Utilizing Neuroscience-Based Learning Principles in the Higher Education Classroom Barbara Nadeau, Quinnipiac University Abstract: Neuroscience has begun to prove what many college professors have long suspected -cramming for exams doesn't work, multitasking is inefficient, and completing the readings before class is helpful. This practice session will explain the current research regarding how the brain learns and the factors that influence learning. Participants will be provided with examples of how to incorporate these principles into their classrooms and will be invited to reflect upon and discuss their own use of these learning principles. Literature Review In the past fifteen years neuroscientists have developed a greater understanding of the way the brain learns and the factors that influence learning (Doyle & Zakrajsek, 2013). It is now understood that learning is a process of building new neural networks and that there are certain learning strategies that can facilitate the development of these new networks (McGinty, Radin, & Kaminski, 2013). Therefore we can now design our courses to take advantage of the revelations from neuroscience. For instance, it has been demonstrated that memory for new information is stronger when studying is spaced over a period of a week rather than all in one day (Jang, Wixted, Pecher, Zeelenberg, & Huber, 2012; Pavlik & Anderson, 2008). Researchers have also found that some of the strategies that are regularly used by students actually inhibit learning. For example multi-tasking is inefficient and impedes learning (Ravizza, Hambrick, & Fenn, 2014) and even the longstanding practice of using flashcards for learning has been shown to be ineffective for retaining detailed information (Reagh & Yassa, 2014). On the other hand, strategies such as the use of concept maps and scaffolded learning are being supported by the neuroscience research. Researchers have shown that it is necessary to fully engage the lateral pre-frontal cortex in learning tasks in order to later be able to generalize the learning to new situations and that the lateral pre-frontal cortex becomes engaged when new learning is linked with prior learning (Cole, Laurent, & Stocco, 2013). Developing an understanding of these neuroscience concepts can improve teaching and learning (Doyle & Zakrajsek, 2013). Objectives for the Practice Session Upon completion of this session, participants will be able to: 1. Identify the primary brain areas implicated in learning 2. Explain, in general terms, what is occurring in the brain during learning 3. Develop classroom strategies that incorporate the neuroscience of learning. Description of the Practice This session will begin with a brief, general, interactive neuroanatomy primer to orient participants to the brain areas implicated in learning. Major findings relative to memory, the impact of elaboration and distributed practice on learning, novelty and mindset will be discussed. The research regarding each concept will be described followed by examples of how these concepts can be incorporated into the classroom. Participants will be invited throughout the presentation to think about which of these concepts they are already utilizing and to share their strategies. Research regarding additional factors that influence learning such as sleep and exercise will also briefly be presented. Discussion Many higher education teachers have already instinctively discovered these principles but science is now providing credence to these practices. I have begun sharing this brain science with my students in order to promote these active learning practices. I have also adjusted my teaching style to incorporate these principles. Similar to others (Stupans, Scutter, & Pearce, 2010), since making these changes I have noticed an improvement in student grades as well as on my teaching evaluations. References Cole, M. W., Laurent, P., & Stocco, A. (2013). Rapid instructed task learning: a new window into the human brain’s unique capacity for flexible cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(1), 1–22. doi:10.3758/s13415-012-0125-7 Doyle, T., & Zakrajsek, T. (2013). The new science of learning. Sterling: Stylus Publishing LLC. Jang, Y., Wixted, J. T., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Huber, D. E. (2012). Decomposing the interaction between retention interval and study/test practice: the role of retrievability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 65(5), 962–75. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.638079 McGinty, J., Radin, J., & Kaminski, K. (2013). Brain-Friendly Teaching Supports Learning Transfer. New Directions for Adult and Continuing Education, 2013(137), 49–59. doi:10.1002/ace.20044 Pavlik, P. I., & Anderson, J. R. (2008). Using a model to compute the optimal schedule of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 14(2), 101–17. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.14.2.101 Ravizza, S. M., Hambrick, D. Z., & Fenn, K. M. (2014). Non-academic internet use in the classroom is negatively related to classroom learning regardless of intellectual ability. Computers & Education, 78, 109–114. Reagh, Z. M., & Yassa, M. A. (2014). Repetition strengthens target recognition but impairs similar lure discrimination: Evidence for trace competition. Learning and Memory, 21(7). doi:342 DOI: 10.1101/lm.034546.114 Stupans, I., Scutter, S., & Pearce, K. (2010). Facilitating Student Learning: Engagement in Novel Learning Opportunities. Innovative Higher Education, 35(5), 359–366. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10755-010-91486