Nancy Eisenberg’s model of Pro-social Reasoning Kohlberg’s and Piaget’s theories both have a number of similarities, as already mentioned. One of these is their emphasis on wrong doing and justice. Social psychologists in the 1970s became more interested in the reasoning behind people’s actions, as well as the actions themselves, and in moral development this trend was manifested by Eisenberg’s theory of pro-social reasoning. Crucially, as implied by the name, her theory also concentrates on positive behaviours and the reasoning behind them, rather than focusing on negative actions. However, Kohlberg is still the starting point, and much of Eisenberg’s theory and her methods show overlap and similarities with her predecessor’s. Methods Nancy Eisenberg and her colleagues (e.g. Eisenberg, 1986; Eisenberg et al, 1987) have explored this type of reasoning by presenting dilemmas to children in which they have to have to take on the role of someone else and act either out of self-interest or in the interests of others. For example they take on the role of Mary who is in her way to a birthday party. On her way she comes across a child who has fallen and suffered an injury. The dilemma being, does Mary stop and help and as a result miss the party, or does she ignore the injured person and continue on her way? On the basis of children's responses to dilemmas dealing with such pro-social behaviour, Eisenberg proposes a series of five levels of pro-social reasoning. Other factors: Feeling Feelings are crucial to her theory. She believes that morality develops through being able to empathise with others, being able to understand things from their point of view and sharing their feelings. Role play As children develop they take on many different roles, for example, daughter, best friend, pupil and party animal. They also become aware of the role played by others and are able to play these roles too. Role playing of this sort, as opposed to the orchestrated role playing of an electron in science, helps the child to see things from the point of view of others and helps in appreciating their feelings. Eisenberg identified five main levels of pro-social reasoning: Age 0 to about 7 (pre-school and primary school children) 1. Hedonistic (self-focused) orientation (pre-school children) Child only cares only for itself. Any apparently altruistic behaviour is motivated by selfishness for example 'I’ll help them because they’ll help me in future’ (reciprocity), or simply because the child likes the person they are helping. Again compare to Freud’s id oral stage dominated by the selfish id. 2. Needs of others orientation (some pre-school and primary school children) The needs of others are being recognised but only to a limited extent. The needs of the specific situation are being addressed rather than a genuine sense of empathy. When asked the child offers simple explanations for their positive behaviour without referring to guilt or self reflection. Age about 7 to adolescence (primary to secondary school children) 3. Stereotyped approval-focused orientation (primary and many high school children) The child acts in a way that will make them liked. For example lending a helping hand in order to impress others. When asked to explain their behaviour they tend to use stereotyped portrayals of good and bad behaviour. Adolescence onwards 4. Empathic orientation (a few high school children and most secondary school children) The child now starts to show genuine empathy by putting themselves in the shoes of others and begins to report feelings of genuine guilt when considering their own actions. 4b. Transitional level (a few secondary school children) The child’s actions are now explained in terms of wider social values and the need to Protect the dignity and self-esteem of others. 5. Internalised orientation (rare in children) The child now has a full set of values and understands their responsibilities towards others. They have self-respect that they can only maintain by behaving with a duty of care towards others. The person’s desire to live up to their own set of principles is also a motivating factor. In summary, the child progresses from a level at which reasoning is 'self-focused' or 'self-centred' ('what feels good to me is right’) to a stance in which social approval guides both reasoning about justice and about doing good. What is right is what other people define as right. Much later, some young people seem to develop internalised, individualised ideas, which then guide both types of reasoning. Eisenberg (1983) found that empathy is not a consistent characteristic. Children act differently towards different people. Clearly they are more likely to help friends and family, but are also more likely to help people from their own ethnic or religious group. Interestingly (Eisenberg 2005) comments on the lack of mention of reward and punishment mentioned by children in explaining their positive actions. This is in marked contrast to Kohlberg’s findings, when it was common for younger children to cite these as reasons for their negative behaviours. Evaluation Comparisons with Kohlberg Kohlberg is a truer stage theory since he believes that once a child progresses past a stage it does not return to earlier stages. However, Eisenberg disagrees, recognising that there are situations in which we may adopt a lower level of morality, particularly in cases were we decide not to help someone in a particular situation. She also believes that some of the reasoning of the higher levels is not always superior to that found in lower levels. Both Eisenberg and Kohlberg (and Piaget obviously) see cognitive development as crucial in guiding moral development. A person’s ability to reason or make moral judgements is in part limited by their ability to think! Eisenberg believes that ‘primitive empathy’ is to be found in children as young as four. This is clearly at odds with Kohlberg who only recognises empathy much later. Eisenberg has more recently recognised the importance of emotion in moral development. On seeing a person in need of help a child (and an adult) is more likely to help if the distressed person arouses sympathy (characterised by lower heart rate) rather than distress (higher heart rate). This is seen as a return more to Piagetian thinking and away from Kohlberg. This model clearly has some parallels with Kohlberg's, however researchers have typically found that children’s reasoning about pro-social dilemmas and their reasoning about Kohlberg's justice and fairness dilemmas, are only moderately correlated. The sequence of stages may be similar, but children seem to move through these stages at different speeds. Eisenberg has found that children’s pro-social reasoning is slightly ahead of their Kohlberg reasoning. Cross cultural comparisons Boehnke et al 1989 found that children in various Western cultures tend to progress through Eisenberg’s stages in the order and at the age that she suggests. However, there do appear to be differences between cultures. In the more collectivist Kibbutzim of Israel children as young as primary school age show signs of stage 5 reasoning, believing that we have a duty to help others. This is not surprising given that the collectivist nature of the Kibbutz system places an emphasis on responsibility for others and not just responsibility for oneself as preached in more individualistic Western society. Other variables According to Eisenberg, it isn’t only cognitive development that determines the level at which the child reasons: 1. Socilisation Children worried about their appearance and about the views of others are more likely to operate at level 3 (approval-focused), whereas a child with a similar cognitive ability who has been bought up to be more empathetic, will most likely reason at level 4 (empathy-focused). 2. Situational factors As the costs involved in helping someone increase (e.g. in terms of time taken, expense or physical harm), the more a child is likely to offer hedonistic explanations of their actions. Specific situational variables may also be important, e.g. if lots of people are watching we may act in order to gain the approval of others.