Transcript

advertisement
Slide 1
Confounding and the Language of Experimentation, Part II – The Importance of Proper Comparisons and
Randomization
Slide 2
This video is designed to accompany pages 13-18 of the workbook “Making Sense of Uncertainty:
Activities for Teaching Statistical Reasoning,” a publication of the Van-Griner Publishing Company
Slide 3
Arthroscopic surgery, which involves the placement of small instruments and a video camera into the
joint, through small incisions, has become the most commonly performed orthopedic surgery in the
United States. It isn’t just athletes who have them, but a diverse group of people, including those
suffering from arthritis. How had doctors traditionally assessed the efficacy of this treatment? It
seemed to make a lot of sense. Patients presenting with the appropriate symptoms would have their
level of function measured and asked to rate their level of pain, both before and after the surgery. By
and large patients both felt they had better function and exhibited an increase in their range of motion.
Slide 4
What is missing with this kind of comparison? What we don’t know is how much the results might have
been confounded by the placebo effect. Is it possible that the patients felt better and had a better
range of motion in part just because they had felt their problem was being addressed, even if the actual
surgery was not effective?
Slide 5
This is the question that was asked by the team of physicians and scientists from the Houston Veterans
Affairs Medical Center at the Baylor College of Medicine.
Slide 6
In this study 180 patients with osteoarthritis of the knee were randomly assigned to the traditional
arthroscopic surgery and a placebo surgery. This type of placebo surgery is called a “sham surgery.”
Those patients received skin incisions and had the sights and sounds of the real surgery simulated. After
surgery the patients in both groups were followed for 24 months and measurements both on pain and
function were taken.
Slide 7
What this team of researchers found was surprising. The outcomes as measured by these pain and
function scales were no better for the real surgery group than for the placebo group. In 2002
recommendations to consider avoiding arthroscopic surgery for knees were released, though the impact
of those recommendations is unknown.
Slide 8
Let’s practice our language here. In the arthroscopic knee surgery example, the response variables in
this case are “level of pain” and “level of function.” The explanatory variable is “type of procedure,”
(real or placebo), and the subjects are the participating patients. Confounding was present in the
original design because although the physicians were doing a before/after comparison there was no
placebo comparison.
Slide 9
It is sometimes helpful to be able to diagram the design of an experiment. Before the placebocontrolled study of arthroscopic surgery, patients’ pain and function werecompared before and after
surgery. So there was a comparison taking place. Comparison alone is not necessarily enough. In this
case, as we’ve just seen, the placebo effect was influencing the post-surgery measurements. So the
right kind of comparison is important.
Slide 10
If we diagram the experiment that was conducted at Baylor then we see that the confounding created
by the placebo effect was controlled for by a direct comparison between the real surgery and an
elaborate placebo surgery, also known as a “sham” surgery. Since patients were randomized to these
two treatments it stands to reason that any differences in post treatment pain and function that exists
between the two groups can be relegated to the differences in the treatments.
Slide 11
Let’s turn now from comparison to randomization. In the mid to late 1980’s the disease AIDs was just
making its way into the public consciousness. It was no surprise that pharmaceutical companies were in
a frantic rush to be the first to offer a drug that would treat patients with early symptoms of the disease.
Milan Panic, then the flamboyant chairman of ICN Pharmaceuticals in California was the first to claim
success in a January 1987 news conference. The FDA disagreed.
Slide 12
Here is a summary of the results that ICN reported from their medical experiments. About the same
number of patients participated in three different treatments, one being a placebo. A close look at the
table of results shows that the drug, especially in 800 mg form, seems to be highly effective compared to
a placebo. What was the FDA’s objection?
Slide 13
To quote a June 5th, 1990 article by Michael Lev in the New York Times, “The agency questioned the
methods used in the test. Dr. Frank E. Young, then Commissioner of the F.D.A., publicly challenged the
tests at an AIDS conference in Washington, D.C., because the group receiving a placebo in the study
might have contained more patients considered seriously ill than were in the group that received
ribavirin, skewing the results in ribavirin's favor.”
In short, there was evidence that ICN had not randomized their subjects to the three treatments.
Hence, the confounding owing to health of the patients when entering the study compromised the
inference of “effective treatment” that Mr. Panic had been hoping for.
Slide 14
It’s worth reviewing the language one more time. In the ribavirin study the response variable was the
“development of AIDS (yes or no),” the explanatory variable was the type of intervention (level of
Ribavirin, placebo),” the subjects were the 163 patients participating in the study,” and the confounding,
as mentioned already, was caused by the “lack of randomization into the treatments.”
Slide 15
So what are the benefits of randomization? Randomization helps to keep the comparison groups as
much alike as possible. This helps insure that any differences observed between the two groups are due
to treatment differences.
Slide 16
This concludes our video on the importance of proper comparisons and randomization . Remember, the
placebo effect and lack of randomization can create very real obstacles to making credible inferences
from experimental data.
Download