The Effects of Cognitive Task Analysis-based Instruction on Students’ Achievement and Retention in Undergraduate Biology Course David F. Feldon, Jie Chao Department of Curriculum, Instruction and Special Education, University of Virginia Abstract: Poor instruction has been cited as a primary cause of attrition from STEM majors and a major obstacle to learning for those who stay (Seymour & Hewitt, 1997). Using a double-blind design, this study tested the hypothesis that the lack of explicit instructions in scientific inquiry skills is a major factor in both low STEM retention and academic underperformance. This project delivered supplemental instruction to students in a laboratory-based undergraduate biology course that was derived either from cognitive task analyses (CTAs) conducted with expert biologists (treatment) or was authored and delivered by an award-winning biology instructor (control). Overall, CTA-based instruction appeared to be more beneficial than the traditional instruction for students, especially those who were low-achieving. This differential effect of CTA-based instruction may be attributed to an expertise reversal effect (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003). Background Poor instruction has been cited as a primary cause of attrition from Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM) majors and a major obstacle to learning for those who stay (Seymour and Hewitt, 1997). This research project aims to improve undergraduate retention and achievement in the biological sciences through the reformulation of instruction in the core course sequences. The core technique employed in the instructional design process is Cognitive Task Analysis (CTA), which is an effective tool for eliciting, analyzing, and representing expert knowledge in a more accurate and complete manner (Clark, Feldon, van Merriënboer, Yates, & Early, 2008). CTAbased training systems that have explicitly accommodated the tacit nature of experts’ knowledge have proven to be significantly more effective than those that have not (e.g., Merrill, 2002). Further, there is substantial evidence that gaps in instructional content resulting from the omission of necessary steps in problem-solving procedures induce higher levels of cognitive load in learners, which interferes with learning and can lower motivation (e.g., Kirschner, Sweller, & Clark, 2006). To evaluate the impact of CTA-based instruction on undergraduate biology students compared to traditional instructions by an award-winning professor, the following hypotheses were tested: (1) CTA-based instruction reduces the rate of attrition in an introductory level biology course. (2) CTA-based instruction leads to increased performance in scientific problem solving as measured by the quality of biology laboratory reports. Methodology A double-blind experimental design was employed to test the effectiveness of CTA-based instructions compared to traditional instruction. Supplemental instructional videos were delivered to students in a laboratory-based undergraduate biology course (n = 1979) at a public, Tier-I university in the Southeast for five semesters from 2008 to 2010. The lectures were delivered by an award-winning biology instructor and were either written by him (traditional) or derived from cognitive task analyses conducted with expert biologists (CTA-based). To ensure that the treatment and control populations were equivalent in both general scientific reasoning ability and motivation, Lawson’s Test of Scientific Reasoning (Lawson) and the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) were both administered at the beginning of the course. Neither measure found significant differences between treatment and control samples. Dependent measures were students’ course completion records and performance on lab reports scored with Universal Lab Rubric (Timmerman et al., in press). Results Overall, CTA-based instruction appeared to be more beneficial than the traditional instruction for the students, especially those low-achieving students. Results from the first semester (Spring 2008) showed that students (n=314) receiving traditional instruction were almost six times more likely to withdraw from the course than students receiving CTA-based instruction (8.1% vs. 1.4% of initial enrollment). Of the students who completed the course, those who received the CTA-based instruction demonstrated significantly higher levels of performance in the discussion section of their written laboratory reports. Significantly higher performances were seen specifically in the areas of analyzing data to formulate valid conclusions, considering alternative explanations, consideration for the limitations of the experimental design and implications of the research (Feldon et al, 2010). Results from the subsequent four semesters (Fall 2008, Spring 2009, Fall 2009, and Spring 2010) showed that biology students who had to repeat the course due to withdrawal or failure in a previous semester receiving traditional instruction were twice as likely as those receiving CTA-based instruction to withdraw from the course a second time or decline to submit the major lab report assignment (61.1% vs. 31.3% of initial enrollment). Of the students who completed the course and viewed all the instructional videos, overall differences in lab report performance between experimental and control groups were not significant. However, significant differences were found among subgroups of participants. For students who repeated the course during this study, CTA-based instruction was significantly more effective than traditional instruction for some skills. These effects appeared to be mostly contributed by female students and non-biology students. Discussion Most students take Biology 101 only once and earn a passing grade. However, students who struggle may withdraw from or fail the course and choose to repeat it in a subsequent semester. Students who repeated the course were assumed to have insufficient prior knowledge to succeed in the course during their first attempt. CTA-based instruction is differentially effective for these struggling students and has greatest and most reliable effects on students who were repeating the course. Further, there were limited negative effects for students who did not repeat the course. This result may be attributed to the expertise reversal effect (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003) which describes the consequences of providing specific instructional support to learners across a wide range of ability. Students with lower levels of prior knowledge benefit tremendously from highly explicit guidance. However, students with more expertise can be hindered by needing to parse instruction that is too basic for them. With regard to gender, it is possible that women in the study experience or perceive a lower level of access to informal support mechanisms than their male counterparts. Supports could include the ability to have their questions answered during class time or instructors’ office hours or access to networks of supportive peers who can provide needed knowledge to bolster student understanding. Such conditions are commonly cited in studies of differential gender outcomes in science education. It is possible that the highly explicit nature of the treatment condition’s instruction reduces reliance on other sources of necessary information. Conversely, use of such resources could bolster performance in the male sub-population, which would diminish observable treatment effects and potentially invoke the expertise reversal effect where CTA-based instruction was interfering with information gained outside the formal course structure. References Clark, R. E., Feldon, D. F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Yates, K. A., & Early, S. (2008). Cognitive task analysis. In: J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. J. G. van Merriënboer, & M. P. Driscoll (Eds.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (3rd ed., pp. 577–593). New York: Routledge. Feldon, D. F., Timmerman, B. C., Stowe, K. A., Showman, R. (2010). Translating expertise into effective instruction: The impacts of Cognitive Task Analysis (CTA) on lab report quality and student retention in the biological sciences. Journal of research in science teaching, 47(10), 1165–1185. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 23-31. Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimally guided learning does not work: An analysis of the failure of discovery learning, problem-based learning, experiential learning and inquiry-based learning. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. Merrill, M. D. (2002). A pebble-in-the-pond model for instructional design. Performance Improvement, 41(7), 39– 44. Paas, F. & van Merriënboer, J. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), pp. 122–133. Seymour, E., & Hewitt, N. (1997). Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave the sciences. Boulder, CO: Westview.