‘Molecular basis of cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis’ Master Thesis - Neuroscience and Cognition Master’s program Gkountidi Anastasia-Olga Supervisors : Geert Schenk1 , Jeroen Geurts1 , Martien Kas2 1 2 VU University of Amsterdam, Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience Utrecht University, Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology Summary Multiple sclerosis is a chronic neurodegenerative disease affecting the central nervous system, i.e brain areas, spinal cord. It is characterized as a neurodegenerative disease because of the absence of myelin that facilitates a good flow of electricity along the nervous system of the brain. Multiple sclerosis can affect people from all ages, but it is more common between ages 20 to 50 years. People of all ancestries can have it although people European descent are more prone to developing multiple sclerosis. The clinical course is variable and among the symptoms observed in patients memory loss, difficulties in processing speed information, attention, learning decision making, problem solving are cognitive processes mainly affected. Depending on the stage of the disease, the cognitive abilities that are affected are presented below as well as the possible molecular mechanisms that are involved. Abstract Multiple sclerosis is a neurodegenerative disorder studied from the beginning of the 20 th century with many unanswered questions so far. For many years, main attention was given to the white matter areas and developing technologies and techniques that would further shed light to the mysteries of this disease. Even though the implication of grey matter was originally noticed in the 1960s, it was at the beginning of the 21st century, that its importance became greater and grey matter damage was found to be more extensive and frequent than lesions in white matter. Attention to grey matter areas has enhanced as the cognitive deficits that accompany multiple sclerosis much more often than wan initially assumed were more linked to it. This report presents evidence of the molecules in grey matter areas that underlie the cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Glossary: Multiple sclerosis = MS, Central nervous system = CNS, Relapsing-remitting MS = RRMS, Secondary progressive MS= SPMS, primary progressive MS = PPMS, Progressive-relapsing MS = PRMS, Magnetic resonance imaging = MRI, Cognitive impairment = CI, information processing = IPS, long term potentiation = LTP, Alzheimer’s disease = AD, Parkinson’s disease = PD, corpus callosum = CC, neurofilament = Nf. Introduction Multiple sclerosis is a debilitating inflammatory-mediated demyelinating disease of the human central nervous system. The name multiple sclerosis refers to scars (better known as plaques or lesions) partially found in the white matter of the brain and spinal cord. The myelin sheaths around the axons are damaged leading to demyelination as well as a broad spectrum of signs and symptoms (Compston & Coles., 2008) which vary depending on the stage of the disease. The disease onset usually occurs in young adults, targeting women more than men (Compston & Coles., 2008) and affects more than two million people worldwide (Hauser & Oksenberg, 2006; Noseworthy, 1999; Noseworthy et al., 2000; Trapp & Nave, 2008; Weinshenker, 1998). The patterns of the disease have led to its classification in four clinical courses: 1) relapsingremitting MS, 2) secondary progressive MS, 3) primary progressive MS and 4) progressive-relapsing MS. The majority of the patients (85%) belong in the first clinical course, where they experience unpredictable episodes of neurological disability followed by remissions that can last months to years without sign of disease disability (Hauser & Oksenberg, 2006; Noseworthy, 1999; Noseworthy et al., 2000; Trapp & Nave, 2008). The secondary progressive disease course (sometimes called ‘galloping MS’) is characterized by steady neurological decline (Noseworthy et al., 2000; Trapp and Nave, 2008 Weinshenker et al., 1989) where occasional relapses and minor remissions may appear (Lublin & Reingold, 1996). The time that intervenes between the disease onset and conversion from PP-MS to SPMS is 25 years for the 90% of the MS patients. Primary progressive MS is characterized by the steady decline in neurological function without recovery. The age onset of PP-MS is around 40 years. A very small percentage of patients (5%) belongs to the last clinical course, PR-MS which prevents a steady neurological decline but also suffer superimposed attacks. Multiple sclerosis has been classified as a white matter disease although later studies (Dawson, 1916; Brownell & Hughes, 1962) have shown that grey matter areas are affected. White matter atrophic areas, studied with MRI techniques can explain some of the physical characteristics of MS patients like epilepsy and depression. However, memory and attention impairment along with reduced information processing are abilities controlled by grey matter brain areas that are affected in 45-65% of MS patients (Rao et al., 1991; Rao et al., 1995).This evidence makes the implication of grey matter even more important. Cognitive impairment is a well-known consequence of MS acknowledged since the early description of MS (Charcot, 1877). It affects roughly 50% of the patients especially in the domains of memory and information processing and it can develop at any time during the course of the disease in the presence or absence of neurological disability. Among the cognitive impairments that patients manifest are also bradyphrenia, impaired abstract thinking and concentration and language deficits (Rao et al., 1991; Newman,2001). Many brain areas, during MRI examination, show grey matter demyelinationthalamus, hippocampus, caudate, putamen, globus pallidus, frontal and temporal cortex, cingulate gyrus, hipothalamus, substantia nigra, amygdala and spinal cord grey matter (Kutzelnigg et al, 2005; Gilmore et al, 2009; Vercellino et al., 2005; Kutzelnigg et al, 2007; Geurts et al,2007; Huitinga et al., 2001; Gilmore et al., 2006). Although improved imaging techniques have opened up the possibility to study grey matter abnormalities in greater detail, not much is known about the underlying molecular mechanisms that lead to gray matter damage and associated cognitive deficits. The aim of this study is therefore to investigate the molecular basis of cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis and identify a possible correlation between affected brain areas that are important for cognition and the molecules involved. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis is a known concomitant of multiple sclerosis affecting 45-65% of cases (Rao et al., 1991; McIntosh-Michaelis et al., 1991; LaRocca, 2000). During the course of their lives, MS patients will experience CI at any time simultaneously to neurological disabilities. Clinically, the effect of the disease on the individual differs depending on the stage of the disease. Among the early beliefs about MS was that cognitive impairment appears several years after the disease onset, manifesting itself as it has already progressed (Butler et al., 2009). However, this assumption has been declined and it is clear that CI can present itself early in the disease process. Many patients diagnosed with clinically isolated syndrome (CIS) indicative of MS, who also had abnormal MRI findings, in a very early stage converted to a diagnosis of MS (Brex et al., 2002; Morrissey et al., 1993; Tintore et al.,2006) showing that cognitive impairment can be early diagnosed (Feuillet et al., 2007; Achiron et al., 2003; Callanan et al., 1989; Feinstein(a) et al., 1992; Feinstein(b) et al., 1992). Neuroimaging studies have provided indications on the brain areas affected. And although white matter was first reported to be affected, the last years more attention is given to grey matter. Memory impairment, attention deficit, bradyphrenia, impaired abstract thinking, information processing and concentration are appearing symptoms in MS because of grey matter damage. The importance of these forms of cognitive impairment in everyday life is needless to be mentioned (Morrow et al., 2010). Areas affected in cognitive impairment (imaging studies) Structural neuroimaging techniques have revealed atrophic brain areas in MS patients. Demyelination of axons in the cortical area is of importance as well as demyelination in the white matter but so far it has not been feasible to assign precisely the brain regions to the features of cognitive impairment presented in this disease (Moriarty et al., 1999; Lazeron et al., 2000; Rovaris et al., 2000; Sokic et al., 2001; Spatt et al., 2001; Benedict et al., 2004; Feinstein et al., 2004). While studying MS, many groups reported that in the tissue they examined (post mortem material), many if not all grey matter areas exceeded in total the white matter volume of demyelination found in SPMS patients (Bo et al., 2003 (a),(b); Geurts & Barkhof 2008; Hulst & Geurts 2011; Lublin & Reingold 1996; Gilmore et al., 2009). So far though, grey matter demyelination seems to be better explaining cognitive deficits like memory impairment and attention deficits. Patients with cognitive decline have more cortical damage than cognitively preserved patients (Amato et al., 2004; Amato et al., 2007), proof that underscores the previous statement. Cortical atrophy is progressive in MS patients and follows a course different than that observed in normal aging. It is more prominent in areas that have extensive connections like the cingulated gyrus and insular, temporal, frontal and parietal cortices (Charil et al., 2007). The neocortex is one of the first regions where grey matter lesions were classified in four different categories. Type I which is mixed white-grey matter lesion and types II, III and IV intracortical lesions (Bö et al.,2003 a,b; Geurts et al., 2007). Volume reduction in the neocortex is linked to cognitive performance that distinguishes cognitively preserved patients and those with cognitive impairment (Benedict et al., 2007). Apart from neocortex and other neocortcal areas, like motor cortex, frontal, temporal and parietal cortex, other deep gray areas like thalamus, caudate, putamen, globus pallidus, amygdala, hypothalamus, hippocampus, substantia nigra and spinal cord grey matter, show demyelination too (Kutzelnigg et al., 2005; Gilmore et al., 2009; Vercellino et al., 2005; Kutzelnigg et al., 2007; Geurts et al., 2007; Huitinga et al., 2001; Gilmore et al., 2006). The hippocampus is a main region for the formation and retrieval of memories through a series of events. It is a region well studied in MS because it is affected by demyelination in a subsequent progressive stage of the disease. In a subgroup of patients studied by Dutta (2011), low white matter lesion load in addition to minimal physical disability, was an indicator that demylination in the hippocampus can lead to memory dysfunction. Magnetic resonance imaging measures correlated increased hippocampal atrophy to memory dysfunction (Roosendaal et al., 2008). Demyelination in the area was found in 53% to 79% in post mortem tissue with significantly decreased synaptic density (Dutta et al., 2011). Atrophy and enlargement of ventricles of corpus callosum is linked to cognitive dysfunction in MS (Tsolaki et al., 2004; Clark et al., 2002; Comi et al., 1993). Although a white matter structure, it connects grey matter areas, having therefore importance to cognitive deficits. Atrophy and axonal integrity is related to speed of information processing, semantic fluency and sustained attention, features impaired in this disease (Cristodoulou et al., 2001). Patients who suffer from these kinds of lesions present decreased information processing speed (Rao et al., 1991), impaired verbal fluency (Pozzilli et al., 1991), visuospatial difficulties (Ryan et al., 1996) and sign of interhemispheric disconnection (Huber et al., 1987). In a detailed study conducted by Swirsky-Sacchetti, the authors tried to correlate the neuropsychological characteristics with different kind of measures of the lesion area in corpus callosum (size of the corpus callosum in frontal, temporal, and parieto-occipital regions, where it was cognitively functional, total lesion area etc.). The total lesion area was the strongest predictor of cognitive impairment. In addition to that, the left frontal lobe was related to memory, word fluency and abstract problem solving deficits. Verbal learning and visuospatial skills were predicted from the left parietooccipital lesions (Swirsky-Sacchetti et al., 1992). In another four year study (Sperling et al., 2001), the authors tried to link the correlation between the lesion burden and the cognitive performance in a group of patients. Most of the lesions related to complex attention and verbal working memory were found in the frontal and parietal white matter. Info processing speed is also another primary cognitive deficit in MS regulated most likely by the basal ganglia and the thalamus. Compared to normal controls the volume of the two areas shows significant reduction (Houtchens et al.,2007; Benedict et al., 2009). The study of Batista (2012), is the first one to show that each area affects independently and predicts the IPS deficit. Left frontal cortical atrophy has been linked to performance on verbal memory tests and right frontal atrophy to visual memory deficits and working memory. Cognitive status of patients is also linked to the width of the third ventricle (Benedict et al., 2004). Molecules related to cognitive decline: comparison to Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease Given the fact that 30-40% of patients demonstrate memory impairment, the hippocampus region related to memory was investigated. Among the studies conducted so far, the one by Dutta (2011) can answer the question of which molecules are involved in cognitive impairment in MS to a large extent. Although the molecular basis of this aspect hasn’t been defined so far, many changes occurring in MS brain tissue are especially attributed to the hippocampal region. Molecules involved in synaptic plasticity, synaptic integrity, axonal transport and memory and learning formation are affected. Comparing morphological and molecular changes, Dutta (2011) found that in more than half of the patients, the demyelination was extensive. Neuronal density was decreased by 10-20% in some of the investigated areas but axonal densities were similar in myelinated and demyelinated hippocampi. Among the genes found to be changed in demyelinated MS hippocampi, the mRNAs that encode neuronal proteins related to memory function and synaptic integrity were altered. The major microtubule motor in charge of fast anterograde axonal transport KIF1A was decreased at the mRNA and protein levels along with KIF3A, KIF15, KIF5B and KIF5C. Kinectin (KTN1), important for kinesin- driven vesicle transport was reduced in the protein level. The synaptic vesicle proteins synaptophysin and synaptotagmin that are related to KIF1A were also reduced in demyelinated tissue. Synaptostagmin is a calcium sensor in the regulation of neurotransmitter release and hormone secretion. Synaptophysin has been studied in more detail in mice, where its elimination lead to reduced spatial learning and impaired novelty recognition (Schmitt et al.,2009). The above evidence show that proteins essential for anterograde and retrograde axonal transport are negatively affected. Similarly to this, in a study by Reddy (2008), the levels of presynaptic protein, synaptophysin, were decreased in Alzheimer’s patients and in another study by 77% (Sze et al., 1997) in the hippocampus. This protein is indeed involved in the progression of AD in areas linked to the magnitude of memory impairment and most likely plays a role in multiple sclerosis (Sze et al., 1997). It has also been found that cognitive dysfunction is enhanced by synaptic pruning that leads to reduced long term potentiation (Neves et al., 2008). LTP is a long-lasting enhancement in signaling between neurons and believed to form the cellular basis of learning and memory formation (Bliss et al., 1993). Demyelinaton in the hippocampus leads to a decrease n the number of synapses in MS tissue. To be more precise, molecules that help contact between pre- and post- synaptic specializations (Sudhof 2008) like neurexins and neuroligins are significantly decreased in demyelinated tissue. Proteins directly related to them, PSD95 and CASK, were also decreased at the protein and mRNA level. The above evidence shows that loss of myelin leads to changes in the number and formation of synapses. Reduced glutamate receptors and transporters were also found in the Dutta study. Reduced mRNAs for AMPA1 and AMPA3 receptors, NMDAR2A, 2B, 2C, 2D, and 3B subunits and MGLUR1, MGLUR2, MGLUR3, and MGLUR4 metabotropic receptors were noticed. Glutamate neurotransmission is also affected in demyelinated hippocampi. Apart from the internal regulation of glutamate, the extracellular compartment is affected as well. Glutamate synthetase that removes glutamate from the synapse and recycles it by metabolism is decreased as well as the glutamate transporters EAAT1 and EAAT2. Continuing the hippocampal investigation, differences were found in the cholinergic neurotransmitter system in MS hippocampus. The cholinergic neurotransmitter system plays an important role in the process of learning and memory (Drachman & Leavitt 1974; Everitt & Robbins 1997) and the hippocampus is a major region of cholinergic input from the basal forebrain (Mesulam, 2004). The activities of the enzyme choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) as well as its absolute protein levels were decreased in contrast to the degrading enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which was unaltered. Compared to controls the ChAT activity was reduced by 43% in MS hippocampi and by 45% in AD hippocampi. In MS patients the ChAT immunoreactivity decreased by 80% in CA4, in CA3-2 by 57% and in CA1 by 59%. On the other hand in AD patients it was decreased by 92% in CA4, by 81% in CA32 and in CA1 by 85%. The AChE activity was found unaltered in CA4 and CA1 but there were no differences found in AChE protein expression in MS and control hippocampus. These findings of Kooi (2011), reveal a selective imbalance in the MS hippocampal area. In relation to other neurodegenerative diseases, Parkinson’s patients with dementia seem to be suffering from extensive reduction of choline transferase and less extensive reductions of AChE in the temporal neocortex, correlated with the degree of mental impairment. Alzheimer- type dementia has almost identical cortical cholinergic biochemical activities but different neocortical involvement. The nucleus of Meynert in AD is accompanied by neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques whereas neuronal loss is regularlyshown in PD (Perry et al., 1985). Decreased glucose metabolism has been associated with a reduced cognitive index in MS patients. Using positron emission tomography (PET) scan to measure regional cerebral glucose metabolism (rCMRglc), Paulesu (1996) screened a group of MS patients. Patients with memory deficits showed bilateral reduction of rCMRglc in the hippocampus, cingulated gyrus, thalamus, associative accipital cortex and cerebellum. MS patients with memory impairment had significantly reduced global CMRglc compared to normal controls but not when compared to MS patients whose memory was intact. Between the memory impaired and unimpaired patients, the first had significant reduction of rCMRglc bilaterally in the hippocampus and in the left thalamus and there was a trend of significance also in the right thalamus. As a region of interest, corpus callosum was also studied in relation to cerebral metabolic rates of glucose (Pozzilli et al., 1992). Cerebral metabolic asymmetries were associated to CC asymmetry. MS patients with CC atrophy have a left predominant hypometabolism indicating a more enhanced involvement of the left hemisphere. Controls as well as MS patients without CC atrophy did not present this type of metabolism, leaving evidence that it is an index of CC atrophy. A variety of factors can regulate LTP by interfering between receptors in excitatory synapses (Bliss & Collingridge, 1993; Malenka, 2003; Feldman, 2009; Kessels & Malinow, 2009; Minichiello, 2009). The isoform amyloid-β1–42, from amyloid-β , aggregates in oligomers that impair synaptic plasticity mechanisms (Klyubin et al, 2005; Shankar et al, 2008; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010). Aβ is the main component of amyloid plaques, found in the brains of AD patients. A number of studies, animal, biochemical and cell biology support the idea that Aβ plaques play a central role in AD pathology (Ghiso and Frangione 2002; Selkoe, 2001). This isoform was consistently found to be reduced in the CSF of AD patients and combined to its so far known implication in LTP, was studied in MS patients (Mori et al., 2011). In their research, the authors claimed that acute inflammation in MS, interferes with amyloid-β1–42 or τ metabolism, like in AD and subsequently is linked to cognitive function and synaptic plasticity. In animals, cognitive performances and LTP induction are altered because of amyloid-β and τ metabolism (Oddo et al, 2003; Klyubin et al, 2005; Rosenmann et al, 2008; Shankar et al, 2008; Polydoro et al, 2009; Walther et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2010), however in humans that option had not been investigated. What was found in fact was that amyloid-β1–42 levels were significantly lower in CSF patients with Gadolinium-enhancing (Gd+) lesions, showing an altered metabolism. The domains that were linked to these altered levels were attention, concentration and information processing speed, cognitive areas primarily known to be affected in MS. The evidence of low amyloid levels, combined to Gd+ lesions that were associated to poor PASAT (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test) performance (Bellmann-Strobl et al, 2009) is an indication of the amyloid implication in synaptic plasticity. As a candidate regulator of plasticity in the human brain, amyloid-β may important to understand the mechanisms of cognitive impairment in MS. Another category of proteins that have been implicated in MS are cytokines. Although the complex interactions that occur among them haven’t made clear their role in cognitive decline, those that worsen cognition are increased in SPMS and those that help cognition are reduced in SPMS (http://www.msforumonline.net/Site/News/default.aspx ). As reviewed in Haase & Faustman (2007), the network created by cytokines has been mainly studied in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis (EAE) and in human MS (Ackerman et al., 1998; Arnason, 1995). Even though MS is considered a demyelinating disease, it is also a T-cell mediated autoimmune disorder, influenced by cytokines. Tumor necrosis factor (TNF) alpha and interferon (IFN) gamma are secreted by activated T-helper cells (CD4+) of Th1- phenotype, affecting oligodendrocytes cytotoxically (Banks et al., 2002).Increased TNF-alpha levels were linked to relapses in MS patients (Banks et al., 2001) and to physical disability (Besedovsky & del Rey, 2002). The anti-inflammatory aspect of the disease includes also the implication of cytokines (Ackerman et al., 1998; Arnason, 1995, Callanan et al., 1989; Cazullo et al., 2003) suggesting a complex interaction within this group of cytokine proteins. INF-gama, TNF alpha, IL-1 alpha/beta and IL-6 have been found to be mediating cognitive skills in animal models. Spatial memory has been linked to elevated IL-1 beta (Pugh et al., 1999) and treatments with it lead to disturbed spatial and non-spatial learning and memory (Larson & Dunn 2001). IL- 1 alpha application in animals lead to extensive changes in the hippocampus and was identified as specific for learning during memory consolidation (Depini et al., 2004; Schneider et al., 1998). Other cytokines were able to determine the extend behavior and cognition in animals (Haddad et al., 2002). In humans these cytokines were regarded as responsible for the sickness and malaise provoked to the patients but not necessarily for cognitive deficits (Haase & Faustmann, 2004; Kühlwein & Irwin, 2001). There is evidence that neurofilaments are also involved in multiple sclerosis. They are abundant axon- and neuron- cytoskeletal components involved in axonal transport and neuronal homeostasis (de Waegh et al., 1992; Petzold, 1995). According to Kuhle and co-workers (2011), NfHSMI35 protein in CSF is increased in the course of the evolution of the disease. This increase is thought as an indicator of accelerating neuronal damage reinforcing the utility of NfH as a biomarker (Kuhle et al., 2011). In order to verify the involvement of neurofilaments in multiple sclerosis, cortical regions were correlated to the proportion of neurofilaments, specifically hyperphosphorylated neurofilament- H, NfHSMI34. The interesting point was that the increase was observed in both demyelinated and non-demyelinated cortex in MS. The observation that the number of neuronal cells with NfHSMI35 in non-demyelinated cortical areas is higher than in controls suggests the existence of other contributing factors outside the lesions, enhancing the idea of neurofilament phosphorylation contributing to the dysfunction of neurons to the progression of the disease (Gray et al., 2012). Disease Region(s) studied Results Dutta et al., 2011 MS HP Hippocampal demyelination leads to synaptic alterations Reddy et al.,2005 AD Frontal and parietal cortices Sze et al., 2007 AD HP, entorhinal cortex, caudate nucleus, occipital cortex Kooi et al., 2011 MS, AD HP Perry et al., 1985 AD, PD All four cortical lobes MS HP, cingulate gyrus, thalamus, associative occipital cortex, cerebellum Possible relation between function of post- and presynaptic proteins to cognitive impairment in AD Synaptic abnormalities in HP correlate with the severity of memory deficits in AD patients MS cholinergic imbalance in HP can be used for future treatment options Degeneration of the cholinergic neurons may have a direct or indirect relation to cog.decline in PD Hypometabolism in thalamic and deep cortical gray structures is associated with episodic memory dysfunction in MS. Paulesu et al., 1996 Number and type of patients investigated Postmortem tissue of 22 MS patients and 9 CP Techniques used Involved pathways qRT-PCR Western blot Glutamate pathway Western blot Immunohistochemistry Synaptic plasticity Immunoblotting Synaptic plasticity Enzymes: choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), acetylcholinesterase (AChE) Immunohistochemistry Cholinergic pathway Enzymes: choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), acetylcholinesterase (AChE) Biochemical analysis Cholinergic pathway MRI (1.5T) Glucose metabolism Involved molecules or substances Axonal transport proteins: KIF1A, KIF3A, KIF15, KIF5B, KIF5C, KTN1 Synaptic vesicle proteins: ( pre-)synaptophysin, (pre-) synaptotagmin Synaptic CAM: neurexins, neuroligins Synaptic density proteins: PSD95, CASK Glutamate receptors: AMPA1, AMPA3, NMDAR2A, 2B, 2C, 2D, 3B subunits, MGLUR1, MGLUR2, MGLUR3, MGLUR4 Glutamate synthetase Glutamate transporters: EAAT1 EAAT2. Postmortem tissue of 18 AD patients and 18 CP Synaptic vesicle proteins: (pre-)synaptophysin Synaptic density proteins: PSD95, synaptopodin Postmortem tissue of 2 AD patients and 13 cognitively intact and possible AD patients Postmortem tissue from 15 MS patients, 10 AD patients and 10 CP Postmortem tissue from 8 AD patients, 14 PD patients and 8 CP Synaptic vesicle proteins: (pre-)synaptophysin 16 RRMS, 12SPMS patients, 13 and 10CP Glucose [ F]FDG PET 18 Pozzilli et al., 1992 MS Corpus callosum (CC) Mori et al., 2011 MS CSF Haase and Faustman, 2007 EAE, MS HypothalamusPituitary-Adrenal (HPA)-Axis Kuhle et al., 2011 MS CSF Gray et al., 2012 MS Cerebral cortex, white matter Left hemisphere hypometabolism indicates a higher involvemet in corpus callosum atrophy in MS Central inflammation in MS can alter amyloid-b metabolism leading to impairment of synaptic plasticity and cognitive function. Review on the current scientifically based experimental and clinical data of neuroimmunological influences on cognition and vice versa in MS. NfH levels can be used to measure the neurodegeneration rate in MS MS is associated with the widespread accumulation of hyperphosphorylated neurofilament protein SMI35 8 patients with CC atrophy, 8 patients without CC atrophy and 10CP Glucose PET Glucose metabolism 30 RRMS, 5 PPMS and 7 CIS patients amyloid-β1-42 t protein MRI TBS Amyloid-b downregulation τ-metabolism Mainly mouse and rat tissue Cytokines: INF-gama, TNF alpha, IL-1 alpha/beta and IL-6 Immunohistochemistry Cytockine network 63 CIS, 39 RRMS, 25 SPMS, 23 PPMS and 73 CP patients NfHSMI35 Electrochemiluminescence immunoassay Cytoskeleton regulators 17 SPMS, 2 PPMS, 2 PRMS, 2 RRMS, 2 of unknown MS course and 17 CP NfHSMI34 Immunohistochemistry Cytoskeleton regulators Table: Summary of all known molecules involved in cognitive impairment and possible pathways. Abbreviations: AD : Alzheimer’s patients, , CAM : cell adhesion molecules, CASK: calmodulin-associated serin/threonine kinase, CIS: clinically isolated syndrome, CP: control patients, [18F]FDG PET: 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography, HP: hippocampus, MRI: magnetic resonance imaging, MS: multiple sclerosis, Nf: neurofilament, PPMS: primary progressive multiple sclerosis, PRMS: progressive-relapsing multiple sclerosis, PSD95: post-synaptic density protein, RRMS : relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis, SPMS : secondary-progressive multiple sclerosis, TBS: θ burst stimulation Pathways involved in multiple sclerosis Synaptic connections between neurons represent the ‘wiring’ that exists in the brain circuitry. Connectivity between neurons is the basic element of brains function that mediates all known neurological functions, learning, memory, etc. Changes in synaptic activity transmission occur because of different forms of plasticity. In order to consolidate short-term memories into long-term memory, synaptic plasticity undergoes changes. LTP is a process that produces a long-lasting increase in synaptic strength, contrary to LTD that produces a long lasting decrease in synaptic strength (Kandel, 2001). Both patterns have different molecular and cellular mechanisms and the brain region that seems to be more important, the hippocampus, is importantly affected. Lesions there prevent the acquisition of new episodic memories. Additionally, synaptic plasticity is a prominent feature of hippocampal synapses. Knowing this background, the correlation between the necessity and sufficiency of synaptic plasticity and acquisition of new information has been validated (Neves et al., 2008). For this reason, and based on the studies presented in the previous sections, it is my belief that the pathways involving synaptic plasticity as well as the neurotransmitters involved should be better studied in multiple sclerosis. Glutamate is the most important neurotransmitter for normal brain function. Almost every excitatory neuron is glutamatergic and over half of all synapses release this neurotransmitter. This predominance has lead to so much attention as a prominent target to study many related neuropathological conditions like anxiety, epilepsy and neurodegenerative diseases. Many CNS diseases have a complex etiology and neuronal dysfunction is normally attributable to a combination of defects involving usually excitatory (or inhibitory) neurotransmission. The general model of signal transmission at a chemical synapse in the glutamate pathway includes AMPA, NMDA and kainite receptors. All three are glutamate gated cat ion channels that allow the inflow and outflow of Na+ and K+. For the same reason Ca2+ channels would be expected to be implicated in the MS pathology as they are a key element in the vesicle fusion. Key enzymes synthesize glutamate which is then transported in synaptic vesicles. When glutamate is released it activates glutamate responsive ion channels initiating downstream G protein signaling to propagate neurotransmission. An important feature of these receptors that are involved is the ability to traffic in and out of synapses. Synaptic plasticity events occur because of the addition or removal of receptors from synaptic membrane (Kandel, 2001). In the case of multiple sclerosis recent studies (Dutta et al., 2011) have provided us with evidence that demonstrate dysregulation in this pathway. Glutamate receptors mediate most of the excitatory synaptic transmission; therefore, reduced levels of mRNAs indicate an obvious dysfunction that could pose in a long-term scale, a therapeutic target in clinical interventions. So far, development of glutamate receptor subunit knockout mice has provided the link between specific types of memory that depend on different glutamate receptor subtypes (Bannerman, 2009). For example, GluA1 AMPAR subunitknockout mice, researchers detected memory mechanisms for short-term memory as well as for memory working tasks (Schmitt et al., 2003; Schmitt et al., 2005). Respectively, similar techniques have been employed to find possible existing links of NMDA receptors and different forms of memory. Their regulation and trafficking plays an important role in many forms of plasticity. The cholinergic system has also been linked to the MS pathology (Kooi et al.,2011). The hippocampus as mentioned before is studied in memory function (Squire et al., 2004) given its known importance in addition to its severely affected status in MS patients. Cholinergic neurons are involved in neuropsychic functions, like memory, learning, arousal, sleep and movement. Their loss was observed in AD (Perry et al., 1978; Whitehouse et al., 1982) as well as in other neurodegenerative diseases (Karson et al., 1996; Mallard et al., 1999). Hippocampus is the main region that receives cholinergic input from the basal forebrain (Mesulam, 2004) and in AD this malfunction has been recognized for some time now (Bartus et al., 1982; Davies & Maloney, 1976; Francis et al., 1999; Henke & Lang, 1983; Perry et al., 1978; Sims et al., 1983; Wilcock et al., 1982). According to Kooi (2011), the directly implicated enzymes in the cholinergic pathway present differences in MS and AD and although more evidence exist in AD, it is possible that the cholinergic neurotransmitter system in MS may show similar changes. Conclusion The current report presents the so far known evidence that have surfaced to answer at some level the possible underlying molecular mechanisms in the cognitive aspect of multiple sclerosis. The body of evidence that highlight the grey matter implication is growing with the help of the existing techniques. The cognitive symptoms and signs present the side of the disease that has been linked to grey matter brain areas in contrast to its first classification as a white matter disease. Unfortunately the mechanisms that create this inter individual variation in grey and white matter pathology haven’t been discovered. The answer to the MS etiopathogenesis would be a major step in towards its treatment. According to the evidence presented previously it is my belief that the pathways regulating synaptic plasticity as well as the neurotransmitters involved can be of essence in the ongoing study in the scientific community. Areas where atrophy is significant and demyelination affects cognitive behaviors have been studied although the accumulation of more information for a better understanding is essential. The neocortex, the hippocampus, the grey matter of the spinal cord are a few of the areas so far mentioned with more to be investigated in the future. Memory impairment is a feature commonly seen in MS patients whose majority has an affected hippocampus. The absence of many important enzymes, primarily observed in hippocampal areas, make the hippocampus and the areas connected to it, important regions for future investigation. The synthesis of neurotransmitters would be a nice additional pathway to be checked, as glutamate and acetylcholinesterase are important for synaptic plasticity. It is well known that there have been many advances in understanding multiple sclerosis, however that still hasn’t been enough. It is reasonable to say that the need for applying strategies for controlling certain aspects of the disease is among the most emergent goals. The need to understand the molecular mechanisms could encourage at a significant level the efforts to approach future treatments. References Achiron, a, & Barak, Y. (2003). Cognitive impairment in probable multiple sclerosis. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry, 74(4), 443–6. Ackerman, K.D., Martino M., Heyman R., et al. Stressor-induced alteration of cytokine production in multiple sclerosis patients and controls. Psychosom Med. 1998;4:484-491 Amato, M.P., Bartolozzi, M.L., Zipoli, V., et al. (2004). Neocortical volume decrease in relapsingremitting MS patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology; 63: 89–93. Amato, M.P., Portaccio, E., Goretti, B., et al. (2007). Association of neocortical volume changes with cognitive deterioration in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol; 64: 1157–61. Arnason, B. The role of cytokines in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1995;45(suppl 6):S54-S55. Banks, W.A., Farr, S.A, Morley, J.E. Entry of blood-borne cytokines into the central nervous system: effects on cognitive processes. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2002;10:319-327 Banks, W.A., Farr, S.A., La Scola M.E., Morley JE. Intravenous human interleukin-1 alpha impairs memory processing in mice: Dependence on blood-brain barrier transport into posterior division of the septum. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;299:536-541 . Bannerman, D.M. (2009). Fractionating spatial memory with glutamate receptor subunit-knockout mice. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 37, 1323–1327. Bartus, R.T., Dean, R.L., Beer, B., Lippa, A,S. (1982).The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. Science 217: 408–414. Batista, S., Zivadinov, R., Hoogs, M., Bergsland, N., Heininen-Brown, M., Dwyer, M. G., WeinstockGuttman, B., et al. (2012). Basal ganglia, thalamus and neocortical atrophy predicting slowed cognitive processing in multiple sclerosis. Journal of neurology, 259(1), 139–46. Bellmann-Strobl, J., Wuerfel, J., Aktas, O, Do¨rr J, Wernecke, K.D., Zipp, F. et al (2009). Poor PASAT performance correlates with MRI contrast enhancement in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 73: 1624– 1627. Benedict, R.H. , Weinstock-Guttman, B., Fishman, I., Sharma, J., Tjoa, C.W., Bakshi. R. (2004). Prediction of neuropsychological impairment in multiple sclerosis: comparison of conventional magnetic resonance imaging measures of atrophy and lesion burden. Arch Neurol; 61: 226–30. Benedict, R.H.B., Bruce, J.M., Dwyer, M.G., et al. (2006). Neocortical atrophy, third ventricular width, and cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol; 63: 1301–06. Benedict, R.H.B. et al (2009). Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: correlation with deep grey matter and mesial temporal atrophy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 80(2):201–206. Besedovsky HO, del Rey A. Introduction: immune-neuroendocrine network. Front Horm Res. 2002;29:1-14 Bliss, T.V., Collingridge, G.L. (1993). A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361: 31–39. (a) Bö, L., Vedeler, C.A., Nyland, H.I., Trapp, B.D., Mork, S.J. (2003). Subpial demyelination in the cerebral cortex of multiple sclerosis patients. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol; 62: 723–32. (b) Bö, L., Vedeler, C.A., Nyland, H., Trapp, B.D., Mork, S.J. (2003). Intracortical multiple sclerosis lesions are not associated with increased lymphocyte infi ltration. Mult Scler; 9: 323–31. Brex, P. a, Ciccarelli, O., O’Riordan, J. I., Sailer, M., Thompson, A. J., & Miller, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal study of abnormalities on MRI and disability from multiple sclerosis. The New England journal of medicine, 346(3), 158–64. Brownell, B., & Hughes, J. T. (1962). The distribution of plaques in the, 265. Butler, M.A., Corboy, J.R., Filley, C.M., (2009). How the conflict between American psychiatry and neurology delayed the appreciation of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychol Rev;19:399–410. Callanan, M.M., Logsdail, S.J., Ron, M.A., et al. (1989).Cognitive impairment in patients with clinically isolated lesions of the type seen in multiple sclerosis. A psychometric and MRI study. Brain;112(Pt 2):361–74. Cazullo, C.L., Trabattoni, D., Saresella, M., et al. Research on psychoimmunology. World J Biol Psych. 2003;4:119-123 Charcot, J.M. (1877). Lectures on diseases of the nervous system. London: New Sydenham Society. Charil, A., Dagher, A., Lerch, J.P., Zijdenbos, A,P,Worsley, K.J., Evans, A.C. (2007). Focal cortical atrophy in multiple sclerosis: relation to lesion load and disability. Neuroimage 34:509–17. Cristodoulou, L., Krupp, W., Huang, D., et al. (2001). Cognitive correlates of quantitative MRI and MR spectroscopy in multiple sclerosis. Neurology; (Suppl 3): A191. Clark, C.M., James, G., Li, D., et al. (1992). Ventricular size, cognitive function and depression in patients with multiple sclerosis. Can J Neurol Sci. 1992; 19: 352-356. Comi, G., Filippi, M., Martinelli, V., et al. (1993). Brain magnetic resonance imaging correlates of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Sci.; 115: 566-573. Compston, A., Coles, A. (2008). Multiple sclerosis. Lancet 372 (9648): 1502–17. Course, C. (2000). Clinical course and diagnosis. Davies, P., Maloney, A.J. (1976). Selective loss of central cholinergic neurons in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 2:1403. Das, P., Lilly, S.M., Zerda, R., Gunning, 3rd, W.T., Alvarez, F.J. and Tietz, E.I. (2008). Increased AMPA receptor GluR1 subunit incorporation in rat hippocampal CA1 synapses during benzodiazepine withdrawal. J. Comp.nNeurol. 511, 832–846. Dawson, J.W., (1916). The histology of multiple sclerosis. Trans R Soc (Edinb); 50: 517–740. Drachman, D.A., Leavitt, J. (1974). Human memory and the cholinergic system. A relationship to aging? Arch Neurol 30:113–121. Depino, A.M., Alonso, M., Ferrari, C., et al.(2004). Learning modulation by endogenous hippocampal IL1: blockade of endogenous IL-1 facilitates memory formation. Hippocampus.;14:526-35. de Waegh, S.M., Lee, V.M., Brady, S.T.(1992). Local modulation of neurofilament phosphorylation, axonal caliber, and slow axonal transport by myelinating Schwann cells. Cell; 68:451– 463. Dutta, R., Chang, A., Doud, M. K., Kidd, G. J., Ribaudo, M. V, Young, E. a, Fox, R. J., et al. (2011). Demyelination causes synaptic alterations in hippocampi from multiple sclerosis patients. Annals of neurology, 69(3), 445–54. Faustmann, P.M., Haase, C.G., Romnerg, S., et al.(2003). Microglia activation influences dye coupling and Cx43 expression of the astrocytic network. Glia;42:101-108. Feuillet, L., Reuter, F., Audoin, B., Malikova, I., Barrau, K., Cherif, a a., & Pelletier, J. (2007). Early cognitive impairment in patients with clinically isolated syndrome suggestive of multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 13(1), 124–127. (a)Feinstein, A., Youl, B., Ron, M., (1992). Acute optic neuritis: a cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain;115(5):1403–15. (b)Feinstein A., Kartsounis L.D., Miller D.H., et al., (1992). Clinically isolated lesions of the type seen in multiple sclerosis: a cognitive, psychiatric, and MRI follow-up study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry;55(10):869–76. Feinstein, A., Roy, P., Lobaugh, N., et al. (2004). Structural brain abnormalities in multiple sclerosis patients with major depression. Neurology; 62: 586–90. Feldman, D.E. (2009). Synaptic mechanisms for plasticity in neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 32: 33–55. Francis, P.T., Palmer, A.M., Snape, M., Wilcock, G.K. (1999). The cholinergic hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: a review of progress. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 66:137–147. Geurts J.J., Bo L., Roosendaal S.D., et al.(2007). Extensive hippocampal demyelination in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol; 66: 819–27. Gilmore,C.P., Christopher, P., Bö, L., Owens, T., Lowe, J., & Esiri, M. M. (2006). Spinal Cord Gray Matter Demyelination in Multiple Sclerosis — A Novel Pattern. Gilmore, C. P., Donaldson, I., Bö, L., Owens, T., Lowe, J., & Evangelou, N. (2009). Regional variations in the extent and pattern of grey matter demyelination in multiple sclerosis: a comparison between the cerebral cortex, cerebellar cortex, deep grey matter nuclei and the spinal cord. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry, 80(2), 182–7. Gray, E., Rice, C., Nightingale, H., Ginty, M., Hares, K., Kemp, K., Cohen, N., et al. (2012). Accumulation of cortical hyperphosphorylated neurofilaments as a marker of neurodegeneration in multiple sclerosis. Multiple sclerosis (Houndmills, Basingstoke, England). Haase, C.G., Faustmann, P.M.(2004). Benign multiple sclerosis is characterized by a stable neuroimmunological network. NeuroImmunoModulation.;11:273-277. Haddad, J.J., Saade, N.E., Safieh-Garabedian, B.(2002). Cytokines and neuro-immune-endocrine interactions: a role for the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal revolving axis. J Neuroimmunol.; 133:119. Hauser, S. L., & Oksenberg, J. R. (2006). The neurobiology of multiple sclerosis: genes, inflammation, and neurodegeneration. Neuron, 52(1), 61–76. Henke, H., Lang, W. (1983). Cholinergic enzymes in neocortex, hippocampus and basal forebrain of nonneurological and senile dementia of Alzheimer-type patients. Brain Res 267:281–291 Houtchens, M.K. et al (2007). Thalamic atrophy and cognition in multiple sclerosis. Neurol 69(12):1213– 1223. Huber, S.J., Paulson, G.W., Shuttleworth, E.C., et al. (1987). Magnetic resonance imaging correlates of dementia in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol.; 44: 732-736. Huitinga, I., De Groot, C.J., van der Valk P., et al.,(2001). Hypothalamic lesions in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol ; 60: 1208–18. Hulst, H. E., & Geurts, J. J. G. (2011). Gray matter imaging in multiple sclerosis: what have we learned? BMC neurology, 11(1), 153. Geurts, J. J. G., & Barkhof, F. (2008). Grey matter pathology in multiple sclerosis. Lancet neurology, 7(9), 841–51. Ghiso, J., Frangione, B. (2002). Amyloidosis and Alzheimer's disease. Adv. Drug Deliv. Rev. 54 (12): 1539–51. Kandel, E.R.(2001). The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses.. Science. Nov 2;294(5544):1030-8. Karson, C.N., Mrak, R.E, Husain, M.M, Griffin, W.S.T, (1996). Decreased mesopontine choline acetyltransferase levels in schizophrenia. Mol.Chem. Neuropathol.; 29: 181-191. Kessels, H.W., Malinow, R. (2009). Synaptic AMPA receptor plasticity and behavior. Neuron 61: 340– 350. Klyubin, I., Walsh, D.M., Lemere, C.A., Cullen, W.K., Shankar, G.M., Betts, V, et al (2005). Amyloid beta protein immunotherapy neutralizes Abeta oligomers that disrupt synaptic plasticity in vivo. Nat Med 11: 556–561. Köller, H., Siebler, M., Hartung, H.P. (1997). Immunologically induced electrophysiological dysfunctions: implications for inflammatory diseases of the CNS and PNS. Prog Neurobiol.;52:1-26. Kühlwein, E., Irwin, M.(2001). Melatonine modulation of lymphocyte proliferation and Th1/Th2 cytokine expression. J Neuroimmunol.;117:51-57. Kutzelnigg, A., Faber-Rod, J. C., Bauer, J., Lucchinetti, C. F., Sorensen, P. S., Laursen, H., Stadelmann, C., et al. (2007). Widespread demyelination in the cerebellar cortex in multiple sclerosis. Brain pathology (Zurich, Switzerland), 17(1), 38–44. Kutzelnigg, A., Lucchinetti, C. F., Stadelmann, C., Brück, W., Rauschka, H., Bergmann, M., Schmidbauer, M., et al. (2005). Cortical demyelination and diffuse white matter injury in multiple sclerosis. Brain : a journal of neurology, 128(Pt 11), 2705–12. LaRocca, N.G. (2000). Cognitive and emotional disorders. In Burks JS, Johnson KP (eds.): Multiple Sclerosis: Diagnosis, Medical Management, and Rehabilitation. New York: Demos: 405-421. Larson, S.J., Dunn, A.J.(2001). Behavioral effects of cytokines. Brain Behav Immun.;15(4):371-87. Lazeron, R.H., Langdon, D.W., Filippi, M., et al.(2000). Neuropsychological impairment in multiple sclerosis patients: the role of (juxta)cortical lesion on FLAIR. Mult Scler; 6: 280–85. Lublin, F.D., Reingold, S.C. (1996). Defining the clinical course of multiple sclerosis: results of an international survey. National Multiple Sclerosis Society (USA) Advisory Committee on Clinical Trials of New Agents in Multiple Sclerosis. Neurology 46 (4): 907–11. Malenka, R.C. (2003). The long-term potential of LTP. Nat Rev Neurosci 4: 923–926. Mallard, C., Tolcos, M., Leditschke, J., Campbell P., Rees S.,(1999).Reduction in choline acetyltransferase immunoreactivity but not muscarinic-m2 receptor immunoreactivity in the brainstem of SIDS infants. J. Neuropathol. Exp.Neurol.; 58:255-264. McIntosh-Michaelis, S.A., Roberts, M.H., Wilkinson, S.M., et al. (1991): The prevalence of cognitive impairment in a community survey of multiple sclerosis. Br J Med Psych.; 333-348. Mesulam, M.M. (2004). The cholinergic innervation of the human cerebral cortex. Prog Brain Res 145:67–78. Minichiello, L. (2009). TrkB signalling pathways in LTP and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 850–860. Mori, F., Rossi, S., Sancesario, G., Codecà, C., Mataluni, G., Monteleone, F., Buttari, F., et al. (2011). Cognitive and cortical plasticity deficits correlate with altered amyloid-β CSF levels in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(3), 559–68. Moriarty, D.M., Blackshaw, A.J., Talbot, P.R., et al.(1999). Memory dysfunction in multiple sclerosis corresponds to juxtacortical lesion load on fast fluid-attenuated inversion-recovery MR images. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol; 20: 1956–62. Morrissey, S.P., Miller, D.H., Kendall, B.E., et al., (1993). The significance of brain magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities at presentation with clinically isolated syndromes suggestive of multiple sclerosis. A 5-year follow-up study. Brain;116(Pt 1):135–46. Morrow,S.A., Drake,A., Zinadinov, R., et al. (2010). Predicting loss of employment over three years in multiple sclerosis: clinically meaningful cognitive decline. Clin Neuropsychol; 24:1131-1145. Neves, G., Cooke, S. F., & Bliss, T. V. P. (2008). Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causality. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 9(1), 65–75. Newman, J.P.(2001). Multiple sclerosis (correspondence). NEJM.; 1: 381-382. Noseworthy, J. H. (1999). Progress in determining the causes and treatment of multiple sclerosis. Nature, 399(6738 Suppl), A40–7. Noseworthy, J.H., Lucchinetti, C., Rodriguez, M., Weinshenker, B.G., (2000). Multiple sclerosis. N. Engl. J. Med. 343, 938–952. Oddo, S., Caccamo, A., Shepherd, J.D., Murphy, M.P., Golde, T.E., Kayed, R. et al (2003). Tripletransgenic model of Alzheimer’s disease with plaques and tangles: intracellular Abeta and synaptic dysfunction. Neuron 39: 409–421. Paulesu, E., Perani, D., Fazio, F., Comi, G., Pozzilli, C., Martinelli, V., Filippi, M., et al. (1996). Functional basis of memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: a[18F]FDG PET study. NeuroImage, 4(2), 87–96. Perry, E. K., Curtis, M., Dick, D. J., Candy, J. M., Atack, J. R., Bloxham, C. a, Blessed, G., et al. (1985). Cholinergic correlates of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: comparisons with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 48(5), 413–421. Petzold, A.(2005). Neurofilament phosphoforms: surrogate markers for axonal injury, degeneration and loss. J Neurol Sci;233:183–198. Perry, E. K., Tomlinson, B. E., Blessed, G., Bergmann, K., Gibson, P. H., & Perry, R. H. (1978). Correlation of cholinergic abnormalities with senile plaques and mental test scores in senile dementia. British medical journal, 2(6150), 1457–9. Polydoro, M., Acker, C.M., Duff, K., Castillo, P.E., Davies, P. (2009). Age dependent impairment of cognitive and synaptic function in the htau mouse model of tau pathology. J Neurosci 29: 10741– 10749. Pozzilli, C., Passafiume, D., Bernardi, S., Pantano, P., Incoccia, C., Bastianello, S., Bozzao, L., et al. (1991). SPECT, MRI and cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry, 54(2), 110–5. Pugh, C.R., Nguyen, K.T., Gonyea, J.L., et al.(1999). Role of interleukin-1 beta in impairment of contextual fear conditioning caused by social isolation. Behav Brain Res.;106:109-18. Rao, S.M., Leo, G.J., Bernardin, L., Unverzagt, F., (1991). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurology; 41: 685–91. Rao, S.M.,(1995). Neuropsychology of multiple sclerosis. Curr Opin Neurol; 8: 216–20. Roosendaal, S.D., Moraal, B., Vrenken, H., et al. (2008). In vivo MR imaging of hippocampal lesions in multiple sclerosis. J Magn Reson Imaging; 27:726–731. Rosenmann, H., Grigoriadis, N., Eldar-Levy, H., Avital, A., Rozenstein, L., Touloumi, O. et al (2008). A novel transgenic mouse expressing double mutant tau driven by its natural promoter exhibits tauopathy characteristics. Exp Neurol 212: 71–84. Rovaris, M., Filippi, M., Minicucci, L., et al. (2000).Cortical/subcortical disease burden and cognitive impairment in patients with multiple sclerosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol; 21: 402–08. Ryan, L., Clark, C.M., Klonoff, H., et al. (1996). Patterns of cognitive impairment in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis and their relationship to neuropathology on magnetic resonance images. Neuropsychology ; 10: 176-193. Schmitt, W.B., Deacon, R.M., Seeburg, P.H., Rawlins, J.N. and Bannerman, D.M. (2003). A withinsubjects, within-task demonstration of intact spatial reference memory and impaired spatial working memory in glutamate receptor-A-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 23, 3953–3959. Schmitt, W.B., Sprengel, R., Mack, V., Draft, R.W., Seeburg, P.H., Deacon, R.M., Rawlins, J.N. and Bannerman, D.M. (2005). Restoration of spatial working memory by genetic rescue of GluR-Adeficient mice. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 270–272. Schmitt, U., Tanimoto, N., Seeliger, M., Schaeffel, F., Leube, R.E. (2009). Detection of behavioral alterations and learning deficits in mice lacking synaptophysin. Neuroscience 162 (2): 234–43. Schneider, H., Pitossi, F., Balschun, D., et al.(1998). A neuromodulatory role of interleukin-1beta in the hippocampus. Proc Nat Acad Sci.; 95(13):7778-83. Selkoe, D.J. (2001). Clearing the brain's amyloid cobwebs. Neuron 32 (2): 177–80. Shankar, G.M., Li, S., Mehta, T.H., Garcia-Munoz, A., Shepardson, N.E., Smith, I. et al (2008). Amyloidbeta protein dimers isolated directly from Alzheimer’s brains impair synaptic plasticity and memory. Nat Med 14: 837–842. Sims, N.R., Bowen, D.M., Allen, S.J. et al (1983). Presynaptic cholinergic dysfunction in patients with dementia. J Neurochem 40:503–509. Sokic, D.V., Stojsavljevic, N., Drulovic, J., et al.(2001). Seizures in multiple sclerosis. Epilepsia; 42: 72– 79. Spatt, J., Chaix, R., Mamoli, B., (2001).Epileptic and non-epileptic seizures in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol; 248: 2–9. Sperling, R.A., Guttmann, C.R., Hohol, M.J., et al.(2001). Regional magnetic resonance imaging lesion burden and cognitive function in multiple sclerosis: A longitudinal study. Arch Neurol.; 58: 115-121. Squire, L.R., Stark, C.E., Clark, R.E. (2004), The medial temporal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:279–306. Sudhof, T.C.(2008). Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature; 455:903–911. Swirsky-Sacchetti, T., Mitchell, D.R., Seward, J., et al.(1992). Neuropsychological and structural brain lesions in multiple sclerosis: a regional analysis. Neurology.; 42: 1291-1295. Sze, C.I., Troncoso, J.C., Kawas, C., Mouton, P., Price, D.L., Martin, L.J. (1997). Loss of presynaptic vesicle protein synaptophysin in hippocampus correlates with cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease. J of Neuropathol and Experim Neurology;56(8): 933-944. Tintore, M., Rovira, A., Rı´o, J., et al., (2006). Baseline MRI predicts future attacks and disability in clinically isolated syndromes. Neurology; 67(6):968–72. Townsend, M, Qu Y., Gray, A., Wu, Z., Seto, T., Hutton, M. et al (2010). Oral treatment with a {gamma}-secretase inhibitor improves long-term potentiation in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 333: 110–119. Trapp, B. D., & Nave, K.-A. (2008). Multiple sclerosis: an immune or neurodegenerative disorder? Annual review of neuroscience, 31, 247–69. Tsolaki, M., Drevelegas, A., Karachristianou, S., et al.(1994). Correlation of neuropsychological and MRI findings in multiple sclerosis. Dementia. 1994; 5: 48-52. dementia, Vercellino, M., Plano, F., Votta, B., et al., (2005). Grey matter pathology in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol ; 64: 1101–07. Walther, T., Albrecht, D., Becker, M., Schubert, M., Kouznetsova, E., Wiesner, B. et al (2009). Improved learning and memory in aged mice deficient in amyloid beta-degrading neutral endopeptidase. PLoS One 4: e4590. Weinshenker, B.G., Bass, B., Rice, G.P., Noseworthy, J., Carriere, W., Baskerville, J., Ebers, G.C., (1989). The natural history of multiple sclerosis: a geographically based study. I. Clinical course and disability. Brain 112, 133–146. Weinshenker, B.G., (1998). Natural history of multiple sclerosis. Ann. Neurol. 36, S6–S11. Whitehouse, P.J, Price, D.L., Strube, R.G., et al. (1982). Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia: loss of neurons in the basal forebrain. Science; 215: 1237-1339. Wilcock, G.K., Esiri, M.M., Bowen, D.M., Smith, C.C. (1982). Alzheimer’s disease. Correlation of cortical choline acetyltransferase activity with the severity of dementia and histological abnormalities. J Neurol Sci 57:407–417.