Experimental techniques Beckman: apparent time analysis across various studies Beddor: ultrasound Beckman & paidologos project The corpus is a database of speech productions recorded in the same way from native speakers for the target language variety in day care centers, nursery schools, or participant homes in a half dozen different locations for as many different languages. The portions of the corpus that are used in the current paper are the Mandarin Chinese recordings made in 2006 in Sōngyuán City, PRC, by the second author, and the Korean recordings made in 2007 in Seoul, South Korea, by the third author. Conditions that give rise to sound change: intrinsic to the signal Many papers are concerned with the conditions under which phonologisation takes place. Kirby: tonogenesis in Phnom Penh Khmer colloquial speech of the capital Phnom Penh /kru/ produced with post-aspiration or breathy voice or falling pitch ... the loss of the trill has been compensated for by the enhancement or emergence of other acoustic cues Cues Beckman: a shift in the phonetic basis of the Korean contrast between lax and aspirated stops from being primarily a contrast in degree of aspiration in phrase-initial position to becoming primarily a contrast in the tone pattern. the size of the difference in VOT values reported in subsequent studies has diminished decade by decade, and both Silva (2006) and Kang and Guion (2008) report apparent-time evidence (i.e., systematic variation across talkers of different ages recorded in the same year) that strongly suggests a change in the contrast from being primarily a difference in degree of aspiration to becoming a difference in voice quality and pitch level on the vowel of the initial syllable of the Accentual Phrase. a gradual shift in the phonetic realization of Korean stops in phrase-initial position whereby the originally moderately long VOT values for the “mildly aspirated” lax stops have lengthened a great deal and the originally very long VOT values in the aspirated stops have shortened so as to considerably reduce the VOT difference. but it is not resulting in a near merger. Instead, other correlates are taking over the role of degree of aspiration in cuing the contrast. In general, Kirby: The implementation of the phonetic precursor discussed here is closely related to the proposal of Beddor (2009), who shows how a gesture initially associated with one segment can come to be interpreted distinctively with a different segment. The implementation of the phonetic precursor discussed here is closely related to the proposal of Beddor (2009), who shows how a gesture initially associated with one segment can come to be interpreted distinctively with a different segment. Beddor’s study focuses on the emergence of contrastive nasalization, whereby the acoustic effects of velum lowering initially associated with a nasal come to be associated with a following vowel, but it is conceptually very similar to the temporal exchange between duration of /r/ and onset VOT hypothesized here. Hypoarticulation Beddor: its starting point and asks whether reduction is especially likely to occur in highfrequency words The theoretical motivation for this extension was to investigate the phonetic underpinnings of sound changes leading to post-vocalic /l/ vocalization or loss. We predicted that a post-lateral consonantal context that does not require an alveolar constriction might contribute to alveolar reduction. Moreover, we predicted that the highly practiced articulatory routines (and, possibly, faster lexical access) for high-frequency words with final /lClabial/ or /lCvelar/ would further contribute to alveolar reduction. Both predictions were upheld. New to this study is the finding that, the more frequent the word ending in / clusters, the greater the aperture in the alveolar region for lateral productions (Figure 5). This lexical frequency effect, while significant, was small. Nonetheless, the frequency effect is not simply one of overall smaller magnitude of lingual constrictions in highfrequency words, as shown by the finding that, for a given high-frequency word (in relation to its low-frequency counterpart), tongue tip and tongue dorsum apertures were not correlated. For these data, then, reduction of the tongue tip constriction is not tied to reduction of the tongue dorsum constriction. Non-linear relationships between acoustics and production Beddor: For reduction of the lateral alveolar constriction in labial and velar contexts to serve as the impetus for sound change, it follows that the reduced gesture must have acoustic consequences...The crucial point here is that, in certain vowel contexts, small changes in F2-F1 proximity in /Vl/ sequences due to reduced tongue tip constriction may result in disproportionately large perceptual shifts. Solé: outlines three types of ambiguity; and it is the third kind Role of statistical frequency/hypoarticulation in sound change Beddor: practiced, efficient articulatory routines for high-frequency words (Bybee 2002) it is reasonable to speculate that, should pronunciation norms begin to shift and alveolar reduction for /l/ become exaggerated, high-frequency words would likely be at the forefront of that shift. Conditions that give rise to sound change: speakers children Beckman: Indeed, already by the late 1970s, Vihman (1980) could conclude that any account of the role of children in phonological change that is predicated on positing such axiomatic homologies between children’s characteristic errors and common sound changes is untenable. (See Foulkes and Vihman 2014 for an updated presentation of more data from Vihman’s own work that supports her original arguments, and Edwards, Beckman, and Munson 2012 for a review of some of the large body of subsequent work that also argues against invoking substantive universals to explain phonological development.) n predicting that the distribution of phonetic values in young children’s productions will match the somewhat more conservative patterns of young men [in Neogrammarian change] rather than the more advanced patterns of young women, and that listeners will expect this we predict that the developmental pattern for children in Songyuan will differ. Specifically, because of the break in transmission, the distribution of phonetic values in Songyuan children will match that of the more advanced talkers in the more abrupt change from above there. On Songyan: how the transmission of this abrupt “change from above” is reflected in the phonological development of children whose grandparents are in the generation before the change (same age as the older speakers in Li 2005) but whose parents are in the generation after the change (same age as the younger speakers in Li 2005). hus, the evidence from the corpus suggests that the system being transmitted to the Songyuan children is the new imported standard system with the robust contrast between retroflex and dental fricatives, and not the older Songyuan system with only the single apical sound in contrast with the alveolopalatal or some “mixed” system that is intermediate between the two-sibilant system and the robust three-sibilant system. The Songyuan children show a developmental progression toward accurate retroflex and dental fricatives that is similar to the progression documented for children in Beijing women Babel: males did not imitate to the extent that females did, although differences between men and women were moderated by voice type so both accommodated to the same degree to the least typical voices; Females accommodated to the most attractive voices more than to the less attractive voices, providing support for the social preferences hypothesis. Beckman: a female-led sound change that was well underway by the 1970s, and is now close to completion in the Seoul dialect. those who have exposure to multiple dialects Clopper cognitive representation of dialect; The preceding discussion of processing benefits associated with local and standard varieties suggests that listeners who have substantial exposure to multiple different dialects as a result of self-reported geographic mobility and/or exposure to a non-local standard through education or the media, will exhibit a processing benefit for all of the varieties with which they are familiar the processing benefits associated with the standard may contribute to dialect leveling and convergence towards the standard The cross-dialect processing benefits observed for participants with exposure to multiple varieties reflect this greater flexibility in perceptual mapping, because this flexibility provides access to both local and standard forms in speech processing tasks. (and inherent ambiguities between speakers because they process in different ways - bring in research by Jannedy here). even brief exposure to previously unfamiliar talkers within an experimental setting can provide listeners with sufficient familiarity with those talkers to facilitate processing. In particular, individuals with exposure to relatively little sociolinguistic variation should have relatively less variable distributions of phonological and/or lexical exemplars, whereas individuals with exposure to more sociolinguistic variation should have more variable distributions. group affiliation Clopper: the locus of sound change in individuals Milroy and Milroy (1993) argued that individuals with weak social networks are more likely to be linguistic innovators than individuals with strong networks, because individuals with weak social group connections are more susceptible to outside influences than individuals in close-knit groups. Jannedy: Many recent experimental results on the categorization of speech in different language and cultural backgrounds suggest that listeners do not categorize speech in a context-free way but by attributing speaker characteristics which may be manipulated through explicit and even very subtle implicit information on the origin of the stimulus (Strand & Johnson 1996; Niedzielski 1999; Hay et al. 2006; Hay & Drager 2010; Brunelle & Jannedy 2007, 2013). young speakers from multiethnic neighborhoods are using this alternation as a social marker implicitly coded social information has an effect on the categorization of acoustic stimuli The third contributing factor may be the youth-style Hood German that can be frequently heard in public spaces like the public transportation, shopping malls or on the streets. It has become a widely distributed youth speech style throughout Berlin, although it was formerly strongly associated with Kreuzberg You process speakers differently depending on who you are listening to Beckman: we predict that in perception experiments, listeners will attend differently to the VOT and the F0 pattern depending on the age of the talker and also (if the talker is an adult) whether the talker is a man or a woman. Beckman: listeners did attend to the VOT when identifying men’s or children’s stops as lax versus aspirated in a way that they did not when listening to women. Jannedy: Thus, the aim of this study is to evaluate the effect of implicitly coded social information (relating to neighborhood in our case) on the categorization of acoustic stimuli with synthesized fricatives ranging from /ç/ to /ʃ / how sound change spreads imitation (i.e. link between conditions that give rise to sound change and its spread) related to e.g. recent studies by Pardo in a study on male roommates, Pardo and colleagues found a correlation between phonetic convergence and roommates’ self-reported closeness (Pardo, Gibbons, Suppes, & Krauss, 2012) and on earlier studies showing that imitation is strongly moderated by social preference in adults Jannedy: The resistance may be due to lack of contact with the primary speakers cognitive novelty drives an imitative response in phonetic accommodation. listeners-turned-speakers modify their speech in response to the characteristics of the voices they are hearing The Least Typical Male voice elicited the most imitative behavior (Figure 9); this was true for shadowers of both genders. Male shadowers only accommodated to the least typical voices. it seems as though the behavior of the male shadowers provides support for the hypothesis that cognitive novelty drives an imitative response in phonetic accommodation. Regular sound change vs. borrowing beckman: regular sound change, which is assumed to be incremental and so should show continuity between phonological development and the age-related variation observed in the speech community undergoing the change. The second type is dialect borrowing, which could show an abrupt discontinuity between developmental patterns before and after the sociohistorical circumstances that instigate it. the Seoul Korean contrast between lax and aspirated stops which is undergoing regular sound change, and the standard Mandarin contrast between retroflex and dental sibilants which has been borrowed recently into the Songyuan dialect. lexical frequency and spread Word frequency effects on leniting sound changes are characteristically viewed as contributing to the propagation of these changes through the lexicon. Bybee (2000, 2002), Phillips (1984, 2001), and Pierrehumbert (2001), for example, all cite the tendency of processes of lenition (assimilations, reductions, deletions) to affect more frequent words before less frequent words. We apply a similar lexical diffusion interpretation to the finding that, in the lateral productions of the speakers in this study, high-frequency words exhibited weaker alveolar constrictions than did phonological change by borrowing always begins with a replacement of one word form by another ; regular sound change by incrementation regular sound change, which is assumed to be incremental and so should show continuity between phonological development and the age-related variation observed in the speech community undergoing the change. The second type is dialect borrowing, which could show an abrupt discontinuity between developmental patterns before and after the socio-historical circumstances that instigate it. change through borrowing (“change from above”) is typically ascribed to a specific historical event that brings adult speakers of two divergent systems into contact. In one community (Seoul, South Korea), there is an on-going shift in the pronunciation of two series of voiceless stops that began more than fifty years ago. This shift is a “change from below” In the other community (Sōngyuán City, PRC), there is a change to the pronunciation of sibilant consonants that has split the apical series in two, resulting in a contrast between retroflex and dental word classes, as in the Běijīng dialect that is the basis for the standard language. This split is clearly a “change from above” – i.e., a pattern of pronunciations from a different dialect of Mandarin Chinese that was borrowed into the Songyuan variety within the last thirty years. computational models of spread - of how bias can spread across populations Given a reasonably accurate characterization of a synchronic start state and a hypothesized phonetic bias factor, this type of model enables predictions about directionality in phonologization from a point in time when the ‘seeds’ of the change still lie below the threshold of perceptibility. The model sketched above employs a relatively simple characterization of the relationship between teacher and learner, whereby the learner receives independent and identical distributed random samples from the teacher and uses these to compute maximum likelihood estimates by making strong assumptions about the form of the distributions. The basic premise of the model of category acquisition outlined here is that learners attempt to parse this potentially noisy speech input in an optimal fashion. Here, I extend that framework in order to consider an additional source of potential discontinuity between teacher and learner: changes in attention to cue This suggests that changes in attention to the number of cue dimensions relevant to a contrast could potentially condition abrupt changes in classification behavior across generations of learners. JMH: So sound change is a combination of optimising the strategy for categorisation and introducing a bias factor In the simulations described above, it was assumed that the devoiced/fricated proportion ρ of /r/ is misparsed by learners as increased VOT. That is, the assumption is that learners fail to compensate for what is in some sense an effect of coarticulation. In this set of simulations, the duration of 20% of all tokens of /r/ were reduced by a random proportion ρ as in Series II, but now this was not added to the VOT of those same training examples. JMH: the technique is to develop a model that unifies both stability and change sigmoidal change across generations Kirby, Beckman Beckman: The model also predicts that the generation-by-generation incrementation will have a logistic shape as the sound change progresses from its beginning to its end. Kirby: Several recent computational treatments of language change argue that the sigmoidal or discontinuous nature of change can only be modeled by characterizing the behavior of populations, noting that models where individuals receive input from a single teacher (e.g., S. Kirby et. al 2007) have a linear dynamics that converge to a single stable state from all initial conditions. Models of sound change Beyond Ohala Kirby: This prediction runs counter to the well-established sigmoidal progression often observed in sound changes involving consonantal contrasts, whereby changes between generations are often discontinuous and abrupt . This prediction runs counter to the well-established sigmoidal progression often observed in sound changes involving consonantal contrasts, whereby changes between generations are often discontinuous and abrupt Exemplar models Jannedy: Pierrehumbert’s (2001) results on modeling category neutralization. In her computational exemplar-based model of mergers, Pierrehumbert (2001) shows that marked and less frequent categories such as /ç/ are subject of a persistent bias towards a more frequent and unmarked category such as /ʃ /. Kirby: parametric vs non-parametric see Vallabha et al. (2007) As the notion of category is ill-defined in exemplar-based approaches – the categories are, in a very real sense, defined by the experienced tokens themselves – it is not clear how variability in the signal should become information for the listener in such a model. Kirby: differences with exemplar models: supervised vs. unsupervised learning As the notion of category is ill-defined in exemplar-based approaches – the categories are, in a very real sense, defined by the experienced tokens themselves – it is not clear how variability in the signal should become information for the listener in such a model. Another approach to phonological category learning and change from a non-exemplar perspec- tive is Boersma and Hamann (2008), who consider the evolution of phonological categories in a constraint-based framework. In order to avoid computing auditory distances, their model dis- cretizes the continuous auditory cue space into an arbitrary number of cue constraints, which are then ranked using a standard constraint-demotion algorithm. However, this approach requires that auditory constraints be ranked relative to a universal ‘ease of articulation’ curve, a concept that has proven particularly difficult to rigorously quantify (Pouplier 2003). In addition, since the authors’ primary focus was on accounting for auditory dispersion, they do not explore the question of how multiple phonetic cues would interact in their model. In spite of the difference between these models, superficial and otherwise, it is important to point out that the basic predictions of all the models mentioned above are consistent with the idea that changes to phonological systems may be emergent, rather than explicitly goaloriented. Clopper: more familiar exemplars are more strongly represented in the exemplar space than less frequently encountered exemplars and are therefore easier to process. In addition, recently encountered exemplars are more strongly represented in the exemplar space than exemplars with a longer time lag, which can account for the rapid adaptation to variation that is observed in the laboratory. aken together, these findings are consistent with a model in which exposure to multiple different varieties leads to more variable representations. These variable representations are defined by distributions with greater variance or bandwidth than the distributions of less variable input, which provides a larger space of potential mappings between the signal and linguistic categories. The cross-dialect processing benefits observed for participants with exposure to multiple varieties reflect this greater flexibility in perceptual mapping, because this flexibility provides access to both local and standard forms in speech processing tasks. That is, a given stimulus can be mapped onto more than one linguistic representation, which increases the likelihood that the stimulus will be accurately identified. However, this one-tomany mapping can also lead to processing costs due to competition between possible linguistic categories. In an exemplar model, the competing linguistic representations would be realized with greater variance and potentially bimodal distributions of exemplars. Crucially, an exemplar-based perspective allows us to account for both maintenance of sociolinguistic variation through the entrenchment of local forms and for dialect leveling through change towards the standard. In particular, maintenance can be achieved when the exemplars associated with the local variety are more frequent and/or more heavily weighted in processing than the standard variety. Other modelling points Kirby: A further assumption made in the present work is that the learner knows the number of category labels in advance. This is a common assumption made in work applying mixture models to the task of phonetic category learning (de Boer and Kuhl 2003; Lin 2005), in part because of the difficulty involved with jointly inferring both the number of mixture components and the parameters of those components. In their computational work on English and Japanese vowel category learn- ing, Vallabha et al. (2007) compared parametric and nonparametric versions of an online mixture estimation algorithm. They found the parametric algorithm significantly outperformed the nonparametric one, which they attributed to the nonparametric estimator’s inherent lack of constraints on the underlying category structure. Topics for Future research Beckman: However, there are substantial gaps in our understanding both of the extent of cross-cultural variability in language socialization and of how this might affect the mechanisms of phonological change that must be addressed before we can fully understand the relationship between the time courses of the two. we need better models of the social forces driving both phonological development in an individual learner and phonological change in a speech community before we can begin to understand the relationship between the two. Beckman: However, we need considerably more detailed ethnographic information on language socialization patterns in early childhood in the two speech communities before we can be confident of our interpretations of these two cases. And we also need more case studies of developmental patterns in other communities where there are changes affecting consonants contrasts like the ones studied here before we can extend the interpretations to be a more confident general hypothesis about differing timelines for regular sound change versus borrowing.