Auxiliaries as Dummies 1 why have, be and do are auxiliaries assuming over- rather than underspecification, it must be that have, be and do have the smallest conceptual content of all verbs i.e. when we need to spell out just the functional features using a verb, the best fitting ones will be those associated with the smallest number of extraneous CUs this doesn’t necessarily mean that all these verbs must have the same content – in fact we need them to differ to be able to account for why they are used in different circumstances have and do have a certain conceptual thing in common they all refer to rather vague events, the actual details of which are provided by the context have dinner have a cigarette have a baby have a car have John fix the car have to leave all of these involve a relationship between two elements but the nature of the relationship depends on what is being related do the dishes do the homework do the garden do the crossword puzzle all of these involve a the performance of an action but the nature of the action depends on what it is applied to be is a little more difficult to show this effect with mainly because it seems to lack any meaning in most of its uses (i.e. it is mostly used as a dummy) we see this by the fact that be is omissible in the following contexts I consider [him stupid] he IS stupid I believed [him a good person] he IS a good person Clark Kent Superman! I don’t believe it Clar Kent IS Superman However, there are some contexts in which be is not so easy to omit ? I consider [him in the garden] * I consider [there life on Mars] incidentally, these are exactly the places where a third person BE must be present in Hungarian ő orvos (* van) ő magas (* van) ő a kertben *(van) *(van) élet a Marson this might suggest that in these cases be carries actual meaning but note, which meaning it carries (locative or existential) depends on the context ‘there is life on Mars’ means life exists on Mars, not life is located on Mars ‘he is in the garden’ means he is located in the garden, not that he exists in the garden in this way, be is like have and do assume therefore that be have and do all have the same sort of conceptual content (with extra things to distinguish them) which is only properly defined in context call this ‘null root content’ and assume that it works rather like a pronoun does a pronoun is provided its actual reference in context in the vocabulary it is associated with ‘null reference’ – i.e. it has potential to refer to an individual null root contend is provided its actual reference (the actual event it picks out) in context in the vocabulary it is associated with ‘null content’ – it has the potential to refer to an event have ... √∅ ... be ... √∅ ... do ... √∅ ... see ... √𝑆𝐸𝐸 ... Finally, we can reasonably assume that null root content is the smallest possible root content as it lacks all specific details it therefore follows that be have and do will automatically be selected as the best fitting vocabulary elements to spell out functional CUs 2 how to differentiate between auxiliaries obviously, different auxiliaries are used in different contexts, so we need to be able to differentiate between them we will do this by assuming that it is not only the functional CUs (such as tense and aspecuals) that they spell out in their auxiliary uses, but other CUs which are positioned near to them. so tense + [x] might be spelled out by ‘had’ and tense + [y] by ‘does’ thus, besides null root content and the tense and aspect CUs, these vocabulary items will also be associated with these other relevant CU had [x] √∅ [past] does [y] √∅ [pres] obviously we need to discover the identity of [x] and [y] and provide an analysis of their distributions they appear near to tense and aspectual elements in the situations in which these particular auxiliaries are selected 3 be as default out of all the auxiliaries, be is used in the widest context with progressive with passive with non-verbal predicates this indicates that be is chosen when no other more specific CU is spelled out other than tense and aspectuals therefore we assume that the vocabulary entries are: be √∅ is, am, are √∅ [pres] was, were √∅ [past] obviously the different agreement forms will have different vocabulary entries, but what these are depends on how we account for agreement phenomena in general one idea is that the different agreement forms are restricted contextually by their subjects is √∅ [pres] / [3] am √∅ [pres] / [1] 4 have and perfect the semantics of perfect are rather complicated they seem to involve both tense and aspect the perfect defines an interval delimited by the tense and a period extending some time before this (anteriority) he has been sick since January universal = he continued to be sick from January till now experiential = he experienced a period/periods of being sick some time between January and now he had been sick since January universal = he continued t be sick between January and some point in the past experiential = he experienced a period/periods of being sick between January and some point in the past we can say that the perfect involves defining a period during which the event described by the root holds (continually or sporadically) and the end point of this period is placed at the time identified by the tense perfect = T e p: p = ti ... tx & AT(tx, T) & ep thus the meaning of the perfect involves a prepositional meaning relating the end point of the perfect period to the tense suppose the prepositional aspect of the perfect is represented by a CU [perfP], in the same way that prepositional aspects of events are represented by CUs which are spelled out by prepositions [perfP] always follows tense in the inflection domain [tense]PDI > [perfP]PDI > [perf]PDI As the root is placed in 2nd to last position in the inflection domain, [perfP] will never be spelled out on the root as it is always accompanied and followed by [perf] thus in perfect contexts both tense and [perfP] need to be spelled out be cannot do this as it is not associated with anything other than the tense and null root content so the choice is between have and do have is associated with tense (has, had) and with root content, but it differs from be in that it expresses a relationship has √∅ [pres] [rel] had √∅ [pres] [rel] Given that [perfP] is a kind of relationship [rel] can be used to spell it out therefore have is the best vocabulary choice for spelling out [tense] [perfP] strings 5 do insertion unlike have, do is not used in a specific semantic context, but a syntactically defined one do is used when tense is the only member of the inflection domain tense is not spelled out by a post root morpheme or with the root itself (in irregular cases) in other words, when tense precedes the root so part of the use of do has to do with the syntax which places tense in front of the root when it is the only member of the inflection domain this has to be special circumstances given that the root is usually second from last in the inflection domain, and therefore precedes tense in this situation obviously this is to do with the presence of negation and whatever causes inversion this cannot be the only thing however, as if it were just a matter of spelling out tense, be would be used so something else must be spelled out with tense in this situation as this something else is not an extra semantic element, it must be something that is normally present, but presumably spelled out by something else – most likely the root, as obviously do must be associated with whatever this is in its vocabulary entry do differs from be and have in that it expresses an activity but this cannot be what determines the use of do in its dummy context as do is used even in non-activity contexts John did not run John did not know John did not see it did not seem ... however, do is only used in cases where the root is expressed as a verb, not an adjective: John is not tall * John did not tall this might be taken in one of two ways either whatever it is that do spells out is associated with verbal predicates and normally spelled out by the verbal root or it is present in all cases but is spelled out by adjectival roots but not verbal ones if we go with the first of these, it might be the very element which defines a root as a verb (as opposed to an adjective) which is involved what is the difference between he sleeps is is asleep the first suggests that ‘he’ is involved in some sort of process the second that ‘he’ has a certain property this might come down to the status of ‘he’ as an argument in one and a ‘subject’ in the other argumenthood seems to be connected with certain linking elements – represented as ‘v’ in current theories it is also assumed that ‘v’ is the verbalising element in that if a root is associated with v it is spelled out as a verb we may therefore suppose that it is [v] that gets distributed with tense which is therefore spelled out by do rather than be this assumes that be is not associated with [v] in many ways be is the least verb-like of verbs unlike other small content verbs, it is never used as a light verb have a sleep take a walk give a damn do a dance make a move * be a sleep it is used in typically non-verbal contexts therefore we can assume that be is not associated with [v] in this case, do will be chosen to spell out tense + [v] sequences as be will not be able to do so, not being associated with [v] and have will not be chosen as it is associated with [rel]