Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 1 Meaning in Life and Intuition: Supplementary Materials Samantha J. Heintzelman & Laura A. King Study 1 PA as a mediator. Despite the weakness of our single item measure of MIL, we conducted analysis to determine whether PA mediated the relationship between FI and MIL. This mediational analysis (Hayes, 2009; Preacher & Hayes, 2005) showed that the indirect effect of FI on MIL through PA was significant, z = 7.01, p < .001; 95% confidence interval (CI) bootstrapped with 3000 re-samplings for the indirect effect = [.08, .15]. Still, the direct path from FI to MIL remained significant, B = 0.16 (.04), p < .001, controlling for PA, suggesting the relationship between FI and MIL was significantly, but only partially mediated by PA. PA as a moderator. In addition to its potential role as a mediator, PA might also serve as a moderator of the relationship between intuition and MIL. Hicks et al. (2010) examined feelings of meaning for specific targets (e.g., ambiguous quotations, works of art, negative life events, and linguistic triads) as a function of FI and PA. In those studies, main effects of individual differences in reliance on intuitive processing on meaning ratings did not emerge. Rather, FI interacted with PA to predict outcomes: PA led to enhanced meaning in quotations and life events, and to superior discrimination between coherent and incoherent linguistic triads (a measure of meaning accuracy, Hicks et al., 2010), as a function of FI. These results were interpreted as indicating that PA was necessary to “clear the mental landscape of rational interference” (p. 976), allowing individual differences in FI to influence outcomes. MIL is, however, different from the circumscribed outcomes used in this research. When judging whether a particular quotation, for instance, is meaningful, or whether a triad is coherent or incoherent, there is a sense that a right answer exists, that the stimulus is either meaningful or Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 2 not. For the question of life’s meaningfulness, however, the “right” answer is generally hoped to be “yes” (Hicks, Schlegel, & King, 2010). In this sense, when judging MIL, PA would seem unnecessary to quell rational override. Indeed, Humphrey (2006) has argued that the feeling of significance is a key (perhaps the key) function of private mental states. Thus, we did not expect PA to serve as a moderator of the relationship between FI and MIL (no such moderation was found by King & Hicks, 2009). Still, we tested this potential moderation. PA and FI did not interact (β = -.00 p = .98) to predict MIL. Religion as a moderator. Religious identification and FI did not interact to predict MIL (β = .05, p = .13). Study 2 Intrinsic religiosity as a moderator. We also tested intrinsic religiosity as a moderator of the relationship between FI and MIL. Intrinsic religiosity did not interact with FI to predict MIL (β = .02, p = .60). Study 3 All mediational analyses. The mediational analyses with all Study 3 variables are shown in Supplementary Table 1. The second and third columns of the table show the standardized beta weights for regression equations, first entering each of the predictors alone, and then when paired with FI. As can be seen in the third column, FI remained significantly associated with MIL when entered with each of the covariates. The final columns of the table show the results of mediational analyses testing FI and each of the covariates as mediators of each other in the prediction of MIL. PA as a moderator. We tested PA as a potential moderator in the relationship between FI and MIL. In this case, significant main effects for PA, β = .33, and FI, β = .25 (both p’s < Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 3 .001), were qualified by a significant (though small) interaction, β = .07, p = .014. Supplementary Figure 1 shows the regression lines generated by this equation for those +/- 1 SD from the mean on PA and FI. As can be seen, the results show two main effects: largely parallel, positively sloped lines representing high vs. low FI. Importantly, FI significantly predicted MIL for those one SD above the mean on PA (n = 183), β = .41, p < .001; as well as for those one SD below the mean on PA (n = 138), β = .27, p = .001, though the magnitude was significantly higher for those high in PA, z = 1.82, p = .034. NC and FI interaction. Next we tested whether NC, might interact with FI to predict MIL. MIL might be expected to be low for those low on FI and high in the tendency to reflect. In a hierarchical regression equation, the main effects contributed significantly, for FI, β = .29, p < .001; for NC, β = .07, p = .03; but there was no interaction, β = .02, p =.52. Study 4 Raw data. We wanted to discern whether the lines generated for the curvilinear interactions for order by MIL predicting the CRT variables were indeed accurate reflections the data. To do so, we first plotted the raw data across levels of MIL for each .5 standard deviation step in the data (representing 6 levels). Results are shown in Supplementary Figure 2. As can be seen, for the CRT measures, the raw data do follow the pattern suggested by the generated lines reported in the manuscript, particularly for those low in MIL who completed the MIL measure first. The raw data, however, do not appear to support a positive uptick in reflection after moderate levels of MIL. ANOVAs. Means for the groups for the low vs. not low MIL X order interaction for the three CRT measures are shown in Supplementary Figure 3. Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 4 Mediational Analyses. Mediational analyses showed that the relationship between FI and MIL was partially mediated by PA, z = 5.11 (p < .001), but the direct path from FI to MIL remained significant B = 0.15 (0.05), p = .006. Mediational analyses showed that agreeableness significantly (z = 4.34, p < .001), 95% CI = [0.05 to 0.14] but only partially mediated the relationship between FI and MIL. Controlling for agreeableness, the direct path from FI to MIL remained significant, B = 0.19 (.06), p < .001. PA as a moderator. In this sample, PA and FI did not interact to predict MIL, β = .05, p = .20. CRT and FI interactions. We also examined whether FI and CRT interacted to predict MIL. In this analysis we used the dummy codes for CRT scores and the interactions of these with FI were used to test the relationships of FI and CRT scores to MIL. Initial analyses included order as a factor but order did not contribute to the equation. As such, the following represent results using the entire sample, collapsed across orders. A main effect of the dummy code for scoring 3 on the CRT, β = -.11 p = .008, was qualified by two interactions, FI X scoring 3, β = .12, p = 03 and FI X scoring 2, β = .11, p = 02. Graphing these interactions separately showed a pattern that was essentially identical to that using the CRT as a continuous measure. Specifically, predicting MIL from the CRT (β = -.10, p = .012) and FI (β = .16, p < .001) showed that these main effects (ΔR2 = .05, p <.001), were qualified by a significant interaction (ΔR2 = .011, p =.009), β = .11, p = .009. Supplementary Figure 4 shows the generated regression lines. Those high in FI reported high levels of MIL regardless of their CRT scores. However, for individuals 1 SD below the mean on FI, CRT performance was negatively correlated with MIL r(106) = -.28, p = .004. This interaction remained significant (β = .08, p = .036), controlling for religiosity (β = .30, p < .001). Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 5 Analyses using dummy codes for heuristic answers produced essentially the same results in the opposite direction, (e.g., β’s for the interactions between FI and the dummy codes for 2 and 3 heuristic answers = .16, p = .02 and .19, p = .005 respectively). These analyses suggest that individual differences in processing styles may relate to MIL in the way we have proposed: Those who are reliant on intuitive processing report high levels of MIL, but those who report themselves as unlikely to rely intuitive impressions and who are in fact capable of overriding intuitive impulses, report the lowest levels of MIL. Interestingly, when NC, the dummy codes for CRT performance, and their interactions were entered into a regression equation predicting MIL, only main effects of NC, β = .14, p = .018, and the dummy code for scoring 3 on the CRT, β= -.20, p < .001, predicted MIL. No interactions were found (all p’s > .18). When CRT was treated as a continuous measure, CRT performance, β = -.18, p < .001, and NC, β = .20, p < .001, predicted MIL, with no interaction, β = .04, p < .27. CRT variables across orders. CRT variables did not differ across orders: For correct answers, M(SD)’s CRT first = 1.25 (1.20), MIL first = 1.21 (1.24), t(610) = 0.32, p = .75, d = 0.03, JZS Prior Bayes factor = 14.82; for heuristic answers, CRT first = 1.47(1.46), MIL first = 1.49(1.18), t(612) = 0.25, p = .81, d = 0.01, JZS Prior Bayes factor = 15.11; and for time spent on the CRT, CRT first = 22.53 (44.90), MIL first = 23.53 (26.23), t(610) = 0.44, p = .66, d = 0.04, JZS Prior Bayes factor = 14.16. Did MIL predict processing independent of affect? The main results suggest that information processing does not affect MIL. Instead, levels of MIL predict subsequent information processing with low MIL being associated with Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 6 reflection. Mood (PA and NA) are subjective states that have been shown to influence styles of information processing (e.g., Clore & Palmer, 2009; Schwarz, 2012; Schwarz & Clore, 1988). As such, it is of interest to see if MIL and/or MIL2 predict cognitive processing controlling for mood. Table 3 of the manuscript shows the results of regression equations predicting the three CRT measures from MIL and MIL2 as well as (centered) PA and NA, for those who completed MIL prior to the CRT. As can be seen, controlling for PA and NA, the contribution of MIL2 remained significant for all CRT measures. In additional equations, we entered quadratic effects for PA and NA, to insure that extremely low levels of PA or extremely high levels of NA did not account for the patterns found for MIL2. In none of the equations did these variables contribute significantly (all p’s > .10), and all of the effects for MIL2 remained significant (β’s = .17, -.17 and .14 for correct answers, heuristic answer and time spent on the task, respectively, p’s < .023). These patterns were only observed in those who completed the MIL assessment first. Among those who completed the CRT first, both PA (β = -.19) and NA (β = -.18) independently predicted fewer correct answers, p’s = .002 and more heuristic answers, β’s = .15 and .16 for PA and NA respectively, p’s < .02. No other significant results were found for this group. Did processing style moderate the effects of order on MIL? Next we examined whether levels of reflection predicted differences in MIL. For these analyses, MIL was the criterion and CRT scores were converted to three dummy codes (for scoring 3, 2, or 1 respectively, with the 0 group representing the baseline). The main effects and interactions of these with order were entered into a hierarchical regression equation predicting MIL. Only a significant main effect of the dummy code for scoring 3 contributed significantly, β = -.20, p = .002. None of the dummy codes interacted with order, β’s < .02, p’s > .79. Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 7 A similar analysis was completed examining dummy codes for heuristic answers to the CRT predicting MIL as a function of order. Again, no interactions with order were found. Rather, main effects for the dummy codes for 2 heuristic answers, β = .16, p = .02; and 3 heuristic answers, β = .19, p = .005 emerged. For the interaction terms, all p’s > .34. These analyses suggest that MIL is associated with heuristic processing but do not support the idea that heuristic processing enhances subsequent MIL. Regressing MIL on the time spent on the CRT and its interaction with order showed only a marginally significant main effect of time, β = -.09, p = .072, and no interaction, β = .05 p = .27. In order to examine whether time spent on the CRT was associated with MIL in a curvilinear fashion (that potentially interacted with order), MIL was regressed hierarchically on time, order, the time X order interaction, as well as time2, and time2 X order. None of the predictors significantly contributed to the prediction of MIL. For time2, β = -.14, p = .19, for the time2 X order interaction, β = -.05, p = .49. Overall, then, no evidence emerged for the possibility that processing styles as evinced on the CRT predict subsequent MIL. Study 5 As in Study 4, we wanted to determine whether generated regression lines represented accurate reflections of the data. A plot of the raw data shown in Supplementary Figure 5 suggests the data are appropriately represented in our analyses. Means for the MIL (lowest to highest quartile) X mindset induction (intuitive vs. reflective) are shown in the Supplementary Figure 6. Did FI moderate the effects of condition on MIL? The lack of effects for mindset induction on MIL does not preclude the possibility that FI might moderate the effects of condition on MIL. In order to examine whether FI interacted with Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 8 condition (or order) to predict MIL, MIL was regressed on main effects of condition, order, and FI, as well as all two and three-way interactions. Results showed only a main effect of FI, β = .22, ΔR2 = .05, p = .003. No other step contributed a significant change in R2 (p’s > .22) and no other predictors approached significance, p’s >.20. We also examined whether FI2 might qualify this linear association in interaction with condition. For the main effect of FI, β= 21, p =.001, for FI2, β = .10, p = .10, and for the FI2 X mindset interaction, β=.01, p = .87. Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 9 References Hayes, A.F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408-420. Hicks, J. A., Cicero, D. C., Trent, J., Burton, C. M., & King, L A. (2010). Positive affect, intuition, and the feeling of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 967-979. Hicks, J. A., Schlegel, R. J., & King, L. A. (2010). Social threats, happiness, and the dynamics of meaning in life judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1305-1317. Humphrey, N. (2006). Seeing Red: A Study in Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2009). Positive affect, intuition, and referential thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 719-724. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36, 717–731. Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 10 Supplementary Table 1. Regression Analyses Predicting Meaning in Life, Study 3 Regression Results Mediational Results Indirect Effect Predictor βx→MIL Faith in Intuition (FI) .31 Positive Affect .37 FI Need for Cognition .23 FI Religiosity .27 FI Self-Esteem .36 FI Need Satisfaction FI .29 βx→MIL.FI z 95% CI BMILX.FI .33 5.76 [.05, .10] 0.27(.02) .24 5.22 [.02, .06] .19 3.84 [.01, .05] .28 4.30 [.03, .09] .24 2.78 [.01, .04] .28 2.82 [.01, .06] .30 6.75 [.06, .12] .22 5.77 [.04, .10] .21 5.71 [.04, .11] .24 6.30 [.06, .13] BMILFI.X 0.26(.03) 0.26(.04) 0.30(.03) 0.29(.03) 0.30(.03) 0.35(.04) 0.24(.03) 0.25(.04) 0.25(.03) Note. N = 1026. The second and third columns show the standardized beta weights predicting MIL for each predictor alone and when entered with FI simultaneously. All are significant, p < .001. The fourth column contains the tests for mediation (z). Z values in the same row as a variable test that variable as the mediator, all are significant, p<.005. The fifth column provides the confidence intervals for the indirect effect of the mediator, using bootstrapping with 3000 resamplings. The sixth column presents the unstandardized regression weights predicting MIL from each predictor controlling for FI. All are significant, p < .001. The final column presents the same for FI, controlling for each predictor. All are significant, p < .001. Meaning in Life was measured using the MLQ presence of meaning subscale. Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 11 Supplementary Figure 1. Positive Affect and Faith in Intuition predicting Meaning in Life, Study 3. 5 4.8 Meaning in Life 4.6 4.4 4.2 4 Low FI 3.8 High FI 3.6 3.4 3.2 3 Low High Positive Affect Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 12 Supplementary Figure 2. Meaning in Life Predicting CRT Outcomes as a Function of Order, Raw Data, Study 4 CRT Performance 3 2.5 2 1.5 MIL First 1 CRT First 0.5 0 Low High Heuristic Responses Meaning in Life 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 MIL First CRT First Low High Time to Complete CRT Meaning in Life 65 55 45 35 MIL First 25 CRT First 15 Low High Meaning in Life cases 150 100 50 0 Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 13 CRT Scores Supplementary Figure 3. CRT Measures as a Function of MIL Levels and Order, Study 4 2 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 MIL First CRT First Low Not Low Heuristic Answers Meaning in Life 2 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 MIL First CRT First Low Not Low Meaning in Life 35 Time on CRT 30 25 20 MIL First 15 CRT First 10 5 0 Low Not Low Meaning in Life Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 14 Supplementary Figure 4. MIL as a Function of FI X CRT, Study 4 4.8 4.6 Meaning in Life 4.4 4.2 4 3.8 Low FI 3.6 High FI 3.4 3.2 3 Low High Cognitive Reflection Task Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 15 6 5.8 5.6 5.4 5.2 5 4.8 4.6 4.4 4.2 4 Reflective Intuitive Low High Meaning in Life 250 200 cases Faith in Intuition Supplementary Figure 5. FI as a function of MIL and Mindset Manipulation Raw Data, Study 5 150 100 50 0 Low High Meaning in Life Meaning and Intuition: Supplementary Materials 16 Supplementary Figure 6. Faith in Intuition as a Function of MIL and Mindset, Study 5 6 5.8 Faith in Intuition 5.6 5.4 5.2 5 Reflective 4.8 Intuitive 4.6 4.4 4.2 4 1st 2nd 3rd Meaning in Life Quartile 4th