Supplemental Materials Extended Examples of Ideas, Institutions, and Practices from Introduction (Self-schemas: Independence, Interdependence) Mainstream American Contexts Pervasive Ideas Institutions and Practices (examples) African American Contexts Individualism and autonomy Collectivism and relatedness to others Importance of personal responsibility and control Importance of collective responsibility and community Notion that an individual should be free from the constraints of history, other people and society Notion that an individual is fundamentally connected to history, other people and society Politics: concern for how issues affect one’s personal control, voting as personal responsibility Politics: concern for how issues affect one’s community, voting as collective responsibility Historical Holidays: celebrate and emphasize individual freedom and independence (e.g., Independence Day, Veteran’s Day) Historical Holidays: celebrate and emphasize related and collective experiences of African Americans (e.g., Black History Month, Kwanzaa) Parenting Practices: associate pride with individual autonomy and adhering to personal responsibility, encourage individual effort Parenting Practices: associate pride with relatedness to others including racial/ethnic group and adhering to collective responsibility, encourage cooperative effort Religion: church as separate and independent from other societal intuitions; focus on improving and helping the individual (e.g., personal salvation) Religion: church as connected and interdependent with other societal institutions (e.g., schools, politics); focus on improving and helping the community (e.g., community outreach) Supplementary Materials (continued) Preliminary Analysis for Pilot Experiment First to establish that the two speaker application tasks were psychologically commensurate (i.e., equally engaging) we compared the word count and emotional positivity of the self-descriptions and speeches. Additionally, we compared mentions of achievements and aspirations in the self-descriptions. To assess word count and emotional positivity we used the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) text analysis program (Pennebaker, Francis, & Booth, 2001). LIWC sorts text into categories using an internal dictionary and indicates the percentage of total words for each category— for our purposes the relevant categories were word count, positive emotion words (e.g., happy), and negative emotion words (e.g., sad). A paired samples t-test confirmed that word count did not differ between the two graduation conditions for the self-descriptions (Mainstream Grad: M= 38.16, SD= 22.73; African American Grad: M= 42.59, SD = 25.30; t(43) = -1.57, p= .12, ns). The word count also did not differ for the speeches (Mainstream Grad: M= 38.7, SD= 18.17; African American Grad: M = 41.16, SD= 20.18; t(43)=.78, p = .44, ns). To test emotional positivity, following Cohn, Mehl and Pennebaker (2004), we computed the difference between the LIWC scores for positive emotion words and negative emotion words. As expected, emotional positivity did not differ by graduation condition. Overall participants expressed positive affect for both conditions in their self-descriptions (Mainstream Grad: M = 7.64, SD = 11.96; African American Grad: M=8.11, SD=13.33; t(43)=-.44, p = .66, ns) and speeches (Mainstream Grad: M = 4.17, SD = 4.35; African American Grad: M=3.94, SD=4.10; t(43)=-.27, p = .79, ns). Supplementary Materials (continued) Results for Separate Study on Course Primes for Study 3 The results of a separate study conducted using a different sample of African American college students (n= 58) provided evidence that the course primes used in Studies 3 and 4 differed only in culturally theorized ways. That is, there was no difference among participants who reviewed the course that included mainstream American culture or African American culture in positive expectations about taking the course (e.g., how much they expected to enjoy taking the course, how much they expected that the course would provide an inspiring and supportive environment, 3 items, 7 point scale, averaged to create a composite measure, α=.84). This composite measure of positive course expectations was negatively skewed (Z=-1.99, p=.047); a square root transformation eliminated the skew (Z=.51, p=.61, ns). For intuitive clarity the means and standard deviations for positive course expectations are reported in their original metric, Mainstream American course condition: M=5.07, SD=1.43; African American course condition: M=5.48, SD=1.31, t(56)=1.19, p=.24. However, consistent with our theorizing that including African American culture within an educational setting should activate an interdependent self-schema that can facilitate a constellation of mutually constituting consequences participants differed in their expectations related to cooperative behavior and their expectations about being valued and included. That is, relative to participants who reviewed the course that included mainstream American culture, participants who reviewed the course that included African American culture reported greater expectations of cooperative behavior (e.g., working cooperatively with classmates, 2 items, 7 point scale, r=.81, p<.001, averaged to create a composite measure) and greater expectations about being valued and included (e.g., “my various identities would be welcomed and valued”, “I would be proud to think of myself as a student in this course”, 2 items, 7 point scale, r=.66, p<.001, averaged to create a composite Supplementary Materials (continued) measure). The composite measure of cooperative behavior expectations was negatively skewed (Z=-2.47, p=.014); a square root transformation eliminated the skew (Z=.79, p=.43, ns). For intuitive clarity the means and standard deviations for cooperative behavior expectations are reported in their original metric, Mainstream American course condition: M=5.02, SD=1.41; African American course condition: M=5.72, SD=1.26, t(56)=2.11, p=.039. The composite measure of expectations about being valued and included was negatively skewed (Z=-2.33, p=.02); a square root transformation eliminated the skew (Z=.72, p=.47, ns). For intuitive clarity the means and standard deviations for expectations about being valued and included are reported in their original metric, Mainstream American course condition: M=4.90, SD=1.45; African American course condition: M=5.67, SD=1.27, t(56)=2.23, p=.03. Additional Analyses for Study 5 (Potential Alternative Explanation) A potential alternative explanation involves participation in extracurricular activities (e.g., Eccles & Barber, 1999; Mahoney & Cairns, 1997). Additional analyses suggested that more general involvement in extracurricular activities or social support from peers did not drive the results for Study 5. To assess more general involvement in extracurricular activities we examined whether (1) or not (-1) respondents reported involvement in one or more extracurricular activities (i.e., participation in residence hall events, participation in religious or spiritual activities, participation in fraternity or sorority events, assisted on a faculty research project, held a campus leadership position). The majority of the respondents reported involved in one or more extracurricular activities, n=313 (96%). To assess social support from peers we examined an item that asked respondents about the extent to which they had turned to other students for “support and encouragement”, 4 point scale, recoded so that higher scores indicate more support, 1-Not at All; 4-A lot; M=3.26, SD=.79. The item was negatively skewed (Z= -1.68, Supplementary Materials (continued) p<.001); a square root transformation eliminated the skew (Z=-1.68, p=.09, ns). Both of these measures were assessed at baseline. To test the potential alternative explanation that these measures associated with more general extracurricular involvement drive the results of Study 5 we re-ran the mediation analyses described and tested in Study 5 controlling for these measures. All of the mediation analyses remained significant. Specifically, the results for a mediation analysis that tested whether an interdependent self-schema explains the effect of engagement with African American culture on African American students’ academic fit and identification beliefs were as follows: Path C, B=.083, t(279)=2.41, p=.02; Path C’, B=.056, t(279)=1.58, p=.12, ns; CI=.0046,.057. The results for a mediation analysis that tested whether African American students’ academic fit and identification beliefs explains the effect of engagement with African American culture on selfreported GPA were as follows: Path C, B=.018, t(264)=2.58, p=.01; Path C’, B=.012, t(264)=1.74, p=.084, ns; CI=.0021,.013. The results for a mediation analysis that focused on enrollment in an advanced degree program were as follows: ; Path C, B=-1.14, SE=.48, Wald=5.70, p=.017; Path C’, B=-.99, SE=.49, Wald=4.16, p=.041; CI=-.49,-.018. References (Supplemental Material) Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15(10), 687-693. Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, volunteering, basketball, or marching band what kind of extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of Adolescent Research, 14(1), 10-43. Mahoney, J. L., & Cairns, R. B. (1997). Do extracurricular activities protect against early school dropout? Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 241-253. Pennebaker, J.W., Francis, M.E., & Booth, R.J. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC): LIWC 2001. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.