquiry and students’ interest in learning science topics were directly assessed in the test, using embedded questions that targeted personal, social and global contexts. In the case of students’ interest in learning science topics, students were able to report one of the following responses: “high interest”, “medium interest”, “low interest” or “no interest”. Students reporting high interest or medium interest were considered to report an interest in learning science topics. For attitudinal questions measuring students’ support for scientific enquiry, students were asked to express their level of agreement using one of the following responses: “strongly agree”, “agree”, “disagree” or “strongly disagree”. Students reporting that they strongly agreed or agreed were considered to support scientific enquiry” (OECD,2007; 123) We see here a clear indication that attitudes regarding science, The Interest in Science, are measureable, things in themselves, which can be an object for study and put on a scale. This trait of mathematical measurement of The Chimera I will label The Measure. Another important frame of the PISA06 Survey is the assessments goal in bonding The Measure with the self - the student’s self-concept and self-efficacy regarding Science (OECD, 2007; 135-138). This bonding enacts a causality, which results with questions in the survey regarding responsibility and awareness of environmental issues - larger issues of moral regarding science and it’s applications (OECD, 2007, 155-162). This trait of The Chimera, I label The Morality of course closely related to pedagogy, which is its tool of disciplination. The last trait this article aims to explore is found is both the above traits, and is such the visible head of the Chimera. Interest in Science is connected to enjoyment, motivation and learning and various other concepts, which is given an intrinsic psychological meaning (OECD, 2007; 139-150) This psychological trait of The Chimera, I label The Mind. The concept of Interest in Science in PISA06 Survey, The Chimera, is thus transversed by three different discourses and traits. The head of the contemporary chimera is The Mind, the proud regal Lion’s head of neuropsychological causality, it is dominating and controlling the body, which is The Measure – the workings of psychometrics supporting the logos of the head. Finally the tail the proverbial hidden trait is The Morality, the steering intrinsic morality of the good of science for the betterment of the world, ecological awareness. All components are perfectly linked and entwined, not like earlier transformations of The Chimera, but this frame is bereaved of notions on power and capital and their connection to Interest in Science and the overall Science dispotif. After an archaeological and genealogical exploration of the concept and statement Interest in Science I will return to the ramifications of what the composition of The Chimera means regarding the way we are allowed to perceive the concept of Interest in Science. Herbart’s mathematical psychology- The Measurement in The Soul One could argue that the concept and especially the statement Interest in Science dates back as long as the human culture has known science, thereby stretching an historical analysis back to the early Greeks and their notions on physis (Helge Kragh Kosmos og Kaos, xxx); since the scope of this article lies in the contemporary form of Interest in Science I will start my argument and construction of the chimera by showing how Newtonian causality first intersected with the concept. Prenzel and Krapp takes Johann Friedrich Herbart theory of education and mathematicalpsychology as the starting point for the concept of Interest in Science (Krapp & Prenzel, 2011); one must bear in mind that a fixing a starting point for a discourse or serious statement is like tracing a line in water, it will always be an arbitrary genesis as there is not ‘one discourse’ or statement producing a such (Foucault, 1972). Why Prenzel and Krapp in their overview of research in Interest in Science discursively states Herbart as the root of the Interest in Science concept is though enough to warrant my analytic demarcation point in an examination of Herbart and the practices following this discourse. Johann Friedrich Herbart (1776-1841) provides a model of the mind, which uses mathematical models akin to Sir Isaac Newton’s model of the solar system. Herbart has discovered a way to expand the notion of Science and it’s laws and regularities into the contemporary science of psychology, which at that time wasn’t regarded as one of the serious sciences (Foucault, xXX). Many discourses were attempting in the late eighteenth- and nineteenth-century to expand mathematics into the science of man (Leary, 1980), but psychology at the time of Herbart was still a dominion of the Soul (Herbart 1890) and before Wundt’s revolutionary attempt to create a experimental psychology (Kuhn, xxx) Herbart’s intent was as an apprentice of Kant to expand his notion of predicting physical events both in the body and in the brain to actual mental events, which was going beyond Kant’s explicit denial of a mathematical psychology(Leary, 1980). It is interesting to note that a rival discourse to Herbart’s mathematical-psychology was the biological/physiological-psychology (xx), but I will show that Herbart’s notion of Interest in Science was absent of this biologicalphysiological dimension; first later (with Berlyne) the bio-phys. conceptualization is seen in a transformation of The Chimera and it still takes more transformations to reach the current contemporary form as seen in PISA06 Survey. Herbart’s key notion was that a Vorstellung (which could roughly be translated to a sense, presentation or idea of) could be measured and predicted with mathematical accuracy(Herbart 1890). A Vorstellung is for the mind, what the atom is for the physical world; the consciousness is composed of combinations of these Vorstellungen behaving according to Newtonian mechanical laws. Herbart distanced himself from Fichte’s concept of Self1 in that the notion of Self first had to be developed in the childhood, thus it could not be the nucleus of origin, hence the concept of Vorstellung. The mental unit of Vorstellung entails thoughts, emotions, visual images, ‘inner speech/voice’ and emotions, it is defined by a measureable strength in the consciousness reflecting the clarity of the Vorstellung (Herbart 1890)(Boudewijnse, 1999). Herbart’s concept of Vorstellung is inspired by his education in music and the concept of ‘’tonelehre’; from music for instance he got the notion of strength and how Vorstellungen could cancel each other out, but it is also with concepts from tonelehre that he goes beyond Newtonian concepts regarding opposing Vorstellungen (Herbart 1890)(Boudewijnse, 1999) In his mechanical and abstract explanation of the behaviour of Vorstellungen he gives an explanation how one Vorstellung helps another one into existence or is fused by it, thereby in effect showing how sequential learning is attained – through repetition (Herbart 1890)(Boudewijnse, 1999) . 1 Johann Gotlieb Fichte (1762-1814) Herbart’s mathematical psychology and his following writings goes beyond a desire to predict and measure the mind on a abstract and theoretical level, he moves his theories and concepts into the practice of education and pedagogy. The writings of Herbart thus follow the function, which other related discourses are taking in the eighteenth century by fusing the respective sciences with The Dictum of Kant2 and the mathematics of Newton/Leibniz (Leary, 1980) In his writings Herbart introduces the concept of pedagogical tact (Pädagogischer Takt), which is a solution to unite the problematic duality of educational theory and educational practice(Herbart 1890). The duality is due to the problem of good and bad practice, and a good practice is dependant on a scientific approach to pedagogy and educational theory (Herbart 1890). In this there is a quite clear demarcation line posited by Herbart, that an educational science may never follow the asserted causality of the natural sciences, hence the need for a concept between theory and practice – pedagogical tact. In the concept of pedagogical tact the educational theory gives you a choice of action, but tact makes you make ‘the right choice’. To further elaborate on this morality in his theories, which is very much in line with Kantian morality, Herbart introduces another concept – Aesthetic necessity, which is the judgement of a specific situation, still a judgement of taste in kantian terms and subjected to the rules of such, but one that the educator can support and improve upon(Kienskel) The improvement of the educators and pupils perception of the world is the cornerstone of Herbart’s educational and pedagogical theory one that is very much in line with his mathematical concept of Vorstellung and how it evolves. (Herbart)(Boudewijnse, 1999(Leary 1980) )(Kien) The notion of repetition and attentiveness is therefore the link between the mathematic-psychology of Herbart and his educational and pedagogical theory. Implemented in Herbart’s project of uniting mathematics, psychology and educational/pedagogical theory there is also the grand failure of the enterprise, he didn’t manage to show how the micro level of his mathematical-psychology was visible in the macro level of schooling and education (Boudewijnse, 1999)(Herbart) This Dark Side of Pedagogy (Herbart) was attributed to general problems regarding education and Herbart’s solution was to emphasize to build learning on what already have been learnt – one could also see this problem as a general problem of Science 2 Any true science must be mathematical. The Chimera of Herbart – A Trinity of Failed Causality The Chimera of Herbart’s Interest in Science is composed of mathematics, psychology and educational/pedagogical theory. This trinity of measurement, soul (psychology) and moral (pedagogy) is The Interest in Science concepts proverbial head, body and tail. In this historical Episteme the proverbial head of the concept of Interest in Science, openly and proudly displayed, is thus the regal mathematical Lion of causality and encompasses the totality of the soul. The goat’s body and main functioning of the concept of Interest in Science is a notion of psychology not founded in biology and physiology, but in Vorstellung and abstract notions of repetition, fusion and attention – still in the nomenclature of Soul. The tail of the chimera, the hidden and steering manifestation of the construct, is morality emphasized by educational and pedagogical theory. The ménage au trois of Herbart shows how Interest in Science is intersected and transversed by other statements; the concept of Interest in Science is in the eighteenth century not explained as a ‘thing in itself’, but a measureable, internal structure, which should be developed according to a judgement of taste. This Chimera remains though a creature of mythos in the various discourses of the century, and with Herbart’s Dark Side of Pedagogy we are witnesses to a failure of linkage between the various parts of the beast. The relation between measurement, soul and morality is never fully realized in the discourse of Herbart German education in the 18th -19th century – A Prussian Discipline The three discursive statements forming The Chimera of Herbart’s notion of Interest in Science The measurement, The Soul and The Moral is the discursive dimension of the construct in the following I will explore the non-discursive – the negative – formations, that the statement is enacted within and without. [under construction] Dewey’s Interest vs. Effort – The fragmentarization of the concept Krapp and Prenzel states Dewey as having adopted Herbart’s ideas regarding of Interest in Science (Krapp 2011) Specifically it is the text Interest and Effort in Education (Dewey 1913)I turn to see how the statement Interest in Science is composed in 1913 in the United States and why it is important in a modern contemporary review to include a historization from Herbart to Dewey regarding the concept Interest in Science (William James is only mentioned in a side note, so Dewey is clearly the carrier of the torch from the nineteenth century). The editor’s wrapping of the text shows us the reason for Dewey importance – failure of the spirit of The Prussian School Regime by repetition and physical disciplination: “To this end we have established a compulsory school attendance age, forbidden child labour, and provided administrative machinery for executing these legal guarantees of the rights of children. Yet, a guarantee of school attendance will never of itself fulfil the purposes of state education. The parent and the attendance officer, reinforced by the police power of the state, can guarantee only one thing, the physical presence of the child at school. It is left to the teacher to insure his mental attendance by a sound appeal to his active interests.” 3 A dichotomy, perhaps if one is glib due to experience, has shown itself between physical attendance and mental attendance and the editor appoints Dewey’s thinking as the solution to the problem. One cannot refrain from drawing a parallel here to Foucault’s work on both the prison and the asylum, which undergoes a similar transformation from the physical disciplination to an inner mental form of disciplination (Foucault xx). [under construction] Berlyne’s Curiosity & Arousal – A new measureable psychology [under construction] Henry Suzzalo, President of the University of Washington, Seattle, Washington (editor) in Interest and Effort in Education, John Dewey, 1913. 3 Litterature: Boudewijnse, G.-J. A. M., David J.; Bandomir, Christina A. (1999). "Herbart's mathematical psychology." History of Psychology 2(3): 163-193. Boudewijnse, G.-J. A. M., David J.; Bandomir, Christina A. (2001). "The fate of Herbart's mathematical psychology." History of Psychology 4(2): 107-132. Broch, T. & Egelund, N. (2001). Elevers interesse for naturfag og teknik. Et elevperspektiv på undervisningen. København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitet. Busch, H. (2005). ROSE-undersøgelsen – hvad ved vi om danske elevers interesse for naturvidenskab og naturfag i folkeskolen? I: S. Sjøberg (red.). Naturfaglig dannelse. Århus: Klim.Egelund, N. & Hulvei, P. (2002). Folkeskoleelevers holdninger til naturfag og teknik. En kvantitativ undersøgelse omfattende 1050 elever. København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitet. Foucault, M. (1972). The archaeology of knowledge. New York,, Pantheon Books. Gardner, P.L. (1975). Attitudes to science: A review. Studies in Science Education, 2, s. 1-41. Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environ- ments (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Jaeger, M.M (2009) Equal Access but Unequal Outcome: Cultural Capital and Educational Choice in a Meritocratic Society. I: Social Forces, 2009, Vol.87(4), p.1943-1971 Krapp, A. & Prenzel, M. (2011): Research on Interest in Science: Theories, methods, and findings, International Journal of Science Education, 33:1, 27-50 Leary, D. E. (1980). "The Historical Foundation of Herbart's Matematization of Psychology." Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 16: 150-163. Troelsen, R. (2005) Unges interesse for naturfag – hvad ved vi, og hvad kan vi bruge det til? I: MONA, 2005, Vol. 2, p.7-21 Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and Effort in Education, Forgotten Books. Foucault, M. (1972). The archaeology of knowledge. New York,, Pantheon Books. Herbart, J. F. (1890). Psychologie als Wissenschaft [Psychology as science]. . Jon. Fr. Herbart's sämtliche Werke in chronologischer Reihenfolge. K. F. Kehrbach, O. Lagensalza, Prussia, Hermann Beyer und Söhne. 5: 177-434. Krapp, A. P., M. (2011). "Research on Interest in Science: Theories, methods and findings." International Journal of Science Education 33(1): 23. Leary, D. E. (1980). "The Historical Foundation of Herbart's Matematization of Psychology." Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 16: 150-163. OECD (2007). PISA 2006 Science Competencies for Tomorrow's World Volume 1 - Analysis.