“THERE IS AN EFFECT ON CONFEDERATES”: A QUALITATIVE STUDY OF THE EXPERIENCES OF RESEARCH CONFEDERATES by Elliott Lee 100 705 694 August 23, 2010 A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE B.A WITH HONOURS DEGREE Confederates’ Experiences Abstract This research used qualitative research methods to systematically document the nature of people’s experiences as confederates in psychological research. Eight confederates (2 males and 6 females), identified using purposive sampling, participated in semi-structured interviews regarding the nature of their experiences. Using the methods of grounded theory (Strauss & Corbin, 1990), the analyses revealed that confederates whose role did not require acting in a way that might distress participants had slightly positive experiences, largely because of what they learned. Confederates whose role did involve acting in a way that might distress participants expressed ambiguous attitudes toward being a research confederate, attitudes that were determined by weighing the challenges, concerns, and negative feelings they experienced against their belief in the necessity of measures, their faith in the scientific process, improvements of their performance over time, their use of personalized coping strategies, and learning experiences. The limitations of this research and its implications for research confederates, researchers, and professional bodies governing research are discussed. Keywords: confederate, experience, grounded theory ii Confederates’ Experiences Acknowledgements Thank you Dr. Connie Kristiansen for helping me finish this and helping me do it right. Thank you to Whitney Taylor for encouraging me. iii Confederates’ Experiences Table of Contents Abstract ......................................................................................................................................... ii Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................................... iii Table of Contents ..........................................................................................................................iv List of Tables .................................................................................................................................vi List of Figures ............................................................................................................................. vii List of Appendices ..................................................................................................................... viii General Introduction ..................................................................................................................... 1 Research Confederates’ Roles ..................................................................................................... 1 Ethical Issues Associated with the Use of Deception .................................................................. 4 Alternatives to deception ......................................................................................................... 6 Informed consent ..................................................................................................................... 7 Debriefing .............................................................................................................................. 10 Summary................................................................................................................................ 13 Studies of Research Confederates’ Experiences ....................................................................... 14 The Current Research ................................................................................................................ 17 Locating myself ..................................................................................................................... 17 Method .......................................................................................................................................... 21 Participants and Sampling ........................................................................................................ 21 Procedure .................................................................................................................................. 23 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................................. 25 Journaling .................................................................................................................................. 27 Results ........................................................................................................................................... 29 Challenges ................................................................................................................................. 31 Acting-related challenges ...................................................................................................... 32 Participant-related challenges ................................................................................................ 33 Concerns .................................................................................................................................... 34 Concern for the experiment ................................................................................................... 34 Concern for participants ........................................................................................................ 35 Negative Feelings ...................................................................................................................... 36 Counteracting Negative Feelings .............................................................................................. 37 Belief in necessity of methods ............................................................................................... 38 Faith in the scientific process ................................................................................................ 39 Improvement of performance ................................................................................................ 42 Personalized coping strategies ............................................................................................... 43 Learning Experience.................................................................................................................. 45 iv Confederates’ Experiences Negative Case ............................................................................................................................ 47 Discussion ..................................................................................................................................... 49 References..................................................................................................................................... 55 v Confederates’ Experiences List of Tables Table 1. Participant information ...................................................................................... 21 Table 2. Participants’ overall experiences and the potential for harm their role held…30 vi Confederates’ Experiences List of Figures Figure 1. Weighing scale model of confederates’ experiences ......................................... 30 vii Confederates’ Experiences List of Appendices Appendix A. Email to Psychology Department Chair for Faculty Members and Graduate Students……………………………………….…………………………………………74 Appendix B. Informed Consent.…………………………………………………………76 Appendix C. Interview Schedule...………………………………………………………78 Appendix D. Face Sheet.…………………………………………………………………80 Appendix E. Debriefing……….…………………………………………………………81 Appendix F. Post Interview Comment Sheet.……………………………………………83 viii Confederates’ Experiences 1 “There is an effect on confederates”: A Qualitative Study of the Experiences of Research Confederates Elliott Lee Psychologists have an array of research methods in their methodological toolboxes, methods that range from national surveys, computerized questionnaires, role playing, field studies, to personal interviews. Among these, one of the most coveted resources is the experiment. Experiments offer the researcher almost complete control over the situation encountered by research participants, including control over the way independent variables are manipulated, the presence (of at least some) extraneous variables, and how dependent variables are measured. Often, psychologists, particularly social psychologists, wish to observe the effects of a person’s behaviour on other peoples’ behaviour, emotions, opinions or some other dependent variable. In such cases, an effective way of manipulating the independent variable involves the experimenter hiring a person, known as a research confederate, to enact the various predetermined behavioural scripts that constitute the manipulation of the independent variable. The present research is concerned with people’s experiences being research confederates in such research. I will begin by briefly describing the types of roles that have been enacted by research confederates. Because the use deception is inherent to any study employing research confederates, a discussion of the ethical issues associated with deception will be presented. Following this, the existing, albeit scant, research examining research confederates’ experiences will be reviewed. Research Confederates’ Roles Confederates’ Experiences 2 Research confederates have enacted all sorts of roles, including roles that involve enacting benign and beneficial behaviours. In Guéguen's (2007) study of tipping behaviour, for example, the research confederate enacted a fairly benign role. Specifically, the confederate stood in line in a bakery and either tipped the cashier 50 cents or gave no tip. As Guéguen anticipated, the tipping behaviour of the people waiting in line behind the confederate was influenced by the confederate’s tipping behaviour. Bartlett and DeSteno's (2006) study of gratitude and prosocial behaviour illustrates the use of confederates to enact positive or potentially beneficial behaviours. In this study a research confederate either offered to help a research participant, whose computer monitor stopped working, or engaged in a friendly discussion with a participant after viewing a humorous video clip. Participants’ gratitude and helping behaviour was then assessed by having them by rate their willingness to participate in an ostensibly separate survey being performed by the confederate. Analyzing these data revealed that the research participants expressed more gratitude in that they were more likely to help the confederate when the confederate had helped them, relative to when the confederate had simply conversed with them. While some research may require confederates to enact relatively positive roles, other studies employ confederates to enact scripts involving negative behaviours, behaviours that sometimes have the potential to distress or harm the research participants. For instance, in his now classic studies of obedience to authority, Milgram (1974) led research participants to believe that they were about to take part in a study of ways of facilitating learning. Each research participant was then informed that she or he had been randomly assigned to be the “teacher” and that the research confederate, who was pretending to be another research Confederates’ Experiences 3 participant, had been randomly assigned to be the “learner.” The participant was further instructed to administer what they believed was an electrical shock whenever the learner failed to learn and responded incorrectly. Further, with each successive error, the participant was instructed to give the learner what appeared to be progressively stronger electrical shocks and, as they did so, the confederate began to increasingly complain of pain and, in some cases, potentially life-threatening medical problems (e.g., heart problems). The actual dependent variable of interest was “obedience,” as operationalized by the level of electrical shock at which the participant refused to continue with the experiment, values that could range from zero to 30. Of the 40 participants in Milgram's (1963) initial experiment, 26 (65.0%) heeded the experimenter's prods to continue despite the supposed learner’s extreme protests and went on to administer the maximum shock level. The remaining 14 participants expressed great agitation and anger and refused to continue with the study after having administered shocks between levels 20 and 25. Milgram’s (1974) methods were subjected to intense scrutiny following the release of the initial results and concerns regarding the participants’ well-being were raised. Because many of the research participants became increasingly distressed as the experiment progressed, as manifest by increases in sweating, trembling, stuttering, and other signs of tension, some declared that the methods were risqué and unethical (Baumrind, 1964). However, none of the issues that were raised concerned the research confederate, who was simply described as “a forty-seven-year-old accountant, trained for the role; he was of IrishAmerican descent and most observers found him mild-mannered and likable” (Milgram, 1974, p.16). That many of the research participants became increasingly distressed as they administered shocks that they believed caused the confederate increasing levels of pain and Confederates’ Experiences 4 harm, suggests that the research confederate may have had a parallel experience. Research indicating that empathic people suffer with those they witness suffering (e.g., Batson, 1998; Hoffman, 2000) suggests that Milgram’s research confederate may have experienced increasing levels of distress as he witnessed research participants’ increasingly distressful responses to his role-related behaviour. Indeed, it is possible that the research confederate obeyed Milgram’s instructions to act in ways that distressed the research participants in the same way that the actual participants obeyed the experimenter’s instructions to punish and thereby distress the supposed “learner.” Given this possibility, the present research is designed to examine the nature of research confederates’ experiences. Ethical Issues Associated with the Use of Deception The use of a research confederate implies the use of deception, which is a divisive topic among psychologists. One of the first discussions of the ethical issues surrounding deception appeared in the 1950s, when Vinacke (1954) expressed his concerns about how readily psychologists resorted to the use of deception to ensure participants’ naivety without considering the potential repercussions of such deception. Vinacke (1954) also questioned whether debriefing procedures were sufficient to remove any negative effects deception may have on research participants, whether the use of deception might harm the public’s image of the psychological community, and whether the benefits of scientific research findings outweigh the costs of any potential harm to research participants. Not only did Vinacke’s (1954) simple commentary stimulate public discussion of the ethics of deception, it also detailed the most common issues raised in subsequent debates regarding the use of deception. Like Vinacke (1954), Kelman (1967) indicated that, “what concerns me most is not so much that deception is used, but precisely that it is used without Confederates’ Experiences 5 question.” (p. 3). Indeed, deception has become almost normative within research, even though governing bodies often suggest that it be used only as a last resort. The American Psychological Association (APA, 2002), for example, asserts that deception is only warranted when the possible results of a study are of great scientific, educational, or applied value and when there are no other research options. Although considerable arguments have been made against the use of deception in psychological research, there are also a number of benefits of associated with the use of deception in psychological research. A main argument for the use of deception is that the purpose of experimental research is to determine the causes of events that occur in the real world (Aronson, Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Gonzales, 1990), which means that experimental paradigms most similar to the real world are optimal. Deception is employed to make an experimental situation similar to a real-life experience by having a naive participant. Deception is ideal because it provides situations to which the participant reacts, versus situations in which they think of how they might react (as in role playing scenarios or experiments that involve vignettes with the question “what do you think you would do in such a situation?”). Deception is also inherent in many measurement devices used by social scientists (Kelman, 1967). For example, most of the questionnaires used by psychologists do not explicitly state what they measure, for fear of biasing results by social desirability and other concerns. Thus, it may not be too much of stretch to extend the deception to the hypothesis of the overall study. This argument only holds in the case of passive deception, where the purpose of the study is concealed, not in studies where a large-scale hoax is involved. Confederates’ Experiences 6 Alternatives to the use of deception. Because most researchers are likely to believe that their research findings are likely to have great scientific, educational, or applied value, using this criterion to justify the use of deception is problematic. There is also some evidence that existing alternatives to the use of deception, such as role playing, may be ineffective. Role playing involves telling participants the purpose of the research and then asking them to act how they think they would if the situation were real. In addition to being ethically sound, role playing avoids some of the negative repercussions of other research methods, such as evaluation apprehension, suspiciousness, and docility toward the experiment (Miller, 1972). In a pioneering comparison of role playing and deception, Willis and Willis (1970) had pairs of participants rank the aesthetic value of ten photographs. They then provided each participant with information about their partner’s ranking and information about the similarity of their own rankings and their partner’s rankings to the rankings of a panel of experts. After considering this information the participants were asked to re-rank the photographs and their conformity to their partner’s rankings was assessed. Two variables were manipulated within this context. “Perceived task competence” was manipulated by the experimenter’s feedback after the participants’ initial ranking indicating that either the participant’s rankings or their partner’s rankings were more in line with the rankings of the expert panel. The second independent variable, “instructional set,” was manipulated by telling some participants that the study was interested in how much they would be influenced by their partner’s rankings or that the study concerned their ability to use additional information to improve their performance. In addition, before the study half of the participants were deceived about the purpose of the study, among other things. The remaining participants were truthfully informed that the experiment involved a comparison of role playing and deception procedures. Confederates’ Experiences 7 The analyses of these data revealed that role playing duplicated the obvious or strong main effect of perceived task competence observed in the deception condition but not the more subtle or weaker interaction between instructional set and perceived task competence. Willis and Willis (1970) therefore concluded that, while there may be some merit to using role-playing over deception when the effects of interest are known to be strong, role playing should be used with awareness of the fact that some effects may go undetected. Miller (1972) also noted a number of weakness in his review of role-playing as an alternative to deception, with one being the fact that participants act differently when in role plays as opposed to deception situations. Similarly, although an infamous role play study replicated Milgram’s (1974) obedience findings, role playing was best when the researcher was absent (Geller, 1978). Overall, then, the evidence that role playing is a viable alternative to deception is at best mixed. Informed consent. In the addition to concerns regarding the effectiveness of alternatives to the use of deception, researchers have debated issues associated with the nature of informed consent when research involves deception. Informed consent refers the process of disclosing the purpose and proceedings of a study to participants in a way that allows them to make an informed decision whether or not to participate in the study. Deception is used whenever the true purpose of the research is not revealed, something that occurs whenever the nature of the research is disguised by a false but believable “cover story.” When deception is used, participants who agree to participate have essentially consented to misinformation (Smith & Richardson, 1983). Indeed, some have argued that when deception is used informed consent serves no purpose other than to supplement the cover story (Adair, Dushenko, & Lindsay, 1985). Confederates’ Experiences 8 Because consent procedures inform participants of what they will be exposed to and required to do during the study, some researchers (e.g., Baron, 1981) have argued that the purpose of informed consent can be served even when deception is used. This belief hinges on the entire proceedings being revealed to the participant prior to their participation, something that does not always occur. For example, the participants in Asch's (1951) conformity study were not informed of either the true purpose of the study or its methods. This experiment involved a single participant in a room with several confederates whom they believed to be other participants. Three lines were presented on a screen and each individual in the room was required identify which of the three lines was most similar in length to a fourth line. Within this context, the research confederates gave the same wrong answer by declaring that they believed one of the incorrect lines matched best with the fourth line. A total of 37 participants conformed to the confederates’ responses by choosing the incorrect line at least once, whereas only 2 of the individuals in the no-confederate control condition made actual errors. Using Baron’s (1981) explanation of informed consent I would suggest, albeit facetiously, that participants should have been informed that the confederates would be giving predetermined incorrect responses. I say facetiously because the results could not be duplicated if the participants had known that the others were giving a prescribed incorrect answer. The APA (1982) agrees with the aforementioned issues regarding informed consent in studies using deception, stating that “deception employed in research intrinsically compromises the agreement on which consent is based” (p. 35). However, the APA does allow deception under the conditions identified earlier. They also offer some guidelines for situations in which informed consent may be waived, such as when the research is not Confederates’ Experiences 9 expected to harm or distress the participant, when the research involves educational research of normal practices or anonymous participant information (such as naturalistic observation or archival research) and in organizations where there are no repercussions for employees (APA, 2002). The APA also provides provisions for handling informed consent procedures in studies involving deception, provisions that are similar to the informed consent guidelines for studies that do not involve deception. In addition, however, participants must be given the opportunity to refuse to participate and withdraw from the study without any negative repercussions and, following the experiment, participants must be informed of the deception and why it was necessary (APA, 1982, 2000). In Canada, the ethical regulations governing psychological research are set in place by the Tri-Council of the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. The Interagency Advisory Panel on Research Ethics (PRE), which oversees the Tri-Council Policy Statement (TCPS), requires researchers to provide people with the opportunity to give informed consent at their own accord (PRE, 2005). However, any or all of the requirements of informed consent can be waived by the Research Ethics Board if the research poses very little risk to participants, if it could not otherwise be conducted, if there is no therapeutic intervention involved, and whenever possible and appropriate, participants are provided with complete information regarding the study following their participation. It is interesting to note that, while the PRE is in charge of interpreting and implementing the TCPS, it does not indicate that they enforce it. Confederates’ Experiences 10 Debriefing. Debates regarding the use of deception in psychological research have also focussed on whether debriefing a participant about the true purpose of an experiment can subsequently undo any harm caused by the deception. Debriefing is a procedure designed to remove any misconceptions created during the research, such as those associated with any deception, and to restore any trust that was lost between researcher and participant (Baumrind, 1979; Sieber, 1983; Smith & Richarson, 1983). Debriefing participants may also involve attempts to help reassure the participant that they have contributed to science and to knowledge and that these contributions are educational and beneficial to themselves and society (Sieber, 1983). Moreover, to ensure the well-being of the participant, the debriefing should clarify all aspects of any deception or hoax (Sieber, 1983). When research confederates have been used, debriefings may also include formally introducing the confederate and their true identity to the participant (Sieber, 1983). Milgram (1963) referred to this arrangement as “a friendly reconciliation” (p. 374) and is deemed important because it provides an opportunity to both appease any harm done and to ensure that the participant holds no antipathy for the confederate. While the intent and goals of debriefing are clearly laudable, debriefing a participant is not necessarily effective in correcting misconceptions and reinstating a participant’s trust in the researcher or psychology more generally. Indeed, findings regarding the “belief perseverance effect” suggest that debriefing may do more to appease the researcher than the participant. In this regard Anderson, Lepper, and Ross (1980) had some participants read case studies suggesting that risk takers make better firefighters and other participants read case studies suggesting that cautious people make better firefighters. The participants were then instructed to think of a theory that would explain the findings of the case studies they read. Confederates’ Experiences 11 After doing so, the participants were then told that the information in the case studies was false and had been made up for the purposes of the study. Despite receiving this information, participants continued to believe the information they had read. Such findings, then, do not bode well for the ability of debriefing procedures to address the misconceptions stemming from the use of deception. Other research suggests that debriefings may effectively address the misconceptions associated with research using deception and that participants may actually enjoy such research experiences. In an early study by Smith and Richardson (1983), individuals who had recently participated in psychological research studies were polled about their perceptions of the informed consent procedure, how considerate the experimenter had been, the debriefing procedure, any effects of their participation on their trust of psychologists, whether any educational benefits were noted, whether deception had properly been explained, and their overall evaluation of the study they had participated in. They were also asked about any harm they experienced during the study and whether the researcher had properly addressed it and helped to reduce or remove it. Interestingly, and contrary to expectations, participants who had been deceived felt more positive about their participation than those who had not been deceived. In addition, individuals who reported harm that they believed had been addressed effectively reported the most educational benefit from their participation while those who reported harm that they believed had not been addressed effectively reported the least trust of psychologists. Finally, the debriefing was reported as being most effective when it removed all negative aspects of their research participation, when the participants felt they had learned something, and when the researcher showed care and concern while conducting the debriefing. These findings suggest that researchers have a responsibility to properly address Confederates’ Experiences 12 any potential harm their experimental situation creates in participants and they should offer the debriefing as an educational element to help mitigate this harm. Oczak and Niedźwienska (2007) recently proposed a new method of debriefing for research involving deception, a method designed to address two issues. First, an educational element is used to help eradicate any effects when participants gaining new and potentially negative knowledge of themselves. Essentially, this consists of an opportunity to use the new knowledge in a positive rather than negative manner. Second, participants are taught how to recognize deception so as to help eradicate the effects of the deception and to equip individuals with the ability to recognize and cope with future occasions when they may be deceived. To test their debriefing method, Oczak and Niedźwienska (2007) had 60 participants participate in a study of suggestibility. Participants were told that they were participating in a study of memory. At the outset, the participants were asked to complete the State Self-Esteem Scale (SSES) and three Semantic Differential (SD) scales assessing their current mood, psychological experiments, and themselves. After completing these measures, the participants completed the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (GSS). Then, half of the participants were debriefed using a traditional approach and the remaining participants were debriefed using the new method. All participants were then met by another researcher who declared that she was interested in people's reactions to participating in psychological research and asked participants to complete the SSES and SD scales again. The experimental participants were also asked to complete the GSS again. Participants who received the new extended debriefing were less suggestible following the debriefing than participants who received the traditional debriefing (Oczak & Confederates’ Experiences 13 Niedźwienska, 2007). In addition, while the individuals who were given the standard debriefing developed more negative moods over the course of the study, those who received the new extended debriefing reported more positive moods. Relative to participants who received the traditional debriefing, those who received the new debriefing also had more positive evaluations of experiments and increases in self-esteem. In sum, this new extended debriefing holds the potential for increasing participants' post-experiment mood, respect for psychological experiments, and self-esteem following their participation in research involving deception. Summary. Given that lack of viable alternatives to the use of deception, some comfort comes from evidence indicating that it is possible to design debriefing procedures that effectively address any distress or harm caused by the use of deception. It is also comforting to know that new approaches to informed consent procedures are being implemented to ensure that participants are able to provide their informed consent to studies using deception. Such innovations include informing participants that the study may or may not use deceptive methods. However, while there are guidelines regarding researchers’ treatment of research participants, there are no professional guidelines for confederates to follow or guidelines to protect them. The only potential guidelines recognizing the welfare of research confederates are statements such as this one made by the APA (1992): “Psychologists take reasonable steps to implement appropriate protections for the rights and welfare of human participants, other persons affected by the research, and the welfare of animal subjects” (6.06d). The reference to “other persons affected by the research” may include confederates, but this is unclear. Confederates’ Experiences 14 The newest version of the APA ethical guidelines states that work should only be delegated to those who can “perform competently on the basis of their education, training, or experience” (2002, 2.05). In view of this, the APA encourages researchers to train their research personnel and to ensure their competence (Tangney, 2000). Again, however, confederates are not mentioned explicitly. Although one might believe there is no need to develop guidelines for the protection of research confederates, research examining the nature of research confederates’ experiences suggests that this belief may be misguided. Studies of Research Confederates’ Experiences To date, only one autobiographical qualitative case study and one experimental study address the nature of people’s experiences as a research confederate. Oliansky (1991) documented his experience as a confederate in a study in which participants were led to believe that the study was designed compare the abilities of untrained student counsellors with counsellors who had been trained. In reality, however, the study was designed to assess how participants’ feelings were affected by their ability versus inability to help someone experiencing emotional distress. To this end, participants were instructed to “counsel” a confederate who divulged a fictitious story of an ex-lover. The manipulation of the independent variable occurred at the end of the so-called “counselling” session when the confederate acted in ways suggesting that he either improved or did not improve. Oliansky (1991) heralded all of the confederates for their abilities. Consistent with the belief perseverance effect (Anderson et al., 1980), he also expressed his concern that, even after being debriefed, the participants may have felt burdened by the emotionally laden information they received. He described the participants who divulged their own personal stories in an attempt to empathize with the confederate as those who were the most affected Confederates’ Experiences 15 when they learned of the deception and that the confederate was not actually in need of counselling for emotional distress. As the confederate, Oliansky (1991) assumed responsibility for initiating such distress and reported regretting being involved in the research. Importantly for the present research, Oliansky’s (1991) account gave voice to the perspective of those involved in conducting deception research. In this regard he claimed that everyone who was involved felt “doubtful and guilty about their part in the study” (Oliansky, 1991, p. 256). He recalled the embarrassment they felt while conducting research in which a participant expressed obvious suspicion and he noted that he and his colleagues experienced “mutual embarrassment” (Oliansky, 1991, p. 257) when they met former participants outside the laboratory. Oliansky (1991) also described his opposition to the use of deception and its relation to a number of the previously mentioned ethical concerns, his belief that scientific knowledge does not outweigh the potential harm or distress such methods might cause participants, and his experiences of various problematic consequences of deception, including participants’ anger and confederates’ discomfort with the study and having enacted the deception. In an experimental study, Laurens and Moscovici (2005) examined whether research confederates are affected by the roles then enact. Based on dissonance theory, they predicted that research confederates would engage in what they called “self-conversion” when, as part of their role, they tried to affect participants’ beliefs. Laurens and Moscovici (2005) had two participants enter their laboratory. Unbeknownst to each other, one of the two participants had been previously chosen to act as a confederate. The confederate remained in the laboratory for the duration of three experiments, Confederates’ Experiences 16 while the participant was replaced for each experiment. The experiment was ostensibly about colour perception and involved slides being presented on a screen for 15 seconds, during which time the participant indicated how green or blue the slide was (100% blue and 0% green to 0% blue and 100% green). As a latent measure of colour perception, participants were also asked to match a colour on a colour chart with the colour of the afterimage that appeared when the screen briefly flashed white between slides. Participants recorded this information on a questionnaire. In a second phase of the experiment, participants were asked to declare aloud their opinion of the colour. The third phase of the experiment was a repeat of the first phase. The experimenter explained the procedure, and then asked potential confederates whether they would like to continue in the role of the confederate. All potential confederates agreed to continue and were then instructed to verbally state that a blue projection was actually green during the second phase of the study. They were also told to complete the questionnaire as if they were truly a participant and to report the actual colour they saw. The members of the control group were given the same instructions as the participants, however the researchers did not designate one of the participants as a confederate. Both of the naive participants participated in the sequence of three experiments without being replaced. The analysis of the confederates' questionnaire responses revealed that they perceived the slides in the same way as other participants. However, in the latent condition, where they were asked to rate the colour of the afterimage and match it to a corresponding colour on a colour chart, the confederates reported less green in the afterimage, indicating that they perceived the actual image as more green than it actually was. Hence, by stating that the slides appeared to be green when, in fact, they were blue, the confederates latently influenced Confederates’ Experiences 17 their perception of the colour. As Laurens and Moscovici (2005) expressed it, “In the process of convincing others, one also convinces oneself” (p. 191). The Current Research That research confederates are affected by their own behaviour begs the question: In what other ways are research confederates affected by their research participation? The current research is designed to address precisely this question. The answer to this question is important because, as noted earlier, there are no professional guidelines for confederates to follow or guidelines to protect them. Semi-structured interviewing, a qualitative research method, was selected as the method of choice for this research in view of the opportunity interviews offer to explore people’s experiences and because they allow participants to express themselves in their own way and provide participants with at least some room to determine the nature of the subject matter that is discussed. A qualitative research method was also deemed appropriate in view of the lack of previous research examining people’s experiences being a research confederate and because a quantitative method (e.g., a questionnaire) would require knowledge of which experiences to ask about. Locating myself. In qualitative research the researcher is the tool and because of this, it is important, for both me and the consumers of this research, that I articulate my personal perspectives regarding issues that might affect my methods and/or interpretations and understandings. In regard I personally find it hard to state ownership of an extreme position either for or against the use of deception and research confederates. Much knowledge has been gained through the use of deception, most of which could not have been achieved otherwise. I do hold that it is only acceptable when it is fully justifiable (which, Confederates’ Experiences 18 coincidentally, is the mantra of numerous regulatory organizations; e.g., APA, 2002; PRE, 2005). Further, I believe deception is justifiable when the knowledge gained has a practical implication and the costs or harm to participants are determined in advance to be short-term and treatable directly following their participation. I also believe it is justifiable only when there are no other sufficient alternatives to using deception. A colleague of mine once said “some of the most interesting research involves confederates.” I feel the need to repeat this sentiment here as I believe it is true. Although some of the most interesting research does involve deception, there are costs and in this regard. Recognition should also be directed toward the experiences of confederates who, like participants, are affected by the research (Laurens & Moscovici, 2005). That research confederates may be affected in both negative and positive ways also needs to be recognized. They may, for example, be adversely affected by having to lie to participants who are often fellow students or by being required to act in ways that have the potential to distress or harm the research participants. However, being a research confederate may also be associated with any of a number of positive experiences, such as learning that they are competent actors and feeling good about themselves as a result, as well as gaining knowledge about both the area examined in the study and psychological research methods. My motivation for pursuing this topic comes from my own experience as a confederate. I was a confederate in a study that required me to express unprovoked harmful statements toward a female participant. This was an extremely negative situation, and I experienced great distress with little support. Because I was not closely involved with other confederates, I would like to know whether others in similar situations experienced any distress or if they felt it was a positive experience, and if any had any effective coping Confederates’ Experiences 19 mechanisms. The potential for a bias toward negative experiences exists, but I do have some measures in place to ensure that this does not occur. For example, I have recently been involved in a positive psychology class that elucidated the importance of focusing on positive aspects of psychology. In addition, I intentionally included questions focusing on both positive and negative aspects of being a confederate. Importantly, I deliberately used the methods of grounded theory to analyze the data because I believed these methods would help me stay grounded in the realities of participants’ experiences, as opposed to taking flight in my own preconceived notions. With respect to my research paradigm beliefs, I cannot say that I ascribe to a single position. I might best be described as a practicing constructivist-interpretivist with shared positivist and postpositivist experiences. I seek to be multi-paradigmatic in order to be able to achieve the highest grade of research possible, not being limited in my methods or perspectives. I hold a constructivist-interpretivist perspective toward ontology defined by “multiple, equally valid, and socially constructed realities” (Ponterotto & Grieger, 2007, p. 410); a constructivist-interpretivist approach towards epistemology defined by an “interactive researcher-participant role” (p. 410) combined with a hint of postpositivism because I search for an unattainable objectivity. Concerning axiology, I hold both postpositivist and constructivist-interpretivist beliefs because I do believe biases are inevitable, valued, and should be embraced, but I also believe that they should be dealt with in all research contexts (e.g., during interviews or participant-researcher interactions before participation). I also believe that research may be discussed from the first person perspective, but a degree of objective and scientific prose is required to at least appear academic. Finally, I believe in the possibility of using both qualitative and quantitative methods as necessary because of the Confederates’ Experiences 20 differing applications of each, which I suppose makes me a postpositivist. It is important to note that I am only in the toddler years of my prospective research career and, although I would like to believe that the fluid property of my attitudes will remain forever, it may not. Confederates’ Experiences 21 Method Participants and Sampling A total of 8 participants (2 males and 6 females) were interviewed regarding their experiences as a research confederate. Purposive sampling was employed to ascertain participants who had acted as a confederate in psychology experiments. For purposes of this study, a confederate is regarded as someone who was hired by the researcher to pretend to be a participant in an experiment. Requirements of the purposive sample were also that participants must have interacted in person with participants (as opposed to a confederate who is simply required to be present with no interaction with participants or a confederate who interacts with participants through a computer) and a belief that they had acted in a way that they would not normally have acted. A summary of participants is presented in Table 1 with participants numbered in the order they were interviewed, their pseudonym which they will be referred to as, their gender, their age, and their year and program of study at university. Table 1 Participant demographic information Participant Pseudonym Gender Age Year and program of study 24 1st year Psychology, Ph.D. Number 1 Male Jake 2 Pants Female 26 4th year Psychology, B.A. 3 Alice Female 20 3rd year Political Science & Law, B.A. Confederates’ Experiences 22 4 Laura Female 20 3rd year Political Science, B.A. 5 Sarah Female 19 3rd year Anthropology, B.A. 6 Kuzco Female 21 4th year Psychology, B.Sc. 7 Bob Male 24 3rd year Psychology, B.A. Female 21 1st year Psychology, M.A. Sarah 8 Smith Due to the limited population of individuals who have acted as confederates, an array of recruitment procedures were used in this study. Posters were the primary method of recruitment. The recruitment posters had the Carleton University letterhead at the top left and read: Have you ever worked as a research confederate in a psychology study? If so, I would like to know more about your experience! I am looking for people who have been research confederates in psychological studies that used some form of deception. In return for participating in a 45 minute interview about your experiences, you will be given $15.00. To learn more, contact Elliott, at (613) 868-1828 or elee8@connect.carleton.ca Note: Answering questions about experiences as a research confederate in research that used some form of deception may be distressing for some participants. This study has been approved by the Carleton University Ethics Committee for Psychological Research. This notice was posted at universities in the Ottawa region and neighbouring cities. Posters were primarily hung nearby the psychology offices and common areas (i.e., Confederates’ Experiences 23 cafeterias) of several large universities in the Eastern Ontario region. There were numerous respondents to these posters, but, unfortunately, only one matched the required criteria of the purposive sample. Posters were placed around the campuses of several Eastern Ontario universities, concentrated in the psychology departments, but also displayed in common areas (e.g. cafeterias). Another recruitment method was the use of two acquaintances of mine who I knew had acted as confederates. I emailed one and spoke with another, asking if they would be willing to participate in an interview about their experiences as a confederate. Both individuals have been involved in the study. An email was also sent to the chair of the psychology department at my current university asking her willingness to distribute a recruitment email to all staff and graduate students, to which she complied (see Appendix A). This form of recruitment produced numerous respondents, but, unfortunately, only two fit the description as prescribed by the purposive sampling. Snowball sampling, which yielded two participants, was also employed. At the conclusion of each interview, participants were asked if they knew anyone else who had been a research confederate and, if so, whether they could provide me with their contact information. Procedure Upon initial contact with potential participants, an attempt was made to verify that they fit the description of the purposive sample. This helped remove some individuals who did not meet the criteria. Potential participants were informed that the focus of the research was in their experience as a confederate and I was interested in conducting a 30 to 45 minute Confederates’ Experiences 24 interview with them in person. If they expressed a willingness to participate, we would decide a mutually convenient time and place to meet to conduct the interview. Interviews began after I offered written and oral versions of informed consent and received the participant’s informed consent to participate in the study and their consent to audio-tape the interview (see Appendix B for the informed consent form). Interviews lasted approximately 18 to 47 minutes, at an average of 27:48 minutes long (SD = 9:30 minutes). Interviews were conducted following a semi-structured interview schedule (see Appendix C for the final version of the interview schedule). The interview schedule was designed according to Rubin and Rubin’s (1995) stages of interviewing. As such, initial questions were designed to be “warm-up” questions that allowed participants to settle in and develop a sense of their competence as an interviewee (i.e., “how did you hear about my study?” and “why did you decide to participate?”). I proceeded to ask for some basic background information about the study in which they been a research confederate, such as how they came to be a research confederate and what the study was about. This was to help me get an idea of their experience to build from and to help participants by beginning the recalling of past events. I then began to ask for more detailed information about their role and experience as a confederate, including what they were required to do and how they felt about doing it. As participants spoke of their experiences as a research confederate, I probed for additional information to ensure that I had understood the participant’s meanings and to gain more insight into relevant topics that needed further elaboration. At the conclusion of the interview, basic participant information was gathered and collected on a face sheet (shown in Appendix D). Interviews concluded with a written and Confederates’ Experiences 25 verbal debriefing, in which the purpose of the study was reiterated, contact information for a counselling service was provided, and they were thanked for their participation. Shortly after each interview, I completed the Post Interview Comment form shown in Appendix E. Using this form I recorded information on the mood or tone of the interview, the participant’s emotional reactions, my emotional reactions to the participant and what they said, the strengths and weaknesses of the interview, and any other comments. This information helped me become aware of aspects of the interview, including my behaviour that might have affected participants’ responses, aspects of myself that may influence the subsequent coding of the interview, and ways of improving the interview. The interview schedule was constantly iterated. It changed slightly with each subsequent interview. Questions were added when new categories emerged that required elaboration and questions were removed as certain categories became fully developed. Iteration of the interview schedule helped to elucidate and develop categories. Data Analysis Each interview was transcribed as soon as possible. The transcribed interviews were then coded and analyzed using Strauss and Corbin’s (1990) version of grounded theory. During the first stage of coding, known as open coding, small meaningful portions of data were summarized with as few words as possible. The use of in vivo codes consisting of the participant’s own words were sometimes used. Throughout open coding, constant comparisons were made between interviews, looking for similar ideas and concepts (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). This helped to recognize the emergence and development of categories. After several interviews had been open coded, axial coding began. This level of coding entailed examining the relationships, differences, and similarities between categories Confederates’ Experiences 26 that emerged during open coding. Axial coding involves defining categories, assigning dimensions, and making connections between categories and subcategories. Strauss and Corbin’s (1990) method of establishing and testing subcategory relationships through the paradigm model was used for all categories and their respective subcategories. The final stage of coding, selective coding, involved refining categories and their properties, as well as their inter-relations. It also involved identifying the “core category,” a category that was linked to all of the other categories. A storyline was designed that surrounded the core category, which eventually became the emerging theory. Selective coding also involved analyzing the relationship of categories to the core category through use of the paradigm model. These relationships between categories and the core category were tested against the data of the interviews, which fully grounded the theory. Throughout this process, any patterns in the data or potential processes were noted, analyzed, and placed in the theory as necessary or possible. A tentative model composed during selective coding was distributed to all eight participants to ensure that it encompassed their experience. A request for feedback, both praises and critiques was request. Participants were reminded that they were not obliged to do this, but it would be greatly appreciated. Two participants responded stating that it fit their experience, helping to ground the theory even further. Consistent with procedures outlined in Strauss and Corbin’s (1990) version of grounded theory, I wrote memos throughout the analysis. Coding is stressed as both an inductive and a deductive process (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). Memos helped to keep track of any deductive theories I may have had, any observations I thought necessary that could not be coded, or to document my conceptual and methodological thoughts as I worked with the data. Confederates’ Experiences 27 They provided a direction for the ongoing data analysis, subsequent interviews and other methodological concerns (e.g., regarding saturation and sampling needs). One memo was utilized as an explanation for the only negative case that was found. Journaling From the time of the initial conception of this study to its end, I maintained a journal tracking my methodological thoughts and research decisions, as well as my subjective experiences during the research process. For example, one journal entry describes an unsuccessful method of recruitment in which I sought contact with confederates from a researcher whom I knew had employed confederates. Upon initially approaching him, he seemed ready to oblige in contacting his confederates on my behalf. However, after what appeared to be a long, stressful day for him, I stood by as he read my letter of permission, which declared my search for: people who have been research confederates in psychological research where their roles required them to deceive and/or mistreat research participants in some way. He stated that he believed that in the above statement, I was referring to him as unethical. He proceeded to say he would speak to his colleague, but expected she would have the same reaction he did which was “where the fuck does this kid get off?” He also blamed me for entering research with a bias. I was taken aback at his reactions, and recorded mine in my reflexivity journal: He believed my letter seeking permission to contact his participants referred to his work as unethical and that it states a preconceived notion of my results and people’s reactions. I hope I can mend this relationship so that I can get some participants. Otherwise, finishing my thesis does not look like a promising prospect. Confederates’ Experiences 28 The relationship was not mended, but I was able to find participants without his help. I did learn that wording and language is instrumental in guiding research. From that point on, I began to refer to participants’ roles as having “potentially caused participants harm or distress.” This change was officially made in an addendum to the poster and any other relevant materials. Confederates’ Experiences 29 Results The analysis of the transcribed interviews revealed that being a research confederate had both positive and negative aspects. As illustrated in Figure 1, participants’ overall attitude toward being as a research confederate can be conceptualized as a resulting value on a scale after the positive aspects of their experiences have placed on one side of the scale and the negative aspects on the other. On the one hand, these research confederates experienced a number of Negative Feelings as a result of the Challenges (e.g., performing their role, acting negatively toward participants) and Concerns (regarding the experiment and the participants) they encountered. On the other hand, these negative feelings were often counterbalanced by confederates’ Belief in the Necessity of the Methods used in the study (e.g., the use of deception; regarding the ends as justifying the means), the way in which their Faith in the Scientific Process (i.e., their confidence in Ethics Review Boards, the researcher and experimenter, and the debriefing process) offset their sense of personal responsibility, Improvement in their Performance over time and their use of Personalized Coping Strategies, as well as by the positivity of confederates’ Learning Experiences. Each of these concepts is outlined in detail below. The favourability of participants’ experiences as a research confederate was largely determined by the nature of the role they were required to enact. Confederates whose role involved acting in ways that could potentially distress or harm the participants (over and above any harm stemming from the use of deception) reported experiencing more negative feelings and ultimately felt ambiguous about their experience as a confederate. Sarah Smith, for example, said “I mean, it wasn’t a bad experience, but it wasn’t exactly a pleasant one.” In contrast, confederates whose role did not require acting in ways that could potentially distress Confederates’ Experiences 30 or harm the participants experienced fewer negative feelings that, together with the positivity of their learning experiences, resulted in slightly positive experiences as a confederate. For example, Bob’s slightly positive attitude was evident when he said, “I thought it was cool to be a research confederate, you know, the whole excitement of doing something. Um, but other than that, it had no personal effect [on me] whatsoever.” Table 1 presents an overview of all participants’ overall experiences, in relation with the potential for harm that their role Figure 1. Model of confederates’ experiences; scale balancing negative and positive experiences. Table 2 Participants’ overall experiences and the roles they enacted while interacting with research participants. Jake Alice Sarah Laura Pants Kuzco Bob Sarah Smith Confederates’ Experiences 31 Role Make sexist remarks Provide support or not Provide support or not Provide support or not Act angry or happy to negotiations Plan a day Engage in conversation Induce anxiety Overall Attitude Ambiguous “Indifferent” “Really good” Ambiguous Ambiguous Easy “Easy, with difficult aspects” Ambiguous Challenges Participants described a number of challenges associated with being a research confederate, some of which were encountered regardless of confederates’ roles and others that were unique to the particular role that a participant was required to enact. Jake, a 24-year-old in the first year of a Ph.D. program in psychology, described the challenging nature of being a confederate in general terms when he said: ...it’s not easy, you know, being a confederate’s never easy. Um, people don’t necessarily like doing it, you know what I mean? Often you’re put in awkward situations, awkward social situations especially. And so it’s always somewhat hard on the confederate, um, you just have to be able to deal with it, and cope with it, you know, your confederate has an outlet. Jake had been involved in a study that required him to act as if he was another research participant and to make sexist comments to female participants or, in the control condition, state that he had a previous engagement and was unable to continue his participation in the study. Not only did he describe the challenges that make being a confederate difficult, as evident above, he also hinted that some intervention may be required to deal with these difficulties. In addition to the general challenges participants faced as confederates, they also described more specific challenges, including acting-related challenges and challenges posed by the research participants. Confederates’ Experiences 32 Acting-related challenges. A confederate's role involves acting as directed by the researcher while appearing to interact spontaneously and sincerely with the research participants. Some participants described the need to accomplish these goals as a test of their improvisational skills. In this regard Sarah, a 19-year-old in her 3rd year of a B.A. program in anthropology, said “you had to be able to interact with people and learn a script, but also be a little bit flexible at the same time.” Participants described the need for such flexibility as challenging because it required reconciling the potentially contradictory demands of adhering to a relatively rigid script so as to ensure that they enacted their role in a way that had the intended effect on participants while simultaneously interacting with the participants, both verbally and nonverbally, in a spontaneous and genuine way that did not arouse their suspicion. Bob described his encounter with these potentially irreconcilable demands while interacting with the actual research participant as if he was another participant waiting for the study to begin: There was a structured list [of lines], um, 'Are you here for a study?' 'What year are you in?' You know, 'what major?' Just general questions to try to get them to talk about themselves a little bit. But, it was structured to the point where like I'd ask a question and then have to wait a minute, and I couldn't continue the conversation. I had to wait for them to, you know, kind of keep it going. And then a minute later, I'd ask the next question, kind of structure it along. Bob’s script required him not to provide the participant with information about himself and not to ask any questions that did not appear in the script. In addition, Bob was instructed to ensure that there was one minute between each question, even when participants’ responses were very brief. Given this inflexibility, Bob found it difficult to interact with the participants in a way that appeared genuine. Confederates’ Experiences 33 Enacting a particular role was also described as challenging when it involved acting in a way that was personally uncharacteristic or atypical. Alice, a 20-year-old in the third year of a double major in political science and law, put it this way: When I was aggressive, which was pretty hard, I had to make them feel bad about the way they reacted. So I’d say, ‘How could you let [the other confederate] talk to you like that? That's so awful! You're an insult to all women, everywhere,’ and stuff like that. It was pretty hard ... It was very difficult to play the unsupportive role, I found, because the woman, it was – I felt like this whole hoax was on this one participant, and she was the only nonconfederate in that study. Which was, … it was just difficult, I found. A conflict of interest in the way that I wanted to be sympathetic to her and I wanted to be supportive, but I had to play a jerk. Participants also found it difficult to act in ways that made participants feel badly. Alice articulated this difficulty, as well as its salience for her, when she said: I remember I was supposed to say stuff like, ‘I can’t believe you would let him talk to you like.’ I remember that one. Uh, ‘why would you let him do that?’ Asking questions like that, making them feel awful, those were the ones that I remember because they were harder for me to say. Alice’s comment also suggests that these confederates’ negative experiences may have more impact on them than any positive experiences they had. Participant-related challenges. The participants also described being challenged by the research participants’ personalities and the unpredictability of their reactions. While they found it quite easy to interact with some research participants, interacting with other participants was more difficult. Sarah Smith, a 21-year-old in the first year of an M.A. program in psychology whose role involved making eye contact with research participants known to be socially anxious, described participants who had strong, negative reactions to her Confederates’ Experiences 34 eye contact as more difficult to be involved with than those who did not have such reactions. As she explained, this difficulty came from the fact that she felt badly for participants who reacted negatively: Like, it was easier with some people because they were easy people, because some people, like the ones who kept laughing and stuff like that. It was hard for me as another [supposed] participant I wanna say, to not laugh with them because naturally you would laugh, but I was instructed to not laugh ... [it also depended on] how anxious they were. Some people were, like, one girl looked like she was going to cry, and like I can't just not feel bad when you're inducing such high anxiety levels in someone. And then other people were just fine. They were just as calm as I was, as relaxed as I was. Concerns Participants described being challenged by the acting skills required of them and adverse reactions to their behaviour, things that were regarded as challenging because they were related to research confederates’ concerns for the validity of experiment and the wellbeing of the research participants. Concern for the experiment. Confederates perform their roles well when the intended manipulation is enacted without raising participants’ suspicions regarding the true purpose of the study. Under these circumstances, participants are likely to act spontaneously and naturally and provide meaningful data. When confederates do not perform well in that the intended manipulation is not enacted effectively and/or the participants become suspicious about the purpose of the study, participants may act in ways that are either consistent or inconsistent with what they believe is expected of them. Such data is at best meaningless and, at worst, may lead to erroneous conclusions. Because they did not want to be responsible for Confederates’ Experiences 35 “ruining the experiment,” these confederates were very concerned about performing their roles well. Alice, for example, talked about worrying about raising participants’ suspicions: …yeah it was uncomfortable to do it at times, because you didn't know how believable it was for the participant. Or you doubted if they knew. You know what I mean? You'd always question: ‘Do they know?’ Do they know I'm a confederate?’ Um, and yeah, it was really hard. Kuzco, a 21-year-old in the fourth year of a B.Sc. program in psychology, was also concerned that she might “mess up the results of the study.” Acting as another research participant, Kuzco’s role involved maintaining a neutral demeanor while planning a day together with the actual research participant. Following this interaction, Kuzco rated the extent to which the participant displayed various personality characteristics during the interaction. She regarded her role as an integral part of the study and had been warned that not performing her role effectively could have adverse effects on the results of the study. Given this warning, it is perhaps not surprising that she said, So I guess when I was doing that, I was like, 'Hmm, am I gonna like offend somebody or like mess up the results of the study because they didn't act a certain way or didn't act not a certain way?’ Concern for the participants. Participants were concerned about the well-being of the actual research participants because they realized that some participants might be distressed by the use of deception and/or how they were treated during the experiment. Kuzco expressed her concern for participants by saying: It was basically like I, you know, I'd finish up my session and I'd be walking back to my car or something and I'd be like, 'Hmm,' you know, a little – It was almost like I wondered how they felt about it because they knew that it was basically all pretend. So that was more my kind of concern there. Confederates’ Experiences 36 Kuzco indicated that her concerns consisted of only fleeting thoughts that occurred as she finished her research work for the day. However, earlier in the interview Kuzco talked about feeling badly for some of the participants because the deception used in the study entirely negated the connection she had created with them. She described this by saying “I kinda felt like I made a connection with them so then I kind of negated that because, like, I'm just here, pretending, kind of thing.” The fact that the connection with the participant was created in situations that were part of a hoax contributed to a feeling that the connection would be lost when the hoax was revealed. This led to feeling bad about those situations where they experienced connections with participants. Feeling badly was most frequently associated with confederate roles that required acting in ways that could potentially distress or harm the research participants. Sarah Smith described feeling badly when she increased participants’ levels of anxiety: It was awkward. Like some of the people, like, I felt bad sometimes because you could tell they were feeling really awkward, like some of them would fidget and couldn't maintain eye contact or they'd laugh. So I felt bad because I didn't want to make people feel anxious. Witnessing their overt signs of anxiety first hand, Sarah Smith had compunctions about being responsible for participants’ anxiety and was concerned about their well-being. Negative Feelings The challenges that confederates encountered as they enacted their roles contributed to their concerns. These concerns, in turn, instilled a number of negative feelings. All of the confederates reported feeling negatively about at least one aspect of their involvement. For example, performing their role and performing potentially distress-inducing actions made some confederates feel awkward. Laura, a 20-year-old in the third year of a political science Confederates’ Experiences 37 B.A., described feeling awkward while acting negatively toward a female research participant because it was inconsistent with her normal behaviour. She said, “I felt really awkward doing it ... when it was unsupportive, I had such a hard time. Like, I had to always remind myself that I was acting, definitely.” Other participants described feeling nervous about their acting ability within the context of their concern for the experiment. They also described feeling nervous about acting in ways that could potentially distress or harm the participants within the context of their concern regarding the participants’ well-being. For Jake, these two sources of feeling nervous converged: Basically I had to do yelling at a certain time period when the stopwatch came to, I think 8 minutes. I had a little stopwatch in there while I was [performing the task]. So at the 8 minute mark is when I would yell. And towards that 8 minute mark, I would always get very nervous. My heart would start beating, I'd start breathing a little bit heavier. I would actually be worried that the participant would find out that I was nervous. So, it was very hard, towards that mark, to start yelling at them. I would often times have to just, I don't know, fake to myself that I didn't like the way [the participant was acting] so that I would have the energy to get angry at them. Of all of the roles enacted by the participants in this study, the role enacted by Jake probably had the most potential to distress or harm the research participants. Given this, it is not surprising that Jake was sufficiently nervous to increase his heart and respiration rates. Although such intense nervousness may be rare, Jake’s experience provides some insight into just how challenging a research confederate’s role can be. Counteracting Negative Feelings Several factors counteracted any negative feelings participants had as a result of being a research confederate. These factors included confederates’ belief in the necessity of the Confederates’ Experiences 38 methods used in the study, their faith in the scientific process, the improvement in their performance over time, and their use of personalized coping strategies. Belief in the necessity of the methods. Confederates are used in experiments to implement a manipulation and, because participants must be naive to this reality, deception is inherent to any study that involves confederates. The participants in this study believed that researchers needed to employ confederates and use deception in order to study some phenomena, and these beliefs counteracted any negative feelings they had regarding their involvement in these procedures. Pants, a 26-year-old in her fourth year of a B.A. program in psychology, talked about how the study she was involved in could not have been conducted without the use of deception: I have a hard time with [deception]. Like, it’s starting to kind of get better, which may or may not be a good thing, but like, I still think it’s a hard thing to do…the negotiation study might have worked if – maybe not, if you told people you were looking at how they react to somebody's emotional statements. It probably wouldn't have worked, but there might have been – they're looking at, again, emotional reaction and how important the initial attitude is on – like how it affects how you argue. So I don't know, I personally can't think of a better way to do what they did. Pants’ role involved negotiating with participants and acting either angry or happy in response to the participant’s offers during the negotiation. Because of the difficulties she had while enacting her role as a confederate, Pants wrestled with whether there was an alternative to using deception. Ultimately she concluded that the use of deception was justified because she believed that the results would have been tainted if the participants had known the true purpose of the research. Confederates’ Experiences 39 Some participants also believed that the results of the experiment were important and justified using research confederates and deception, as well as potentially distressing or harming the participants. Bob put it this way: ...there were some times where, um, people really didn't want to talk and I still had to push the boundaries. Um, those were the few times where I actually thought, ‘Mmm...maybe this isn't the best thing to be doing’ thing, but at least if it generates, you know, research data, stuff like that, you know, that might help other people. That kind of stuff. Although Bob felt somewhat negatively about pushing participants' personal boundaries, these feelings were alleviated by his belief that he was contributing to science and his hope that the results of the study would be helpful for others. Faith in the scientific process. While believing that confederates and deception were necessary for scientific progress helped to reduce any negative feelings participants had as a result of their personal concerns, these beliefs did not necessarily reduce any negative feelings associated with their concerns regarding the well-being of the participants in the research they were involved with. Instead these research confederates’ concerns regarding participants' well-being were alleviated by their faith in the scientific process, faith that included their confidence in the procedures of Ethics Review Boards, their confidence in the competence of the research and the experimenter, and their confidence in the effectiveness of debriefing procedures. Moreover, confederates’ faith in the scientific process also reduced any sense of personal responsibility they might have felt for participants’ distress. The participants in this study had a great deal of respect for Ethics Review Boards and were confident that the members of such boards acted in ways that ensured the well-being of the participants in the research they were involved with. Sarah explained her trust in the ethics committee: Confederates’ Experiences 40 I guess, simply put, it was approved by the ethics committee so people who know more about it were – approved it. I guess that given the fact that it was a very short experience and that at any point, she could say, you know, ‘I really don't feel like continuing the study.’ Or um, that kind of thing. Sarah held the ethics committee in a high regard, believing that they had a good understanding of the potential effects of the study on research participants and how any potential negative effects could be minimized and addressed. She also recognized that safeguards ensuring participants' well-being are required in order to receive the approval of an Ethics Review Board. As a result, the fact that an Ethics Review Board had approved the study she was a confederate in essentially eliminated any concerns Sarah had regarding participants’ wellbeing. The confederates’ also described their confidence in the researcher’s ability to design an ethically sound study and the experimenter’s (i.e., research assistant’s) ability to conduct the study appropriately as alleviating any concerns or negative feelings they had about their role-related behaviour. Jake’s faith in the researchers and research assistants involved in the study he participated in was evident when he said: …those times when I didn't come back in, I think, I'm pretty sure the experimenter could handle the situation, could debrief without me. I saw her debrief a couple of times and she was very good at it. … I have faith that, you know, that the people who designed the study thought about [the procedure] and came up with the best way to do it. And that was, unfortunately, me yelling at [the participants]. And it was bad at first, but it eventually got better. Although the method used to manipulate the independent variable was somewhat extreme (i.e., having the confederate yell at the participants), Jake believed that that the researcher who designed the study was competent and, as a result, selected a way of manipulating the independent variable that was both effective and appropriate. Jake also believed that the Confederates’ Experiences 41 experimenter (i.e., the research assistant) was capable of effectively debriefing or de-hoaxing the participants in a way that overcame any distress the participants may have experienced. Taken together, these beliefs reduced Jake’s concerns and negative feelings about the effects of his role-related behaviour on the participants. Many of these confederates expressed confidence that their study’s debriefing procedures effectively removed any distress or harm the participants may have experienced. Laura, for example, believed that the study she was involved with would have minimal negative effects on the research participants and that these effects would stem from issues related to the use of deception rather than her role-related behaviour. Laura’s role involved pretending to be a research participant and either providing support or not providing support to female research participants who had faced discrimination. Laura also believed that the debriefing procedure would help participants understand why the deception was necessary and reduce any ill feelings on the part of the participant. As she put it, I think [the study is] still ethical, because it's all explained afterwards. It's obviously nothing that's personal and I feel for most people that wouldn't affect them later on, other than just feeling like, ‘Oh I get it. I didn't see that coming ahead of time.’ ... The debriefing process was full and explained everything and kind of rectified these weird things [i.e., negative effects felt by participants or issues in how the participant perceived the confederate] that came up. Bob also believed that the debriefing removed any negative effects the study had on the participants but, like Jake, he was primarily concerned about any distress or misperceptions that occurred as a result of his role-related behaviour: But you know, hopefully everything will be later explained and the whole process will take care of that. And once they find out, ‘Ooh, ok, it was a job,’ or something like that, you know. Most people were fine with it. Confederates’ Experiences 42 Confederates’ faith in the scientific process reduced any concerns or negative feelings they had about the contribution of their role-related behaviour to participants’ distress. Specifically, believing that their study had been examined and approved by the members of a well-informed, competent Ethics Review Board, that the study had been designed by a competent researcher and conducted by a competent experimenter, and that any problematic experiences for the participants would be dealt with effectively during the debriefing procedure allowed the confederates to minimize any sense of being personally responsible for any distress or harm experienced by the research participants. Alice described her lack of personal responsibility for any negative effects the participants experienced when she stated, “Of course [having others involved in the research] was going to alleviate the guilt. I didn’t plan it. I wasn’t the one telling myself to be unsupportive because, like I said before, I was sincerely like ‘oooo, this is really awful.’” Although Alice did not enjoy acting in way that distressed the participants, she did not accept any responsibility for participants’ distress because she did not design the study. Instead, she assigned this responsibility to the researchers who designed the study and told her how to act during the study. Improvement of performance. Confederates’ concerns about their ability to enact the role required of them, which often involved acting in ways that were alien to their normal reallife behaviour, dissipated with practice. Pants described how enacting her role became easier over time: The first time I had to get really angry I was really nervous. I was like, ‘shit, I don’t know if I’m going to do this well enough,’ or like I was afraid of how the people were going to react. Um, but then by the time – like, we ran it with 80 people – by the time we hit 25, it was just like I was coming to work. So it was a lot easier to act. Confederates’ Experiences 43 Like many of the confederates, Pants was initially nervous because she feared that she would not be able to act convincingly and because she was afraid of the participants’ reactions. With practice, however, her performance improved and her nervousness subsided. Indeed, that enacting her role became like “coming to work” suggests that she became desensitized or habituated to her role. Laura also described comparable improvements in her performance of being mean and unsupportive: Yep, at first I was hardly able to be that unsupportive. Like I would kind of water it down and be just sort of mediocre supportive and [the researchers would] be like, ‘that’s not helpful for this study.’ But yeah … I got meaner in the unsupportive role over time. Practice and the habituation that resulted from the repetitive re-enactment of their role reduced confederates’ concerns and anxiety regarding their ability to effectively enact the role required of them. Any remaining negative feelings would have to be dealt with in a manner that was unique to each confederate. Personalized coping strategies. Believing in the necessity of the methods (i.e., in the need for deception and that the ends justify the means), having faith in the scientific process (i.e., their confidence in Ethics Review Boards, the researcher and the experimenter, and the debriefing procedures), and the increasing ease with which they enacted their role-related behaviours reduced many of the concerns and negative feelings that these confederates shared. Other issues were unique to the particular role enacted by a confederate and, as a result, the confederates developed their own personalized way of reducing these negative feelings. Sarah Smith described finding excuses to reduce her negative feelings about the effects of her role-related behaviour on the research participants: Confederates’ Experiences 44 ...[staring at the participant] would cause the person anxiety. It’s just with me always being the person it just kept it consistent, because if you had two really high anxious people, they’d both be laughing or whatever. Um, so yeah, I found excuses why not to feel bad. Excuses helped Sarah Smith feel better about increasing anxiety levels in already socially anxious participants. Her excuses included recalling that participants entered the laboratory with preexisting anxiety. This helped her feel less responsible for their anxiety, resulting in less negative feelings for her. She explained this saying, “They’re naturally anxious people. We knew that with them coming in, uh, so that would help, kind of taking the blame off of me and put it on the situation.” Pants had been involved in negotiating with participants. Her role required her to respond either happily or angrily towards participants’ offers during negotiations. Pants described how she deliberately ignored the participants’ reactions because their reactions made it difficult for her to perform her role: …sometimes during the study [participants would] be like, ‘okay, relax, like, it’s just an experiment. And then you’d be like ‘whatever!’ or ‘you suck’ [sarcastically] or whatever. I’d have to kind of brush it off and still not laugh at myself because it was absurd what I was doing. Sometimes Pants felt that her responses were unjustified given the offers participants were giving during negotiations. This led to Pants’ need to ignore participants’ reactions while trying to enact her role. Jake talked about a poor initial experience that made him decide to meet with participants: I felt really bad about that first experience. So, needless to say, for the next little while, I was very, um, very nervous about doing the study. Um, I didn’t really want to do it, but I knew I had to be very careful. And I decided then, Confederates’ Experiences 45 that I would come back in afterwards. I would stick around and make sure that participants were okay after because I felt it was important that I make sure that what I did to them didn’t affect them badly, in any sort of psychological way. Jake actively made the decision to meet with participants at the conclusion of the experiment. This served to further the debriefing of the participant by appeasing and assuring them that he was enacting a role and the discriminatory remarks he had made were untrue. Jake’s concerns for the participants were also addressed through this reconciliation. Such personalized strategies helped reduced the unique role-related difficulties these confederates encountered and thereby reduced the overall negativity of their experiences as a research confederate. Learning Experience The confederates did not describe being a research confederate in solely negative terms. To the contrary, they also described being a confederate as a positive learning experience. Some described gaining valuable research experience and learning about research methods, while others spoke of the lessons they learned from the topic of the research. Sarah described the research experience she gained from her involvement: ...at the time I was considering doing a double major in anthropology and psychology and that kind of pushed me towards doing the psych aspect because it was like a hands-on experience and you got to learn more about how it actually worked rather than always just the theory. Although Sarah was only in her first year of university when she was confederate, she had already learned about the research process and different psychological theories in her classes. Being a research confederate, however, provided her with a more practical and experiential basis for her knowledge of psychology. Confederates’ Experiences 46 Kuzco described how being a confederate provided her with access to an insider’s rather than outsider’s perspective regarding experimental research methods, knowledge that would be useful to her in her future studies. I figured, hey, it's good experience if I'm gonna do an honours thesis at some point. So I, you know, I can kind of see how some of how the experiment works from a different point of view, as opposed to being just a participant. It was just kind of like, ‘Oh, so this is the way you can do experiments.’ Interestingly, Kuzco’s revelation about research methods was due to her experience of being deceived by the researcher. She described her reactions when she learned that the researcher had kept her “blind” to the true purpose of the study saying: I guess there was more to it, but I didn't know that until the end… it turns out that the participants would be assigned to act introverted or extraverted and I didn't know that so sometimes I'd, you know, I'd be like “why is this person being so shy or why are they, like, really happy and extraverted?” She had noticed participants were acting suspiciously introverted or extraverted, and only learned at the conclusion of her involvement that they had been asked to act introverted or extraverted. Being a confederate taught her about the utility of research personnel who are “blind” to the purpose and full procedure of experiments. In addition to learning about the research methods used in psychology, these confederates also described learning about the psychological topic of the study they were involved with and how this knowledge was useful for them. For example, Pants said: It let me kind of learn a little bit more about negotiation and how to get what I want from people, just because this was what the study was based on. I had done a little bit of literature review before I came to kind of know a little more about the study. So now I know how to win an argument [laughter]. Confederates’ Experiences 47 Similarly, Sarah Smith described immediately applying what she learned about rumination in the course of being a research confederate. In particular, she described learning that she should not ruminate about the anxiety she felt as a result of her role-required behaviour, which involved making the research participants feel anxious. As she put it, “our research was on rumination and stuff like that, so I knew not to ruminate on how I feel.” Hence being a research confederate was described as beneficial in that it increased participants’ knowledge regarding the practical details of psychological research methods and the psychological phenomenon under investigation in personally useful ways. Negative Case These confederates described their experiences as having both positive and negative aspects and, overall, the participants had slightly positive feelings about their experience unless their role required them acting in a way that distressed the research participants, in which case they had ambiguous attitudes about being a research confederate. Sarah’s experience as a research confederate was largely, but not completely, consistent with these findings. Sarah described her experience as a research confederate as “really good, considering that I was manipulating people. [laughs] Um, yeah, no, it was an enjoyable learning experience.” As a confederate, Sarah was required to be supportive or unsupportive to female participants who had faced sexual discrimination. Sarah was the only confederate in the present research who reported being able to empathize with the participants and their reactions. As she explained, this probably occurred because of the sexual discrimination she had encountered in her own life: You know, I’ve been confronted with situations that are kind of like, “wow” and you know an hour later you think of this really good comeback. But, like, Confederates’ Experiences 48 in that case you’re struck with silence. It was interesting to actually see that happen with other people and to relate to it, I guess. Sarah also indicated that being involved in the study provided her with a learning experience. As she explained, her involvement “kind of made me think, you know, if I am confronted with this, maybe that’s something that should be confronted by myself, not lying down and taking it.” Although Sarah was concerned about the distressing effect that her rolerelated behaviour had on the research participants, she regarded her experience as a research confederate as positive because being a confederate taught her an important life lesson, namely that she ought to confront instances of sexual discrimination. Moreover, despite the distress that participants endured during the study, Sarah believed that the experiment was beneficial to the participants because it provided them with the same useful learning experience: Well it definitely is a learning experience, not only for myself, but also for the other person who’s coming in. To know that you should be able to say something and you should speak up for yourself whether … what they say is correct or not. Although believing that the experiment provided participants with a useful learning experience may have been a way of defensively discounting the distress that participants experienced as a result of her role-related behaviour, this belief allowed Sarah to feel positive about what she did as a research confederate. Confederates’ Experiences 49 Discussion The present research is the first to offer systematic, grounded insight into the nature of people’s experiences being research confederates. As research confederates, the participants in this study had enacted a number of roles, ranging from engaging in conversation with participants to deliberately inducing anxiety in participants. All of the participants, regardless of their role, experienced at least some distress in the course of enacting their role. Initially, this distress stemmed from their concerns about their ability to adequately enact their role, a concern also noted in Oliansky’s (1991) autobiographical account. Fortunately, this concern was addressed by improvements in role performance that came with practice, as well by the desensitization or habituation associated with having enacted the role on countless occasions. Consistent with the variability in their roles, these confederates also described qualitatively different experiences as research confederates. Those whose role did not require them to act in ways that had the potential to distress or harm research participants generally described their experiences as being slightly positive, largely because they regarded it as a learning experience. In particular, confederates said they learned about both how to conduct psychological research and the subject matter of the research. In contrast, confederates whose roles required them to act in ways that could distress or harm the participants described their experiences as having both positive and negative elements and, as a result, were ambiguous about having been a research confederate. The positivity of their experiences stemmed from what they learned in the course of being a research confederate while the negativity of their experiences arose from the various challenges they faced, particularly those associated with acting negatively toward research participants within the context of their role. A number of beliefs helped confederates Confederates’ Experiences 50 overcome the negative feelings associated with their role related behaviour. These beliefs included their belief that the methods used to conduct the experiment were necessary to study the phenomenon in question, that the ends justify the means, and their faith in the scientific process. Finally, these confederates dealt with any remaining negative affect by developing their own, personalized coping strategies. Interestingly, participants’ discussions of their beliefs suggest that, contrary to Kelman’s (1967) concern that deception “is used without question” (p. 3), confederates are aware of and think about the ethical issues associated with the use of deception. Nevertheless, by having faith in the scientific process, which included faith in the appraisal of the Ethics Review Board, faith in the competence of the researcher and the experimenter, and faith in the ability of the debriefing to sooth participants’ distress, these confederates were able to shift the responsibility for any negative effects associated with their role-related behaviour from themselves to others. In this sense, it does appear that research confederates engage in the role-required behaviours researchers ask of them in much the same way as the participants in Milgram’s study followed the experimenter’s instructions to punish and thereby distress the supposed “learner.” On the surface, the experiences of the confederates in the current study appear to be inconsistent Oliansky (1991), who noted that “[e]veryone who was involved in the experiment was, and continues to be, doubtful and guilty about their part in the study” (p. 256). This inconsistency, however, is more apparent than real in view of the fact that Oliansky did not share the beliefs that allowed the current participants to offset their negative feelings. For example, in regard to the belief that the ends justify the means, Oliansky (1991) wrote, “in the context of psychological experimentation, however, the concept of a cost-benefit ratio is Confederates’ Experiences 51 ridiculous, because harm to the subjects cannot be balanced against benefit to the science” (p. 257). Given this, the negativity of Oliansky’s (1991) experience is consistent with the additive model proposed here, whereby a person’s experience as a research confederate is a function of the sum of both the positive and negative aspects of those experiences and experience-relevant beliefs. Laurens and Moscovici’s (2005) “self-conversion” research in which confederates altered their colour perceptions in ways consistent with the position they were required to argue as part of their role has potentially serious implications if the findings generalize to the other types of role-required behaviours. For example, the self-conversion effect suggests that, to reduce the cognitive dissonance stemming from their role-related behaviour, Jake might come to believe the sexist comments he made to female participants, Alice might ultimately regard the female participants as being the “insult to women” that she accused them of being, and Pants might end up believing that she really was angry or happy about a participant’s offers during the negotiation. The extent to which the self-conversion process generalizes to confederates’ behaviour more generally, then, clearly merits investigation. Relatedly, one might also wonder how research confederates are affected by their deceptive behaviour toward research participants. In this regard Pants recalled her partner’s concern, saying: I was talking to [my partner] and she was saying because this study had deception and the study that I’m doing now has deception like, ‘Now you’re going to be like this great liar, and I don’t know, like, I’m never gonna know when you’re telling the truth or not.’ She goes, ‘You think you’re gonna start lying all the time now? Like because you lie so much, it’s gonna make it seem like it’s an okay behaviour.’ Which is a weird thing to say, like, it never occurred to me that would happen, but it’s an interesting observation. Confederates’ Experiences 52 The possibility that the self-conversion observed by Laurens and Moscovici (2005) may extend lying as part of a confederate’s role seems all the more likely in view of confederates accounts of how habituation made the deception easier for them to perform. As noted earlier, a number of confederates used their own, personalized strategies to cope with the negative aspects of their experiences, particularly aspects associated to their deceptive roles. Of note is that some of the participants, specifically some of those who had the time, described using Milgram’s (1963) “a friendly reconciliation” (p. 374) procedure. Because little if any research has assessed the extent to which such reconciliations are actually beneficial to research confederates and participants, research in this area is warranted. The current findings have a number of implications for research confederates, researchers and professional bodies overseeing psychological research. Given that research confederates appear to offset any negative feelings stemming from their role-related behaviour by having faith in the scientific process, research confederates might endeavour to ensure that their faith is not misplaced by ensuring that the research they are involved with has received the approval of an Ethics Review Board, that the experimenter has been trained adequately, and that debriefing procedures are thorough and effective. The current findings also suggest the need for researchers to forewarn potential confederates of the negative feelings they may encounter as a result of their role-related behaviour, that they provide sufficient training to offset research confederates’ performance-related anxieties, and that they monitor confederates’ experiences and affect throughout the research process in ways that ensure their well-being. In addition, these findings suggest that professional bodies overseeing psychological research, such as the American Psychological Association and the Interagency Advisory Panel on Research Ethics (within Canada) might usefully develop ethical guidelines Confederates’ Experiences 53 for research confederates and incorporate safeguards for research confederates’ well-being into any revisions of existing ethical guidelines. Like all research, the current research has a number of strengths and weaknesses that qualify any conclusions. As a novice researcher, I faced a number of methodological challenges in the course of conducting this research. One of these challenges was interviewing. For example, along the way I learned not to do any interviews after having spent a day at work and being too tired to attend to and pursue what participants said. Although a novice, I am pleased to report that when I asked the participants how they found the interview, how I had done, and whether I had concentrated too much on a particular topic, all of them felt positively and did not offer any criticism. Due to time constraints and the uncooperativeness of researchers who would not allow me access to their research confederates, theoretical saturation was not achieved for all of the potential categories that emerged. However, very few new categories emerged during the final interviews and only those categories that were fully developed were used to construct the model outline in this research. Categories that still required elaboration were that of confederates’ helpful previous experiences, confederates’ requirement for closure, and confederates’ beliefs in participants’ responsibility to take care of themselves. Conceivably, these could be described as Personalized Coping Strategies. Other limits stemmed from the fact that I did all of the interviewing, transcription, coding, and analysis. As a result, this research was based on only my perspective, without potential benefits of depth and breadth afforded by additional perspectives. In an attempt to address this limitation, after coding all the interviews, I spent some time away from the data. When I returned, I recoded all of the interviews to capture the essence of each interview in the Confederates’ Experiences 54 most comprehensive manner. This process made it easier for me to engage in the method of constant comparisons (Glaser, 1965). After developing a thorough understanding of each interview, I then returned to the interviews and, with my knowledge of the other interviews, I was better able to identify and develop relevant codes and categories. This iterative process, I believe, contributed to a more thorough analysis and trustworthy findings that are well grounded in the reality of the participants’ experiences. Confederates’ Experiences 55 References Adair, J.G., Dushenko, T.W., & Lindsay, R.C.L. (1985). Ethical regulations and their impact on research practice. American Psychologist, 40(1), 59-72. American Psychological Association (1982). Ethical principles in the conduct of research with human participants. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. American Psychological Association (1992). Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. Retrieved August 11, 2009, from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/code1992.aspx American Psychological Association (2000). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. In B.D. Sales & S. Folkman (Eds.) Ethics in research with human participants (pp. 129-160). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. American Psychological Association (2002). Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. Retrieved August 11, 2009, from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx# Anderson, C.A., Lepper, M.R., & Ross, L. (1980). Perseverance of social theories: The role of explanation in the persistence of discredited information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1037-1049.Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P.C., Carlsmith, J.M., & Gonzales, M.H. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology (2nd ed.). Toronto: McGraw-Hill Publishing Company. Asch, S.E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.) Groups, leadership and men: Research in human relations (pp. 177-190). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. Baron, R.A. (1981). The 'costs of deception' revisited: An openly optimistic rejoinder. IRB: Ethics and Human Research, 3(1), 8-10. Confederates’ Experiences 56 Bartlett, M.Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science, 17(4), 319-325. Batson, C.D. (1998). Altruism and prosocial behaviour. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 282-316). New York: McGraw-Hill. Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics of research: After reading Milgram's “Behavioral study of obedience.” American Psychologist, 19(6), 421-423. Baumrind, D. (1979). IRBs and social science research: The costs of deception. IRB: Ethics and Human Research, 1(6), 1-4. Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (1998 with 2000, 2002, and 2005 amendments). Tri-council policy statement: Ethical conduct for research involving humans. Retrieved from http://www.pre.ethics.gc.ca/policypolitique/tcps-eptc/docs/TCPS%20October%202005_E.pdf Geller, D.M. (1978). Involvement in role-playing situations: A demonstration with studies on obedience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(3), 219-235. Glaser, B.G. (1965). The constant comparative method of qualitative analysis. Social Problems, 12(4), 436-445. Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company. Guéguen, N. (2007). The effect of modeling on tipping behavior. Studia Psychologica,49(3), 275-282. Confederates’ Experiences 57 Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implicatons for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kelman, H.C. (1967). Human use of human subjects: The problem of deception in social psychological experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 67(1), 1-11. Laurens, S., & Moscovici S. (2005). The confederate’s and others’ self-conversion: A neglected phenomenon. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145(2), 191-207. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371-378. Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. London: Tavistock Publications Ltd. Miller, A.G. (1972). Role playing: An alternative to deception? A review of the evidence. American Psychologist, 27(7), 623-636. Oczak, M., & Niedźwienska, A. (2007). Debriefing in deceptive research: A proposed new procedure. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 2(3), 49-59. Oliansky, A. (1991). A confederate’s perspective on deception. Ethics & Behavior, 1(4), 253258. Ponterotto, J.G., & Grieger, I. (2007). Effectively communicating qualitative research. The Counseling Psychologist, 35(3), 404-430. Rubin, H.J., & Rubin, I.S. (1995). Qualitative Interviewing: The Art of Hearing Data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sieber, J.E. (1983). Deception in social research III: The nature and limits of debriefing. IRB: Ethics and Human Research, 5(3), 1-4. Confederates’ Experiences 58 Smith, S.S. & Richardson, D. (1983). Amelioration of deception and harm in psychological research: The important role of debriefing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(5), 1075-1082. Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. Tangney, J. (2000). Training. In B.D. Sales & S. Folkman (Eds.), Ethics in research with human participants (pp. 97-105). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Vinacke, W.E. (1954). Deceiving experimental subjects. American Psychologist, 9(4), 155. Willis, R.H., & Willis, Y.A. (1970). Role playing versus deception: An experimental comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(3), 472-477. Confederates’ Experiences 59 Appendix A. Email to Psychology Department Chair for Faculty Members and Graduate Students Dear Faculty Member/Graduate Student Researcher; My name is Elliott Lee. I am an undergraduate student in the Psychology Department at [name of university]. For my B.A. thesis research, I am conducting 45 minute interviews with people who have been research confederates in psychological research that involved any form of deception (i.e., where the question regarding the use of deception on the ethics application was answered ‘yes’). I am interested in confederates’ experiences in such research, how they felt about being a confederate in research that used deception (be it positive, negative or neutral), and the basis of their feelings. In order to obtain participants, I would be grateful if you would forward the e-mail message that appears on the next page to your former and current research confederates. Alternatively, you are welcome to send me the names and e-mail addresses of your former and current confederates so that I can e-mail them myself [email address]. Note that, to facilitate the anonymity of everyone involved in the research, the names of any individuals mentioned by the participants, the name of the university where the research was conducted, and any identifying details regarding the particular research project will not be provided in any written or verbal presentations of the findings. In addition, all identifying information will be deleted from the typed transcript of the audio recordings, the audio recordings and any other data will be stored in a locked filing cabinet which only the researchers associated with the project have access to, and the audio recordings will be destroyed upon completion of the study. You might also note that this study has been Confederates’ Experiences 60 reviewed and approved by the [name of university] Research Ethics Committee for Psychological Research. The email distributed to former and current research confederates read: My name is Elliott Lee and I have asked ___(name of researcher)___ to forward this message to the people who have acted as confederates in his/her research. I am currently doing my honours BA thesis research on the experiences of people who have been research confederates in psychological research that involved any form of deception (i.e., where the question regarding the use of deception on the ethics application was answered ‘yes’). I am interested in confederates’ experiences in such research, how they felt about being a confederate in research that used deception (be it positive, negative or neutral), and the basis of their feelings. Given this, I am interested in learning about your experiences as a research confederate and, in return for participating in a 45 minute interview about your experiences, you will be given $15.00. You should note, however, that answering questions about experiences as a research confederate in a study that used deception may be distressing for some participants. If you want to learn more about the study or you want to participate, please contact me, Elliott, at [telephone number] or [email address]. Your help will be very much appreciated. Thanks for your time, Elliott Lee Confederates’ Experiences 61 Appendix B. Informed Consent This study has been reviewed and approved by the Carleton University Research Ethics Committee for Psychological Research Informed Consent: Study of Research Confederates’ Experiences The purpose of this consent from is to ensure that you understand the purpose of this study and the nature of your involvement. This consent form must provide you with sufficient information so as to allow you to determine whether you wish to participate. Please ask the researcher to clarify any questions you may have. This is study is being conducted by Elliott Lee under the supervision of Dr. Connie M. Kristiansen, both from the Department of Psychology at Carleton University. This study is investigating the experiences of people who have acted as research confederates. To gain insight into your experience as a research confederate you will be asked to participate in a tape-recorded 45 to 60 minute interview during which you will be asked to describe the research you were involved with, your role in this research, how the research appeared to affect the participants, how being a confederate affected you and how you dealt with these feelings. As a way of reimbursing you for your time and effort, you will be given $15.00. Some people may experience some distress in response to questions asking about what they did as a research confederate, especially when this involved deception and/or the mistreatment of research participants. It is therefore important that you understand that you can refuse to answer any question and may stop your participation in the study at any time, for any reason, without explanation and without penalty. All of the information you provide will be stored in a locked cabinet separate from any documents containing your name and only researchers associated with this project will have access to this information. In addition, your identity will not be revealed in any written or verbal presentations of the findings and a pseudonym will used to further conceal your identify. In addition, the tape recording of the interview will be destroyed upon completion of the study. As a result, the data you provide will ultimately be anonymous. Confederates’ Experiences 62 If you have any concerns about the study or would like to make an inquiry, feel welcome to contact Elliott Lee at elee8@connect.carleton.ca or Dr. Connie M. Kristiansen at ckristia@connect.carleton.ca. For any ethical concerns about this research, please contact Monique Sénéchal (Ethics Chair, Carleton University Research Ethics Committee for Psychological Research) at monique_sénéchal@carleton.ca or 613-520-2600 ext.1155. For any other concerns or comments you are welcome to contact Janet Mantler (Chair of the Department of Psychology, Carleton University) at psychchair@carleton.ca or 613-520-2600 ext. 4173. My signature below indicates I have read the above information and consent to participating in this study. _________________ Participant’s Name __________________ Witness’ Name _______________________ Participant’s Signature _______________________ Witness’ Signature ________________ Date ________________ Date Consent to Audiotape I hereby consent to the audio tape recording of this interview. I understand that it is being audiotaped for accuracy purposes only and that the tape will be destroyed upon completion of the study. __________________ _______________________ ________________ Participant’s Name Participant’s Signature Date __________________ ________________________ ________________ Witness’ Name Witness’ Signature Date Confederates’ Experiences 63 Appendix C. Interview Schedule Interview Schedule: Experiences as a Research Confederate Study Go Through Consent Procedure Warming Up Perhaps the easiest way to start would be by asking where you heard about the study. And why did you decide to participate? The Research As you know, I’m interested in your experiences when you acted as a research confederate. Perhaps you could start at the beginning by telling me how you came to be involved in this research? (e.g., volunteer, paid employee) And what was the study about? What was your role as a confederate? Did you receive any training? Was it easy or hard to do this? How did you feel about doing this? Were you surprised you felt this way? Did it get easier with time? How did the research participants react when you did this? [get examples to illustrate their reactions] How did the participants’ reactions affect you? [positive/negative; get examples] How long did these feeling last? o Why do you think you felt that way o If negative, how did you cope? Did anyone ever warn you that you might feel like that? Did anyone ever warn you about what their reactions might be? Did anyone ever debrief you or offer to help you with your feelings? Did you talk to anyone about your feelings? Did doing these things in the research affect your behaviour outside the lab in any way (e.g., did it spill over – get examples; any positive effects - examples) Did anyone ever tell you if the study had been approved by an ethics review board? Were you offered an incentive for being a confederate? o What was your incentive for being a confederate? (e.g., get to know a professor, obligation, payment) Confederates’ Experiences 64 o Did the incentive help offset any negative feelings? What are your thoughts on deception? What are your thoughts on the ethics of the study you partook in? Closing Down Is there anything else that you think I should know about the nature of the research that you were involved with and how it affected you? Do you have any questions for me? Pseudonym You would like to receive a summary of the study? Request signed permission for future contact for clarification, additional info and/or feedback interview. Go through debriefing – written and verbal Payment Confederates’ Experiences 65 Appendix D. Face Sheet Face Sheet Participant # _____ Participant’s Pseudonym _____________________________ Participant’s Age ______ Participant’s Year and Program of study / Occupation _____________________________ Would like to receive summary of findings Y / N If yes, e-mail address: ___________________________ Willing to participate in clarification e-mail or interview? Y / N Confederates’ Experiences 66 Appendix E. Debriefing Debriefing: Study of Research Confederates’ Experiences While informed consent, debriefing procedures, ethics review boards and other resources focus on research participants’ well-being, surprisingly little attention has been given to the welfare of research confederates. In particular, the potential for confederates to experience anxiety, distress or other negative consequences as a result of the demands of their role has been neglected until now. The present study is therefore being conducted to shed light on the experiences of people who have been research confederates and, if they had any troubling experiences, how they dealt with them. Should you have any further inquiries about this study feel free to contact the research, Elliott Lee, by email at elee8@connect.carleton.ca or Dr. Connie Kristiansen by email at ckristia@carleton.ca. If you have any ethical concerns regarding this study, you can contact Dr. Monique Sénéchal (Chair, Carleton University Research Ethics Committee for Psychological Research) at monique_sénéchal@carleton.ca or (613) 520-2600, ext. 6026. If you have any questions or comments about any other aspect of the study, you are welcome to contact Dr. Janet Mantler (Chair of the Department of Psychology, Carleton University) at psychchair@carleton.ca or (613) 520-2600, ext. 4173. Because the study addresses a potentially sensitive topic, some of the questions may be emotionally challenging or distressing for some people. If you feel the need to talk to someone regarding any issue that may have arisen from participating in this study, then please do not hesitate to contact the 24-hour Distress Centre of Ottawa. This service provides confidential assistance and support by staff trained to deal with sensitive issues, and can be contacted at 613-238-3311 24-hours a day, 7 days a week. If you are a student at Carleton University, you may also contact Carleton University’s counselling service at hcs@carleton.ca or 613-520-6674. Finally, note that I may be contacting some participants to take part in a follow-up interview that will last about 30 minutes. During this interview, you may be asked to a) comment on aspects of your earlier interview that were unclear; b) provide more information about things you said during the earlier interview; and/or c) provide feedback about the extent Confederates’ Experiences 67 to which the researcher’s integration and interpretation of the interview data from all of the participants is consistent with your own experiences. Thank you so much for your participation and assistance in this research. Confederates’ Experiences 68 Appendix F. Post Interview Comment Sheet Mood/Tone of the Interview Participant’s Emotional Reactions throughout Interview My Emotional Reactions to the Participant My Emotional Reactions to what the Participant Said Strengths of the Interview Weaknesses of the Interview Additional Comments/Notable Features of the Interview