Perfectionism, Failure and Self-conscious Emotions: A role for Self-compassion? Natalie Rebecca Almond Submitted for the degree of Doctor of Psychology (Clinical Psychology) School of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences University of Surrey Guildford, Surrey United Kingdom September 2015 Abstract Objectives This study investigated: (1) whether maladaptive perfectionism predicted the experience of self-conscious emotions such as shame, guilt and pride following an imagined failure and (2) whether self-compassionate writing could reduce shame and guilt and increase pride relative to a control and self-esteem writing task. Design The study used a correlational design to assess the relationship between maladaptive perfectionism and self-conscious emotions. The study also used an experimental between-subjects design to investigate the effect of writing task on self-conscious emotions controlling for initial levels of self-conscious emotion using ANCOVA. Methods Ninety-five University of Surrey students completed an online study that manipulated imagined failure on an academic assignment, and measured maladaptive perfectionism and shame, guilt and pride. Participants were then randomly allocated to either a self-compassionate, self-esteem or a control writing task. Self-conscious emotions were then measured again. Results Following imagining failure maladaptive perfectionism was positively associated with state shame and guilt and negatively associated with state pride. When measured after the writing tasks, the means for shame and guilt were lowered and the mean for pride was increased. However, contrary to predictions, shame was not predicted by writing condition, guilt remained highest following completion of a self-compassionate writing task and pride was highest following the completion of the control-writing task. Conclusions Maladaptive perfectionism is correlated with self-conscious emotions following imagined failure. Self-compassionate writing tasks do not appear to be more effective at improving self-conscious emotion than other writing tasks. 2 Acknowledgements I would like to thank all of the service-users I have worked with over the course of the last three years. I have learnt something valuable from each person and I sincerely hope that the work was valuable for the service-users. I would also like to thank all of the clinical psychologists that welcomed me into their teams and have supervised my clinical practice during training. I have learnt vast amounts from each supervisor in terms of your therapeutic skills, working in teams, self-care, and what makes good supervision! I would like to thank all of the research supervisors that have been involved in contributing to this research and my personal and professional development as a clinical psychologist, in particular Dr Laura Simonds, and Dr Linda Morison. In addition, Dr Clara Strauss, Dr Jason Spendalow, and Dr Kate Gleeson all played important roles in the process of developing this project for which I am grateful. I would also like to thank Andrew Barnes for his technical help and support in setting up the online questionnaire. I am indebted to the kindness of the University of Surrey students who completed my online questionnaire: without them there would not be any research data. I would like to recognise the numerous clinical and academic psychologists who contributed to the taught programme. I have learnt a great deal about a great many topics and I am sure that this comprehensive programme of teaching will provide a useful resource to inform my thinking for many years to come. I would also like to thank Elly Tomlinson who acted as my mentor throughout training. Elly’s experience, wisdom, kindness and support throughout the rollercoaster of training has been invaluable. Finally, I would like to thank my wonderful husband Paul without whom I could not have completed this training course. I’m not sure either of us knew at the beginning of this course what we had let ourselves in for! Over the last three years I have developed as a psychologist and as a person and you have made space for this change and supported it. You have provided emotional support, practical support, friendship and love, unwaveringly. Thank you for celebrating my successes and supporting me in the face of difficulties. Bowlby, (1973) describes the benefits of an attachment relationship that provides a secure base as follows: “Human beings of all ages are at their happiest and are able to deploy their talents to their best advantage when they are confident that standing behind them, there is a trusted person who will come to their aid should difficulties arise.” Thank you for being my secure base and supporting me to flourish. 2 Table of contents Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 1 Objectives ................................................................................................................... 1 Design ........................................................................................................................ 1 Methods ...................................................................................................................... 1 Results ........................................................................................................................ 1 Conclusions ................................................................................................................ 2 Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................... 1 Table of contents .......................................................................................................... 3 MRP paper ................................................................................................................... 8 Perfectionism, Failure and Self-conscious Emotions: A role for Self-compassion? ........................................................................................................................................ 8 Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 9 Statement of journal choice....................................................................................... 11 Introduction ................................................................................................................ 12 What is perfectionism? ............................................................................................. 12 Perfectionism and failing to meet high standards ................................................... 12 Failing to meet standards and shame ...................................................................... 13 Failing to meet standards, perfectionism and shame .............................................. 13 Perfectionism and self-conscious emotions ............................................................. 13 Maladaptive perfectionism and self-conscious emotions ........................................ 14 Shame and self-compassion ..................................................................................... 17 Overview of study ...................................................................................................... 19 3 Method ........................................................................................................................ 19 Ethical Approval ...................................................................................................... 19 Study Design ............................................................................................................ 19 Procedure ................................................................................................................. 19 Dependant measures ................................................................................................ 24 Maladaptive Perfectionism. ................................................................................. 24 Shame, guilt, and pride. ....................................................................................... 25 Participants .............................................................................................................. 25 Results ......................................................................................................................... 27 Data handling .......................................................................................................... 27 Analytic approach .................................................................................................... 28 Correlational analysis ............................................................................................. 28 Effect of writing condition on Time 1 variables....................................................... 30 Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) Analyses .......................................................... 31 Content Analysis ...................................................................................................... 35 Discussion.................................................................................................................... 41 Maladaptive perfectionism, and self-conscious emotions ....................................... 41 Self-compassion and self-conscious emotions ......................................................... 43 Limitations and suggestions for future research ..................................................... 46 References ................................................................................................................... 48 List of Appendices ...................................................................................................... 56 Appendix A ................................................................................................................. 58 Appendix B ................................................................................................................. 62 Appendix C ................................................................................................................. 63 4 Appendix D ................................................................................................................. 64 Appendix E ................................................................................................................. 78 Appendix F ................................................................................................................. 79 Appendix G ................................................................................................................. 84 Appendix H ................................................................................................................. 85 Appendix I .................................................................................................................. 88 Appendix J .................................................................................................................. 89 Appendix K ................................................................................................................. 90 Appendix L ................................................................................................................. 92 Appendix M ................................................................................................................ 93 Appendix N ................................................................................................................. 99 Appendix O ............................................................................................................... 100 MRP Research Proposal.......................................................................................... 101 Can compassionate-imagery reduce perfectionist’s experience of shame? ........ 101 Introduction .............................................................................................................. 102 Background and Theoretical Rationale ................................................................. 102 Research Question ................................................................................................. 105 Main Hypotheses .................................................................................................... 105 Method ...................................................................................................................... 106 Participants ............................................................................................................ 106 Design .................................................................................................................... 107 Measures/Interviews/Stimuli/Apparatus ................................................................ 107 5 Perfectionism measure ........................................................................................... 107 Manipulation of failure .......................................................................................... 108 Measures of shame and guilt ................................................................................. 108 Manipulation of compassionate-imagery .............................................................. 108 Measure of mood.................................................................................................... 109 Procedure ............................................................................................................... 109 Ethical considerations ........................................................................................... 110 R&D Considerations .............................................................................................. 111 Proposed Data Analysis ......................................................................................... 111 Service User and Carer Consultation / Involvement ............................................. 112 Feasibility Issues .................................................................................................... 112 Dissemination strategy........................................................................................... 113 Study Timeline........................................................................................................ 113 References ................................................................................................................. 114 MRP Literature review ........................................................................................... 120 Perfectionism, parental bonding and attachment. ................................................ 120 Journal choice and justification .............................................................................. 121 Abstract ..................................................................................................................... 122 Introduction .............................................................................................................. 123 Perfectionism ......................................................................................................... 123 Measures of perfectionism ..................................................................................... 124 Defining perfectionism ........................................................................................... 125 Development of perfectionism................................................................................ 126 Attachment and perfectionism................................................................................ 127 6 Measuring attachment ........................................................................................... 128 Method of the literature search .............................................................................. 130 Inclusion/ Exclusion criteria .................................................................................. 130 Results ....................................................................................................................... 131 Perfectionism and Parental Bonding Inventory .................................................... 133 Overall summary of perfectionism and parental bonding inventory ..................... 138 Adult attachment .................................................................................................... 139 Overall summary of perfectionism and adult attachment ...................................... 146 Discussion.................................................................................................................. 149 Theoretical implications ........................................................................................ 149 Limitations and implications for future research .................................................. 151 Conclusion ............................................................................................................. 153 References ................................................................................................................. 154 Clinical Experience .................................................................................................. 160 An overview of the clinical experiences gained on placement. ............................ 160 Year 1 ..................................................................................................................... 161 Year 2 ..................................................................................................................... 161 Year 3 ..................................................................................................................... 162 Assessments .............................................................................................................. 164 Year I Assessments ................................................................................................. 165 Year II Assessments................................................................................................ 165 Year III Assessments .............................................................................................. 166 7 MRP paper Perfectionism, Failure and Self-conscious Emotions: A role for Selfcompassion? 8 Abstract Objectives This study investigated: (1) whether maladaptive perfectionism was associated with the experience of self-conscious emotions such as shame, guilt and pride following an imagined failure and (2) whether self-compassionate writing could reduce shame and guilt and increase pride relative to a control and self-esteem writing task. Design The study was a randomised control study. Following an imagined failure the effect of three different writing tasks (control, self-esteem and self-compassion) on shame, guilt and pride was assessed. Methods Ninety-five University of Surrey students completed an online study that manipulated imagined failure on an academic assignment, and measured maladaptive perfectionism and shame, guilt and pride. Participants were then randomly allocated to either a self-compassionate, self-esteem or a control writing task. Shame, guilt and pride were then measured again. Results Following imagining failure maladaptive perfectionism was positively associated with state shame and guilt and negatively associated with state pride. When measured after the writing tasks, the means for shame and guilt were lowered and the mean for pride was increased. However, contrary to predictions, there was no significant effect of writing condition on shame. Writing condition did significantly effect guilt and pride but not as hypothesised. Guilt was least reduced by the self-compassion writing, and pride was most increased following control 9 writing. Conclusions Maladaptive perfectionism is correlated with shame, guilt and pride following imagined failure. Self-compassionate writing tasks do not appear to be more effective at improving self-conscious emotion than other writing tasks. 10 Statement of journal choice I plan to submit this paper to the British Journal of Clinical Psychology. The guidelines for authors submitting to this journal can be seen in Appendix A. This journal is appropriate because the content of the current paper falls under the overall aims and scope of the journal. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology publishes original research that is empirical in nature and covers all aspects of clinical psychology. The journal considers psychological treatment interventions of individuals, which is important, because this is what one part of my study considers. Also the journal consider papers considering all psychological disorders regardless of age groups or setting, this is important given my use of a non-clinical sample. The journal has previously published empirical investigations of perfectionism in nonclinical samples. I have chosen this particular clinical psychology journal because it has a good impact factor of 2.377. Much research on self-compassion has been published in social and personality psychology journals and whilst some of these may have higher impact factors, I believe it is important to publish these findings in a clinical psychology journal because this journal is more likely to be read by more clinical psychologists. 11 Introduction In this study I consider whether perfectionism is associated with the extent to which an individual experiences self-conscious emotions (such as shame, guilt and pride) following the experience of failing to meet their self-defined standards. I also consider whether a brief self-compassionate writing task can reduce shame and guilt and increase pride relative to a self-esteem and control writing task. What is perfectionism? In this study perfectionism is defined as “… the setting of, and striving to meet, very demanding standards that are self-imposed and relentlessly pursued despite causing problems. It involves basing one’s self-worth almost exclusively on how well these high standards are pursued and achieved.” (p. 9, Shafran, Egan, & Wade, 2010). Perfectionism is associated with predictors of maladjustment such as negative affect (see Stöber & Otto, 2006) and predicts and maintains a range of mental health difficulties and distress, (see Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011 for a review). Perfectionism and failing to meet high standards A perfectionist’s sense of self-worth is overly dependent on their ability to strive for and meet their own self-defined (unrealistically high and inflexible) standards. Thus perfectionists direct much effort self-evaluating their progress in meeting these standards. Shafran et al (2010) proposed that perfectionists often fail to meet their self-defined standards for performance and this failure leads to counterproductive behaviours (such as procrastination or avoidance) and selfcriticism. These outcomes maintain the perfectionistic striving. The perfectionist will continue to strive to improve their self-worth by re-attempting to meet their high standards. Ironically, then perfectionists commonly experience failure, and this 12 failure perpetuates further perfectionistic striving (and further failure). The emotional consequences of failing to meet these standards for maladaptive perfectionists are currently unclear. Failing to meet standards and shame Failing to meet ideals, standards and live up to expectations has been consistently linked to the experience of shame (Miller, 1996; Reimer, 1996; Sorotzkin, 1985, see Gilbert, 1998 for a review). Tangney (2002) describes shame as a painful negative scrutiny of the entire self and a feeling that “I am an unworthy, incompetent, or bad person.” Gilbert (1998; 2003) suggests that shame is a perceived failure to create a desirable, attractive self and suggested that there may be two types of shame, internal shame and external shame. Internal shame originates inside the self and involves self-generated criticism, and negative self-evaluation. External shame, which originates outside the self, involves a distressing awareness that others view the self negatively. Failing to meet standards, perfectionism and shame Mascolo and Fischer (1995) suggested that shame is generated by the appraisal of having failed to live up to personal (and other peoples) standards of worth and that shame is functional because it highlights behaviours that threaten self-worth. Perfectionists are continually failing to live up to their own personal standards and therefore never attain the self-worth that they are seeking. Furthermore perfectionists monitor and evaluate whether they are meeting these standards. When perfectionists fail to meet their standards they engage in negative self-evaluation and self-criticism and thus are likely to experience an internal sense of shame. Perfectionism and self-conscious emotions Shame is one of a number of self-conscious emotions, along with guilt and 13 pride. People experience self-conscious emotions when they evaluate some aspect of the self (personality characteristics, abilities, or behaviour) against a set of standards (Tangney, 2002). Perfectionists are continually self-evaluating and often conclude that they have underachieved, thus there has been repeated suggestions in the literature then that perfectionists may be prone to experiencing shame and guilt and may be unable to experience pride (e.g. Hamacheck, 1978; Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Sorotzkin, 1985). Shame and guilt are often perceived to be similar and overlapping so it is plausible that perfectionists may also be more likely to experience guilt. Both shame and guilt arise from negative self-evaluations and may be elicited by similar events and scenarios. However, different types of appraisal may trigger guilt and shame: a negative evaluation of the self will result in shame; whereas a negative evaluation of the action or behaviour will result in guilt. To clarify, shame is associated with appraisals that “I am bad”, whereas guilt is associated with appraisals that “I did a bad thing” (Lewis, 1971). Pride on the other hand is a positive self-conscious emotion arising from positive self-evaluations that contribute to a person’s sense of self-worth pride is associated with feelings of accomplishment and satisfaction. Given perfectionists tendency to chronically self-evaluate and conclude that they have underachieved it seems likely that perfectionists may experience less pride. Maladaptive perfectionism and self-conscious emotions Stöber, Harris, and Moon, (2007) note that despite the longstanding hypotheses that perfectionists are likely to experience more shame and guilt and less (if any) pride, empirical support for this hypothesis is sketchy. There are a number of reasons why this is the case. Firstly, perfectionism has been conceptualized in a number of ways and secondly there is variation in whether proneness to self- 14 conscious emotions, or state or trait self-conscious emotions is considered. Perfectionism has often been considered as a multi-factorial concept and thus the most commonly used measures of perfectionism measure different aspects of perfectionism. The Multifactorial Perfectionism Scale (F-MPS: Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990) measures concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, personal standards, parental expectations, parental criticism, and organization and the Multifactorial Perfectionism Scale (H-MPS: Hewitt & Flett, 1991) measures selforiented perfectionism, other-oriented perfectionism and socially-prescribed perfectionism; and the Almost Perfect Scale- Revised (APS-R: Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi & Ashby, 2001) measures discrepancy, high standards, and order. Research using these multifactorial measures of perfectionism to investigate selfconscious emotions has demonstrated that self-oriented perfectionism is associated with proneness to guilt (Hewitt & Flett, 1991), socially-prescribed perfectionism is associated with proneness to guilt and shame (Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Tangney, 2002) and that negative perfectionism is associated with both state and trait shame and guilt (Fedewa, Burns & Gomez, 2005). More recently numerous factor analytic investigations have attempted to identify the key aspects of perfectionism (Bieling, Isralei, & Antony, 2004; Dunkley, Blankstein, & Berg, 2012; Enns, Cox, & Clara, 2002; Rice, Ashby, & Slaney, 1998; Suddarth & Slaney, 2001). Reviewing these factor-analyses Stöber & Otto (2006) concluded that perfectionism has two factors; an adaptive factor that captures having high personal standards and striving for excellence and a maladaptive factor that seems to capture self-evaluation/ criticism and monitoring of whether an individual is meeting their own standards. This has lead Stöber, et al. (2007) to argue that only maladaptive perfectionists should experience increased shame and guilt and reduced 15 (or absent) pride. Only one study has examined how maladaptive perfectionists state shame, guilt and pride were affected by failing a task (Stöber, et al, 2007) and there were numerous problems with this study that make the results difficult to interpret. Success and failure was defined in relation to objective standards (university grades) rather than in relation to the individual’s self-defined standards and expectations for their own performance. It is failure to meet self-defined standards that is central to a maladaptive perfectionist’s experience. Therefore this study will consider whether the extent to which an individual endorses maladaptive elements of perfectionism is associated with the extent to which they experience state shame, guilt and pride following failure to meet their own self-defined standards for performance. Based on the existing literature it is predicted that following a failure (to meet self-defined standards of performance) higher levels of maladaptive perfectionism will be associated with higher levels of state shame, and may be associated with higher levels of state guilt and lower levels of state pride. In the second part of this study the aim is to consider whether a brief intervention at the point of failing to meet self-defined standards can reduce the emotional impact of this failure. As noted above failing to meet standards may elicit both shame and guilt. In shame the object of negative evaluation is the whole self (Lewis, 1971). The experience of shame is associated with stable and global attributions about the self, an inward attentional focus, the desire to escape/avoid, and meaning-making that the individual is bad or worthless (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Guilt is often attributed to the action taken and so may be less damaging for the individual in the longer term. However, guilt and shame are often both elicited by similar situations therefore, guilt will still be measured in the second part of the 16 study but reducing shame is the primary focus. Research has suggested that the development of self-compassion may serve to reduce an individual’s experience of shame. Shame and self-compassion Self-compassion may provide a means to reduce overly self-critical responses to negative events (Neff, 2003a, 2011). Self-compassion consists of three elements “being open to and moved by one’s own suffering, experiencing feelings of caring and kindness toward oneself, taking an understanding, non-judgmental attitude toward one’s inadequacies and failures, and recognizing that one’s experience is part of the common human experience” (Neff, 2003a p. 224). These three elements of self-compassion are referred to as mindful awareness of feelings, self-kindness and an acceptance of common humanity respectively. Johnson and O’Brien (2013) argue that self-compassion can reduce the experience of shame in response to negative events. They propose that each of the components of self-compassion can reduce specific components of shame. Specifically, being kind to oneself can soften self-criticism and negative selfevaluation, acceptance of one’s common humanity can reduce the perceived need for behavioral withdrawal and social isolation, and mindful awareness of feelings can decrease the tendency to avoid or suppress emotional reactions to negative events. Similarly it is plausible that being kind to oneself could soften the negative selfevaluations present in guilt and acceptance of one’s common humanity may serve to reduce feelings of guilt. Consistent with this conceptualization, there is evidence that self-compassion is negatively correlated with shame (Barnard & Curry, 2012), and therapeutic interventions that promote self-compassion have been shown to reduce shame 17 (Gilbert & Procter, 2006; Judge, Cleghorn, McEwan & Gilbert, 2012). However, these interventions are time-consuming and costly and use a combination of tasks (e.g., compassionate writing, compassionate imagery and mindfulness) to create all three elements of self-compassion. Consequently research has begun to consider the effect of briefer self-compassionate interventions. One promising intervention is the use of self-compassionate writing. Selfcompassionate writing has been shown to reduce negative affect (compared to a selfesteem or control writing task) following a negative event (Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen & Hancock, 2007) and three doses of self-compassionate writing have been shown to reduce state shame (compared to expressive writing) about a shameful event (Johnson & O’Brien (2013). These studies suggest that self-compassionate writing could reduce the experience of shame and possibly guilt. However, this remains open question because Johnson and O’Brien’s study failed to include both a control writing condition and a self-esteem condition, thus the effects of the selfcompassionate writing task cannot be distinguished from the effect of writing, or from the effect of raising self-esteem. Whilst self-esteem can be thought of as positive feelings about the self and feeling valued by others, (Leary & MacDonald, 2003), self-compassion can be thought of as an orientation to care for oneself (Leary et al., 2007). Therefore the current study focuses on whether a self-compassionate writing intervention can produce lower levels of shame than a self-esteem intervention and a control writing condition. It is predicted that a brief self-compassionate writing exercise will lead to lower levels of shame than a writing control task, and a self-esteem writing task. It is possible that self-compassionate writing will have a similar effect on other selfconscious emotions producing lower levels of guilt and higher levels of pride than a 18 writing control task. Overview of study A flowchart summarising the procedure can be seen in Appendix B. The study was completed in one sitting by the participant. The use of Part 1 and Part 2 when describing the study is to aid clarity by separating out the aspects of the study that addressed the two research questions: (1) to explore the association between maladaptive perfectionism, shame, guilt and pride following imagined failure and (2) to assess whether a writing task manipulation will have a differential effect on shame, guilt and pride following imagined failure. Part 1 of the study addresses question 1 and Part 2 of the study addresses question 2. Method Ethical Approval The project was submitted for ethical review to the University of Surrey Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences ethics committee and this project received a favourable ethical opinion (see Appendix C for proof of review and approval). Study Design The study was a randomised control study. Following exposure to an imagined failure task the study assessed the effect of three different writing tasks (control, self-esteem and self-compassion) on shame, guilt and pride. Shame, guilt and pride were measured twice, once following the imagined failure task and once following the writing task. Procedure All of the measures and materials used in this study can be seen in full in Appendix D. The study was hosted via an online portal, affiliated to the University of Surrey, which provided a URL that was accessible from any internet-connected 19 computer. Participants completed all of the study online at this URL. The online portal had a fixed design template for the screen- this had a white background with a University of Surrey symbol at the top right corner. All of the text used was black in colour and was presented in the Arial font at size 14 point. All items that participants were asked to rate were presented with the item text centred in the middle of the rating scale. Each individual measure or manipulation was presented on a new screen and was followed at the bottom by a button to click to move to the next screen unless otherwise specified. The first screen provided participants with introductory information that explained the purpose of the study, the payment for the study, and how data would be used and stored. Participants were asked to read this information and then provide consent to complete the study. Participants were asked to click yes to complete the study, or to close the window if they did not consent to complete the study. If participants consented to complete the study a new screen appeared that displayed the following text “The following items are designed to measure attitudes people have toward themselves, their performance, and toward others. There are no right or wrong answers. Please try to respond to all of the items. Use your first impression and do not spend too much time on individual items in responding. Respond to each of the items using the scale below to describe your degree of agreement with each item.” Participants were then provided with all of the items from the measure of maladaptive perfectionism in a random order on one screen. Participants rated their agreement by clicking the radio button to indicate their selection. All participants were then asked to complete the manipulation of failure. For a perfectionist failure is defined as a failure to meet their own self-defined 20 standards. Therefore an imagery task was used to create a manipulation where each participant could experience a failure to meet their own self-defined standards. There is good evidence that imagery can create similar emotional and motivational responses to those of real experiences (Dadds, Bovbjerg, Redd, & Cuttmore, 1997) and imagery and re-scripting is an important aspect of clinical assessment and intervention (Hackmann, Bennett-Levy, & Holmes, 2011). The imagery task was a modified version of an imagery task used by Gilbert, Baldwin, Irons, Baccus, and Palmer (2006). Participants were shown the following instructions on screen “The following task is timed and you will not be able to progress to the next page until the time allowed has passed.” Participants then read the instructions that were designed to create the self-defined standards of performance “We would like you to imagine a piece of coursework you have been working on and putting a lot of effort into as part of your university studies. Imagine the grade you want to get and how you would feel if you achieved it . . . Close your eyes or look down and try to imagine this for 30 seconds." This screen was presented for 50 seconds, allowing participants 20 seconds to read the instructions and 30 seconds to complete the task. After the 50 seconds had elapsed a button appeared on screen so that participants could click to progress to the next screen. This screen provided the manipulation of failing to meet these standards. Participants were provided with the following written instructions “Now imagine that when your essay is marked and comes back to you, the grade it is given is two grades lower than you wanted. Close your eyes or look down and try to imagine this for 1 minute." below these instructions there were three written prompts of what to think about during this minute (e.g., What would this lower grade mean to you?). This screen was displayed for 80 seconds, allowing 20 seconds to read the instructions and one minute to 21 complete them. After this time has elapsed a button appeared on screen to allow participants to move to the next screen. All participants were presented with the following text to read “The following are some statements which may or may not describe how you are feeling right now, following imagining the scenario. Please rate each statement using the 5point scale below. Remember to rate each statement based on how you are feeling right at this moment.”. Participants were then presented with all items from the measure of state shame, guilt and pride in a random order, on one screen and again clicked a radio button to indicate their agreement. The failure manipulation had been conducted using imagery so it was difficult to objectively determine the extent to which participants had completed this task. An adapted version of Gilbert et al. (2006) imagery manipulation check was used to assess this. Participants saw the following text on screen “Please answer the items below by clicking your answer on the scale”. Participants were asked to rate their agreement with three questions to measure imageability of the failure scenario: for example, “how easy was it to imagine the situation?”, on a ten-point scale anchored at 1 (not easy to imagine) and at 10 (easy to imagine). The three items had a Cronbachs alpha = .87. These three items were averaged together and the range of possible scores was from 1-10. Participants were also asked to complete an item that assessed the percentage of allotted time spent imagining; “how much of the allotted time did you spend thinking about this situation?” anchored at 0% (none of the time) and at 100% (all of the time). All participants were then randomly assigned by the online software to one of three writing tasks: writing control, self-esteem, or self-compassion. These writing tasks were based on those used by Leary et al., (2007). The self-esteem and self- 22 compassion conditions had minimal changes from Leary’s self-esteem and selfcompassion writing tasks. Leary’s writing control condition was based on Pennebaker, Colder, and Sharp’s (1990) task that encouraged self-disclosure of emotion but this task had been shown to change how people feel about an event. In this study the aim was to create an emotionally neutral control task that would control for the process of writing, without changing the participants’ emotions. Therefore participants were asked to describe buildings on their university campus, write about their university, and being a student at their university. The following text was then displayed to all participants “In the next task we would like you to continue to think about the assignment that you did not do as well on as you had anticipated. We would like you to write about this by answering the questions below by typing in to the text boxes provided.” The remainder of the information on this screen diverged dependent on writing task. Each writing task asked participants to respond to three questions. For each questions there was a specified minimum number of characters required. The character required minimums were set based on pre-testing of these questions. The character required minimums were the same across the three conditions, the first question was set at 150 characters, the second question and third questions were set at 400 characters. At the bottom of these questions participants were presented with a button to click to proceed to the next screen. If participants had not typed enough to meet the minimum threshold they were prompted to provide more detail in their answer to that question. The next screen was a repetition of the state shame, guilt and pride measure. This screen was presented in the same way as the first time the measure was presented but the items were presented in a random order each time, for each 23 participant. Participants were presented with the following information on the next screen “We would be grateful if you would complete a few final questions about this study.” They were asked “What do you think this study was about?” which they completed by typing into a text box and “Were you suspicious at any point that the study was looking at something other than what was stated?” and were asked to click one of three options “Not at all”, “A little”, or “ A lot”. All participants were then asked to provide demographic information and to read the debriefing information. Dependant measures Maladaptive Perfectionism. Maladaptive perfectionism was measured using the discrepancy subscale of the APS-R (Slaney et al, 2001). The 23-item scale has three subscales: discrepancy, high standards, and order. The APS-R is the only perfectionism scale designed to capture the adaptive elements of perfectionism in one scale (high standards) and the maladaptive components of perfectionism in a separate scale (discrepancy). The APS-R has acceptable construct, concurrent, convergent and discriminant validity (Rice, Ashby & Slaney, 2007). The three factors have been shown to be reliable with a confirmatory factor analysis producing Cronbachs alphas that ranged from .85- .92 for the subscales (Slaney et al., 2001) and test-retest reliability is good (Rice & Aldea, 2006). In this sample Cronbachs alpha= .94. The discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) subscale has 12 items (e.g., “Doing my best never seems to be enough”) that participants rated from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Scores on these items were averaged together (the range of possible scores was from 1 to 7) and a higher mean indicated higher levels of discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism), 24 Shame, guilt, and pride. Shame, guilt, and pride were measured using the State Shame and Guilt Scale (SGSS: Marschall, Sanftner, & Tangney, 1994). Robins, Noftle, and Tracey (2007) note that the subscales have good Cronbachs alphas; shame (α = .89), guilt (α = .82), pride (α = .87). No other psychometric information is available however, this measure is often used in the published literature (Fedewa et al., 2005; Stöber et al., 2007). In this sample Cronbachs alpha for shame, guilt and pride at Time 1 = .87, .88, and .92 respectively and at Time 2= .90, .87, and .92 respectively. The SGSS has 15 items, (five items measure shame, guilt, and pride) which participants rated from 1 (not feeling this way at all) to 5 (feeling this way strongly). Example items for shame, guilt, and pride respectively are as follows “I want to sink into the floor and disappear”, “I feel remorse, regret”, and “I feel good about myself”. For each of the subscales the five item scores were averaged together (the range of possible scores was from 1 to 5) and higher scores represented higher state levels of that emotion. Participants One hundred and twenty-nine student participants were recruited from the University of Surrey. The study was advertised to undergraduate and postgraduate psychology students via the School of Psychology research participant recruitment system. Psychology students were offered either research credit or entry to a prizedraw for £50 (first prize) or £25 (two runner-up prizes) as payment for their participation. The study was also advertised to other University of Surrey students, by emailing the secretaries of other University of Surrey departments and asking them to circulate an advertisement for the study. Participants recruited via the latter method were offered entry to the prize draw only because they were not eligible for 25 research credit. Twenty-eight participants were removed from the final dataset because they had not completed the study. Eleven of these did not progress as far as completing the first task (measure of perfectionism), one dropped out after the measure of perfectionism, and the remaining 16 dropped out before completing the writing task. The online nature of the study meant that it was difficult to determine whether people had completed the study in one sitting, therefore, a further six participants who took longer than two hours to complete the study were excluded, leaving N=95. A power analysis conducted using G*Power version 3.1.9.2 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) suggested that a sample size of 158 would be required to detect the proposed main effect in ANCOVA if it had a medium effect size. Thus it is possible that this study was underpowered. It was plausible that individuals who were more perfectionistic may have been more likely to dropout of the study because they did not want to imagine failing, or may have been more likely to procrastinate and been removed from the sample for taking too long to complete the study. In order to test this possibility independent t-tests were computed. In order for t-tests to be valid the data needed to be normally distributed on discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) for both groups, and there should be homogeneity of variance across the two groups. Histograms on discrepancy for the two groups with Z scores of skew, and kurtosis suggested that both groups had an approximately normal distribution (see Appendix E) and the Levine’s test suggested that there was a marginally non-significant difference in variance across the two groups (F (1, 116)= 3.79, p = .05). Given this marginal result the statistics for the adjusted t-test that does not assume equal variances are reported. The t-test revealed no significant differences in discrepancy (maladaptive 26 perfectionism) between those who did not complete the study and those in the final sample (t (47.36) = -1.10, p = .27, Mnon-completers= 4.01 vs. Mcompleters= 4.26). This suggests that those who completed the study did not differ in how perfectionistic they were compared to those who did not complete the study. Results Data handling All data were collected by the online survey software Qualtrics. The online study had been programmed to code the data such that any items were reverse-coded as necessary. The data was downloaded from the online survey software and checked for any errors. There were no errors and no missing data that needed to be addressed. All variables were explored for outliers using boxplots and z-scores as described in Field, (2009, p.99-103). For variables where significant outliers were identified the outlying data-points were removed. This resulted in 2 outliers being removed from the imageability scale. Means were calculated for each scale/ subscale with items reversed as necessary. The means, standard deviations, ranges and for all variables can be seen in Table 1. Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, Ranges for all Variables. Variable Discrepancy N 95 Mean (SD) 4.26 (1.27) Range 5.75 Imageability (3 items) 93 8.06 (1.47) 9.00 Percentage time (1 item) 95 67.4 (21.0) 95.00 Time 1 shame 95 2.62 (1.08) 4.00 Time 2 shame 95 1.97 (0.96) 3.60 (Maladaptive perfectionism) 27 Time 1 guilt 95 2.79 (1.06) 4.00 Time 2 guilt 95 2.06 (0.90) 3.60 Time 1 pride 95 2.08 (0.85) 3.20 Time 2 pride 95 2.72 (1.00) 4.00 Analytic approach All analyses were conducted in SPSS Statistics (Version 21, IBM Corporation, 2012). A correlational analysis was conducted to consider whether maladaptive perfectionism, was associated with shame, guilt, and pride (following imagined failure). ANCOVA were conducted to consider whether following imagined failure there was a significant effect of writing task on Time 2 levels of shame, guilt and pride. Finally a content analysis was conducted to aid understanding of the findings. Correlational analysis Pearson’s product-moment correlations were calculated to assess the extent to which discrepancy, shame, guilt and pride and the manipulation check variables percentage time and imageability were linearly associated. For significance tests of correlation coefficients to be valid it is necessary that one of the variables has an underlying Normal distribution (Altman 1991). Examination of histograms of the variables and Z scores for skew and kurtosis (see Appendix F) suggests that there was some deviation from Normality for Time 1 shame, Time 1 pride, imageability, and percentage time, Time 2 guilt and Time 2 shame. Therefore Spearman's correlation coefficients were also calculated. Comparison of Pearson's correlation (Table 2) with Spearman's (Appendix G) shows that the pattern of results was similar for both methods. The pairs of variables involved in the predicted 28 Table 2. Bivariate Pearson’s Correlations for Time 1 Variables. Imageabili Percentage Time 1 Time 1 Time 1 Time 2 Time 2 Time 2 ty time shame guilt pride shame guilt pride) Discrepancy r = .17 r = .33** r = .48*** r = .40*** r = -.33** r = .50*** r = .43*** r = -.30** (n=95) p = .10 p = .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .003 Imageability r = .17 r = .21 r = .09 r = -.17 r = .10 r = .13 r = -.09 (n=93) p = .09 p = .05 p =.39 p =.10 p = .34 p = .23 p = .41 Percentage time r = .37*** r = .32** r = -.16 r = .26* r = .12 r = .03 (n=95) p < .001 p = .002 p = .12 p = .01 p = .25 p = .78 Time 1 shame r = .78*** r = -.68*** r = .63*** r = .51*** r = -.33** (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .001 Time 1 guilt r = -.49*** r = .61*** r = .65*** r = -.22* (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .03 Time 1 pride r = -.44*** r = -.30** r = .52*** (n=95) p < .001 p = .003 p < .001 *** Time 2 shame r = .83 (n=95) p < .001 r = -.58*** p < .001 Time 2 guilt r = -.51*** (n=95) p < .001 Note- Asterisks denote the significance level * p < .05, ** p < .01 and *** p <.001 29 correlations between discrepancy and Time 1 emotions met the assumptions for Pearson’s correlations (Altman, 1991) because discrepancy appeared normally distributed and so are reported below. There was a positive association between discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) and Time 1 shame and between discrepancy and Time 1 guilt. There was also a negative association between discrepancy and Time 1 pride. These findings supported the initial hypotheses. It was also important to consider whether the manipulation check measures of imageability and percentage time spent imagining were associated with maladaptive perfectionism. Discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) was not related to the imageability of the failure scenario, however, there was a significant positive relationship between discrepancy and estimated percentage of time spent thinking about the imagined failure scenario. This suggests that those who endorsed maladaptive perfectionism more strongly also reported spending more of the allotted time thinking about the imagined failure scenario. Effect of writing condition on Time 1 variables Prior to conducting the ANCOVA it was important to consider whether there was a significant effect of writing task on the Time 1 variables. Participants were randomly allocated to writing task by the survey software (after completing all of the Time 1 variables) so any differences would be due to chance. A series of 3 (writing task: control vs. self-esteem vs. self-compassion) one-way ANOVAs were conducted to assess this. In this study the group sizes are approximately equal and so ANOVA is robust and accurate and can cope with deviations from normality and homogeneity of variance (Field, 2013). However, these assumptions were still considered. Histograms of the residuals (with Z scores for skew and kurtosis) and Levine’s test for homogeneity of variance can be seen in Appendices H-I. These results show that 30 there was some lack of normality of distribution for imageability, Time 1 shame, and Time 1 pride, and there was heterogeneity of variance for discrepancy. Although due to equal group sizes ANOVA should be robust to these deviations, the analysis was repeated (for those variables that showed wither a lack of normality or heterogeneity of variance) using Kruskal-Wallis tests to check the robustness. The Kruskal-Wallis tests can be seen in Appendix J and they do not materially differ from the one-way ANOVA presented below. The one-way ANOVAs revealed that there were no significant effects of writing conditions on discrepancy (F (2, 92) = .10, p > .05, η2= .0021) imageability (F (2, 90) = .15, p > .05, η2= .0033), percentage time spent on failure scenario (F (2, 92) = 1.37, p > .05, η2= .029), Time 1 shame (F (2, 92) = .06, p > .05, η2= .0013), Time 1 guilt (F (2, 92) = .39, p > .05, η2= .0084), nor Time 1 pride (F (2, 92) = 1.57, p > .05, η2= .033). The randomisation had not resulted in an unequal distribution of characteristics across the three groups therefore the ANCOVA could be conducted as planned. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) Analyses The Time 1 and Time 2 means for shame, guilt and pride can be seen in Figure 1, 2 and 3 respectively. These figures suggest that for all conditions the levels of shame and guilt are lower and the levels of pride are higher at Time 2 (following completion of writing tasks) than following completion of the imagery failure task. 31 3 2.5 Shame 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 Time 1 Control Self-esteem Time 2 (model 3) Self-compassion Figure 1. Means for Shame as a function of writing condition at Time 1 and Time 2 (model 3) N=95. 3.5 3 Guilt 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 Time 1 Control Self-esteem Time 2 (model 3) Self-compassion Figure 2. Means for guilt as a function of writing condition at Time 1 and Time 2 (model 3) N=95. 32 3.5 3 Pride 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 Time 1 Control Self-esteem Time 2 (model 3) Self-compassion Figure 3- Means for pride as a function of writing condition at Time 1 and Time 2 (model 3) N=95. ANCOVAs were conducted to assess whether there was a significant effect of writing task on Time 2 emotion, after Time 1 emotion and discrepancy were controlled for. Three separate ANCOVAs were conducted for shame, guilt and pride respectively. ANCOVA was chosen rather than examining change from baseline (by conducting a 3 (writing task) x 2 (time) repeated measures ANOVA) because change from baseline methods make more assumptions about the relationship between Time 1 and Time 2, which potentially introduces bias and reduces statistical power (Everitt & Wessely, 2008 p. 93-96; Senn, 2006). Due to the equal group sizes meeting the assumptions required for ANOVA is not critical (Field, 2013). Despite this these assumptions were still considered and the histograms of the residuals for ANCOVA with Z scores for skew and kurtosis and the Levine’s tests can be seen in Appendices K-L. These results show that normality assumptions were met for all variables. There was some heterogeneity of 33 variance for Time 2 guilt but given the equal group sizes the results are robust to this (Field, 2013). In each of the three ANCOVAs the covariate Time 1 emotion was significantly related to Time 2 levels of emotion. Time 1 shame was significantly related to Time 2 shame (F (2, 90) = 35.00, p < .001, r = .53, Time 1 guilt was significantly related to Time 2 guilt (F (2, 90) = 52.05, p < .001, r = .61 and Time 1 pride was significantly related to Time 2 pride (F (2, 90) = 20.85, p < .001, r = .43 The covariate discrepancy was significantly related to Time 2 shame F (2, 90) = 8.18, p < .05, r = .29, and Time 2 guilt F (2, 90) = 5.79, p < .05, r = .25, but not to Time 2 pride F (2, 90) = 3.05, p > .05, r = .18. It was hypothesised that there would be a significant effect of writing task on Time 2 emotions. Specifically it was predicted that the self-compassionate writing would lead to lower levels of shame (and guilt and higher levels of pride) than the writing control task, and the self-esteem writing task. Contrary to these predictions there was no significant effect of writing task on Time 2 shame after controlling for the effect of Time 1 shame and discrepancy (F (2, 90) = 2.40, p > .05, partial η2= .05, Mcontrol = 1.80 Mself-esteem= 1.93, Mself-compassion= 2.17). There was a significant effect of writing task on Time 2 guilt after controlling for the effect of Time 1 guilt and discrepancy (F (2, 90) = 4.24, p < .05, partial η2= .09) and a significant effect of writing task on Time 2 pride after controlling for the effect of Time 1 pride and discrepancy (F (2, 90) = 4.72, p < .05, partial η2= .09). An examination of the means for Time 2 guilt and Time 2 pride (see Table 3) suggests that whilst writing task does predict Time 2 emotions it does not predict Time 2 emotions in the way that was hypothesised. Post hoc tests (Bonferroni) were conducted to decompose the significant 34 effects of writing task on Time 2 emotions (see Table 3). There were significant differences between the control condition and the self-compassion condition on both pride (p < .01) and guilt (p < .05) such that pride is higher in the writing control condition than the self-compassion condition and guilt is higher in the selfcompassion condition than the writing control condition. There was also a marginally non-significant difference (p = .05) suggesting that guilt was also higher in the self-compassion condition than the self-esteem writing condition. All other comparisons failed to reach significance (p > .05). Table 3. Means for Time 2 Emotion Variables where there was a Significant Effect of Writing Condition for Post-hoc Analysis (N = 95). Control Self-esteem Selfcompassion Time 2 guilt 1.91a 1.92ab 2.32b Time 2 pride 3.07a 2.70ab 2.44b Note- Superscripts denote the post hoc Bonferrni tests. Within rows means that do not share a superscript letter differ at p < .05. Content Analysis The effect of the writing tasks on pride, shame and guilt were not as originally predicted. Therefore a content analysis was conducted on participant’s responses to writing tasks. The answers to all three questions provided in the writing task were considered as one text for each participant. The text was analysed using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC). LIWC is a text analysis software program designed by Pennebaker, Booth, and Francis (2007). LIWC analyses text on a word-by-word basis and calculates the number of words people have used and the degree to which people use different categories of words. Each word is compared against a file of more than 2,000 reference words, divided into 72 categories. After 35 counting the number of words in each category, the output is given as a percentage of the total words in the text sample. Newman, Pennebaker, Berry, and Richards (2003) note that although computerized word count approaches are typically blind to context they show promising and reliable results in personality, social, and clinical psychology (Mergenthaler, 1996; Pennebaker et al., 2001; Rosenberg & Tucker, 1979; Stone, Dunphy, Smith, & Ogilvy, 1966). The dimensions captured by LIWC have been used to predict social judgments (Berry, Pennebaker, Mueller, & Hiller, 1997), personality (Pennebaker & King, 1999), psychological adjustment (Rude, Gortner, & Pennebaker, 2004), and health (Pennebaker, Mayne, & Francis, 1997). The LIWC considers categories that fall under four superordinate category areas; these are linguistic processes, psychological processes, personal concerns, and spoken categories. Herein, 11 categories from across the first three superordinate categories were selected on the basis of their potential to explain the findings. The categories selected were (1) overall word count, (2) first-person pronouns singular, (3) third-person pronouns singular, (4) third-person pronouns plural, (5) past-tense, (6) present-tense, (7) future-tense, (8) positive emotions, (9) negative emotions, (10) work and (11) achievement. The rationale for selecting these categories is outlined below. Word count was chosen to determine whether the different writing tasks had generated different amounts of written text. It was predicted that there would be no differences in word count between writing tasks because they had been constructed in similar ways to try and reduce this possibility. It is possible that the way the failure imagery task was written about could increase or decrease the extent to which participants experience self-conscious emotions. Specifically writing about the imagined failure in the third rather than first-person may decrease the extent to which the person feels shame or guilt about 36 their performance, indeed this is what the self-compassion condition encourages (thinking about others who may have had similar experiences). Similarly the tense in which the imagined failure is written about may also change the effect of the failure; there may be less shame if the person has consigned the failure to the past, rather than construing it in the present. The negative and positive emotion words were considered because the frequency of these emotion words could have affected the extent to which participants reported feeling shame, guilt or pride. One may have expected higher levels of positive emotion words in the self-esteem condition. Finally because the task failed was an academic assignment it is plausible that, in the different writing conditions, people may have varied in the extent to which they wrote about work and achievement, for example in the self-esteem condition people may have talked about achievement more and work less than in other conditions. LIWC calculated the frequency scores for these variables. Word count is a total of words typed, however, for the other variables LIWC calculates a frequency percentage by dividing the frequency of the occurrence of the category words by the total number of words. The variables calculated from the content analysis were explored for outliers using boxplots and Z-scores as described in Field, (2009, p.99103). The outlier analysis highlighted that a number of data-points from two participants were outliers on a number of these variables. These participants’ datapoints were outliers because the percentage frequency scores were inflated due to small total word counts (14 and 16 words). Therefore although these participants responses were only identified outliers on particular variables they had the potential to bias all variables calculated as percentage of total text, therefore these participants data are only included in the analysis of the word count variable and are excluded from the subsequent analysis of variables calculated as a percentage of total text. 37 Due to the equal group sizes meeting the assumptions for ANOVA is not critical, but despite this the assumptions were still considered. The histograms of the residuals with Z scores for skew and kurtosis and Levine’s tests can be seen in Appendices M-N. Some of the histograms revealed a skew that was consistent with a floor effect. This was particularly pronounced for the third-person singular category where 91 out of 95 participants had not written any words of that category in their text (a score of 0%). Therefore no further analysis was conducted on this variable. There was evidence of non-norrmality and heterogeneity for most of the variables so although results should be robust based on equal group sizes (Field, 2013) the analysis was repeated using Kruskal-Wallis to check the robustness. The results for the Kruskal-Wallis are shown in Appendix O and do not differ from the ANOVA in terms of significance or pattern of means and mean ranks. The results of the one-way ANOVA on writing task are summarised in Table 4. Reviewing the statistics in Table 4, the number of words written (word count) did not vary across writing conditions. This suggests that it is content of participants writing rather than the amount of writing that influenced their guilt and pride following the writing tasks. There was a significant main effect of writing condition on the following LIWC variables first-person singular, third-person plural, past-tense, future-tense, negative emotions, work and achievement words. Post-hoc tests (Bonferroni) were conducted to decompose the significant main effects of writing task on the LIWC variables (see Table 4.) Below I discuss only the LIWC variables that differed significantly by writing condition. 38 Table 4. Means, Standard Deviations and Statistics for One-way ANOVAs investigating the effect of writing condition on LIWC variables Dependant variable Means Control SD F (DOF, p η2 Self- Self- esteem compassion 128.83 134.14 118.40 53.95 0.71 (2, 92) > .05 .02 1st person singular (% of text) 2.14a 8.94 b 5.05 c 3.87 23.26 (2, 90) <. 001 .43 3rd person plural (% of text) 0.20 a 0.29 a 1.85 b 1.12 22.93 (2, 90) <. 001 .33 Past-tense (% of text) 0.50 a 4.79 b 5.60 b 2.40 41.45 (2, 90) <. 001 .50 Present-tense (% of text) 10.04 10.98 10.27 4.86 0.31 (2, 90) > .05 .05 Future-tense (% of text) 0.05 a 1.69 b 2.74 c 1.43 29.34 (2, 90) <. 001 .39 Positive emotions (% of text) 5.20 6.61 4.68 4.22 1.73 (2, 90) > .05 .02 Negative emotions (% of text) 0.76 a 2.23 b 5.12 c 1.97 42.09 (2, 90) <. 001 .50 Work (% of text) 8.88 a 6.04 b 5.26 b 3.12 11.93 (2, 90) <. 001 .25 Achievement (% of text) 2.23 a 5.66 b 4.96 b 3.15 10.19 (2, 90) <. 001 .19 Word count DOE) Note- Superscripts denote the post-hoc Bonferrni tests. Within rows Means that do not share a superscript letter differ at p < .05. 39 The use of first-person singular words is highest in the self-esteem condition and lowest in the control condition, with the self-compassion condition falling between these two means and all of these differences are significant. The use of third-person plural words is significantly higher in the self-compassion condition than, in the self-esteem condition or in the control condition. It is notable that all of the percentage scores for third-person plural words are low indicating that there is little use of these types of words in the text produced in each condition. The use of past-tense words was significantly lower in the control condition than in the selfesteem and self-compassion conditions. The use of future-tense words was lowest in the control condition and highest in the self-compassion condition, with the selfesteem condition falling between these two conditions, all of these differences were significant. It is notable that the use of future-tense words was relatively low across conditions. The use of negative emotions words was much lower in the control condition than in the self-compassion condition, with the self-esteem condition falling between these two conditions, all of these differences were significant. The use of words about work was significantly higher in the control condition than in the self-esteem and self-compassion conditions, which did not differ from each other. It is interesting to consider how the results of the analysis of the written text fit with the effects of writing condition of self-conscious emotions. The means suggested that Time 2 guilt and shame were lower than Time 1 guilt and shame and that Time 2 pride was higher than Time 1 pride. The ANCOVA revealed that there was a significant effect of writing condition on both Time 2 pride and Time 2 guilt. Time 2 guilt was significantly higher in the self-compassion writing condition than in the control condition and there was a trend towards guilt being higher in the selfcompassion than the self-esteem writing condition. Time 2 pride was higher in the 40 control condition than in the self-compassion condition and the self-compassion and self-esteem conditions did not differ from each other. Comparing the pattern of means on pride with the patterns of means on the LIWC variables, the only variable that shows a similar pattern of means is the “work” words variable. It is possible that the opportunity to describe their University is related to this increase in pride. Comparing the patterns of means on guilt with the patterns of means on the LIWC variables, the only variable where self-compassion was clearly different from both self-esteem and control conditions (which were broadly similar to each other) is on the third-person plural pronouns variable. Discussion This study aimed to address two separate research questions. The first research question was to identify if there was a relationship between maladaptive perfectionism and the experience of self-conscious emotions following an imagined failure to meet self-defined standards for performance. The second research question was to determine whether a self-compassionate writing task could reduce the experience of shame and guilt and increase the experience of pride compared to selfesteem or writing control tasks. Below I discuss the findings of this study in relation to the research literature considering each of the research questions in turn. Maladaptive perfectionism, and self-conscious emotions It was hypothesised that those who endorsed maladaptive perfectionism more strongly would also report more shame and guilt and less pride following imagining failing to meet their self-defined standards on a piece of academic coursework. Maladaptive perfectionism (discrepancy) was found to be positively associated with Time 1 shame and Time 1 guilt and negatively associated with Time 1 pride. The findings support these hypotheses. 41 A number of studies have investigated the relationship between perfectionism and self-conscious emotions (Fedewa et al, 2005; Klibert, et al, 2005; Tangney, 2002; Stöber et al, 2007) but as noted above the variation of in the way that perfectionism and self-conscious emotions were measured makes these findings difficult to interpret. However, there was some evidence that an individual’s level of perfectionism was related to their proneness to experience shame and to their experience of state shame and guilt. The current findings appear to conceptually replicate the findings of Stöber et al (2007) who demonstrated that ‘unhealthy’ perfectionists experienced more shame and guilt and less pride than ‘healthy’ perfectionists. However, surprisingly Steober’s unhealthy perfectionists did not differ in shame, guilt and pride from those he defined as non-perfectionists. Comparing Stöber et al.’s findings to the current findings may explain why. Stöber et al. used median splits to categorise their sample on the basis of participants’ ratings on both high standards and discrepancy subscales to create the three groups. “Unhealthy perfectionists” were those with high scores on both the high standards and the discrepancy subscales of the APS-R, and “nonperfectionists” were those with below median scores on high standards and any score on discrepancy. Hence, in Stöber et al.’s study, unhealthy perfectionists and nonperfectionists could have the same score on discrepancy. The current study argues that variation in discrepancy alone represents the best measure of maladaptive/ unhealthy perfectionism. By categorising participants using their ratings on the subscale for high standards, Stöber et al. implicitly assume that what represents high standards is the same for everyone. However, individuals vary in their level of ability to complete various tasks and so the same objective “high standards” could be achievable and realistic for one person and excessively high for another individual. 42 This highlights one of the strengths of the current study, which was to ensure that the standards that participants failed to meet were self-defined standards for performance. Interestingly the findings from the first part of the study suggest that those participants who endorsed discrepancy more strongly also reported that they spent a greater percentage of the allocated time thinking about the failure scenario. Assuming that these self-reports are accurate, this hints at several possibilities. First, that those participants who report spending longer on the failure task experience more shame and guilt and less pride (regardless of level of perfectionism), second that maladaptive perfectionists focus more on evidence that they have failed to meet their standards, or third, both of the above. Disentangling these explanations represents an interesting question for future study. Self-compassion and self-conscious emotions The second part of the study tested two hypotheses: first, whether a selfcompassionate writing intervention reduced state shame and guilt and increased state pride relative to a self-esteem and control writing task. The means suggested that all conditions lead to reduced shame and guilt and increased pride. However, none of these hypotheses about the differential effects of writing condition were supported. Levels of shame did not significantly vary across writing condition. Although this study may have been underpowered there was variability in the means for guilt and pride, so it appears that this may only have been problematic for shame. Levels of guilt and pride did vary across writing condition but this variation did not support the predictions. Higher levels of pride were found in the control condition compared to the self-compassion condition, and higher levels of guilt were found in the selfcompassion condition compared to the control condition. 43 An exploratory content analysis was conducted using LIWC to consider what might underlie these differences. These analyses revealed that the length of written content did not vary across writing conditions. However, individuals who completed the self-esteem writing task used more first-person singular words than those who completed other writing tasks. Individuals who completed the self-compassion writing task used more third-person plural words that those who completed other writing tasks. These findings suggest that the task instructions were followed because the self-esteem condition promoted a focus on the self and the selfcompassion condition a focus on common humanity. Similarly there was evidence that there was a lower frequency of past-tense words and future tense words used in the control condition than in the other two writing conditions. This most likely reflects the nature of the control task. Furthermore the frequency of both past- and future-tense words was highest in the self-compassion condition suggesting that these participants were locating this experience amongst their own and others’ similar previous experiences and anticipated future experiences. The frequency of achievement words is lower in the control condition that the other writing conditions This is likely because in the control condition individuals are describing their university whereas the other writing tasks ask individuals to write about their response to the failure scenario. Comparing the pattern of means and post-hoc tests on guilt and pride with the LIWC variables suggested that the pattern of means for Time 2 pride was similar to the pattern of means for frequency of words about work. Participants were asked to write about their University in the control condition and so higher frequency of words about work and higher levels of pride makes intuitive sense. Whist participants were asked to describe the campus, list the buildings, and describe what 44 it was like to be a student, an examination of the written text reveals that some students used the task to describe their pride at being a University of Surrey student. It is notable that during the period the study was conducted the University of Surrey was ranked 6th in the Guardian league table and this was heavily publicised on the University campus. The Social Identity approach (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher & Wetherall, 1987) suggests that individuals will consider themselves in terms of their most salient identity. For participants who had just been asked to imagine failing a University assignment and who were subsequently asked to describe their University, their identity as a University student was particularly salient. Furthermore, identifying with this group identity provides a higher status and represents an adaptive means of raising self-esteem - whilst they as an individual have failed, they will succeed as a University of Surrey student. The findings suggested that guilt scores were higher following selfcompassionate writing than following control writing (although an examination of the means suggests that both means were lower than the scores at Time 1). Similarly, there was evidence of a higher frequency of negative emotion words in the selfcompassion condition than in the control condition. This finding could be explained by the nature of the self-compassionate writing task instructions. Indeed, Neff (2003b) suggests that self-compassion creates coping by encouraging individuals to approach their emotions, rather than avoid their emotions. Thus it may be that those who completed self-compassionate writing tasks are experiencing more negative emotions. This argument suggests that an increased frequency of negative emotions words should also be accompanied by higher levels of shame following selfcompassionate writing. Indeed, a consideration of the Time 2 means for shame suggests a similar albeit non-significant pattern. This may be attributable to a lack of 45 power. Considering the self-compassion task instructions may also provide an explanation for why the reduction of shame is not impaired to the same extent as guilt. The instructions focussed on the imagined failure of the task, and so the negative emotions were about a particular event rather than about the individual person. Thus the task instructions may have been more likely to impair reduction in guilt, because guilt is often about an action, whereas shame is about the person (Lewis, 1971). Limitations and suggestions for future research The current study has a number of potential limitations. The hypotheses were tested in a specific context, which was that of academic performance and imagined failure. This represents only one domain in which individuals may express their perfectionism, and one type of failure within this domain. Similarly, self-compassion was manipulated using only one approach, that of writing. In clinical interventions a variety of methods are used including imagery, compassionate letter writing, and compassionate mindfulness. It is possible that the use of different methods may have led to different results. The measure of state shame, guilt and pride (Marschall et al., 1997) was used because it is commonly used within the literature and was the only measure of state shame, guilt and pride that was available. In this study a nonclinical population (university students) was used. All ratings of self-conscious emotions were around or below the midpoint of the scale and so perhaps there was not a great deal of guilt and shame to reduce. It is currently unclear to what extent self-compassionate writing is a useful strategy for individuals from non-clinical population. It may be that self-compassionate approaches are more effective than other approaches such as self-esteem and control writing in clinical populations only. One strength of the study was that the failure that participants were exposed 46 to was imagined. Whilst this could be considered a weakness compared to placing participants in a manipulated objective failure scenario, there is good evidence that imagery can create similar emotional and motivational responses to those of real experiences (Dadds, et al, 1997) and indeed imagery and re-scripting is an important aspect of clinical assessment and intervention (Hackmann, et al, 2011). Furthermore it was the use of imagery that allowed participants to individually self-define the standards that they expected to meet. It is failure to meet one’s own standards that is central to the definition of perfectionism and so with this in mind, it seems that using a task that allowed for failure to meet self-defined standards represented a strength of this approach. This study represent a first step in examining the relationship between selfcompassion, shame, guilt and pride. The findings would be strengthened by increasing their generalizability by considering alternative domains of perfectionism, different control conditions, different manipulations of failure and self-compassion and different measures of self-conscious emotions. It would also be important to clearly define the populations who may benefit from self-compassionate writing interventions. Future research should consider whether a self-compassionate writing task reduces state shame, when compared to self-esteem, expressive writing and writing control tasks, and whether any reductions in state shame are accompanied by reductions in shame-proneness. Research should also consider whether repeated exposures to self-compassionate writing would increase the usefulness of the selfcompassionate writing task in improving self-conscious emotions following failure, and whether there is an optimal number of exposures required to create this effect. 47 References Altman, D. G. (1991). Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman and Hall. Barnard, L. K., & Curry, J. F. (2012). The relationship of clergy burnout to selfcompassion and other personality dimensions. Pastoral Psychology, 61, 149– 163. doi: 10.1007/s11089-011-0377-0 Berry, D. S., Pennebaker, J. W., Mueller, J. S., & Hiller, W. S. (1997). Linguistic bases of social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 526-537. doi: 10.1177/0146167297235008 Bieling, P. J., Israeli, A. L., & Anthony, M. M. (2004). Is perfectionism good, bad, or both? Examining models of the perfectionism construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1373-1385. doi: 10.1016%2FS01918869%2803%2900235-6 Dadds, M. R., Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd, W. H., & Cutmore, T. R. (1997). Imagery in human classical conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 89 –103. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.122.1.89 Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., & Berg, J. (2012). Perfectionism dimensions and the five-factor model of personality. European Journal of Personality, 26, 233-244. doi: 10.1002/per.829 Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: A clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 203–212. doi: 0.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.009. Enns, M.W., Cox, B. J. & Clara, (2002). Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism: developmental origins and association with depression proneness. Personality and Individual Differences 33, 921–935. doi:10.1016%2FS0191- 48 8869%2801%2900202-1 Everitt , B. S., & Wessely, S. (2008). Clinical Trials in Psychiatry (2nd edition). Chicester, West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191. Fedewa, B. A., Burns, L. R., & Gomez, A. A. (2005). Positive and negative perfectionism and the shame/guilt distinction: Adaptive and maladaptive characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1609-1619. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.026. Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (2nd Edition). London, UK: Sage. Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th Edition). London, UK: Sage. Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C. M., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468. doi: 10.1007%2FBF01172967 Gilbert, P. (1998). What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. In, P. Gilbert & B. Andrews, (eds) Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology and culture. (pp 3-38). New York: Oxford University Press. Gilbert, P. (2003). Evolution, social roles, and differences in shame and guilt. Social Research: An International Quarterly of the Social Sciences 70, 1205-1230. doi: not available. Gilbert, P., Baldwin, M., Irons, C., Baccus, J., & Clark, M. (2006). Self-criticism and self-warmth: An imagery study exploring their relation to depression. Journal 49 of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 20, 183–200. doi: 10.1891%2Fjcop.20.2.183. Gilbert, P., Baldwin, M. W., Irons, C., Baccus, J. R., & Palmer, M. (2006). Selfcriticism and self-warmth: An imagery study exploring their relation to depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 20 (2),183-200. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/jcop.20.2.183. Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame and self-criticism: A pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 353–379. doi: 10.1002/cpp.507 Hackmann, A., Bennett-Levy, J., & Holmes, E. A. (2011). Oxford Guide to Imagery in Cognitive Therapy. OUP: Oxford. Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism. Psychology, 15(1), 27-33. doi: not available. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456–470. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.60.3.456. IBM Corporation. (2012). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. [Computer software] Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. Johnson, E. A., & O’Brien, K. A. (2013). Self-compassion soothes the savage egothreat system: Effects on negative affect, shame, rumination and depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32(9), 939-963. doi:10.1521/jscp.2013.32.9.939. Judge, L., Cleghorn, A., McEwan, K., & Gilbert, P. (2012). An exploration of groupbased compassion focused therapy for a heterogeneous range of clients presenting to a Community Mental Health Team. International Journal of 50 Cognitive Therapy, 5(4), 420–429, doi: 10.1521%2Fijct.2012.5.4.420 Klibert, J. J., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Saito, M. (2005). Adaptive and maladaptive aspects of self-oriented versus socially prescribed perfectionism. Journal of College Student Development, 46, 141–156. doi: 10.1353%2Fcsd.2005.0017, Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Hancock, J. (2007). Selfcompassion and reactions to unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications of treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 887–904. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.887. Leary, M. R., & MacDonald, G. (2003). Individual differences in self- esteem: A review and theoretical integration. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 401–418). New York: Guilford Press. Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press. Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transformations in appraisals for pride, guilt and shame. In Fischer, K. W. & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.), Self-Conscious Emotions: Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment and Pride (pp. 64-113). New York: Guilford.Mas Marschall, D. E., Saftner, J., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). The State Shame and Guilt Scale. George Mason University, Fairfax, VA. Mergenthaler, E. (1996). Emotion-abstraction patterns in verbatim protocols: A new way of describing psychotherapeutic processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1306-1315. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.64.6.1306 Miller, S. B. (1996). Shame in context. London: The Analytic Press. Neff, K. D. (2003a). The development and validation of a scale to measure self- 51 compassion. Self & Identity, 2, 223–250. doi:10.1080/15298860390209035. Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–102. doi:10.1080/15298860390129863. Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-Compassion. New York: HarperCollins. Newman, M. L., Pennebaker, J. W., Berry, D. S., & Richards, J. M. (2003). Lying words: Predicting deception from linguistic styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(5), 665-675. doi: 10.1177/0146167203251529. Pennebaker, J.W., Booth, R.J., & Francis, M.E. (2007). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2007. Austin, TX: LIWC (www.liwc.net). Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 528–537. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.58.3.528. Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles: Language use as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 12961312. doi: 0.1037//0022-3514.77.6.1296 Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 863-871. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.4.863 Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2001. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Reimer, M. S. (1996). “Sinking into the ground”: The developmental consequences of shame in adolescence. Developmental Review, 16, 321-363. doi:10.1006/drev.1996.0015 Rice, K. G., & Aldea, M. A. (2006). State dependence and trait stability of 52 perfectionism: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 205-213. doi: 10.1037%2F0022-0167.53.2.205 Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. B. (1998). Self-esteem as a mediator between perfectionism and depression: A structural equation analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 304-314. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F00220167.45.3.304 Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. B. (2007). Perfectionism and the five-factor model of personality. Assessment, 14, 385-398. doi: 10.1177/1073191107303217 Robins, R. W., Noftle, E. E., & Tracey, J. L. (2007). Assessing Self-Conscious Emotions: A Review of Self-Report and Nonverbal Measures (pp. 443-469). In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. M. (Eds.) The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford. Rosenberg, S. D., & Tucker, G. J. (1979). Verbal behavior and schizophrenia: The semantic dimension. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 1331-1337. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1979.01780120061008 Rude, S.S., Gortner, E.M., & Pennebaker, J.W. (2004). Language use of depressed and depression- vulnerable college students. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 1121-1133. doi:10.1080/02699930441000030 Senn, S. (2006). Change from baseline and analysis of covariance revisited. Statistics in Medicine, 25, 4334-4344 . doi:10.1002/sim.2682 Shafran, R., Egan, S., & Wade, T. (2010). Overcoming perfectionism. London: Constable and Robinson. Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised Almost Perfect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling 53 and Development, 34, 130–145. doi: not available. Sorotzkin, B. (1985). The quest for perfection: Avoiding guilt or avoiding shame? Psychotherapy, 22, 564–571. DOI: 10.1037/h0085541 Stöber, J., Harris, R. A., & Moon, P. S. (2007). Perfectionism and the experience of pride, shame, and guilt: Comparing healthy perfectionists, unhealthy perfectionists, and non-perfectionists. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 131-141. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.012. Stöber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295– 319. doi: 10.1207%2Fs15327957pspr1004_2 Stone, P. J., Dunphy, D. C., Smith, M. S., & Ogilvy, D. M. (1966). The General Inquirer: A computer approach to content analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Suddarth, B. H., & Slaney, R. B. (2001). An investigation of the dimensions of perfectionism in college students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 157-165. doi: not available. Tajfel, H. & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (eds), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole, pp. 33-47. Tangney, J. P. (2002). Perfectionism and the self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. In P. L. Hewitt & G. L. Flett (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 199-215). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford. Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes P. J., Reicher S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). 54 Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory. Oxford and New York: Basil Blackwell. 55 List of Appendices Appendix A ............................................................................................................... 58 Instructions for authors- British Journal of Clinical Psychology .......................... 58 Appendix B ............................................................................................................... 62 Summary of procedure ........................................................................................... 62 Appendix C ............................................................................................................... 63 Proof of ethical review ........................................................................................... 63 Appendix D ............................................................................................................... 64 Information sheet ................................................................................................... 64 Consent sheet ......................................................................................................... 66 Measure of Perfectionism ...................................................................................... 67 Failure imagery task .............................................................................................. 70 State Shame and Guilt scale ................................................................................... 71 Manipulation check ................................................................................................ 73 Writing tasks .......................................................................................................... 74 Feedback and demographics.................................................................................. 75 Debrief.................................................................................................................... 77 Appendix E ............................................................................................................... 78 Histograms for discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) ..................................... 78 Appendix F................................................................................................................ 79 Histograms of variables for correlations ............................................................... 79 Appendix G ............................................................................................................... 84 56 Appendix H ............................................................................................................... 85 Histograms of residuals from one- way ANOVA’s ................................................ 85 Appendix I ................................................................................................................ 88 Appendix J ................................................................................................................ 89 Appendix K ............................................................................................................... 90 Histograms of the residuals for all ANCOVAs on shame, guilt and pride. ........... 90 Appendix L ............................................................................................................... 92 Appendix M .............................................................................................................. 93 Histograms of residuals for ANOVA on LIWC variables ...................................... 93 Appendix N ............................................................................................................... 99 Appendix O ............................................................................................................. 100 57 Appendix A Instructions for authors- British Journal of Clinical Psychology The British Journal of Clinical Psychology publishes original contributions to scientific knowledge in clinical psychology. This includes descriptive comparisons, as well as studies of the assessment, aetiology and treatment of people with a wide range of psychological problems in all age groups and settings. The level of analysis of studies ranges from biological influences on individual behaviour through to studies of psychological interventions and treatments on individuals, dyads, families and groups, to investigations of the relationships between explicitly social and psychological levels of analysis. The following types of paper are invited: • Papers reporting original empirical investigations • Theoretical papers, provided that these are sufficiently related to the empirical data • Review articles which need not be exhaustive but which should give an interpretation of the state of the research in a given field and, where appropriate, identify its clinical implications • Brief reports and comments 1. Circulation The circulation of the Journal is worldwide. Papers are invited and encouraged from authors throughout the world. 2. Length The word limit for papers submitted for consideration to BJCP is 5000 words and any papers that are over this word limit will be returned to the authors. The word limit does not include the abstract, reference list, figures, or tables. Appendices however are included in the word limit. The Editors retain discretion to publish papers beyond this length in cases where the clear and concise expression of the scientific content requires greater length. In such a case, the authors should contact the Editors before submission of the paper. 3. Submission and reviewing All manuscripts must be submitted via http://www.editorialmanager.com/bjcp/. The Journal operates a policy of anonymous peer review. Before submitting, please read the terms and conditions of submission and the declaration of competing interests. 4. Manuscript requirements • Contributions must be typed in double spacing with wide margins. All sheets must be numbered. • Manuscripts should be preceded by a title page which includes a full list of authors and their affiliations, as well as the corresponding author's contact details. A template can be downloaded from here. • Tables should be typed in double spacing, each on a separate page with a selfexplanatory title. Tables should be comprehensible without reference to the text. They should be placed at the end of the manuscript with their approximate locations indicated in the text. • Figures can be included at the end of the document or attached as separate files, 58 carefully labelled in initial capital/lower case lettering with symbols in a form consistent with text use. Unnecessary background patterns, lines and shading should be avoided. Captions should be listed on a separate sheet. The resolution of digital images must be at least 300 dpi. • All papers must include a structured abstract of up to 250 words under the headings: Objectives, Methods, Results, Conclusions. Articles which report original scientific research should also include a heading 'Design' before 'Methods'. The 'Methods' section for systematic reviews and theoretical papers should include, as a minimum, a description of the methods the author(s) used to access the literature they drew upon. That is, the abstract should summarize the databases that were consulted and the search terms that were used. • All Articles must include Practitioner Points – these are 2–4 bullet points to detail the positive clinical implications of the work, with a further 2–4 bullet points outlining cautions or limitations of the study. They should be placed below the abstract, with the heading ‘Practitioner Points’. • For reference citations, please use APA style. Particular care should be taken to ensure that references are accurate and complete. Give all journal titles in full and provide DOI numbers where possible for journal articles. • SI units must be used for all measurements, rounded off to practical values if appropriate, with the imperial equivalent in parentheses. • In normal circumstances, effect size should be incorporated. • Authors are requested to avoid the use of sexist language. • Authors are responsible for acquiring written permission to publish lengthy quotations, illustrations, etc. for which they do not own copyright. For guidelines on editorial style, please consult the APA Publication Manual published by the American Psychological Association. 5. Brief reports and comments These allow publication of research studies and theoretical, critical or review comments with an essential contribution to make. They should be limited to 2000 words, including references. The abstract should not exceed 120 words and should be structured under these headings: Objective, Method, Results, Conclusions. There should be no more than one table or figure, which should only be included if it conveys information more efficiently than the text. Title, author name and address are not included in the word limit. 6. Supporting Information BJC is happy to accept articles with supporting information supplied for online only publication. This may include appendices, supplementary figures, sound files, videoclips etc. These will be posted on Wiley Online Library with the article. The print version will have a note indicating that extra material is available online. Please indicate clearly on submission which material is for online only publication. Please note that extra online only material is published as supplied by the author in the same file format and is not copyedited or typeset. Further information about this service can be found at http://authorservices.wiley.com/bauthor/suppmat.asp 7. Copyright and licenses If your paper is accepted, the author identified as the formal corresponding author for the paper will receive an email prompting them to login into Author Services, 59 where via the Wiley Author Licensing Service (WALS) they will be able to complete the license agreement on behalf of all authors on the paper. For authors signing the copyright transfer agreement If the OnlineOpen option is not selected the corresponding author will be presented with the copyright transfer agreement (CTA) to sign. The terms and conditions of the CTA can be previewed in the samples associated with the Copyright FAQs. For authors choosing OnlineOpen If the OnlineOpen option is selected the corresponding author will have a choice of the following Creative Commons License Open Access Agreements (OAA): - Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License OAA - Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial -NoDerivs License OAA To preview the terms and conditions of these open access agreements please visit the Copyright FAQs and you may also like to visit the Wiley Open Access Copyright and Licence page. If you select the OnlineOpen option and your research is funded by The Wellcome Trust and members of the Research Councils UK (RCUK) or the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) you will be given the opportunity to publish your article under a CCBY license supporting you in complying with your Funder requirements. For more information on this policy and the Journal’s compliant self-archiving policy please visit our Funder Policy page. 8. Colour illustrations Colour illustrations can be accepted for publication online. These would be reproduced in greyscale in the print version. If authors would like these figures to be reproduced in colour in print at their expense they should request this by completing a Colour Work Agreement form upon acceptance of the paper. A copy of the Colour Work Agreement form can be downloaded here. 9. Pre-submission English-language editing Authors for whom English is a second language may choose to have their manuscript professionally edited before submission to improve the English. A list of independent suppliers of editing services can be found at http://authorservices.wiley.com/bauthor/english_language.asp. All services are paid for and arranged by the author, and use of one of these services does not guarantee acceptance or preference for publication. 10. Author Services Author Services enables authors to track their article – once it has been accepted – through the production process to publication online and in print. Authors can check the status of their articles online and choose to receive automated e-mails at key stages of production. The author will receive an e-mail with a unique link that enables them to register and have their article automatically added to the system. Please ensure that a complete e-mail address is provided when submitting the manuscript. Visit http://authorservices.wiley.com/bauthor/ for more details on online 60 production tracking and for a wealth of resources including FAQs and tips on article preparation, submission and more. 11. The Later Stages The corresponding author will receive an email alert containing a link to a web site. A working e-mail address must therefore be provided for the corresponding author. The proof can be downloaded as a PDF (portable document format) file from this site. Acrobat Reader will be required in order to read this file. This software can be downloaded (free of charge) from the following web site: http://www.adobe.com/products/acrobat/readstep2.html. This will enable the file to be opened, read on screen and annotated direct in the PDF. Corrections can also be supplied by hard copy if preferred. Further instructions will be sent with the proof. Excessive changes made by the author in the proofs, excluding typesetting errors, will be charged separately. 12. Early View British Journal of Clinical Psychology is covered by the Early View service on Wiley Online Library. Early View articles are complete full-text articles published online in advance of their publication in a printed issue. Articles are therefore available as soon as they are ready, rather than having to wait for the next scheduled print issue. Early View articles are complete and final. They have been fully reviewed, revised and edited for publication, and the authors’ final corrections have been incorporated. Because they are in final form, no changes can be made after online publication. The nature of Early View articles means that they do not yet have volume, issue or page numbers, so they cannot be cited in the traditional way. They are cited using their Digital Object Identifier (DOI) with no volume and issue or pagination information. E.g., Jones, A.B. (2010). Human rights Issues. Human Rights Journal. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9299.2010.00300.x 61 Appendix B Summary of procedure Click on study URL Information about study Informed consent Yes No Close window do not complete study Measure of perfectionism Part 1 Manipulation of failure imagery T1 measure of state shame, guilt and pride Manipulation check of failure imagery Part 2 Instructions for writing task Control Self-esteem Self-compassion T2 measure of state shame, guilt and pride Feedback and demographics questions Debrief 62 Appendix C Proof of ethical review 63 Appendix D All measures and manipulations are presented below in the order they were completed by participants. Information sheet Principal Investigator Natalie Almond, Natalie.Almond@surrrey.ac.uk Co-Investigator Dr Laura Simonds, L.Simonds@surrey.ac.uk Information sheet Project title: Writing, personality and appraisals My name is Natalie Almond and I am a Trainee Clinical Psychologist, based in the Psychology department at the University of Surrey. This means that I am currently studying for a doctorate qualification in Psychology. As part of my training I have to complete a research project. I am researching how writing can influence the way that individuals feel and think, and I am particularly interested in how certain personality factors may influence the relationship between writing and appraisals such as thoughts and feelings. This study is important in helping us to consider whether writing used in psychological therapy is equally beneficial to people with different personality characteristics. If you choose to complete this study you will be directed to an online study. You will be asked to complete four different questionnaires and to perform an imagery task and a writing task. In the imagery task you will be provided with some instructions and ask to try and picture an image, and hold it in mind before answering a number of questions about this image. It is important that you take time to complete the imagery task as described. In the writing task you will be provided with some prompts of what to write about. This study will take approximately 30 minutes. In return for your participation you will be asked to choose whether you wish to be awarded 1 lab token OR entry into a Prize Draw. If you choose Prize Draw you will be entered into a prize draw for Amazon vouchers, one randomly selected person will receive vouchers worth £50 and two randomly selected people will receive vouchers worth £25. The draw will take place on February 20th 2015 and you will be contacted by email if you win one of the prizes. You will then need to collect the prizes in person from the researcher. The data that you provided by participating in this study will be anonymised and will remain confidential. You have the right to withdraw from this study for two weeks after completing the study. You do not have to give a reason to withdraw from the study. This study has received a favourable ethical opinion by the University of Surrey FAHS Ethics Committee. If you have any complaint or concerns about any aspects of the way you have been dealt with during the course of this study, then please 64 contact Dr Laura Simonds at the email address given above. [ ] I have read the information above and wish to proceed with this study 65 Consent sheet Principal Investigator Natalie Almond, Natalie.Almond@surrrey.ac.uk Co-Investigator Dr Laura Simonds, L.Simonds@surrey.ac.uk Consent form Project title: Writing, personality and appraisals ·I the undersigned voluntarily agree to take part in the study on Writing, personality and attitudes. ·I have read and understood the Information Sheet provided. I have been given a full explanation by the investigators of the nature, purpose, location and likely duration of the study, and of what I will be expected to do. I have been given the opportunity to ask questions on all aspects of the study and have understood the advice and information given as a result. ·I agree to comply with any instruction given to me during the study and to cooperate fully with the investigators. I shall inform them immediately if I suffer any deterioration of any kind in my health or well-being, or experience any unexpected or unusual symptoms. ·I understand that all personal data relating to volunteers is held and processed in the strictest confidence, and in accordance with the Data Protection Act (1998). I agree that I will not seek to restrict the use of the results of the study on the understanding that my anonymity is preserved. ·I understand that I am free to withdraw from the study for up to two weeks after completing the study, without needing to justify my decision and without prejudice. I understand that after two weeks I will not be able to withdraw my data. ·I acknowledge that in return for signing up for this study I shall earn one lab token OR that I will be entered into a prize draw where one person chosen at random will win £50 and 2 people chosen at random will win £25 in Amazon vouchers. ·I confirm that I have read and understood the above and freely consent to participating in this study. I have been given adequate time to consider my participation and agree to comply with the instructions and restrictions of the study. Full Name: Email address (required for lab token OR entry to prize draw): Please select which of the following you wish to receive for your participation [ ] Lab tokens (course credit for psychology students) [ ] Entry in to the prize draw 66 Measure of Perfectionism APS-R (Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001). The following items are designed to measure attitudes people have toward themselves, their performance, and toward others. There are no right or wrong answers. Please try to respond to all of the items. Use your first impression and do not spend too much time on individual items in responding. Respond to each of the items using the scale below to describe your degree of agreement with each item. 1. I have high standards for my performance at work or at school. Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree 2. Agree Strongly Agree Agree Strongly Agree I am an orderly person. Slightly Disagree 4. Neutral Slightly Agree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree Agree Strongly Agree Neatness is important to me. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree If you don’t expect much out of yourself, you will never succeed. Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree 6. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree My best just never seems to be good enough for me. Slightly Disagree 7. Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree I think things should be put away in their place Slightly Disagree 8. 9. Strongly Disagree Slightly Agree I often feel frustrated because I can’t meet my goals. 3. 5. Neutral Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree I have high expectations for myself. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree I rarely live up to my high standards. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 67 10. Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree I like to always be organized and disciplined. Slightly Disagree 11. Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree Doing my best never seems to be enough. Slightly Disagree 12. Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree I set very high standards for myself. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 13. I am never satisfied with my accomplishments. Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 14. I expect the best from myself. Neutral Slightly Agree 15. I often worry about not measuring up to my own expectations. Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 16. My performance rarely measures up to my standards. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 17. I am not satisfied even when I know I have done my best. Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 18. I try to do my best at everything I do. Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 19. I am seldom able to meet my own high standards of performance. Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 20. I am hardly ever satisfied with my performance. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 21. I hardly ever feel that what I’ve done is good enough. Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 22. I have a strong need to strive for excellence. Strongly Disagree Slightly Neutral Slightly Agree Strongly 68 Disagree Disagree Agree Agree 23. I often feel disappointment after completing a task because I know I could have done better. Strongly Disagree Disagree Slightly Disagree Neutral Slightly Agree Agree Strongly Agree 69 Failure imagery task The following task is timed and you will not be able to progress to the next page until the time allowed has passed. We would like you to imagine a piece of coursework you have been working on and putting a lot of effort into as part of your university studies. Imagine the grade you want to get and how you would feel if you achieved it . . . . close your eyes or look down and try to imagine this for 30 seconds." (this page is shown for 50 seconds, allowing 20 seconds to read the instructions and 30 to complete them after this time has elapsed a button to progress to the next page appears) Now imagine that when your essay is marked and comes back to you, the grade it is given is two grades lower than you wanted. Close your eyes or look down and try to imagine this for 1 minute." During this minute imagine: How would you feel about this lower grade? What would this lower grade mean to you? What would you think about yourself? (this page is shown for 80 seconds, allowing 20 seconds to read the instructions and 60 to complete them, after this time has elapsed a button to progress to the next page appears) 70 State Shame and Guilt scale (Marschall, Sanftner, & Tangney, 1994). The following are some statements which may or may not describe how you are feeling right now, following imagining the scenario. Please rate each statement using the 5-point scale below. Remember to rate each statement based on how you are feeling right at this moment. 1. I feel good about myself Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 2. I want to sink into the floor and disappear. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 3. I feel remorse, regret. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4. I feel worthwhile, valuable. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 5. I feel small. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 6. I feel tension about something I have done. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 7. I feel capable, useful. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 71 8. I feel like I am a bad person. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 9. Not feeling this way at all 1 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 I cannot stop thinking about something bad I have done. 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 10. I feel proud. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 11. I feel humiliated, disgraced. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 12. I feel like apologizing, confessing. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 13. I feel pleased about something I have done. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 14. I feel worthless, powerless. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 15. I feel bad about something I have done. Not feeling this way at all 1 2 Feeling this way somewhat 3 4 Feeling this way very strongly 5 72 Manipulation check Please answer the items below by clicking your answer on the scale. How easy was it to imagine this situation? Not easy to imagine 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Very easy to imagine 10 9 How clear was your visualisation of the situation? No clear image 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Very clear image 10 How easy was it to imagine how you would feel in this situation? Not easy to imagine 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Very easy to imagine 10 9 How much of the allotted time did you spend thinking about this situation? None of the time 0% - - - t o - - - All of the time 100% Enter you answer as a number between 0 and 100 below- there is no need to use a % sign. 73 Writing tasks General instructions for all conditions In the next task we would like you to continue to think about the assignment that you did not do as well on as you had anticipated. We would like you to write about this by answering the questions below by typing in to the text boxes provided. Having thought about not doing as well on an assignment as you anticipated we would like you to: Writing control condition 1. List the building names at the University of Surrey. 2. Write 4 to 5 sentences describing the University of Surrey campus. 3. Write 4 to 5 sentences describing what it is like to be a student at the University of Surrey. OR Self-esteem writing 1. List your positive characteristics—indications that you are competent and valuable. 2. Write 4 to 5 sentences explaining how what happened was not entirely your fault and to think about the event in a way that makes you feel better about yourself. 3. Write 4 to 5 sentences describing why the event does not really indicate anything about the kind of person you are. OR Self-compassion writing 1. List what you think a friend might think and feel about themselves following the experience of a similar event. 2. Write 4 to 5 sentences that express understanding, kindness, and concern to yourself in the same way that you might express concern to a friend who had undergone the same experience. 3. Write 4 to 5 sentences describing your experience of the event in a factual way without making any judgements or evaluations about yourself or the event. 74 Repetition of State Shame and Guilt Scale Feedback and demographics We would be grateful if you would complete a few final questions about this study. 1) What do you think this study was about? 2) Were you suspicious at any point that the study was looking at something other than what was stated? [ ] Not at all [ ] A little [ ] A lot Please could you tell us your: Gender: [ ] Male [ ] Female [ ] Nationality First language: [ ] English [ ] Other Religion: _______________ Age: _________________ Subject studied at University: _____________________ Year of study: [ ] Undergraduate Year 1 [ ] Undergraduate Year 2 [ ] Undergraduate Year 3 [ ] Masters 75 [ ] Doctoral study [ ] Other 76 Debrief In this study we were interested in finding out, if people’s personality influenced how they experienced the imagery task. The aspect of personality that we were interested in was Perfectionism. Perfectionists are individuals who have unrealistically high standards that they adhere to rigidly and for these individual’s their self-worth is defined in relation to meeting these high standards. We were interested in what feelings were created by imagining failing to meet your own standards on a coursework assignment and whether those who were more perfectionistic experienced different feelings, or felt the same feelings more strongly. We anticipated that those who were more perfectionistic, may experience more shame and guilt than those who were less perfectionistic. We were also interested in whether writing (particularly compassionate writing) would reduce the extent to which individuals experienced shame and guilt and whether there would be less reduction in shame and guilt for those who were more perfectionistic. It was important that we did not tell you that we would ask you to think about failing as this may have made it very difficult to recruit participants who had more perfectionistic tendencies! If you have felt upset or distressed as a result of completing this study then please visit the Centre for Well-being at the University of Surrey. Information about the Centre and how to contact them can be found here Centre for well-being webpage (copy paste in to your browser) http://portal.surrey.ac.uk/portal/page?_pageid=729,1&_dad=portal&_schema=POR TAL 77 Appendix E Histograms for discrepancy (maladaptive perfectionism) ZSkewness= 1.83 ZKurtosis= .37 Figure E1. Distribution of discrepancy for those who did not complete the study. ZSkewness= .88 ZKurtosis= -1.05 Figure E2. Distribution of discrepancy for those who completed the study (final sample). 78 Appendix F Histograms of variables for correlations ZSkewness= .88 ZKurtosis= -1.05 Figure F1- Histogram of discrepancy. ZSkewness= 2.61 ZKurtosis= -.60 Figure F2- Histogram of Time 1 pride 79 ZSkewness= .68 ZKurtosis= -2.01 Figure F3- Histogram of Time 1 shame ZSkewness= -.11 ZKurtosis= -1.90 Figure F4- Histogram of Time 1 guilt 80 ZSkewness= -2.23 ZKurtosis= -.05 Figure F5- Histogram of imageability check. ZSkewness= -2.76 ZKurtosis= .70 Figure F6- Histogram of percentage time spent on imagery. 81 ZSkewness= 4.15 ZKurtosis= .59 Figure F7- Histogram of Time 2 shame. ZSkewness= 2.24 ZKurtosis= -1.08 Figure F8- Histogram of Time 2 guilt. 82 ZSkewness= .70 ZKurtosis= -1.20 Figure F9- Histogram of Time 2 pride. 83 Appendix G Table G1. Bivariate Spearman’s Correlations for Time 1 Variables. Imageabili Percentage Time 1 Time 1 Time 1 Time 2 Time 2 Time 2 ty time shame guilt pride shame guilt pride) Discrepancy rs = .15 rs = .30** rs = .44*** rs = .37*** rs = -.33** rs = .43*** rs = .36*** rs = -.27** (n=95) p = .16 p = .003 p < .001 p < .001 p = .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .008 Imageability rs = .25* rs = .21 rs = .10 rs = -.27* rs = .09 rs = .06 rs = -.08 (n=93) p = .02 p = .05 p =.33 p =.04 p = .42 p = .56 p = .45 Percentage time rs = .39*** rs = .37*** rs = -.21* rs = .12 rs = .10 rs = .03 (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 p = .04 p = .05 p = .35 p = .79 Time 1 shame rs = .77*** rs = -.69*** rs = .61*** rs = .47*** rs = -.31** (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .002 Time 1 guilt rs = -.49*** rs = .61*** rs = .63*** rs = -.21* (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p = .04 Time 1 pride rs = -.46*** rs = -.310** rs = .53*** (n=95) p < .001 p = .002 p < .001 Time 2 shame rs = .79*** rs = -.54*** (n=95) p < .001 p < .001 Time 2 guilt rs = -.48*** (n=95) p < .001 Note- Asterisks denote the significance level * p < .05, ** p < .01 and *** p <.001 84 Appendix H Histograms of residuals from one- way ANOVA’s ZSkewness= .71 ZKurtosis= -.97 Figure H1- Histogram of standardised residuals for discrepancy. ZSkewness= -2.22 ZKurtosis= -.03 Figure H2- Histogram of standardised residuals for imageability. 85 ZSkewness= -3.14 ZKurtosis= 1.80 Figure H3- Histogram of standardised residuals for percentage time spent ZSkewness= .57 ZKurtosis= -2.05 Figure H4- Histogram of standardised residuals for Time 1 shame. 86 ZSkewness= -.06 ZKurtosis= -1.72 Figure H5- Histogram of standardised residuals for Time 1 guilt. ZSkewness= 2.71 ZKurtosis= -.32 Figure H6- Histogram of standardised residuals for Time 1 pride. 87 Appendix I Table I1- Levine’s test for homogeneity of variance for one-way ANOVA on writing task for Time 1 variables. Levene Statistic df 1 df 2 Sig. 4.00 2 92 < .05 Imageability 1.04 2 92 > .05 Percentage time spent 0.23 2 92 > .05 Time 1 shame 2.78 2 92 > .05 Time 1 guilt 1.54 2 92 > .05 Time 1 pride 1.03 2 92 > .05 Discrepancy (Maladpative perfectionism) on failure scenario 88 Appendix J Table J1- Kruskal-Wallis tests on writing condition for Time 1 variables. H (DOF) p .19 2 > .05 Imageability .39 2 > .05 Time 1 shame .06 2 > .05 Time 1 pride 3.76 2 > .05 Discrepancy (Maladpative perfectionism) 89 Appendix K Histograms of the residuals for all ANCOVAs on shame, guilt and pride. ZSkewness= .73 ZKurtosis= .21 Figure K1- Histogram of the residuals for Time 2 shame in ANCOVA on writing condition with Time 1 shame and discrepancy as covariates. ZSkewness= .87 ZKurtosis= -.42 Figure K2- Histogram of the residuals for Time 2 guilt in ANCOVA on writing condition with Time 1 guilt and discrepancy as covariates. 90 ZSkewness= 1.39 ZKurtosis= -.46 Figure K3- Histogram of the residuals for Time 2 pride in ANOVA on writing condition with Time 1 pride and discrepancy as covariates. 91 Appendix L Table L1- Levine’s test for homogeneity of variance for ANOVAs and ANCOVAs on shame, guilt and pride. (N=95) Levene df 1 df 2 Sig. Statistic Time 2 shame .032 2 92 > .05 Time 2 guilt 4.51 2 92 < .05 Time 2 pride .484 2 92 > .05 92 Appendix M Histograms of residuals for ANOVA on LIWC variables ZSkewness= .87 ZKurtosis= .02 Figure M1 Histogram of residuals word count for writing task ZSkewness= 2.46 ZKurtosis= 2.24 Figure M2- Histogram of residuals first-person singular words percentage score 93 ZSkewness= 21.00 ZKurtosis= 58.32 Figure M3- Histogram of residuals third-person singular words percentage score. ZSkewness= 6.94 ZKurtosis= 14.84 Figure M4- Histogram of residuals third-person plural words percentage score. 94 ZSkewness= 3.55 ZKurtosis= 6.82 Figure M5- Histogram of residuals of past-tense words percentage score. ZSkewness= -1.14 ZKurtosis= .45 Figure M6- Histogram of residuals of present-tense words percentage score. 95 ZSkewness= 1.67 ZKurtosis= 2.31 Figure M7- Histogram of residuals of future-tense words percentage score. ZSkewness= 5.99 ZKurtosis= 8.80 Figure M8- Histogram of residuals of positive emotion words percentage score. 96 ZSkewness= 6.76 ZKurtosis= 14.12 Figure M9- Histogram of residuals of negative emotion words percentage score. ZSkewness= .18 ZKurtosis= -1.07 Figure M10- Histogram of residuals of work words percentage score. 97 ZSkewness= 9.06 ZKurtosis= 21.04 Figure M11- Histogram of residuals of achievement words percentage score. 98 Appendix N Table N1- Levine’s test statistics for ANOVAs conducted on LIWC variables Levene Statistic df 1 df 2 Sig. Word count 1.98 2 92 > .05 First-person singular 4.00 2 90 < .05 Third-person singular 7.07 2 90 < .01 Third-person plural 18.67 2 90 < .001 Past-tense 9.63 2 90 < .001 Present-tense 0.45 2 90 > .05 Future-tense 24.43 2 90 < .001 Positive emotions 0.22 2 90 > .05 Negative emotions 10.46 2 90 < .001 Work 3.76 2 90 < .05 Achievement 0.84 2 90 > .05 99 Appendix O Table O1. Mean Ranks and Statistics for Kruskal-Wallis tests investigating the effect of writing condition on LIWC variables. Dependant variable Mean Ranks Control Self- Self- esteem compassion H (DOF) p 1st person singular (% of text) 22.78 69.32 49.90 43.93 (2) <. 001 3rd person plural (% of text) 34.62 37.14 65.50 32.10 (2) <. 001 Past-tense (% of text) 18.03 55.02 65.41 53.94 (2) <. 001 Future-tense (% of text) 22.10 53.61 63.06 43.02 (2) <. 001 Positive emotions (% of text) 49.95 51.07 41.21 2.60 (2) >. 05 Negative emotions (% of text) 22.12 42.66 71.80 56.06 (2) <. 001 Work (% of text) 64.13 42.61 35.83 18.83 (2) <. 001 Achievement (% of text) 27.77 58.46 54.31 22.87 (2) <. 001 100 MRP Research Proposal Can compassionate-imagery reduce perfectionist’s experience of shame? 101 Introduction Background and Theoretical Rationale Perfectionism is associated with a variety of physical and psychological disorders (Burns, 1980; Pacht, 1984; Shafran & Mansell, 2001). Egan, Wade and Shafran (2011) have argued that perfectionism is a transdiagnostic process and have provided evidence that perfectionism is associated with a range of symptoms associated with psychological distress and that treating perfectionism can be helpful in reducing psychological distress. Shafran and Mansell (2001) have described perfectionists as individuals holding unrealistically high standards that they adhere to rigidly and note that the individual’s self-worth is defined in relation to meeting these high standards. Despite this straightforward definition, there are currently three scales with different focuses that are typically used to measure perfectionism in the research literature these are: Multifactorial perfectionism scale (F-MPS; Frost et al., 1990), Multifactorial perfectionism scale (H-MPS; Hewitt & Flett, 1991), and the Almost Perfect Scale- Revised (APS-R: Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi & Ashby, 2001). Factor-analytic investigations of these perfectionism scales typically reveals two higher-order factors which have been variously described as adaptive vs. maladaptive perfectionism (Enns, Cox, & Clara, 2002; Rice, Ashby & Slaney, 1998; Suddarth & Slaney, 2001), positive striving vs. maladaptive concerns (Bieling, Isralei, & Antony, 2004), personal standards vs. evaluative concerns (Dunkley, Blankstein, & Berg, 2012) and perfectionistic strivings vs. perfectionistic concerns (Dunkely, Blankstein, Masheb, & Grillo, 2006; Stöber & Otto, 2006). Reviewing these factor analyses Stöber & Otto, (2006) suggest that there are two distinct factors underlying perfectionism; the adaptive factor captures having high personal 102 standards, and striving for excellence and the maladaptive factor seems to capture concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, fear of disapproval by others, and discrepancy between expectations and results. The maladaptive factor seems to capture self-evaluation, and self-criticism about whether an individual is meeting their own standards and it is this factor that is associated with predictors of maladjustment such as negative affect (see Stöber & Otto, 2006 for a review). Therefore it is this aspect of perfectionism that is of interest. Tangey (2002) argues that when people evaluate themselves against a set of standards they often experience the self-evaluative emotions (shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride). Tangey notes that perfectionists are continually selfevaluating (especially maladaptive perfectionists) and predicts they will experience these emotions more than others. There is good evidence that socially-prescribed perfectionism (an aspect of maladaptive perfectionism) is positively correlated with proneness to experiencing shame and guilt (Lutwak & Ferrari, 1996: Tangey, 2002) and with feelings of shame and guilt (Klibert, Langhinrichsen-Rohling, & Saito, 2005). Fedewa, Burns and Gomez (2005) also demonstrated that negative perfectionism showed positive correlations with proneness to shame and guilt, and with state shame and guilt. Stöber, Harris and Moon, (2007) extended these findings demonstrating that healthy perfectionists experienced more state pride and less state shame and state guilt than unhealthy perfectionists and non-perfectionists. Collectively, these findings suggest that shame and guilt are associated with maladaptive or negative aspects of perfectionism (see also Ashby, Rice, & Martin, 2006). In summary, individuals who endorse maladaptive perfectionism are thought to be engaging in more self-evaluation and self-criticism, and these individuals experience more shame and guilt than others. 103 The literature on perfectionism and self-criticism suggest that they develop in a similar manner. Drawing on attachment theory Gilbert and Procter (2006) note that insecure attachment styles developed in childhood can lead the individual to view others as a source of threat to the self, and that this results in heightened selfmonitoring, self-blaming, self-criticism and striving to meet other people’s expectations of the self. Gilbert and Procter also note that modelling, safety strategies/behaviours with hostile others, shame (Andrews, 1998; Gilbert, 1998), inabilities to process anger (Ferster, 1973), and lack of internal schema of others as safe/supportive (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2004), all potentially contribute to the development of self-criticism. Interestingly similar factors are proposed to contribute to the development of perfectionism (See Almond & Strauss, 2013) and in both cases insecure attachment styles are implicated. Although self-criticism and associated shame may develop through a range of processes, self-criticism is typically negatively correlated with abilities to be compassionate and self-affiliative (Gilbert, Clark, Hempel, Miles, & Irons, 2004; Gilbert, McEwan, Gibbons, Chotai, Duarte, & Matos, 2011). Gilbert (2009) proposes that there are three type of emotion-regulation system (achieving and activating, threat-protection and affiliative-soothing) that interact to regulate our emotions dependent on context. Compassion-focussed therapy aims to bolster the affiliative and soothing system, which is often underdeveloped in those experiencing psychological distress who may tend to rely on achievement and activating strategies and/or threat-protection strategies. Compassion-focussed therapy has been shown to produce significant reductions in depression, anxiety, self-criticism, shame, and increase the individual’s ability to be self-soothing (Gilbert & Procter, 2006; Judge, Cleghorn, McEwan & Gilbert, 2012). 104 Perfectionists are likely to rely achievement/activating and threat-protection emotion-regulation systems, and tend to experience self-criticism and shame. It is plausible that compassionate-mind training for perfectionists, could increase soothing-affiliative emotion-regulation and reduce their experience of self-criticism and shame. Compassionate-mind interventions are often 12-14 weeks in duration, however, given perfectionists are proposed to experience self-criticism at the point of failing to meet standards and this self-criticism plays a role in maintaining perfectionism (Shafran, Egan, & Wade, 2010), it is pertinent to consider whether a brief compassion intervention at the point of failure, could reduce perfectionists experience of shame and self-criticism. Addressing this question is the focus of this project. One aspect of compassionate-mind training lends itself to a brief intervention is compassionate-imagery (Gilbert & Irons, 2004; Gilbert, Baldwin, Irons & Palmer, 2006). Indeed, Rockcliff, Gilbert, McEwan, Lightman, & Glover (2006) provide evidence that a single dose of compassionate-imagery can have soothing effects. One commonly used form of compassionate-imagery is to develop an image that represents or embodies warm and compassionate qualities, this is sometimes referred to as an ideal or perfect nurturer (Lee, 2005). Therefore this research will investigate whether such compassionate-imagery can reduce perfectionist’s experience of shame and self-criticism following failing to meet their standards. Research Question Can compassionate-imagery reduce maladaptive perfectionist’s experience of shame, and guilt, following failure? Main Hypotheses 105 Individuals who score more highly on the measure of maladaptive perfectionism will report lower levels of shame and guilt following exposure to compassionate-imagery compared to non-compassionate imagery. Individuals who score more highly on the measure of maladaptive perfectionism will show a greater reduction in their ratings of shame and guilt following exposure to compassionate-imagery than following exposure to noncompassionate imagery. Method Participants A power analysis was conducted to determine the sample size required for this study (alpha= 0.05, and power= 0.80). A medium effect size was anticipated for this study and based on Cohen’s (1988) descriptions of F squared effect sizes, the effect size was estimated to be= .015. There would be three predictors discrepancy, compassionate-imagery condition and their interaction. This power analysis suggested a sample of 77 would be required. The experimental design of this study uses imagery, therefore it is plausible that some participants will not actually engage in this imagery and should be removed from the sample. To account for this possibility the data will be checked as recruitment is on-going so that more data can be collected to replace those who have not engaged with the task appropriately. Participants will all be University of Surrey students (both undergraduates and postgraduates). Psychology students will be recruited initially as they are easily accessible to the researcher. The study will be advertised via the Psychology research panel. The recruitment will be widened out to all University of Surrey students if sufficient numbers cannot be recruited from within Psychology. 106 Participants who are not students at the University of Surrey will not be included in the sample. There will likely be approximately 240 undergraduate psychology students and 90 postgraduate psychology students to collect data from. From my own experience collecting data in University settings, the response rate is usually relatively high in Psychology students who will be required to collect data for their own study at some point in their student careers. Recruiting enough participants would require 2/7 of the psychology student population to complete the study. From my own experience conducting research in University settings, online studies are typically preferable to this sample, therefore it seems reasonable that 2/7 students could be recruited. The University of Surrey website suggests that in 2011-2012 their were 15,209 undergraduate and postgraduate students, therefore expanding the study out to the whole student population should ensure that the required sample size could be collected. Design The study has a 2 (Compassionate-imagery: compassionate-imagery vs. noncompassionate-imagery) between-subjects design. Maladaptive perfectionism (measured used APS-R discrepancy scale) will be a measured independent variable and will be treated as a continuous variable. The dependent variables are the emotions of shame and guilt. Measures/Interviews/Stimuli/Apparatus All measures and manipulations can be seen in Appendix 1 in full. Perfectionism measure Perfectionism will be measured using the APS-R (Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi & Ashby, 2001). This is a 23-item scale with three subscales Discrepancy, 107 High standards, Order. The APS-R is the only perfectionism scale that was designed to capture the adaptive elements of perfectionism in one scale (High standards) and the maladaptive components of perfectionism in a separate scale (Discrepancy). APS-R has acceptable construct, concurrent, convergent and discriminant validity (Rice, Ashby & Slaney, 2007) the three factors were reliable with a confirmatory factor analysis producing Cronbachs alphas that ranged from .85- .92 for the subscales (Slaney et al., 2001) and test-retest reliability is good (Rice & Aldea, 2006). Manipulation of failure Participants will be asked to complete modified version of an imagery task taken from Gilbert et al., (2006). Participants will be asked to imagine completing a piece of university coursework and finding out that they do not do a well as they expect to. This is an appropriate manipulation of failure because it concerns a task that most students would likely want to succeed in, and using imagery in this way means that the experimenter can manipulate a discrepancy between what was hoped for and what was achieved. Both of these can be hard to achieve in tasks where failure is induced experimentally via task-completion and negative feedback. Measures of shame and guilt Shame and guilt will be measured using the State Shame and Guilt Scale (SGSS: Marschall, Sanftner, & Tangney, 1994). Robins, Noftle, and Tracey (in press) note that the subscales have good cronbachs alphas; shame (5 items; α = .89), guilt (5 items; α = .82), pride (5 items; α = .87). No other psychometric information is available but this measure is often used in the published literature (Fedewa et al., 2005; Stöber et al., 2007). Manipulation of compassionate-imagery 108 Similar to Gilbert et al., (2006) participants will then be asked to complete a subsequent imagery task. This task will manipulate the use of compassionate versus non-compassionate-imagery. The compassionate-imagery task is a composite of a task described in Gilbert & Procter (2006) and from Lee and James’ (2011) perfect nurturer task. The non-compassionate imagery task is matched to the compassionateimagery task as closely as possible removing all references to compassion. The task can be seen in full in the appendix but requires participants to read the instructions and type notes as they develop a compassionate/ non-compassionate image and then bring this image to mind for three minutes. Manipulation check of experience of failure-imagery / compassionate-imagery Given that participants will be completing tasks using imagery it will be important to check to what extent they have engaged in /completed the imagery tasks, how easy or difficult they found it to complete these imagery tasks and how clear the image was. Participants will be asked these questions (adapted from Gilbert et al., 2006) once after the failure-imagery and once following the manipulation of compassionate/ non-compassionate-imagery. The items can be seen in the Appendix. Measure of mood A 20-item self-report measure of positive and negative affect (PANAS: Watson, Clark, and Tellegen (1988) will be used to determine whether the manipulations of compassionate and non-compassionate-imagery have divergent effect on mood more generally. The PANAS is considered to have good reliability Cronbachs alpha .89 for the Positive Affect scale, and .85 for the Negative Affect scale (Crawford & Henry, 2004). Procedure An opportunity sample of students will be recruited from the University of 109 Surrey by advertising the study on the Psychology research panel where participants will receive research credit for completion of the study. Participants will also be recruited by advertising the study throughout the University campus using posters and emails. A prize draw will be offered to incentivise participation. Students who chose to complete the study will be directed to a URL to complete the study online. Participants will be initially asked to complete the measure of perfectionism. All participants will then complete the failure-imagery task followed by measure of shame and guilt. At this point the procedure diverges by condition, with half of the participants being asked to complete a manipulation of compassionate-imagery and half non-compassionate-imagery. All participants will then repeat the measure of shame and guilt, before completing the measure of mood, and the imagery manipulation checks. Finally participants will provide demographic information, and will be thanked for participation and debriefed. Ethical considerations The University of Surrey Faculty of Arts and Humanities Ethics Committee will be asked to review the ethics of conducting this research. Deception It will be necessary to deceive participants about the true purpose of the study. Participants will be told that the study investigates the effects of imagery on their appraisals of situations. If participants are told that the study is about failure it is likely that this may deter those who are more perfectionistic in particular and it is necessary to recruit students with a wide range of perfectionism scores. Distress It is possible that asking students who are perfectionist to think about failure could cause a small amount of distress, in particular, self-criticism and shame. 110 However, it is important to note that not all participants will be perfectionists and so not all participants will experience distress. However, the purpose of this experiment is to investigate whether self-compassionate-imagery can reduce the distress experienced by perfectionists, so in order to test the efficacy of such an intervention it is necessary to induce a modicum of distress. Furthermore, investigating this topic via imagery, may allow participants to distance themselves from this distress more easily, as this is not a real experience of failure. R&D Considerations There are no R and D considerations for this project. Proposed Data Analysis For all scales the subscale means will be calculated by reversing items as necessary and replacing any missing items with the subscale mean score. The discrepancy scale of the APS-R will be considered as a continuous independent variable that represents the extent to which participants endorse maladaptive aspects of perfectionism where higher scores represent more maladaptive perfectionism. Moderated regression analyses (Aiken & West, 1991) regressions will be conducted to assess whether discrepancy (continuous) and compassionate-imagery condition (categorical) and their interaction predict initial shame and guilt following the failure-imagery manipulation for all participants. It is anticipated based on the previous research evidence that shame, and guilt should be predicted by discrepancy. It would be preferable that compassionate-imagery condition did not predict initial shame or guilt because this would suggest that there were differences between our experimental groups levels of shame and guilt prior to the manipulation of compassionate-imagery. Moderated regression analysis will be used to determine whether 111 discrepancy, self-compassionate-imagery condition, and their interaction predict the experience of 1) shame and 2) guilt following the compassionate-imagery manipulation. This will require the continuous variables to be centred around zero, and compassionate-imagery condition to be dummy coded (0,1) and an interaction term calculated by computing the product of these variables. The design of the study will also make it possible to consider whether the change in shame and guilt (from post failure manipulation to post compassionateimagery manipulation) is predicted by discrepancy, compassionate-imagery condition and their interaction again using a moderated regression analysis. However, this would only be considered if shame and guilt are not predicted by compassionate-imagery condition following failure imagery. Service User and Carer Consultation / Involvement I have presented my research plans to a member of the university/ service user and carer panel via my oral presentation of this research proposal. They raised concerns about how ethnicity may predict the experience of perfectionism; I will measure ethnicity as one of the demographic variables collected at the end of my study. I plan to consult the carer/service user representatives to help me consider the clarity of my materials in particular how easy the imagery manipulations are to follow. Feasibility Issues It may be difficult to recruit the required numbers of participants from the psychology research panel. Therefore, I will plan to recruit from the university student population in addition to the psychology panel and include this in my initial ethics proposal so that I can recruit more broadly if required without needing to reapply for ethical approval. 112 Analysing the data using a moderated regression approach allows for consideration of perfectionism/ discrepancy as a continuous variable so that one can consider how increases in perfectionism predict the other variables. It will be important to ensure that there are a comparable range of discrepancy scores in each of the compassionate-imagery conditions. This will be achieved by regularly downloading the data from the online questionnaire. The way in which participants are recruited/ allocated to condition could then be adjusted to account for their perfectionism score to distribute this equally across conditions. It is possible that participants will not actually engage in the imagery manipulations as requested, therefore, a question will be included in the imagery manipulation check to assess how much of the allotted time participants spent on the imagery task. Dissemination strategy Following analysis of the findings, if appropriate the results will be submitted to an academic conference for presentation and/ or to an academic journal for publication. Study Timeline Oct13Dec13 Jan14Mar14 Apr14Jun14 Jul14Sep14 Oct14Dec14 Jan 15Feb 15 MRP Course approval Ethics submission Ethics approval Draft introduction Draft methods Data collection Data analysis Draft results Draft discussion Finalise draft Complete draft to supervisor 113 References Almond, N. R., & Strauss, C., (2013). Perfectionism, parental bonding and attachment. Unpublished manuscript, University of Surrey. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, London, Sage. Andrews, B. (1998). Shame and childhood abuse. In P. Gilbert, & B. Andrews (Eds), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology and culture (pp. 176–190). New York: Oxford University Press. Ashby, J., Rice, K., & Martin, J. (2006). Perfectionism, shame, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Counseling & Development, 84, 148-156. doi: 10.1002%2Fj.1556-6678.2006.tb00390.x Bieling, P. J., Israeli, A. L., & Anthony, M. M. (2004). Is perfectionism good, bad, or both? Examining models of the perfectionism construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1373-1385. doi: 10.1016%2FS01918869%2803%2900235-6 Burns, D. D. (1980). Feeling good: the new mood therapy. New York: New American Library. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 245– 265. doi: 10.1348%2F0144665031752934 Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., & Berg, J.-L. (2012). Perfectionism Dimensions and the Five‐factor Model of Personality. European Journal of Personality, 114 26, 233–244. 10.1002/per.829 Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Personal standards and evaluative concerns dimensions of “clinical” perfectionism: A reply to Shafran et al. (2002, 2003) and Hewitt et al. (2003). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 63–84. doi:10.1016%2Fj.brat.2004.12.004 Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: A clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 203–212. doi: 0.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.009. Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). The nature and assessment of perfectionism: A critical analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 33-62). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Fedewa, B. A., Burns, L. R., & Gomez, A. A. (2005). Positive and negative perfectionism and the shame/guilt distinction: Adaptive and maladaptive characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1609-1619. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.026 Ferster, C.B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. American Psychologist, 28, 857–870. doi: 10.1037%2Fh0035605 Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C. M., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468. doi: 10.1007%2FBF01172967 Gilbert, P. (1998). What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. In P. Gilbert, & B. Andrews (Eds), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology and culture (pp.3–36). New York: Oxford University Press. Gilbert, P. (2009). The compassionate mind. Londo: Constable & Robinson. 115 Gilbert, P., Baldwin, M., Irons, C., Baccus, J., & Clark, M. (2006). Self-criticism and self-warmth: An imagery study exploring their relation to depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 20, 183–200. doi: 10.1891%2Fjcop.20.2.183 Gilbert, P., Clarke, M., Kempel, S., Miles, J.N.V., & Irons, C. (2004). Criticizing and reassuring oneself: An exploration of forms style and reasons in female students. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 31–50. doi: 10.1348/014466504772812959 Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2004). A pilot exploration of the use of compassionate images in a group of self-critical people. Memory, 12, 507–516. doi: 10.1080/09658210444000115 Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Gibbons, L., Chotai, S., Duarte, J., & Matos, M. (2011). Fears of compassion and happiness in relation to alexithymia, mindfulness and self-criticism. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 84, 239–255. doi: 10.1111%2Fj.2044-8341.2011.02046.x Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame and self-criticism: A pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 353–379. doi: 10.1002/cpp.507 Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456–470. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F00223514.60.3.456 Judge, L., Cleghorn, A., McEwan, K., & Gilbert, P. (2012). An exploration of groupbased compassion focused therapy for a heterogeneous range of clients presenting to a Community Mental Health Team. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 5(4), 420–429, doi: 10.1521%2Fijct.2012.5.4.420 116 Klibert, J. J., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Saito, M. (2005). Adaptive and maladaptive aspects of self-oriented versus socially prescribed perfectionism. Journal of College Student Development, 46, 141–156. doi: 10.1353%2Fcsd.2005.0017 Lee, D. A., (2005). The perfect nurturer: A model to develop a compassionate mind within the context of cognitive therapy p 326-351. In P. Gilbert (Ed.) Compassion: Conceptualisations, Research and Use in Psychotherapy. Hove: Routledge Lee, D. A., and James, S. (2011). The compassionate-mind guide to recovering from trauma and PTSD. Oakland, CA: New harbinger publications. Lutwak, N., & Ferrari, J. R. (1996). Moral affect and cognitive processes: Differentiating shame from guilt among men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 891-896. doi: 10.1016%2FS01918869%2896%2900135-3 Marschall, D. E., Saftner, J., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). The State Shame and Guilt Scale. George Mason University, Fairfax, VA. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P.R. (2004). Security-based self representations in adulthood: Contents and processes. In N.S. Rholes, & J.A. Simpson (Eds), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications. (pp. 159–195). New York: Guilford. Pacht, A. R. (1984). Reflections on perfection. American Psychologist, 39, 386-390. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0003-066X.39.4.386 Rice, K. G., & Aldea, M. A. (2006). State dependence and trait stability of perfectionism: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 205-213. doi: 10.1037%2F0022-0167.53.2.205 117 Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. B. (1998). Self-esteem as a mediator between perfectionism and depression: A structural equation analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 304-314. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F00220167.45.3.304 Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. B. (2007). Perfectionism and the five-factor model of personality. Assessment, 14, 385-398. doi: 10.1177/1073191107303217 Robins, R. W., Noftle, E. E., & Tracey, J. L. (in press). Assessing Self-Conscious Emotions: A Review of Self-Report and Nonverbal Measures. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. M. (Eds.) The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford. Rockliff, H., Gilbert, P., McEwan, K. Lightman, S., & Glover, D. (2008). A pilot exploration of heart rate variability and salivary cortisol responses to compassion-focused imagery. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 5 (3), 132-139. doi: not avaliable Shafran, R., Egan, S., & Wade, T. (2010). Overcoming perfectionism. London: Constable and Robinson. Shafran, R., & Mansell, W. (2001). Perfectionism and psychopathology: A review of research and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 879−906. doi: 10.1016%2FS0272-7358%2800%2900072-6 Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised Almost Perfect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 130–145. doi: not available. Stöber, J., Harris, R. A., & Moon, P. S. (2007). Perfectionism and the experience of pride, shame, and guilt: Comparing healthy perfectionists, unhealthy 118 perfectionists, and nonperfectionists. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 131-141. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.012 Stöber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295–319. doi: 10.1207%2Fs15327957pspr1004_2 Suddarth, B. H., & Slaney, R. B. (2001). An investigation of the dimensions of perfectionism in college students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 157-165. doi: not available. Tangney, J. P. (2002). Perfectionism and the self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. In P. L. Hewitt & G. L. Flett (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 199-215). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 47, 1063-1070. doi: 10.1037//00223514.54.6.1063 119 MRP Literature review Perfectionism, parental bonding and attachment. 120 Journal choice and justification I plan to submit a version of this literature review to Clinical Psychology Review because the aims and scope of the journal are suitable to the topic of the review I have written. The journal states that it publishes reviews on topics that have a clear relationship to research or practice in Clinical Psychology. The literature review includes a section that justifies why the topic is relevant to Clinical Psychology. Furthermore, the journal has a good impact factor (7.07) suggesting that publishing in this journal would mean that the research would be read and cited by others and promote good exposure for my work. 121 Abstract This review aims to determine whether there is a relationship between perfectionism and parental bonding or attachment. Perfectionism has been associated with psychological disorders since the 1980s but recent reviews have highlighted the importance of classical perfectionism as a risk factor for developing and maintaining psychological disorders. Despite the importance of perfectionism, there has been little work that has investigated the origins of perfectionism. One factor that has been mooted to be important to the development of perfectionism is attachment, however, there has been very little examination of whether attachment or parental bonding is indeed related to classical perfectionism. Addressing this question forms the focus of this review. A literature search identified 10 peer-reviewed articles that had assessed this relationship. Systematically reviewing this literature provided good evidence that certain elements of perfectionism (Concern over mistakes, Doubt over Actions, Discrepancy, and Self-Oriented Perfectionism) were correlated with early experiences of parental bonding and attachment style whereas other elements of perfectionism (High Standards, Personal Standards) were not. Whilst the review identifies that there is a relationship between elements of perfectionism and parental bonding or attachment it also highlights the need to fully consider the causal direction and mechanisms underlying these relationships. The review also highlighted the need to consider these relationships using diverse samples, and a range of methods that allow for a test of causality. Future work should seek to address these issues. 122 Introduction Perfectionism Burns (1980) defined perfectionism as a set of interrelated cognitions including expectations, interpretations of events, and evaluations of the self and others. At this time perfectionism was considered to be a pervasive and debilitating condition. Perfectionism has been associated with a variety of physical and psychological disorders including alcoholism, irritable bowel syndrome, abdominal pain, and Type A coronary-prone behaviour, a range of anxiety disorders, depression, anorexia nervosa, and obsessive compulsive personality disorder, (Burns, 1980; Pacht, 1984; Shafran & Mansell, 2001). Indeed Egan, Wade and Shafran (2011) have argued that perfectionism is a transdiagnostic process and have provided evidence that perfectionism is associated with, and can be helpful in treating, a range of symptoms associated with psychological distress. Consequently, perfectionism is an important area of study for clinical psychology and given this importance there has been much discussion about how perfectionism should be defined. Frost, Marten, Lahart, and Rosenblate, (1990) noted that perfectionism research has suffered due the lack of a precise definition of perfectionism. Initial work tended to focus on the presence of high standards, which as Hamachek, (1978) notes does not distinguish between individuals who could be considered adaptive perfectionists (those with high standards and are able to relax these standards when the situation permits) and maladaptive perfectionists (those with high standards who criticise their performance as never good enough and are unable to relax their standards). Attempts to better define perfectionism have resulted in a proliferation of measures and subscales, which has led Shafran and Mansell (2001) to call for better 123 measures of perfectionism that were more closely related to what they termed the “classical concept” of perfectionism. Shafran and Mansell describe perfectionists as individuals holding unrealistically high standards that they adhere to rigidly and note that the individual’s self-worth is defined in relation to meeting these high standards. Below we briefly review three commonly used measures of perfectionism to highlight how perfectionism will be defined in this review. Measures of perfectionism The three measures of perfectionism that seem to be most commonly used in the literature are: Multifactorial perfectionism scale (F-MPS; Frost et al., 1990), Multifactorial perfectionism scale (H-MPS; Hewitt & Flett, 1991), and the Almost Perfect Scale- Revised (APS-R: Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi & Ashby, 2001). F-MPS The F-MPS consists of six factors, Concern over Mistakes (CoM), Doubts about Actions (DoA), Personal Standards (PS), Parental Expectations (PE), Parental Criticism (PC), and Organization (OR). CoM includes negative responses to mistakes, equating mistakes with failure and beliefs about loosing other people’s respect following failure. DoA refers to a sense that projects are not completed properly. PS was defined as the setting of very high standards and the inflated importance placed on these high standards for self-evaluation. PE was the tendency to believe that one's parents set very high goals and PC the tendency to believe that one’s parents are overly critical. OR was defined as including an emphasis on the importance of order and the preference for order. H-MPS The H-MPS consists of three factors; Self-Oriented Perfectionism (SOP), Other-Oriented Perfectionism (OOP) and Socially-Prescribed Perfectionism (SPP). 124 Hewitt and Flett (1991) describe SOP to consist of behaviours such as setting exacting high standards for oneself, stringently evaluating and critiquing one's own performance and striving to avoid failures and attain perfectionism. OOP consists of beliefs and expectations about the capabilities of others. SPP is about the individual’s perception of their significant other’s expectations of them being perfect. APS-R The APS-R consists of 3 factors Discrepancy (D), High standards (HS) and Order (OD). D measures the inconsistency between an individual’s high personal standards and their perceptions of success in meeting those standards. HS is very similar to the measure of PS described above, and OD is very similar to the measure of OR described above. Defining perfectionism This brief review of perfectionism measures highlights the multitude of constructs that have been considered to form part of perfectionism. It is notable that across these different measures there are some similarities, a focus on having high personal standards (PS, HS, SOP) appears on all measures. Likewise all measures attempt to capture some element that relates to monitoring or evaluating whether these standards have been met (CoM, DoA, SOP, D). Similarly, a number of the scales consider expectations and criticism from others (SPP, PE and PC). Shafran and Mansell (2001) note that SOP, PS and CoM come closest to measuring the classical concept of perfectionism defined as the pursuit of excessively high standards that are rigidly adhered to and define self-worth. Therefore in this review perfectionism is defined in relation to Shafran and Mansell’s definition (and subscale choice) of the classical concept of perfectionism. 125 Shafran and Mansell did not consider the APS-R in their review as it was published contiguously, however, given the similarities between HS and PS and between SOP and D noted above it is proposed that HS and D could equally be viewed as measuring the classical concepts of perfectionism and so these will be included in this review. Shafran and Mansell argue that DoA does not measure of perfectionism given its overlap with OCD symptomology. Yet, there is good evidence that CoM and DoA may be overlapping concepts: Stöber’s (1998) factor analysis of the F-MPS suggests that CoM and DoA should be collapsed on to one factor, therefore DoA is considered as an element of perfectionism in this review. Subscales assessing expectations of perfection in others (e.g., OOP) and subscales assessing the expectations and criticism of significant others and parents (e.g., PE, PC and SPP) will not be included in this review. PE and PC in particular are not appropriate because they are retrospective (Shafran & Mansell, 2001) and confound the assessment of current perfectionism (Rhéaume et al., 2000). Furthermore the way in which children are parented may be related to the development of perfectionism (Frost et al., 1990; Rice, Lopez, & Vegara, 2005) rather than a current assessment of perfectionism. The development of perfectionism is considered below. Development of perfectionism There has been much theorising around the role of parenting in the development of perfectionism. Barrow and Moore (1983) theorised that four different types of early experience may contribute to the development of perfectionism: overly critical and demanding parents; excessively high parental standards and expectations (indirect criticism); perfectionistic parents modelling the behaviour for children; and parental approval being absent, inconsistent or 126 conditional. Similarly, in a theoretical review chapter, Flett, Hewitt, Oliver and Macdonald (2002) described four potential models that may explain the development of perfectionism: 1) Social expectations, 2) Social learning, 3) Anxious rearing, and 4) Social reaction/harsh environment. The social expectations model suggests that approval is contingent on the child being perfect, the social learning model suggest that the child learns to imitate the parent who is perfectionistic, the anxious rearing model suggests that anxious parenting promotes a focus on mistakes and negative consequences of mistakes which leads to perfectionistic striving and anxious overconcern. Finally, the social reaction/ harsh environment model suggests that a variety of difficult early experiences such as abuse, maltreatment, withdrawal of love, or exposure to a chaotic family environment may lead to the use of perfectionism as a coping strategy. The literature reviewed above on the development of perfectionism shares clear similarities with the literature on the development of attachment as Rice and Mirazdeh (2000) have noted. Attachment and perfectionism An attachment is the representation of an affective bond between individuals, and contains the behaviours that are useful for maintaining contact with the significant other(s) (Ainsworth, 1989; Bowlby, 1969; 1988; Sroufe, 1979). Secure attachments are optimal, and emerge when parents have been emotionally accessible to, and nurturing of, their children. Insecure attachments result from unpredictable, harsh, or unsupportive parent-child interactions or overly involved and anxious parenting and these types of experiences are clearly represented in the theorizing around the development of perfectionism as noted above. Children with insecure attachment bonds have difficulties managing developmental challenges and experience a variety of personal and interpersonal adjustment problems (Bowlby, 127 1973). It is possible that the development of perfectionism represents one of these interpersonal adjustment problems. Rice and Mirazdeh (2000) suggest that children who experience inconsistent parental approval may develop an insecure attachment and learn to emphasize the importance of their performance rather than their emotional needs supporting the development of perfectionism. In such cases being perfect provides a means to receive or maintain recognition from parents or to escape or avoid critical or punitive responses from parents. There have been few research studies that have considered the relationship between attachment and perfectionism. Flett et al. (2002) reviewed a couple of studies that were of variable quality; with aspects of parental relations being contained in both the measure of perfectionism and attachment, and the use of atypical measures of perfectionism or attachment. Although Flett et al. argue there is a relationship between attachment and perfectionism this is based on evidence of an association between SPP and fearful and preoccupied attachment styles. This evidence is questionable because SPP is arguably not part of the classical conception of perfectionism because it takes into account significant others expectations. Therefore it is possible that this evidence of a relationship between SPP and attachment is confounded. The literature reviewed thus far suggests that there may be a relationship between attachment and perfectionism but before this can be considered it is important to define the scope of attachment experiences that will be included in this review. Measuring attachment A child’s attachment to their primary caregiver can be assessed in children 128 during childhood using the strange situation (Ainsworth, 1978), and retrospectively in adults using the adult attachment interview (AAI: George, Kaplan, & Main, 1984). It is unlikely that there will be many, if any, studies where a child’s attachment style has been assessed and recorded in childhood alongside concurrent or subsequent measures of perfectionism, in part due to an absence of measures of perfectionism in children. Furthermore, both the strange situation and the AAI require researchers to undergo specific training in their administration and scoring and so consequently studies using these methods may be infrequent in the literature. These assessment tools also produce a categorical measure of attachment, and as such these measures have been criticised because they do no take into account variation between individuals within the same category (Mikiluncer & Shaver, 2007). Categorical measures may make it more difficult to identify a relationship between attachment and perfectionism, therefore this review will only consider dimensional measures of attachment. In an attempt to access early experiences of an individuals’ parenting retrospectively some researchers have used the parental bonding instrument (PBI: Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979). The PBI assesses the participant’s perceptions of their mother’s and father’s parenting in terms of two dimensions, care and overprotection. Although this is not a measure of attachment, the PBI does capture the individual’s perceptions about their early experiences and these early experiences of parenting are thought to contribute to the individual’s attachment style. Attachment can also be measured in terms of an adult’s attachment to adults that they are in close relationships with. There is evidence that attachment security remains moderately stable over the first nineteen years of life (Fraley, 2002) and that adult attachment is in part determined by early attachment to the primary caregiver 129 (Hazen & Shaver, 1987). Adult attachment can therefore be considered as a proxy for early attachment. In summary, perfectionism is an important concept for mental health and well-being and the literature theorises a relationship between attachment and perfectionism. However, there have been no been no published reviews nor investigation of this relationship and consequently it is unclear if there is a relationship. This review addresses this gap in the literature by investigating whether there is a relationship between attachment orientation and perfectionism. Method of the literature search The literature search was conducted on the 15th January 2013 using Psycinfo, Scopus and Web of Knowledge (WoK) databases. The search consisted of two components combined with an “AND” the first was attachment OR “parental bonding” and the second perfection*. There were no date limitations imposed on the search. Differences between databases meant that in Psycinfo the search was conducted on Abstracts, in Scopus the search was conducted on Article title, abstract and keywords, in Web of Knowledge (WoK) the search was conducted on topic (which considers titles, abstracts, keywords and keywords plus). In Scopus the Health sciences, Life sciences, and Social sciences and humanities databases were included, in WoK default settings were used which included all the databases available to WoK, and in Psycinfo the Psycinfo database alone was used. Inclusion/ Exclusion criteria In order to be included in the final set of papers it was required that sources were peer-reviewed to ensure that sources were of high quality. In Psycinfo this was achieved by selecting the peer-reviewed only option. In Scopus and WoK this was achieved by limiting document type to those most likely to have been peer-reviewed, 130 (in Scopus, article or review, and in WoK article, review, case report or clinical trial). Sources identified by Scopus and WoK were checked and excluded if they were not peer-reviewed or if this could not be determined. The search results were combined and duplicates removed leaving 107 results. The inclusion/exclusion criteria are explained briefly below, the number of results lost at each stage is shown in Figure 1. The full-text version of the source had to be available in English to be included. All results were checked by the author and were excluded if the full-text version was not available in English. The following inclusion criteria were addressed in turn. The sources identified had to: 1) contain both a measure of attachment and a measure of perfectionism in the same study and report the relationship between these variables, 2) the measure of attachment had to be dimensional and there had to be evidence of convergent validity with other attachment measures as defined by Ravitz, Maunder, Hunter, Sthankiya, and Lancee (2010) and 3) the measure of perfectionism had to include the at least one of the subscales noted to represent the classical definition of perfectionism in the introduction (that is SOP, HS, PS, D, CoM or DoA) or a composite of the subscales, and the measure had to have convergent validity with another measure of perfectionism as noted by Enns and Cox (2002). Papers that did not meet any of the above criteria were excluded. The final set consisted of 10 papers that met these criteria. Results The results will be presented in two sections. The first section will consider studies where early experiences of attachment or parental bonding have been measured and the results are summarised in Table 1. The second section will consider studies where adult attachment has been measured and the results are 131 Psycinfo search results 57 Minus 20 Peer-reviewed only 37 Scopus search results 59 Minus 2 WoK search results 92 Minus 7 Articles and reviews only 57 Article, review, case-report, clinical trial only 85 Total search results 179 Minus 72 Duplicates removed 107 Minus 9 Papers written in English only 98 Minus 79 Must include a measure of attachment and perfectionism in the same study and consider the relationship between them 19 Minus 2 Must include a dimensional measure of attachment with concurrent validity with another measures 17 Minus 7 Must measure at least one subscale of classical perfectionism, SOP, DoA, CoM, HS, PS, SOP (these are from scales with concurrent validity with other perfectionism measures) 10 Figure 1. Schematic representation of the literature selected for inclusion in the review. 132 summarised in Table 2. In both sections the results will consider each perfectionism subscale in turn, reviewing and evaluating what evidence there is for a relationship between that perfectionism subscale and attachment. Subscales will be presented so that those that are conceptually similar appear sequentially. All studies had large sample sizes (n ranged from 138 to 604) and so statistical power was unlikely to be a concern therefore sample sizes for each study are recorded only in the Tables. Perfectionism and Parental Bonding Inventory Personal standards (PS) Enns, Cox, and Clara (2002) investigated the relationship between score on the two indices of the PBI-care and overprotection (Parker et al., 1979) and PS in Canadian University students. In this study the care dimension was assessed in terms of lack of care1. Participants were asked to complete the PBI once for each parent and the PBI scores were calculated by averaging responses for mother and father for each participant. There were no significant correlations. PS was not associated with either perceived lack of care from parents or perceived overprotection by parents. Self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) Two studies investigated the relationship between SOP and the two indices of the PBI in outpatients with depression (Enns, Cox, & Larsen, 2000) or Canadian college students (Enns et al., 2002). In both studies the PBI was completed once for each of the participant’s parents. Enns et al. (2000) analysed their results by considering PBI scores for each parent separately whereas Enns et al. (2002) examined responses on the PBI towards parents by averaging responses for mother and father. Furthermore, Enns et al. (2000) analysed responses by participant gender 1 The PBI is usually measured so that higher scores indicate care, however, in this study the authors chose to recode the answers so that higher scores indicated a lack of care. 133 whereas Enns et al. (2000) averaged across participant gender. For the sake of brevity when reporting Enns et al. (2000) findings only women’s responses about their mothers are considered. Women were chosen because they represented the majority of participants and responses about mothers were chosen on the assumption that women are usually the primary caregiver. It is noteworthy that Enns et al. report that there were no statistical differences between men and women’s responses to the PBI. In Enns et al. (2000) the PBI dimension is coded as specified by Parker et al., (1979) however, in Enns et al. (2002) the care dimension was assessed in terms of lack of care1. Enns et al. (2000; 2002) both demonstrate a weak correlation between participant’s SOP and their perceptions of their mothers or parents being overprotective. The findings on the care dimensions were also consistent: Enns et al. (2000) found a weak negative correlation between perceptions of maternal care and SOP and Enns et al. (2002) found a weak positive correlation between perceptions of parental lack of care1 and SOP. Thus both studies support the idea that higher perceptions of parental care are associated with lower levels of SOP. The consistency between studies is noteworthy because one study considered an average of parents PBI scores whereas the other considered the scores separately. Furthermore, different samples were used in the two studies and the samples are divergent in age (Enns et al., 2002 M= 20 years, SD= 4.4 years; Enns et al., 2000 M= 40 years, SD= 10.0 years). However, the findings on the care subscale should be interpreted with some caution. In both studies the correlations between SOP and (lack of) care are significant only at the p< .05 level, in Enns et al. (2000) they compute 64 correlations therefore one would expect 3.2 of the 64 correlations to be significant at the p> .05 level. Given the risk of a type 1 error, considering these 134 results with a more stringent p level would be advisable and would suggest that there was no significant evidence of a relationship between SOP and (lack of) care on the PBI. Self-oriented perfectionism and Personal standards: Summary of findings SOP and PS subscales both measure HS so it is pertinent to consider these findings alongside each other. There is little evidence that PS are associated with early experiences of parents in terms of lack of care or overprotection. SOP however is positively associated with perceptions of parental overprotection. The SOP subscale assesses not only setting exacting high standards for oneself, but also stringently evaluating and critiquing one's own performance and striving to avoid failure. Therefore it is possible that it is these later aspects that are responsible for the differing pattern of results on SOP compared to PS. Without further research that adopts a more stringent approach, it is unclear whether perception of parental care (or lack thereof) is significantly associated with SOP. Concern over mistakes (CoM) Enns et al. (2000; 2002) also considered the relationship between CoM and the two dimensions of the PBI. Both studies found evidence of a significant (at p< .01) weak to moderate positive correlation between perceptions of parental (or maternal) overprotection and CoM. Again there was consistency between the two studies on the care dimension with Enns et al. (2000) finding evidence for a weak negative correlation between perceptions of maternal care and CoM and Enns et al. (2002) finding evidence for a weak positive correlation between perceptions of parental lack of care1 and CoM. It is interesting to note that the positive correlation in Enns et al. (2002) is stronger and significant at a more stringent level on this 135 subscale providing good evidence that the greater the perception of parental lack of care, the greater the CoM reported. Doubts over action (DoA) Enns et al. (2000; 2002) considered the relationship between DoA and the two subscales of the PBI in clients with depression, and in Canadian college students retrospectively. Whilst Enns et al (2000) found no evidence for an association between female depressed participant’s DoA and their perceptions of care from their mothers, Enns et al. (2002) found evidence of a significant positive but weak association between college student’s DoA and their perceptions of lack of care from their parents (Enns et al., 2002). Enns et al. (2000; 2002) both found evidence of a (weak to moderate) positive correlation between participant’s DoA and their perceptions of their parents being overprotective. This relationship was stronger in the Enns et al. (2002) study where the participants were college students and the perceptions of mothers and fathers being overprotective were averaged. Collectively these two studies provide evidence that participants perceptions of overprotection by parents is associated with DoA, however it is less clear to what extent there is an association between care and DoA. It is unclear whether the absence of a significant association between perceptions of maternal care and DoA in the Enns et al. (2000) study is borne of considering just women and their relation to their mothers because when perceptions of parental lack of care (mothers and fathers averaged) are considered in Enns et al. (2002) there is a significant positive association with DoA. However, it is unclear why this issue of gender and parents would impact on the DoA subscale and not any of the other subscales. Likewise the absence of an association between perceptions of care and DoA in the Enns et al., (2000) study (compared to the Enns et al., 2002 study) may result from the sample 136 comprising outpatients with depression but again it is unclear why the DoA subscale would be the only subscale affected by the differing samples. One potential explanation is provided by evidence that severity of depression is positively correlated with DoA (Enns & Cox, 1999) Therefore, it is possible that the depressed participants in the Enns et al. (2000) had inflated DoA scores and that there was little variation in DoA scores, which would make it difficult to detect a correlation with perfectionism. However, Enns et al. (2000) do not provide any mean scores for the perfectionism subscales so it is difficult to assess whether this suggestion can explain the divergence in results across the two samples. Furthermore given their conceptual similarities it is unclear why the same issue would not have beset the results on CoM also. Concern over mistakes and Doubts over action: Summary of findings In keeping with the idea that CoM and DoA subscales may represent slightly different aspects of the same construct (Stöber, 1998), the findings for the two subscales are considered alongside each other. The findings for perceptions of parental (or maternal) overprotection on the PBI and both CoM and DoA are very consistent. There are significant moderately strong, positive correlations between parental overprotection and CoM and between parental overprotection and DoA in both studies considered (see Table 1). The results on the care dimension of the PBI are more variable. Whilst there is consistent evidence for an association between perceptions of care with CoM across the two studies, the relationship between perceptions of care and DoA is inconsistent. 137 Table 1. Correlation r-values between perfectionism and parental bonding subscales Sample Attachment target PBI (lack of) care PBI overprotectio n SOP Enns et al. (2000) OD (n=138) Mother -.22* .36** Enns et al. (2002) US (n=261) Parents (.14*) .18** US (n=261) Parents (.04) .10 Enns et al. (2000) OD (n=138) Mother -.25* .42** Enns et al. (2002) US (n=261) (.35**) .38** PS Enns et al. (2002) CoM Parents DoA Enns et al. (2000) OD (n=138) Mother -.10 .36** Enns et al. (2002) US (n=261) (.33**) .45** Parents Note- *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. US- University students. OD- Outpatients with depression. Parentheses represent correlations where the PBI care measure was coded to represent lack of care. Overall summary of perfectionism and parental bonding inventory The examination of the relationships between parental bonding and dimensions of perfectionism has provided good evidence that participants’ perception of higher levels of parental overprotection is associated with higher levels of SOP, CoM and DoA. Furthermore, this examination has also provided good evidence that participants’ perception of higher levels of parental lack of care is associated with higher levels of SOP, and CoM. Notably, there was no evidence that perceptions of parental overprotection, nor parental care were associated with PS. 138 This is interesting and may reflect the notion that high personal standards are associated with both adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Although most of the findings reviewed are consistent and consider differing samples (in terms of diagnosis and age), care should be taken when interpreting these results because they represent the findings of only two separate studies, both of which were conducted in the same country. These results may be generalizable to other Western cultures, but there is not any evidence to support this yet. Adult attachment Personal standards (PS) Two studies also investigated the relationship between personal standards and adult attachment dimensions measured using the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR, Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) in University students in the USA (Rice, Lopez, & Vergara, 2005) and in a sample of athletes recruited from Universities and sports clubs from across the UK (Shanmugam, Jowett, & Meyer, 2012). In both studies there was no significant correlation between PS and attachment anxiety nor between PS and attachment avoidance. High standards (HS) Two studies considered the relationship between HS and adult attachment dimensions (measured using the ECR) in University students in the USA (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012 Rice et al. 2005). Both studies found no evidence of an association between HS and attachment anxiety. Iannantuono and Tylka found no evidence of an association between HS and attachment avoidance, and Rice et al. found a significant negative correlation (at p < .05) between HS and attachment avoidance. This correlation was very weak, but suggested that higher scores for HS was associated with lower scores for attachment avoidance. 139 Ulu and Tezer (2010) considered the relationship between HS and adult attachment dimensions using the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) in University students in Turkey. The RSQ asks participants to rate their agreement with four subscales that represent four different adult attachment styles secure (most optimal), preoccupied, dismissing and fearful. The RSQ had been translated in to Turkish previously and was found to be reliable and valid (Sümer & Güngör, 1999). The authors adapted the RSQ so that it produced dimensional measures attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance by performing the following calculations avoidance = (fearful + dismissive) − (preoccupied + secure); anxiety = (preoccupied + fearful) − (secured + dismissing). This approach was supported by a factor analysis (Kurdek, 2002). Ulu and Tezer found no evidence of an association between HS and attachment anxiety nor any evidence of an association between HS and attachment avoidance as measured by the RSQ. Considering the results from these three studies these it appears that there is very little evidence of a correlation between attachment dimensions and HS. However, given the adaption made to the RSQ it is important to be wary in interpreting the absence of significant findings in this study. Only one study provided evidence of a correlation between attachment avoidance and HS, and it is unclear if this correlation is meaningful. Rice et al. measured 13 variables and considered the 78 correlations of these variables, without adjusting the significance level to reduce type 1 error. Results are only reported in terms of whether they reach conventional significance levels of p < .05, so it is unclear whether these findings are significant if a more stringent p value is adopted. Self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) Reis and Grenyer (2002) investigated the relationship between SOP and 140 attachment in University students in Australia. Attachment to partner was measured using the RSQ. There was no significant association between SOP and secure, nor SOP and dismissing styles. However, there was a significant weak positive correlation between SOP and preoccupied attachment subscales and between SOP and fearful attachment subscales. More recent measures of adult attachment styles (e.g., ECR; Brennan, et al., 1998) consider attachment to be best represented by the crossing of two dimensions: attachment anxiety (anxiety about abandonment) and attachment avoidance (avoidance of intimacy). These two dimensions can be applied to the categorical views of attachment style such that: secure represents low attachment anxiety, low attachment avoidance; preoccupied represents high attachment anxiety, low attachment avoidance; dismissing represents low attachment anxiety, high attachment avoidance; and fearful represents high attachment anxiety, high attachment avoidance. Thus Reis and Grenyer’s findings could be interpreted to suggest that higher attachment anxiety is associated with SOP because higher attachment anxiety is associated with both preoccupied and fearful attachment styles. However, the RSQ does not categorise individuals into attachment styles, it simply asks them to rate their agreement with items pertaining to each of the four styles and so this interpretation should be viewed cautiously. Furthermore, the RSQ has lower reliability and validity than other measures of attachment according to Ravitz et al. (2010) and within this study the internal reliability of the secure and dismissive scales were low (alpha=.48 and .50 respectively), so this could explain the lack of correlations in these subscales. In summary, the idea that attachment anxiety is associated with SOP should be viewed cautiously unless this finding is replicated in other studies. Rice et al. (2005) investigated the association between SOP and adult 141 attachment style using the ECR (Brennan et al., 1998), in University students in the USA. There was no significant association between SOP and attachment anxiety nor between the SOP and attachment avoidance. Overall there is a mixed picture as to whether SOP is associated with adult attachment. There was some tentative evidence that there may be an association between attachment anxiety and SOP but Rice et al. (2005) did not replicate this finding. Composite Personal standards (SOP and PS) Dunkley, Berg, and Zuroff, (2012) investigated the relationship between perfectionism and attachment in Canadian University students. They used a two factor model of perfectionism derived from a factor analysis of the Depressive experiences Questionnaire, (DEQ: Blatt, D'Afflitti, & Quinlan, 1976) the F-MPS (Frost et al., 1990) and the H-MPS (Hewitt & Flett, 1991). The two-factors were Personal standards (comprised of SOP and PS) and Self-criticism (comprised of DoA, COM, SPP, and DEQ). The self-criticism factor is comprised of subscales of perfectionism that we have excluded from our analysis (SPP) for reasons outlined above, therefore we focus only on the Personal standards composite scale (PScomp). Dunkley et al. (2012) measured attachment using a version of the RSQ (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) specifically focusing on the subscales from Collin and Read (1990) which are fear of closeness, fear of dependency and fear of loss. Brennan, et al., (1998) note that fear of closeness and fear of dependency both correlate with attachment avoidance and fear of loss correlates with attachment anxiety. The results reveal significant weak positive correlations of PScomp with fear of dependency and fear of closeness (both of which are associated with attachment avoidance) and a significant but very weak correlation between PScomp and fear of 142 loss (associated with attachment anxiety). Consequently these results could be viewed as evidence that PScomp is weakly associated with attachment avoidance and very weakly associated with attachment anxiety. Self-oriented perfectionism, personal standards and high standards: Summary of findings Given that SOP, PS and HS subscales are all measure high personal standards it is pertinent to consider these findings in relation to each other. Examining the results collectively there is little evidence that adult attachment dimensions are related to high personal standards. Considering those studies that used the ECR and the RSQ there is no evidence that attachment anxiety is associated with high personal standards, and scant evidence that attachment avoidance is associated with high personal standards (only one very weak negative correlation at p < .05 in a study that did not adjust for multiple correlations). The remaining studies that considered whether there is an association between attachment and HS and PS used measures of attachment that are more difficult to consider simply in terms of attachment avoidance and anxiety. Rice et al. (2005) provided evidence that SOP was significantly positively associated with preoccupied and fearful styles of attachment, albeit weakly, and this pattern may reflect that higher levels of attachment anxiety are associated with higher personal standards. Dunkley et al. (2012) found a weak but highly significant association between a composite of SOP and PS with fear of dependency and fear of closeness (both avoidance), and weaker and less significant relationship with fear of loss (anxiety). Collectively these findings seem to suggest that there is little evidence for associations between high personal standards and attachment. However, there is 143 better evidence of an association between SOP and attachment anxiety and avoidance. SOP is not a pure measure of high standards, it also measures stringently evaluating and critiquing one's own performance and striving to avoid failure. This suggests that it may be the additional concepts captured by SOP that are not apparent in HS or PS subscales, which may be responsible for this relationship. However, this interpretation should be viewed cautiously because the studies that provide evidence for a relationship between SOP and attachment both use less reliable measures of attachment (Ravitz et al., 2010). It is possible that the discrepancy subscale of the APS may be similar to the SOP in terms of capturing the evaluation and critique of an individual’s own performance and so considering the results on the discrepancy subscale may provide supportive evidence for a relationship between monitoring and evaluating performance and attachment. Discrepancy (D) Four studies considered the relationship between D and adult attachment dimensions (using the ECR) in University students in the USA (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Rice et al., 2005; Wei, Heppner, Russell, & Young, 2006; Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell, & Abraham, 2004). All of these studies found significant positive but weak correlations between attachment avoidance and D and all demonstrated significant positive correlations (that varied in strength from weak to moderately strong) between attachment anxiety and D (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Rice et al., 2005; Wei et al., 2004; 2006) 2. Ulu and Tezer (2010) also found 2 Wei et al. (2004; 2006) were testing a structural equation model with other variables and so they split attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety variables in to 3 parcels. Therefore for Wei et al. (2004), there were three correlations with each attachment dimension for each perfectionism subscale. In Wei et al. (2006), discrepancy was also split into 3 parcels resulting in nine correlations considering 144 evidence of significant weak positive correlation between D and attachment avoidance and significant moderate positive correlation between D and attachment anxiety when attachment was measured using an adapted RSQ in a sample of Turkish University students. The studies reviewed provide very good evidence of significant positive correlations between discrepancy and both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. The relationship between discrepancy and attachment anxiety seems to be stronger than the relationship between discrepancy and attachment avoidance. This evidence is consistent with the same relationships found with two different measures of attachment, and in both US and Turkish samples, however, it is noteworthy that all samples are University students and so replication with an alternative sample to test the generalizability of this finding would be useful. Discrepancy and Self-oriented perfectionism: Summary of findings It is worth comparing the findings on discrepancy and SOP because the subscales share some conceptual similarities. Although the findings on SOP are tentative there was evidence that SOP is significant positively correlated with higher levels of attachment anxiety (preoccupied and fearful) and PScomp (SOP and PS) scale is significantly positively correlated with attachment anxiety (fear of loss) and with attachment avoidance (fear of dependency and fear of closeness). These findings are strengthened by the conceptual replication of these findings on the discrepancy subscale. Concern over mistakes (CoM) Rice et al. (2005) and Wei et al. (2004) considered the relationship between discrepancy and attachment anxiety and nine correlations considering discrepancy and attachment avoidance. 145 CoM and adult attachment dimensions (measured using the ECR) in University students in the USA. Both studies revealed significant weak positive correlations between CoM and attachment anxiety and CoM and attachment avoidance. Doubts over action (DoA) Two studies considered the relationship between DoA and adult attachment dimensions (measured using the ECR) in University students in the USA (Rice et al., 2005; Wei et al., 2004). Both studies revealed significant weak positive correlations between DoA and attachment anxiety and DoA and attachment avoidance. Concern over mistakes and Doubts over action: Summary of findings In keeping with Stöber’s (1998) proposal that CoM and DoA are related constructs it is pertinent to consider these results alongside each other. All of the results reviewed on these subscales provide consistent evidence of significant weak positive associations between CoM and DoA with both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. However, it is notable that all of these studies used the ECR and used University students as a sample so these results are replicable but lack generalizability. Overall summary of perfectionism and adult attachment The examination of the relationships between attachment and dimensions of perfectionism has provided good evidence that both higher levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance are associated with higher levels of CoM, DoA and D. There was some evidence that the relationship between attachment anxiety was stronger, particularly in relation to the discrepancy subscale. Notably, there was little evidence that either attachment anxiety or avoidance were associated with PS or HS but there was some evidence that attachment dimensions are associated with SOP. This is interesting and may reflect the notion that high standards are associated with both 146 Table 2. Correlation r-values between perfectionism and adult attachment subscales Sample Attachment ECRtarget anxious ECRRSQavoidanc anxious e RSQFear of Fear of avoidanc dependen loss e cy Fear of Secure closeness Preoccup Dissimiss Fearful ied ing SOP Reis and Grenyer (2002) Rice et al. (2005) US (n=245) Students (n=241) Partner - - - - - - - -.09 .24** -.02 .17*** Partner .08 .03 - - - - - - - - - PS Rice et al. (2005) Students (n=241) Partner .04 <. 01 - - - - - - - - - Shanmugam et al. (2012) US, athletes (n=411) general, partners, coaches teammat es .40 .80 - - - - - - - - - Partners -.08 -.05 - - - - - - - - - Partner -.08 -.12* - - - - - - - - - Partner - - .10 -.20 - - - - - - - Close others - - - - .36*** .16* .30*** - - - - Partners .44*** .26*** - - - - - - - - - Partner .36* .36* - - - - - - - - - ECRavoida RSQanxio RSQavoida Fear of Fear of loss HS Iannantunono and Tylka (2012) Rice et al. (2005) Ulu and Tezer, (2010) US (n=249) Students (n=241) US (n=604) PScomp Dunkley et al. (2012) D Iannantunono and Tylka (2012) Rice et al. (2005) US (n=163) US (n=249) Students (n=241) Sample Attachme nt target ECRanxiou Fear of Secur e Preocc upied Dissi missi Fearf ul 147 Ulu and Tezer (2010) Wei et al. (2006) Wei et al. (2004) US (n=604 ) s nce us nce depen dency - - .40** .25** - .48*** .48*** .49***. 49*** .49*** .49***. 52*** .53*** .55*** .38** .39** .41** .17*** .18***. 19*** .19*** .20*** .21***. 21*** .22***. 23*** .32** .24** .21** Partner .28* Partner Partner US (n=372 ) Partner US (n=310 ) Partner closen ess ng - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - .25* - - - - - - - - - .37** .34** .33** .27** .21** .20** - - - - - - - - - Partner .35* .28* - - - - - - - - - Partner .32** .33** .32** .34** .27** .23** - - - - - - - - - COM Rice et al. (2005) Wei et al. (2004) Student s (n=241 ) US (n=310 ) DOA Rice et al. (2005) Wei et al. (2004) Student s (n=241 ) US (n=310 ) Note- *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. US-University students. Dashes represent correlations that were untested or unreported. 148 adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Although most of the findings reviewed are very consistent, they predominantly consider University students in Western cultures and so care should be taken when interpreting these results because they warrant replication in more diverse samples. Discussion In summary, the literature reviewed provides good evidence for a relationship between some elements of perfectionism and parental bonding and good evidence for a relationship between some elements of perfectionism and adult attachment. Specifically, Self-oriented perfectionism (SOP), Concern over mistakes (CoM) and Doubts over action (DoA) are positively associated with perceived parental overprotection, and SOP and CoM are positively associated with perceived parental lack of care. Similarly, CoM, DoA, and Discrepancy (D) are positively associated with attachment avoidance and anxiety. There is some evidence that SOP is positively associated with attachment dimensions, including fear of loss, dependency and closeness, and preoccupied, and fearful attachment styles. Interestingly, there is little evidence that Personal standards (PS), or High standards (HS) are associated with parental bonding or adult attachment. It is interesting to note the consistency of the findings on parental bonding and attachment, especially when it is noted that there were no measures of discrepancy taken in the studies of parental bonding. This consistency is notable because the parental bonding inventory is not a measure of attachment per se, but a retrospective measure of early experience that may capture some of the experiences that lead to the formation of attachment security or insecurity. Theoretical implications Defining perfectionism 149 Hamachek (1978) highlighted that high standards were necessary but not sufficient to define maladaptive perfectionism. Similarly Shafran and Mansell (2001) noted that the perfectionism was characterised by the pursuit of excessively high standards and a rigid adherence to them. Recently Dunkley, Zuroff, & Blankstein, (2003) factor analysed the F-MPS, H-MPS with the DEQ in an attempt to identify the factors underlying perfectionism. This produced 2 factors: High standards (SOP and PS) and Self-criticism (DEQ, DoA, CoM, SPP). Interestingly those measures of perfectionism that tap self-critical/evaluative perfectionism seem to be more associated with psychological problems than perfectionism defined by high standards (Dunkley, Blankstein, Masheb, & Grilo, 2006; Stöber & Otto, 2006). This review considers different subscales to Dunkley et al., and seems to highlight three dimensions of perfectionism High standards (PS and HS), Monitoring, evaluation and criticism of whether standards are met (D and SOP), and Concerns and doubts (CoM and DoA). Interestingly the last two factors seem to capture self-critical evaluative aspects of perfectionism and the subscales proposed to be in these factors are those most reliably associated with attachment dimensions, supporting the idea that insecure attachment is associated with maladaptive perfectionism. Considering the findings of this review in a broader theoretical context suggests that high standards are not associated with adult attachment nor with maladaptive perfectionism, but that selfcritical and evaluative aspects of perfectionism are associated with both attachment and maladaptive perfectionism. Development of perfectionism The findings of this review strongly suggest that aspects of maladaptive perfectionism are associated with adult attachment style. Scholars have previously argued that a variety of different types of early experience may contribute to the 150 development of perfectionism, (Barrow & Moore, 1983; Flett et al., 2002). Specifically, anxious rearing where parents promote a focus on mistakes and are overly critical, or provide inconsistent approval could all lead to the development of an anxious attachment style. Whereas, parental approval being absent or inconsistent, a harsh environment with difficult early experiences such as abuse, maltreatment or withdrawal of love, could lead to the development of a more avoidant attachment style. Indeed in both cases perfectionism could develop as a coping strategy in order to try and maintain and achieve love and affection, or in an attempt to avoid punishment or withdrawal of love. However, the findings of this review do not demonstrate a causal direction or explain this relationship but represents an important step in considering the relationship between attachment and perfectionism. Limitations and implications for future research Diversity of samples Both studies that considered parental bonding and perfectionism were conducted in Canada, either with depressed patients or with University students. This variation in samples allowed for some variation in age, but in both samples women were a majority. All of the studies that considered adult attachment and perfectionism were conducted in University students, this means the samples were predominantly well-educated, White, female and between the ages of 18-22. Furthermore, most of the studies were conducted in Westernised cultures, therefore, future work should look to increase the diversity of the populations considered, targeting populations that are experiencing psychological distress and those from different cultural backgrounds. Self-report measures All of the studies included in this review used self-report measures of parental bonding, attachment, and perfectionism. This is in part borne of the inclusion criteria 151 identified. A dimensional measure of attachment was specified, however, a byproduct of this decision is that this may have excluded studies that contained interviewer-assessed measures of attachment because these are typically scored categorically. Similarly all of the measures of perfectionism included in this report are self-report measures of perfectionism, this is representative of the measures of perfectionism available. The use of self-report measures means that biases such as social desirability can affect the validity of the results. Future work should consider whether the relationship between attachment, parental bonding and perfectionism is replicated on interviewer-assessed measures of attachment. Correlational designs All of the results reviewed are correlational in nature. As a result the causal directions and specific mechanisms are unclear and unspecified. Some of the studies reviewed used more complex regression modelling such as SEM or moderated regressions in an attempt to better specify these relationships. However, studies that use these types of techniques tended to address questions that are outside the remit of this review. It is clear that some elements of perfectionism are related to parental bonding and adult attachment, however the causal pathways between attachment and perfectionism warrant further research. Cross-sectional data All of the studies reviewed considered cross-sectional data on attachment and perfectionism. Given that attachment style develops early in life and is moderately stable until early adulthood (Fraley, 2002) a longitudinal design may be better able to capture the causal relationships between attachment and perfectionism. The studies that use the PBI arguably capture some early experience as the PBI is a retrospective measure of earlier parenting. However, this measure is not without fault and its 152 retrospective nature means that it may be biased by the individual’s memory of their parenting. Future work should attempt to consider the relationships between attachment and perfectionism in a longitudinal manner to help elucidate the causal relationship. Experimental investigations None of the studies identified by this review adopted an experimental approach. An experimental investigation could provide a means of assessing the causal relationship between attachment and perfectionism. Although it is not possible to manipulate attachment style, a variety of mechanisms have been used to activate attachment style, usually via priming an attachment threat (e.g., Crisp et al., 2009; Mikiluncer, Birnbaum, Woddis & Nachmias, 2000). Attachment moderated behaviours are often observed under conditions of attachment-related threat (Fraley & Shaver, 1998) so it would be possible to investigate whether following an attachment threat, those individuals with an insecure attachment style, endorse aspects of perfectionism more strongly and engage in more perfectionistic behaviour. Conclusion This review assessed whether perfectionism was related to early experiences of parenting and attachment style. The literature reviewed provided evidence that Concern over mistakes, Doubts over actions, Discrepancy, and Self-oriented perfectionism were correlated with early experiences of parental bonding and attachment style whereas high standards and personal standards were not. However, the studies reviewed were limited in their generalizability and the causal nature of the relationship is underspecified and so future work should seek to address these issues. 153 References Ainsworth. M. D. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709-716. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F0003-066X.44.4.709 Barrow, J. C., & Moore, C. A. (1983). Group interventions with perfectionist thinking. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 61, 612– 615. doi:10.1111%2Fj.2164-4918.1983.tb00008.x Blatt, S. J., D'Afflitti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976). Experiences of depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 383-389. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0021-843X.85.4.383 Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation. London: Hogarth Press. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic Books. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C.L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: an integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.) Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford. Burns, D. D. (1980). Feeling good: the new mood therapy. New York: New American Library. Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 58, 644–63. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.58.4.644 Crisp, R. J., Farrow, C. V., Rosenthal, H. E. S., Walsh, J., Blisset, J., & Penn, M. K. (2009). Interpersonal attachment predicts identification with groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 115–122. doi10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.006 154 Dunkley, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., & Blankstein, K. R. (2003). Self-critical perfectionism and daily affect: Dispositional and situational influences on stress and coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 234–252. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.84.1.234 Dunkley, D. M., Berg, J.-L., & Zuroff, D. C. (2012). The Role of Perfectionism in Daily Self-Esteem, Attachment, and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality 80 (3), 633-663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00741.x Dunkley, D. M., Blankstein, K. R., Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Personal standards and evaluative concerns dimensions of “clinical” perfectionism: A reply to Shafran et al. (2002, 2003) and Hewitt et al. (2003). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 63–84. doi:10.1016%2Fj.brat.2004.12.004 Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: A clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 203–212. doi: 0.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.009. Enns, M.W., & Cox, B. J. (1999). Perfectionism and depression symptom severity in major depressive disorder. Behavior Research & Therapy, 37, 783-94. doi:10.1016%2FS0005-7967%2898%2900188-0 Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). Nature and assessment of perfectionism. In G. L. Flett & P.L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 33−62).Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Enns, M.W., Cox, B. J. & Clara, (2002). Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism: developmental origins and association with depression proneness. Personality and Individual Differences 33, 921–935. doi:10.1016%2FS01918869%2801%2900202-1 Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., & Larsen, D. K. (2000). Perceptions of parental bonding and 155 symptom severity in adults with depression: Mediation by personality dimensions. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, 263–268. doi: not available. Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123-151. doi:10.1207%2FS15327957PSPR0602_03 Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C. M., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468. doi: 10.1007%2FBF01172967 George, C., Kaplan, N. & Main, M. (1996). Adult attachment interview. Unpublished manuscript. Griffin, D. W., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). The metaphysics of measurement: The case of adult attachment. In K. Bartholomew and D. Perlman (Eds.) Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment process in adulthood (pp. 17–52). London: Kingsley. Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Oliver, J. M., & Macdonald, S. (2002). Perfectionism in Children and Their Parents: A Developmental Analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.). Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 89-132). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 15, 27– 33. doi: not avaliable Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987) Romantic Love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511-524. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0022-3514.52.3.511 Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of 156 Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456–470. doi:10.1037%2F%2F00223514.60.3.456 Iannantuono, A. C. & Tylka, T. L. (2012). Interpersonal and intrapersonal links to body appreciation in college women: An exploratory model. Body Image, 9, 227– 235. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01.004. Kurdek, L. A. (2002). On being insecure about the assessment of attachment styles. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 811–834. doi: 10.1177%2F0265407502196005 Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative and intraindividual components of attachment theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 509–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.509. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Measurement of attachment-related constructs in adulthood. In Attachment in adulthood: structure, dynamics, and change (pp. 81–115). New York: Guilford Press. Pacht, A. R. (1984). Reflections on perfection. American Psychologist, 39, 386-390. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0003-066X.39.4.386 Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1–10. doi:10.1111%2Fj.20448341.1979.tb02487.x Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., & Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment measures: A 25-year review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 69, 419–432. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.08.006 Reis, S., & Grenyer, B. F. S. (2002). Pathways to anaclitic and introjective depression. Psychology & Psychotherapy: Theory, research & Practice, 75, 445- 157 459. doi: 10.1348%2F147608302321151934 Rhéaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Bouchard, C., Gallant, L., Talbot, F., & Vallières, A. (2000). Functional and dysfunctional perfectionists: Are they different on compulsive-like behaviors? Behaviour Research & Therapy, 38, 119–128. doi: 10.1016%2FS0005-7967%2898%2900203-4 Rice, K. G., Lopez, F. G., & Vergara, D. (2005). Parental/social influences on perfectionism and adult attachment orientations. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 580–605. doi:10.1521%2Fjscp.2005.24.4.580 Rice, K. G., & Mirzadeh, S. A. (2000). Perfectionism, attachment, and adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 238–250. doi: 10.1037%2F%2F00220167.47.2.238 Shafran, R., & Mansell, W. (2001). Perfectionism and psychopathology: A review of research and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 879−906. doi: 10.1016%2FS0272-7358%2800%2900072-6 Shanmugam, V., Jowett, S., & Meyer, C. (2012). Eating Psychopathology amongst athletes: Links to current attachment styles. Eating Behaviors, 13, 5-12. doi: 10.1016%2Fj.eatbeh.2011.09.004 Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised Almost Perfect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 130–145. doi: not available. Stöber, J. (1998). The Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: More perfect with four (instead of six) dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 24(4), 481-491. doi: 10.1016%2FS0191-8869%2897%2900207-9 Stöber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295–319. 158 doi: 10.1207%2Fs15327957pspr1004_2 Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early care, attachment, and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34, 834-841. doi:10.1037%2F%2F0003-066X.34.10.834 Sümer, N., & Güngör, D. (1999). Yetişkin bağlanma stillerinin Türk örneklemi üzerinde psikometrik değerlendirmesi ve kültürler arası bir karşılaştırma. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 14(43), 71-106. doi: not avaliable Ulu, İ. P., & Tezer, E. (2010). Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism, adult attachment, and big five personality traits. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 144, 327-340. doi:10.1080%2F00223981003784032 Wei, M., Mallinckrodt, B., Russell, D. W., & Abraham, T. W. (2004). Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between attachment and negative mood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 201–212. doi: not available. Wei, M., Heppner, P. P., Russell, D. W., & Young, S. K. (2006). Maladaptive perfectionism and ineffective coping as mediators between attachment and subsequent depression: A prospective analyses. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 67-79. doi: not available. 159 Clinical Experience An overview of the clinical experiences gained on placement. 160 Year 1 I spent the first year of training in a Community Mental Health and Recovery Service for working age adults. I worked with individuals with diagnoses of Complex trauma, PTSD, bipolar affective disorder, low self-esteem, borderline personality disorder, avoidant personality disorder, psychosis, obsessive compulsive disorderhoarding, social phobia, agoraphobia, and emetophobia. I worked with individuals with underlying autistic spectrum disorders, and with individuals whose attachment style and relationships were affected by an attachment figure having a probable autistic spectrum condition. I used a variety of therapeutic interventions including psychoeducation, CBT, Dialectical Behaviour Therapy, Mindfulness, and Compassion-focussed therapy. I co-facilitated two groups: a four-week Understanding Personality Disorder group and an eight-week Mindfulness group. I also conducted a neuropsychological assessment to consider whether a woman with longstanding hoarding disorder was experiencing the onset of dementia. I taught the multi-disciplinary team (MDT) about motivational interviewing and taught carers about communication skills. I also conducted an audit to consider whether the team were NICE guideline compliant in their management of individuals with schizophrenia/psychosis and shared the findings with the team. I attended a variety of team meetings including business meetings and allocations meetings, and team reflective meetings. I also attended local psychology meetings, carer support groups and the Forum for Carers and people who use services. Year 2 My second placement was in a Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service. I worked with a number of children, adolescents, parents and teachers to help support young people with a range of presentations including chronic pain, anxiety, low 161 mood, ADHD, autistic spectrum conditions, obsessive compulsive disorder, and anger. I used both second wave CBT approaches and third approaches including ACT and Mindfulness. I conducted two neuropsychological assessments: one with an adolescent girl who was presenting with anger, but had attention and memory difficulties and one with an adolescent boy with a history of epilepsy who described having difficulties with his memory. I also conducted a school observation and formulated the child’s difficulties and shared this with the parents and school. I also supervised an assistant psychologist to complete a school observation. I taught Year 10 pupils about anxiety in a school-setting with a primary mental health worker. I also attended regular team meetings, and psychology local area meetings and service-user group meetings and observed social work consultation meetings. My third placement was in an Older People’s Community Mental Health Team. I worked with a gentleman who had been experiencing depression since his retirement using a CBT approach and with a woman with bipolar affective disorder who was experiencing depression and anxiety. On an inpatient ward for older adults with mental health difficulties I provided weekly CBT groups for anxiety, depression and grief and a recovery group. I taught the ward staff about the nature of the groups and what would make an appropriate referral. I also conducted neuropsychological assessments of two women who were concerned that they had developed dementia. Both women had longstanding mental health difficulties. I also helped to setup team formulation meetings to help facilitate the MDT understanding of the service-users difficulties. Year 3 My fourth placement was in a Chronic Pain out-patient team. In this 162 placement I worked with a number of individuals, and couples with a variety of types of pain disorders, and co-morbid diagnoses including complex regional pain syndrome, chronic fatigue, chronic pelvic pain, fibromyalgia, anxiety, depression, and borderline personality disorder. I used both second-wave CBT approaches and third wave approaches such as mindfulness and ACT. I co-facilitated an eight-week Living with Pain course with a physiotherapist and my supervisor. I lead a case formulation and discussion group with the MDT and designed and delivered a workshop on Depression and chronic pain to service-users. I also worked in more generic health settings. I observed a neuropsychologist conduct an assessment on the stroke ward and I co-facilitated a level two psychological skills training in CBT for oncology nurses. In addition I worked therapeutically with a diabetic woman to address her fear of needles. I also supervised an assistant psychologist to develop some leaflets as a piece of service development work. My final placement was in Community Team for People with Learning Disabilities. Within this placement I worked with individuals, their families and carer’s and with day services and residential homes. I worked with people with a range of learning difficulties including Angelman’s syndrome, Down’s syndrome, foetal alcohol spectrum conditions, and Autism and other conditions including epilepsy, hydrocephalus, and bipolar affective disorder. I developed and shared formulations with a number of clients for a variety of difficulties including loneliness, gambling, and anger and conducted a systemic intervention with a care home. I also conducted a range of neuropsychological assessment to assess service-users strengths and weaknesses as part of dementia, and autism assessments. I supervised an assistant psychologist to complete a joint piece of work to understand a challenging behaviour. 163 Assessments 164 Year I Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT Fundamentals of Theory and Practice in Clinical Psychology (FTPCP) Practice case report Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account Research – Literature Review Adult – case report Adult – case report Research – Qualitative Research Project Research – Major Research Project Proposal TITLE OF ASSIGNMENT Short report of WAIS-IV data and practice administration Assessment and formulation of a young woman with PTSD. Reflective account of personal and professional learning from the process of completing a group problem-based learning task. Perfectionism, parental bonding and attachment. Assessment and intervention of an individual with complex trauma. Neuropsychological assessment of memory difficulties What do First Year Clinical Psychology Trainees Think about Clinical Supervision? Can compassionate-imagery reduce perfectionist’s experience of shame? Year II Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT Research - SRRP Research Professional Issues Essay Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account Child and Family – Case Report Personal and Professional Learning TITLE OF ASSESSMENT NICE guidelines audit of psychological and psychosocial interventions being offered to those with Schizophrenia in a CMHRS. Research Methods and Statistics test Critically explore the statement that clinical psychology should “move away from psychiatric diagnoses…., which have significant conceptual and empirical limitations, and develop alternative approaches which recognise the centrality of the complex range of life experiences…” (Position Statement on the Classification of Behaviour and Experience in Relation to Functional Psychiatric Diagnoses: Time for a Paradigm Shift, BPS, 2013). Reflective account of personal and professional learning from the process of completing a group problem-based learning task. Assessment and intervention of a young person with chronic pain, fatigue and anxiety. A reflective account of the personal and professional learning discussion group process. 165 Discussion Groups – Process Account Older People – Oral Presentation of Clinical Activity Case consultation skills. Year III Assessments PROGRAMME COMPONENT Research – MRP Portfolio Personal and Professional Learning – Final Reflective Account Specialist – Case Report ASSESSMENT TITLE Perfectionism, Failure and Self-conscious Emotions: A role for Self-compassion? On becoming a clinical psychologist: A retrospective, developmental, reflective account of the experience of training. Exposure therapy with a young woman with needle phobia and type 1 diabetes. 166