Some Remarks on The Morphology of Decausativization João Paulo Lazzarini Cyrino (USP/FAPESP) - jpcyrino@usp.br Since Chomsky (1995), the emergence of a syntactic category (little-v) responsible for the projection of the external argument of a verb has been welcomed among the works on generative theory. Although the original formulation of such category was as a licenser of a verbal external argument, it didn't take too long to emerge a kind of little-v which comes in two flavours: one that introduces an external argument and one that does not. One consequence of this definition of v is that the absence or the presence of an external argument of every verb must be licensed by the contents of v. In terms of morphology, it is expected that such a model derives, basically, the causative/anticausative alternation type called equipollent in Haspelmath (1993): "In equipollent alternations, both [causative and anticausative counterparts of a verb] are derived from the same stem which expresses a basic situation by means of different affixes, different auxiliary verbs or different stem modifications." Languages that follow this pattern, at least, apparently are Greek and Japanese (see 1). This non-directed kind of marking the members of an alternation is not the most popular among languages though. For instance, many languages derive anticausative counterparts of a transitive verb by adding a clitic/affix to the transitive form (see 2). Languages may also derive transitive verbs from a basic intransitive, non-causative form. In contrast with decausativisation, which are only marked with clitcs or affixes (see Lazzarini Cyrino, 2012), there is a relative freedom of form for causativization marks (see 3). Taking, for instance, the corpus used by Haspelmath (1993) in his typology of the causative/inchoative alternation, from 651 verbs spread among 21 languages only 20% (125 verbs) are considered equipollent. When the basic form predicted by a structure is found in only 1/5 of the cases, some explanation must be provided about what is between such structure and the surfacing output of its derivation that makes, in 4/5 of the cases, the latter follow a different system from that one predicted by the former. Recent approaches to morphology such as the Distributed Morphology theory, or Minimalist approaches claiming a great part of the morphological operations are result of PF requirements, say they won't have problems with that, by stating the disparity between the predicted form from a structure and the surfacing system is a result of derivations held between Spell Out and PF. In this work, however, I show such kind of analysis fail in capturing essential characteristics of the morphological phenomenon of decausativization marking: the necessary bound-form characteristic of such marks vs. the free form characteristic of causativization marks, issues on ergativity and the transitive characteristics of reflexives; besides the poor empirical support of a v-system, which derives only up to 1/5 of what is seen among languages. As an alternative, I present an expletive analysis of decausativization marks, based in Schäfer (2008) and in the Late Insertion Principle of Distributed Morphology, concerned with the hypothesis such expletive should be incorporated to the verb in order to surface. This approach inserts itself in a program of researching explanatory adequacy in morphological operations. Examples and Tables: (1) (2) Modern Greek a. plén-i wash-3rd.sg.ACTIVE b. plén-ete wash-3rd.sg.NON-ACTIVE Japanese (Cf. Haspelmath, 1993) a. atum-aru gather-TRANSITIVE b. atum-eru gather-INTRANSITIVE Directed Decausativization Language Portuguese Clitic/ Affix se Russian -sja Arabic ta-/in- Hebrew ni-/hit- Otomi2 ni- (3) (CAUSATIVE) (ANTICAUSATIVE) Transitive Intransitive Language quebrou 'broke' otkryl 'opened' quebrou-se Turkish Clitic/ Affix -il- otkryl-sja Georgian -i- baddala 'change' patah 'open' tsih-ti 'swing' ta-baddala Finnish ni-ftah Udmurt1 -ua/utua -sky- Transitive kaalu-c Language Indonesian Mark me- faire fondre Cantonese dou (particle) Transitive Intransitive yık-dı 'destroyed' da-mal-a 'you hid x' yık-ıl-dı avata 'open' vostyny 'change' da-i-mal-a 'you disappeared ava-utua vosi-sky-ni. ni-tsih-ti Directed Causativization Language Telugu Mark -c French faire (v) Intransitive kaalu 'burn' fondre 'melt' Intransitive tutup 'stop' kong 'enlarge' Transitive me-nutup dou x kong 'enlarge x' References: Chomsky, Noam (1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass. The MIT Press. Haspelmath (1993) More on the typology of causative/inchoative verb alternations. In: Comrie, B. & Polinsky, M. Causatives and Transitivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Schäfer, F. (2008) Middles as Voiced Anticausatives. E. Efner and M. Walkow (eds.), Proceedings of NELS 37, 183 - 196. Amherst, MA: GLSA. 1 2 Uralic Language. See Haspelmath (1993). Indigenous language from Mexico. See Maldonado (2009).