Acoustic analysis of intentional vocalizations by autistic and non-autistic monozygotic twins. Claire Scoular Supervisor: Jonathan Delafield-Butt MSc Psychological Research Methods The University of Edinburgh 2008 1 Statement This statement is to confirm that all the work presented in this document is my own work. Claire Scoular 2 Contents Abstract............................................................................... 4 Introduction......................................................................... 5 Methods and Materials.............................................................................. 15 Results..................................................................................19 Discussion............................................................................ 31 Appendices........................................................................... 44 3 Abstract This study focuses on early development communication to help determine autism at an earlier stage. Infant cries have frequently been studied and analysed as a reliable and interesting form of early communication (Corwin, Lester, and Golub, 1996).This study examine the cries of a young infant who is later diagnosed with autism. Her cries will be compared with those of her identical twin sister who does not later get diagnosed with autism. Trevarthen and Daniel (2005) have used home video tapes to analyse dynamic and timing elements the monozygotic twins, one of which later developed autism while the other developed normally. Their research found differences in the children’s movement and interactions. In particular, the twin developing autism exhibited a lack of purposeful movement suggesting a core deficit in prospective control. Further studies have concluded that there are differences in the vocal movements of children with autism. A novel acoustic analysis designed by the University of Edinburgh’s Perception in Action laboratory was used to examine the cries of the twins. The analysis is based on the General Tau Theory of movement which claims that all movements are perceptually and intrinsically guided by an internal Tau guide. Successful movement is based on the simultaneous closing of motion gaps. If unsuccessful their prospective control is weak. It is suggested that autistic sufferers may not possess this internal intrinsic guide and it may explain their motor difficulties. The Tau coupling percentages (%), pitch (T), amplitude (A) and timbre (K) of the twins’ cries was examined to determine any differences between the two. Many significant differences within these variables were found between the twins. This study sets the foundations for further, more in depth research in this area. The focus of the study was a small sample and very focused but it simply aims to highlight a possible route into further studying the area of autism at an early age in order to detect signs of autism earlier. 4 Literature Review Autism Background Autism is a neuro-developmental disorder that produces difficulties in socialising, communicating and displays of repetitive behaviour Autism was initially described by Kanner (1943) as a triad of impairments. He claimed that autism manifests itself as specific behavioural signs, and impairments in socialising and communicating. Autism is presented on a spectrum and all individuals vary in capabilities depending on their intellect and social development as an individual. A lack of social and communication skills are amongst the earliest and most common signs of autism (Osterling & Dawson, 2006). Children with autism generally don’t attend to others often or share attention with them (Entremont & Yazbek, 2006). Autistic children also find it difficult to understand the mental states of others (BaronCohen, 2006). One theory that may explain these and other deficits in autism is the ‘theory of mind’. Also known as ‘Mind blindness’ it is believed that autistic sufferers do not possess this theory of mind which makes it difficult for them to interpret and interact with their environment and the people in it. It involves difficulties in understanding others thoughts and actions from their prospective, their intentions and how their own actions affect others (Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith, 1995). Without this notion that other people have separate thinking it is hard for an autistic child to understand the intentions of others. This was confirmed in a study by Entremont and Yazbek (2006) who found that autistic children do not show an understanding or appreciation of the intentions of others. Another study by Pierno et al. (2006) found that autistic children fail to correctly interpret another person’s actions or gaze leading to a failing in planning their next action. These difficulties in comparison to normally developing children will produce a lack of understanding of their own and others motor intentions (Pierno et al., 2006). If an autistic child struggles to define and understand the intentions of their actions they may struggle with controlling their actions. Glazebrook, Elliott and Szatmari (2008) found that autistic individuals used advance information to plan their movements when the information was direct. However, their actions were less controlled when their strategies for movement were self-generated. Autism presents itself as a neurological developmental disorder. The precise mechanisms involved in autism are still not completely understood but it is believed to be related to abnormal neurotransmission and specific brain regions that mediate motor control 5 such as the cerebellum and sub cortical white matter (Bauman & Kemper, 2005; Courchehesne, 2002). This may potentially affect motor performance. Every autistic will have different deficits and their severities in each of these will vary. Although these impairments are known, there is no single reliable measure found that diagnoses autism. All of the existing measurements draw on Kanner’s (1943) triad and none of them focus on the early development of children, particular in their first year of life. Typically children are not diagnosed until around 3 years old despite many claiming that there are evidential signs long before this age (Werner et al., 2000). Early diagnosis is always preferable for any conditions so that intervention can be applied so alternative methods have tested to determine symptoms of autism earlier. General Tau Theory In order to move successfully we humans and animals need to successfully control their movements. One theory on movement is General Tau Theory. General Tau theory aims to explain how movement is perceptually and intrinsically guided which involves the closure of gaps in motion and the use of sense organs to reach the desired end goal (Lee, 2005). Adequate control of a movement is prospective, suggesting it is constantly based on sensory information that can be accessed at some point in the future. The internal intrinsic guide enables people to gain prospective control of their movements. Lee (2005), states that ‘an adequate theory must explain how movements are perceptually and intrinsically guided. It must explain the form of the guiding perceptual information that enables prospective guidance of movement. And it must be biologically plausible.’ It is unclear where this intrinsic Tau guide originates from but suggestions centre on the brain’s energy flows and the patterns of the nervous system (Lee, 1998). Lee bases his theory on the motion gap which represents the movement between the current state and the end goal of an action (1999). All movements can be described by the closure of a motion gap and Tau represents the time it takes for a motion gap to close. For an action to be completed successfully motion gaps need to close simultaneously, this is called Tau coupling. Tau coupling is when two Tau remain in constant proportion over a period of time. Motion gaps in movement need to be simultaneous when closing in order to have a successful 6 movement. Simultaneous closure of the motion gap and therefore a successful movement is represented by a high tau coupling percentage of >90%. A disproportion in Tau can result in an unsuccessful movement and are represented by a lower percentage. Higher percentage couplings represent more successful movements. Analysis based on general Tau theory has been conducted many times before on many different species. Studies have been conducted on the suckling of neo-natal babies (Craig and Lee, 1999; see Lee, 2005). Human running movements have been studied as well as the landing movements of bats and pigeons (see Lee, 2005). Research has been found to suggest people who have movement difficulties such as Parkinson’s disease do so as they lack to ability to use this intrinsic Tau guide and therefore, present low tau coupling percentages (<90%) (Lee et al., 1999). Without this guide they find it difficult to couple their movement to anything and as a result have difficulty in self-generated movement. This could also explain the difficulties many autistics face with physical and vocal movements. This intrinsic Tau guide may be left out of autistics in development which could explain their differences in movement from normal developing children. The Tau guide process of control can be mathematically described by the equation, = kg T represents the sound wave and K represents the kinematic s of a movement produced. By applying this equation a person can vary the length (T), shape (K) and size (A) of a wave. By measuring the size of a wave and applying the Tau guide analysis to how it varies over time, the changes in duration can be tested to decide if they were controlled in a way that implies Tau coupling. If the changes in duration of a wave are measured and analysed by the Tau guide analysis, it may suggest that the changes in duration are controlled in a way that suggests Tau coupling. The values in the equation of Pitch (T), Timbre (K) and Amplitude (A) are all represented as different values of the sound wave. A coupling constant K value represents the dynamics precipitousness of a movement (Lee, Schloger & Pepping, 2008.).The K value is considered the timbre of a sound. It is based on the characteristic quality of a sound, where its origin can be understood. Higher K values represent a loud, collision type noise to the end of a sound and suggests a long initial acceleration followed by 7 a short and quick decline in sound (Lee, 1998) and lower K values represent a softer, quieter end to a sound (Lee, Schogler, & Pepping, 2008). Within the present study, a significant difference in k values for each twin is expected to be found. Amplitude is represented by the A values. It suggests the greatness of extent to a sound, the scope and measure of a sound. Pitch is represented as the T values. It determines the fundamental frequency of a sound wave. Autism Movement Deficits Autistic children do not commonly have major motor disturbances. There have been many studies that have reported minor motor deficits which manifest themselves as motor delay and difficulties in development and coordination. There are often also abnormal displays of movement, for example, repetitive movements (Ming et al, 2007). Leary and Hill (1996) claimed that such motor deficits impair the communicative and social development of a child. Motor difficulties in autistic children have been categorised as associated symptoms (Ming, Brimacombe & Wagner, 2007). Common ‘symptoms’ are motor delay, hypotonia, motor apraxia toe-walking and reduced ankle mobility. A study with a cohort of 154 autistic children found that Hypotonia was the most common problem found in 51% of the children (Ming et al, 2007). In the same study motor apraxia was found in 34% of the children and toe-walking in 19%. Gross motor delay was reported in 9% of the children. It is not uncommon for people with autism to also have dyspraxia which manifests itself as a difficulty in controlling movement (Siegal & Blades, 2003). Whether oral or manual dyspraxia or both, it is often easier to teach autistic individuals to use syllables of vocabulary and pictures than speech. Language can develop a lot faster when using symbols over speech. Notordaeme et al. (2007) conducted study that found speech deficits in many autistic children. They concluded that not only motor deficits but also speech deficits are concurrent with autism. Communication manifests itself in deficits of language. Every autistic will have different deficits and their severity in each of these will vary. One quarter of sufferers will have normal grammatical and vocal skills while another quarter may never be vocal at all (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Lord & Paul, 1997). Generally though, organisation of 8 language, word meaning and syllable production can all be affected. Speech and recognition and understanding of language can also be affected. It has been suggested that the level of language ability is relative to the severity of autism (Happe, 1995). Tager-Flusberg (2006) defined three subtypes of language ability of autistic children: high on verbal IQ, high on non-verbal IQ and equal verbal and non-verbal IQ. They found that high non-verbal IQ associated with a lack of communication and social interaction. This supports previous findings that claim language development has a large influence in abilities of an autistic individual (Happe, 1995). The cause of difficulties in language development is unknown. Many researchers believe the interplay of genes and the environment is the root of this disorder (Askhoomoff et al., 2002). It is suggested the impairment of language is biological but it can be aggravated by other difficulties such as hearing or visual impairments, motor difficulties or physical disabilities (Boucher, 2003). Alternatively, many theorists attribute the communication problems in autism are due to an impairment of their ‘theory of mind’. Theory of mind as mentioned previously, describes the ability to think of or consider other people thoughts or mindset. Autistic individuals are believed to not develop theory of mind, which leads to their social and emotional malfunctions. This could explain why autistic children find it difficult to interpret and interact with their environment and the people in it. However, there are many autistic children who pass the theory of mind task and who display symptoms before this task can be administered (Siegal & Blades, 2003). Many researchers are looking for alternative ways to define the causes of autism and they are increasingly looking to the roots of language impairments in autistic individuals. Development of language in autism is often uneven. For example, an autistic child may accelerate in reading but may fail to comprehend what they have read (Gowen, Stanley & Miall, 2008). Often, if speech is intact, it may lack context or content. An autistic individual may repeat words or numbers or echo words or sentences inappropriately. Comprehension is found to be more impaired than expression. Autistic individuals often express language that may be well formed, analysed and learned often through echoic. However, this language may be misunderstood and revealed out with the appropriate context. Communicating with others is an apparent problem for autism disorder. The communication deficits of the disorder are closely related to the social deficits. Autistic 9 individuals lack the understanding of conventions and rules for socialising presenting difficulties in attending to conversations with others. Autistic individuals also lack the linguistic knowledge to form coherent and meaningful replies in conversation. These linguistic and non-linguistic pragmatic impairments are strong within the disorder therefore, language as a means of communicating with others is rare for many autistic individuals. Autistic people may recall at length conversations they have heard or about things that interest them but they are not capable of holding interactive conversations about these. Language is usually repetitive and only relevant to the autistic individual (Tager-Flusberg, 1996). Conversation tends to be within the individual, for personal satisfaction and with no expectations of external feedback from others (Fine et al., 1994). Due to the lack of understanding of others emotions and how to interact, their language can often be inappropriate and out of context. Semantic organisation relates to the meaning of language. Semantic impairments vary along the spectrum but generally autistic children will not comprehend non-literal language such as irony or sarcasm (Happe, 1995). More severe cases have difficulties in understanding terms that change dependant on time and places. Dunn et al. (1999) explored semantic categorisation tasks with autistic children. They found that they could not distinguish between deviant and target, as they could not determine the external contexts or attribute the more common semantics to suggest which were deviant. Dawson et al. (1998) believed these difficulties in vocabulary were related to differences in the limbic system. Many autistic children have limited word knowledge and often cannot comprehend the meaning of connected speech (Dunn, Vaughan, Kreuzer & Kurtzberg, 1999). Words relating to their mind or mental state such as ‘believe’ or ‘think’ are often not within their vocabulary (Happe, 1995). This lack of semantic knowledge could reflect on their lack of ability to understand other people’s emotions and language. However, it is known that grammatical errors are common in autism, especially in spontaneous speech (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001). Phonology is not usually badly impaired in an autistic individual and language is generally phonologically accurate. However, if an autistic individual is non verbal it is assumed that just as grammatical and semantic language cannot be acquired, phonological language will not be acquired either (Boucher, 2003). Phonology in these disorders has been measured by event related potentials. This is an electro physical response resulting from 10 thoughts or perceptions. When considering the auditory processing, autistic children have shown a shift in the first positive peak in the ERP (P1) (Molfese et al., 2006). More recently researchers have been trying to define subtypes of autism to better understand the underlying causes and tailor treatments to the individual sufferers (TagerFlusberg, 2006). Researchers have claimed that subtypes of autistic children who are verbal but have impaired language have similar language phenotypes to those of children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). SLI is a developmental language disorder that can affect expressive and receptive language and they display phonological and grammatical language difficulties, all of which are often found on autistic children. MRI scans have also suggested that within both of these disorders there are differences in brain asymmetry compared to normal development. Language regions in the left hemisphere are enlarged relative the right hemisphere in normal development however, the opposite is often apparent in children with autism and SLI (Siegal & Blades, 2003). Acoustic Analysis Methods to better understand infants, has long been conducted by analysing their cries. Cry abnormalities have previously been measured on a sound spectrograph which provided a picture of the cry that displayed the acoustic and descriptive characteristics in a visual pattern. With the age of new technology these methods have been greatly enhanced and computer technology has allowed acoustics to be viewed and measured from various parameters instead of just visual patterns. Acoustic analysis in infants was initially used to determine medical problems but is now being used to gain a better understanding of issues such as autism. Crying is a child’s earliest form of communication and most of the initial communication between parent and child involves crying. While cries can sound similar to an untrained ear most parents can distinguish a painful cry from a hunger cry. Communication through crying is an acoustic phenomenon (Corwin, Lester & Golub, 1996.). The onset of a cry, pitch, dynamic and patterns of a cry can determine the message of a cry. Cry analysis is an acoustic analysis of the sub-second psychoacoustic parameters in it, such as timbre, pitch, intensity. Crying analysis has been useful in researching many medical and social difficulties. 11 As a result there have been many medical and social problems that have been found to affect the cry production system such as brain damage (Corwin, Lester & Golub, 1996). They found that brain damaged infants cries were higher pitched, more dynamics in terms of the rising and falling of waves, and a more unstable fundamental frequency (Ostwald, Peltzman, Greenberg, & Meyer, 1970). It is hoped that autism supports these findings and affects the cry production system so that it can be traced at a much earlier prognosis stage. The communication between a parent and child is not just thought to be one sided but is more representative of a conversation between two people despite neither side understanding the language being spoken. The communication must be deeper than just lexical meaning and syntax (Malloch, 1999). Both parties appear ‘attuned’ to the physical and vocal gestures of each other and the communication bears resemblance to a rhythm or a melody, representative of music. This attunement between the two is thought to be an important aspect of the parent and child’s bond and without it can lead to many problems for either party such as depression in the parent (Beebe & Lachmann, 1994) and developmental delay in the child (Hauge & Hallan Tensberg, 1996). ) This co-operative rhythmical communication between a parent and child has been termed ‘communicative musicality’ (Malloch, 1999). Communicative musicality describes mother-infant proto-conversations. Malloch (1999) describes this as the pulse, quality and narrative in a musical dialogue. He considers these elements to be the aspects of human communication that are represented in music and allow a conversation to occur. Pulse is “the regular succession of expressive events through time” (Malloch, 1999). Such events could be a change in the pitch, the start or end of a vocalisation or a louder or quieter moment. Quality is the second component of communicative musicality. It represents the timbre and melody of the vocalisations. If we were relating the vocalisations to physical movement, the quality would represent the speed and contour of the body movements. The final component relates to the narrative. The narrative is based on the pulse and quality of the vocalisations. Narratives represent the act of sharing a communicative experience with another person. Combining these three components communicative musicality represents the act of emotional transference and companionship between two people. Each of these three areas represents a movement that occurs between a parent and child to allow an understanding with one another. Pulse, quality and narrative are the three main factors that represent this musical element to human communication. This rhythm and attunement attained in the communication between parent and child may not 12 always function or be present which can explain some development delays such as autism (Hauge & Hallan Tensberg, 1996). Just as people are, many infants are better at communicating than others. With the difficulties in communication that autistic children experience it could be suggested that their cries are more difficult to interpret. This suggestion depends on a couple of things. Whether the later language problems experienced by autistic children stem from early communication difficulties in their cries and also whether the onset of autism can even be determined and examined that early. This study aims to examine the cries of a young infant who is later diagnosed with autism. Her cries will be compared with those of her identical twin sister who does not later get diagnosed with autism. The pitch, intensity, length, dynamic and frequency of their cries will be examined to determine any differences using a new acoustic analysis technique based on prospective control, as described above. The methodology is based on the principles of General Tau Theory and works to measure the degree of control the children have of the pitch (T), amplitude (A) and timbre (K) of their vocalisations. This study focuses on early development communication to help determine autism at an earlier stage. Trevarthen and Daniel (2005) have used home video tapes to analyse dynamic and timing elements of monozygotic twins, one of which later developed autism while the other developed normally. Their research found differences in the children’s movement and interactions. In particular, the twin developing autism exhibited a lack of purposeful movement suggesting a core deficit in prospective control. The current study will use the same infant data, extracting the acoustics of vocalisations made by the twins to test for differences in their prospective control of the voice. General Tau Theory will be employed in this analysis and tau coupling will be used to determine whether there is a difference in percentage between the twins. This study will analyse the acoustics of each twin’s vocalisations to measure their intentional, prospective control of pitch, timbre, and intensity using a novel methodology based on general, amodal principles of prospective control (General Tau Theory; Lee et al., 1999). It is hoped there will be a significant difference in the amount of intentional control each twin has on their vocalisations. The twin who later developed autism should exhibit less 13 acoustic control if these findings are to support previous research (Trevarthen and Daniel, 2005; Malloch, 1999). It is suspected that the child who is later diagnosed with autism will present lower tau coupling percentages and different values regarding the control of pitch, timbre and amplitude than the normally developing twin. This study aims to establish the acoustic analysis programme as a reliable and valid means for determining vocal actions which can be widely used among other studies. This study aims to aid further the research into early diagnosis of autism by establishing a new approach of how to analysis children at an earlier age. It is hoped this study will further establish home videos a useful tool in analysing the early signs of children with autism. Hypothesis There are differences in a neural, amodal prospective control system that are detectable shortly after birth in the vocal ‘movements’, the acoustic change of the baby’s cry. 14 Methods and Materials Design This study analysed the acoustics of eight infant cries from two monozygotic twins taken from home video tapes three days after birth, to determine if there were differences in the intentional control of the infants’ vocalisations. The control of their pitch (T), timbre (K), and intensity (A) were measured to determine the degree of control of the voice. This analysis of the voice was carried out using proprietary software designed for this purpose. Baby B refers to the child who went onto develop autism. Baby J refers to the baby who went on to have a normal development. They are labelled according due to the first letter of each of their names. Participants The study involved two monozygotic twins. The footage used examined their vocal actions from birth to 11 months of age. One of the twins was diagnosed with autism at the age of two using the ICD-10, while the other twin had a normal development. Materials The materials used in this study included video footage of the twins from birth to 11 months of age given by the consent of the parents, the acoustic analysis programme, and a computer. The acoustic analysis required several types of software available within the computer. To listen to and edit out the clips of sound, pro cut or QuickTime can be used. Microsoft excel was used to achieve spreadsheets of the data. A programme called Praat (available to download online) was used to analyse the wave processing. The analysis TauG programme was based at the Perception in Action Laboratory at the University of Edinburgh. Procedure Two home video tapes of the twins ranging in age from birth to 11 months old were examined for any vocal utterances. Vocalisations, providing they were of audible quality, were selected and analysed to determine the amount of control they had on their intentional vocalisations. This was analysed using an acoustic analysis programme based on General Tau Theory developed by the Perception and Action Laboratory at the University of Edinburgh. Intentional control was measured through the pitch, timbre and intensity of their vocalisations. 15 Statistical Analysis This acoustic analysis draws on the basic principles from General Tau Theory (Lee et al., 1999). This form of analyse has previously been used to study Parkinson’ disease (Freeman et al., 1993.) and neonatal babies (Craig and Lee, 1999; see Lee 2005). General Tau Theory is a neurological mechanism that our bodies use to achieve movement. It represents a means for researchers to examine how different aspects of a sound (duration, size and shape) may be controlled for through communication. This theory claims that in order to produce prospectively controlled behaviour of vocalisations; we perceive the amount of change in a sound wave relative to the variable being measured. This process of control is described mathematically by General Tau Theory and is now integrated into a working programme which analyses the data through a computer. General Tau Theory measures the motion gaps of a movement. A successful vocal movement requires the motion gaps to close at the same time, this is called Tau Coupling. The Tau coupling percentages will be analysed. Higher percentages (>90%) representing more successful movements. There will further be two levels of analysis. The level of analysis measures each wave so that each waveform has three variable; T, K, and A. T represents the ‘pitch’ or fundamental frequency (F0). K is the coupling constant from the Tau Theory and represents the ‘timbre’. A represents the amplitude of the wave and is considered to reflect the intensity of a wave. The second stage of analysis presents T, K, and A wave glides. The T, K, and A values are expected to fit with the prospective control of movement. This analysis aims to determine the amount of perspective control each twin has in their vocalisations. 16 Sound wave 1ST Tier analysis 2nd Tier analysis % r2 Pitch glides T K % T A r2 A T= Duration % K= r2 Timbre T Timbre glides K A A= Amplitude % r2 T A K Amplitude glides Figure 1. Schematic of the accoustic analysis performed in this study. The waveform was measured for basic parameters T, K, and A, which are measures of the fundamental frequencey (F 0), timbre, and intensity of the sound wave. Each of these measures was achieved using a calculation of the ‘up’ and ‘down’ movement of the pressure ramps in the sound data. T is a measure of the duration of a complete up and down cycle, and thus represents the primary, or fundamental frequency. A is a measure of the height of the wave, and so gives an approximate indication of the psychoaccoustic intensity of the sound. K is a value derived from a tauG analysis of the ‘up’ and ‘down’ portions of the wave. It loosely represents the compiled harmonics (Fn+1) in a single value and is specifically the coupling constant of a tauG measure of each ‘up’ and ‘down’ portion of the wave. These data produce the ‘1 st Tier Analysis’. The ‘2nd Tier Analysis’ measures the control of the shifting T, K, and A values over the course of the cry using a tauG analysis. The values %, r2, T, K, and A each give information about the form of the control of the changes in the pitch, intensity, and timbre over the course of the cry. 17 Procedure of Statistical Analysis (See appendix 1 for more detailed description of the protocol for the analysis stages). Video edit and audio transfer Sound clips of either twin’s cries were edited out of the video using Final Cut Pro. Times and occurrences in each clip were noted also. The clips were then edited as sound wave audio files. The audio files were each opened using Praat and converted into text files. First tier of analysis Matlab was opened and within it the analysis program, called TauGUI was opened. In TauGUI the newly converted text files of the sounds were opened. Criteria were placed into the program as follows: Sample rate; 44000Hz, Smoothing filter- 4. Once this was completed the ‘Run section seeker’ button was selected until the program informs you that it is completed. The following criteria were then inputed: max duration- 0.5, min duration0.00005, max amplitude- 1.5, and min amplitude- 0.001. The ‘Run test’ was selected until the program once again informs you that it has completed that part of the analysis. All of the data was selected and analysed and the results were exported as text files. These stages were completed for every sound clip. To determine the percentage tau coupling criteria the data was pasted into Kaliedograph and smoothed. To determine the T, K, and A values the data was opened in Microsoft excel and then into Kaliedograph, again to be smoothed with a sigma of 4. This presented us with the T, K, and A values and the tau coupling % for the first stage of the analysis. Second tier of analysis In matlab, the workspace was cleared and the data was imported. The ‘Resample’ file was opened in the current directory. The start and end time of the sound clip was divided by 0.002 gives the number of steps. The number of steps was added into line 8 of the file and the ‘save and run’ button was pressed. In the workspace new files appeared and these were exported into text files and saved. Each individual file represented the T, K, or A glides. The TauGUI file was opened again in Matlab and the T, K, and A files were each put through the analysis again. A sample rate of 500Hz was selected along with a sigma value of 4 and a 5% velocity. Again, the files were exported and saved as text files. This presented us with the T, K, and A glides and the % tau couplings for the second part of the analysis. 18 Results Both twins displayed high percentage tau couplings. Both of the babies mean percentage tau couplings were 86.60%. However, neither presented significant percentage couplings (>90%). This suggests a great deal of control is equally present in both babies. For K values baby B had a lower tau coupling % than baby J although neither percentages were significant (>90%). Baby b had much higher values for the K value overall. Baby B had significantly higher amplitude and timbre than baby J which suggested that baby B’s vocalisations were much more intense and dynamic in the timbre than baby J. Baby B also had much higher K values for pitch but not a significant amount more. Baby B showed a higher mean amplitude % tau coupling than baby J. The A values for timbre, amplitude and pitch were all higher for Baby J. However, only the A values for timbre were significant which suggests that there is more dynamic in the amplitude of baby J’s vocalisations. The amplitude and pitch were higher but not significantly so. Baby J had a higher T value tau coupling percentage than baby B although it was not significantly high (>90%). Baby B had higher T values for timbre, amplitude and pitch in comparison to baby J. Baby b had significantly higher amplitude and dynamics regarding pitch. Baby B also had higher frequency for the pitch but it was not significant. There were two parts to analysis and the results are presented accordingly. The first tier of the analysis examines Tau coupling percentages for T, K and A waves. The second tier of analysis examines the glides of the waves with regards to the duration (T), timbre (K) and amplitude (A) of each wave (see the diagram of statistical analysis in the methodology). Wave Percentage Couplings (%) The wave percentage represents the amount of Tau coupling present in the sound. If the Tau coupling percentage is higher than 90% then this suggests good control of vocalisations. If the Tau coupling percentage is below 90% then internal or external causes may have affected the amount of control the child has on their vocalisations (e.g. neuromotor, intrinsic Tau guide). Baby B, who later develops autism, is expected to have lower 19 percentages. This is because autistic children are expected to have less control and perhaps be more affected by other causes. The duration of each clip varied slightly. The longest clip lasted 0.043 seconds for baby B and the shortest clip lasted 0.011 seconds for baby J (see Table 1). Baby B had a longer mean length of clips (0.023 seconds) than baby J did (0.012 seconds). Table 1: Duration of each clip Duration BabyB Clip 1 0.018 BabyB Clip 2 0.043 BabyB Clip 3 0.018 BabyB Clip 4 0.015 Baby J Clip 1 0.013 Baby J Clip 2 0.012 Baby J Clip 3 0.011 Baby J Clip 4 0.013 Table 1. The duration of each clip in seconds. There were eight clips in total, with four for each baby. The clip duration was the whole length of the baby’s utterance. The utterances were not clipped in length. Table 2: Tau coupling percentages means (%) of both twins’ wave forms Baby B Baby J Clip 1 88.70 86.59 Clip 2 86.33 85.80 Clip 3 87.00 87.07 Clip 4 85.20 88.11 20 Table 2. Represents the Tau coupling percentages means of both twins’ wave forms. The separate percentages are presented for each of the four clips for each baby. Mean percentages of Tau couplings for both babies were compared. There was not a large amount of variability between the clips regarding percentage coupling for baby B. Clip 4 had the highest percentage coupling of 88.11% yet it still did not reach the significant level of 90%. There is a similar degree of variability in baby J compared with baby B (see Figure 1). Clip 1 had the highest percentage coupling for baby J (88.70%) but again it was still not high enough to reach the 90% significance level. Neither of the babies had a great range of percentage couplings. Both babies’ had similar and consistent percentages which suggest little difference between the controls of their vocalisations. Both twins presented high percentage couplings (%) but neither reached the significant level of 90%.The overall percentage coupling for each baby was the same (86.60%). This suggests that there was little difference overall in percentage coupling between the vocalisations of the twins. However, there was a slight difference in standard deviation. Baby B had a standard deviation of 9.5 and baby J had a standard deviation of 11. This indicates baby J has a greater variability in the percentage coupling. Although neither reached the significant percentage level, an average of 86.60% was still very close. This could suggest a great deal of control is presented in both babies regarding their vocalisations. K value (Timbre) 1st Tier analysis The mean percentages for timbre values were examined in the 1st tier of analysis. It was found that baby J had a much more consistent % of waves for K over the four sound clips (see Figure 2). In comparison baby B’s % was much more diverse in wave % of K over the four clips. Baby B had much more variance in the % (4.89) than baby J (0.84) and a much higher standard deviation (2.21) than baby J did (0.91). 21 Baby B (who later developed autism) had a lower k value percentage overall (85.52%) than baby J (86.31%), but this was not significant (<90%). However, baby B had a higher standard deviation (17.45) than baby J (16.32). Figure 2. Presents the wave percentage of timbre. The timbre relates to the dynamics of a movement. The percentage amount represented the closure gaps of the timbre of their vocal movements. There are four clips displayed for each baby with a mean percentage each. Higher percentages (>90%) represent a good prospective control of the timbre of their vocal movements. 2nd Tier analysis In three out of the four clips baby B had higher K glides than baby J (see Figure 3). This means in these three clips they had a much more intense quality of sound. The mean K glide for amplitude in baby B was higher (0.035) than for baby J (0.028). The standard deviation for both baby B (0.01) and baby J (0.01) were very similar. A t-test was conducted that determined the difference in K glides for amplitude between the two twins was significant (t= 0.075, df=6, p<0.05). This confirms that the baby B displayed much more dynamic, intense quality of sound and ended with louder, collision sounds at the end of vocalisations. A further ANOVA was conducted on the data which confirmed the significance difference of baby B’s much louder and dynamic vocal sounds in comparison to baby J’s quieter vocal sounds (F=0.56, df= 1,6, p=<0.05). 22 Figure 3. Displays the K glides for amplitudes. The numbers represent the amount of K value of amplitude in each of the four clips for each baby. The K glides reflect the timbre or dynamics of a vocal movement. Amplitude represents the intensity of a vocal movement. From the K glides of the timbre it is clear that the dynamics of baby B’s vocalisations are much higher in every clip than for baby J (see Figure 4). This data again confirms that baby B presents much louder and dynamic ends to their sounds suggesting this twin has less control of her vocalisations. The mean K glide for baby B was higher (0.40) than baby J (0.37) as well as the standard deviation was much higher for baby B (0.02) than for baby J (0.01). A t-test was conducted which confirmed that baby B’s vocalisations were significantly louder and more dynamic than baby J’s (t= 2.65, df= 6, p= <0.05).An ANOVA was conducted which also confirmed that baby B’s vocal sounds were significantly more dynamic than baby J’s (F=7.04, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). 23 Figure 4. Displays the K glides of the timbre. The K values that are presented represent the amount of K that is in the timbre of the vocal movements of each of the four clips displayed for each of the babies. K represents the timbre or dynamics of a vocal movement. From the K glides of the pitch it is clear that baby B has higher K glides. In every clip baby B has a higher k glides for timbre than baby J (see Figure 4). Baby B has a higher mean (0.02) than baby J (0.01). The standard deviation is also higher for baby B (0.01) than for baby J (0.00). A t-test was conducted which suggested the difference in pitch for K glides in baby B was not significantly different to baby J (t=1.71, df=6, p= >0.05). An ANOVA was conducted to confirm that there was no significant difference between the twins in their pitch for K glides (F=0.077, df=1,6, p>0.05). Figure 5. Presents the K glides of pitch. The amounts presented represent the K glides of pitch found in each of the four clips for each of the babies. K values represent the timbre or dynamics of a vocal movement. Pitch represents the fundamental frequency of a vocal movement. A value (Amplitude/ Intensity) This value considers the loudness and intensity parameters controlling. This value represents the greatness of extent of the vocalisation. 24 1st Tier analysis Baby B (who went on to develop autism) showed a higher amplitude percentage coupling overall (86.44%). Baby J (who had developed typically) showed a slightly lower percentage coupling overall (84.77%) (see Figure 6). The standard deviation was higher for baby J (19.21) than for baby B (17.20). Baby J had much more variance in the % (7.87) than baby B (6.22) and a higher standard deviation (2.81) than baby B did (2.49). Figure 6. Displays the wave percentage of amplitude. Each value presented represents the amount of tau coupling percentage present in each of the four clips for each of the babies. Amplitude reflects the amount of intensity in a vocal movement. Higher percentages (>90%) represent a good prospective control of the intensity in their vocal movements 2nd Tier analysis The A glides for amplitude were analysed and it was suggested that the mean A glide for amplitude in baby B was lower (0.022) than for baby J (0.023). The standard deviation for baby B was much higher (0.007) than for baby J (0.005) (see Figure 7). A t-test was conducted that determined the difference in A glides for amplitude between the two twins was not significant (t= 0.10, df=6, p>0.05). A further ANOVA was conducted on the data which confirmed no significance difference in amplitude between the twins (F=0.011, df= 1,6, p=>0.05). 25 Figure 7. Represents the A glides for amplitude. The numbers displayed represent the amount of A glides for amplitude found in each of the four clips for each baby. A glides reflect the amount of intensity in a vocal sound. While analysing the A glides for timbre it was clear that in every clip baby J has a higher amplitude quality than baby B (see figure 8). The mean A glide for timbre is higher for baby J (0.42) than for baby B (0.36). The standard deviation was also higher for baby J (0.07) than for baby B (0.03). A t-test was conducted that determined the difference in A glides for Timbre between the twins was a significant one (t=0.17, df= 6, p=<0.05).An ANOVA was also conducted which confirmed the significance of the difference in A glides for Timbre between the twins (F=2.5, df=1,6, p=<0.05). Figure 8. Displays the A glides in timbre. The A values presented reflect the amount of A glides of timbre in each of the four clips for each baby. A glides represent the amount of 26 amplitude or intensity in vocal movements. Timbre represents the dynamics of vocal movement. From analysing the A glides of pitch it was found that in three out of the four clips, baby J had higher A glides for pitch (see Figure 9). The mean A glides for pitch was higher for baby J (0.02) than for baby B (0.01). The standard deviation was also higher for baby J (0.005) than for baby B (0.002).A t-test was conducted which confirmed that the difference in A glides for pitch between the twins was not a significant one (t=1.29, df=6, p= >0.05).An ANOVA was conducted which confirmed that there was no significant difference between the twins for A glides for pitch (F=4.82, df= 1,6, p= >0.05). Figure 9. Displays the A glides of pitch. This figure displays the amount of A values of pitch presented in each of the four clips for each baby. The A glides represent the amplitude or intensity of a vocal movement. The pitch (T) represents the amount of fundamental frequency within the vocal movement. T value (Frequency/Pitch) 1st Tier analysis While analysing the wave % of pitch it was found that baby B (who later developed autism) had a lower T value percentage overall (84.49%). Baby J (who developed typically) had a slightly higher percentage overall (85.33%). The standard deviation is higher for baby J (2.97) than for baby B (1.42). 27 Figure 10. Presents the wave percentage of Tau couplings for Pitch (T). The numbers given represent the amount of percentage given to each of the four clips for each of the babies. The higher the percentage (>90%) the more controlled the vocal movement is considered to be. Higher percentages (>90%) represent successful tau coupling and therefore, more successful movements. Pitch (T) represents the fundamental frequency of a vocal movement. 2nd Tier analysis In three of the four clips baby B had higher T glides for Amplitude. The mean T glides of amplitude is higher for baby B (201) than for baby J (180) (see Figure 11). The standard deviation however, is much higher for baby J (103.07) than for baby B (12.09). A ttest was conducted which concluded that the difference in T glide of amplitude between the twins was a significant one (t=0.42, df= 6, p= <0.05). An ANOVA was conducted which confirmed that Baby B had significantly higher T glides for amplitude than baby J did (F=0.176, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). 28 Figure 11. Presents the T glides of amplitude. The numbers presented represent the amount of T values of amplitude in each of the four clips for each of the babies. T glides reflect the fundamental frequency of a vocal movement. Amplitude represents the intensity of a vocal movement. While analysing the T glides for timbre it is clear that in three of the four clips baby B had slightly higher T glides for Timbre (see Figure 12). The mean T glide for timbre was slightly higher for baby J (0.42) than for baby B (0.40). The standard deviation however, was higher for baby B (0.04) than for baby J (0.03). A t-test was conducted which concluded that there was no significant difference in T glides for timbre between the twins (t= 0.57, df= 6, p= >0.05).An ANOVA was conducted which confirmed there was no significance difference between the twins for T glides of Timbre (F=0.32, df= 1,6, p= >0.05). 29 Figure 12. Displays the T values of timbre. The numbers presented in figure 12 represent the amount of T values of timbre in each of the four clips in each of the babies. The t values reflect the fundamental frequency of a vocal movement. The timbre (K) represents the dynamics of vocal movements. When considering T glides for pitch, all four clips baby B had much higher T glides for pitch (see figure 13). Baby B had a higher mean of T glides for pitch (0.02) than baby J did (0.01). Baby B also had a much higher standard deviation (0.01) than baby J (0.0). A ttest was conducted which confirmed the difference in T glides between the twins was a significant one (t=1.72, df= 6, p= <0.05). An ANOVA was also conducted which confirmed that baby B did have significantly higher T glides for pitch than baby J had (F= 2.94, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). Figure 13. Displays T glides of pitch. The figure above represents the amount of T values of pitch present in each of the four clips for each of the babies. The T values represent the pitch or fundamental frequency of a vocal movement. 30 Discussion Wave percentage Tau couplings (%) The wave percentages of Tau couplings for each twin were looked at, taking into consideration all four clips from each twin to make an average percentage. The percentages of tau coupling were examined to determine the control of each wave to see if that could highlight how successful their vocal movements were for each twin. It was suggested that had there been a difference in the percentage couplings of the twins, there would have been differences in how each baby managed the closure of motion gaps. If the % coupling is higher than 90% then good control of vocalisations is displayed. If below 90% then internal or external causes affect the amount of control they have. (E.g. neuromotor, intrinsic tau guide). Baby B (the autistic twin) was expected to have lower percentages as they are expected to have less prospective control and perhaps be more affected by other causes. This study of course is particularly speculative and serves as a foundation for further research into this area. It was thus predicted that baby B who later went on the develop autism would have a significantly lower percentage for the tau coupling than the normally developing child, baby J. This would support the hypothesis that autistic children lack the internal tau guide to aid their movements which would explain their movement difficulties. However, such differences in percentages were not evident between the twins. Concerning the data showing the control of the gap closures of each wave, there was not a great deal of variability between the clips regarding percentage coupling for baby B. Clip 4 had the highest percentage coupling of 88% yet it still did not reach the significant level of 90%. There is a similar degree of variability in baby J compared with baby B. The highest percentage coupling for baby J was 89%, but again it was still not high enough to reach the 90% significance level. Neither of the babies showed a great range of percentage couplings in their sound wave motion-gap closures. Both babies had the same mean tau percentage coupling of 89%. These same and consistent percentages suggest little difference between the twin’s vocalisations, but it is interesting to note that this may refer to a specific newborn form of acoustic control that can be explored further in subsequent research. In general, percentages were high for both babies however; no clip’s percentage reached the 90% significant level. This suggests that neither baby had great prospective control of their vocalisations. However, there was a slight difference in standard deviation 31 which indicates baby J has a greater variability in the percentage coupling. The mean percentage couplings for each twin are the same which indicates there is no difference in the control of closing the motion gaps. This indicates no differences in prospective control of vocal movements between the twins. These results suggested that there is very little difference between the twins control of their sound waves indicating that baby B’s autistic development has not affected this area of vocalisation. The specific variables of timbre, amplitude and pitch are now considered to determine if there is a difference within them between the twins. This would highlight more specific differences between the twins that will further research more focused. K value (Timbre) The K value depicts the amount of Tau coupling percentage for timbre and the K glides within the sound waves. The K value represents the dynamics of a movement. This value looks at the timbre changes parameters controlling. High K represents a sound that ends with a loud, collision type end to a sound and a low K represents a much quieter end to a sound. Baby B (who developed autism) had a lower Kvalue percentage overall (86%) (See Table 3). Baby J (who developed typically) had a slightly higher percentage coupling. This supports the idea that baby B would have lower tau coupling percentages which represent less prospective control of their vocalisations. A significant difference in K values for the amplitude of the timbre (K) glides between each twin was expected to be found. The results found that there were differences in K values between the two babies. Baby B was found to have significantly higher K values regarding the amplitude of the K glides. (F=0.56, df= 1,6, p=<0.05).This suggests that baby B’s vocalisations had a much more intense quality of sound and tended to end with much more dynamic and louder sounds. A further ANOVA was conducted on the data which confirmed the significance difference of baby B’s much louder and dynamic vocal sounds in comparison to baby J’s quieter vocal sounds. It could be suggested that the increased intensity and varying dynamic of baby B’s vocalisations present a less controlled vocalisation. A prospectively controlled vocalisation may present itself as less stressed and rounded at the ends of the sound waves and the quality of sound may not be as intense. The K values for timbre glides also presented differences between the twins (F=7.04, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). Again, baby B’s vocalisation were much louder, they were more dynamic and ended 32 with more collision force. Comparing these findings with the wave percentages it is clear that the higher K values represent less quality in the tone of the voice and therefore less prospective control of their vocalisations. These findings suggest again that baby B has less prospective control over vocalisations. The K values from pitch also suggested differences between the twins. Baby B presented higher K values for pitch than baby J. However, unlike the other differences this one was not significant. (F=0.077, df=1,6, p>0.05). Beginning and ending with more forceful, collision style sounds is representative of the irregularities within autistic language abilities (Dunn, Vaughan, Kreuzer & Kurtzberg, 1999). This was supported by the general K value results. It was expected that the data would indicate different K values between the twins and the results supported this hypothesis. The term ‘pulse’ described by Malloch (1999) represents events within a sound, such as louder or quieter beginnings or endings to a sound. Within the results it was found that baby B displayed much higher values of K glides which suggest a more forceful sounding vocalisation. This relates to the Malloch’s term ‘pulse’ within communicative musicality since, baby B has more dynamics at the beginning and end of their vocalisations (1999). This suggests that baby B has a different pulse form baby J. This may suggest a difference in rhythm of communication between baby B communicating with their parents and baby J communicating with their parents. Malloch’s term ‘quality’ refers to the timbre of a sound (1999). Given the current results it is clear that baby B and baby J have different qualities to their sounds. Further research could be done to consider the K values here and specifically analysis whether the different rhythmical styles of the twins to their parents suggest that one style is less effective. If this is the case, then the ineffective communication between parent and child could be an early indicator for further communicative difficulties including autism. A value (Amplitude/ Intensity) The A value represents the loudness and intensity parameters controlling. The Tau coupling percentages (%) for the amplitude glides (A) were examined. This value represents the greatness of extent of the vocalisation. Baby B (who went on to develop autism) showed a higher amplitude Tau coupling percentage overall (86%). Baby J (who had developed typically) showed a slightly lower Tau coupling percentage overall (84%). This suggests that baby B had a more intense sound and more prospective control over their vocalisation than baby J. Although neither of the values were significant at the 90% level. However, the 33 specific amplitude glides were not more significantly intense for amplitude for baby B (t= 0.10, df=6, p>0.05). The A glides for timbre were much higher for baby J than baby B. This suggests that baby J displayed a significantly higher quality of intense vocalisations than baby B (F=2.5, df=1,6, p=<0.05). This would support the hypothesis that baby J’s vocalisations have a better quality and this may be because she developed normally in comparison to baby B. Malloch’s (1999) term, ‘quality’, would suggest that baby J had better contours of movement within the sound waves of her vocalisations. The quality of a sound is crucial for the interpretation of its meaning therefore; the lower quality of amplitude that is presented by Baby B may suggest a difficulty or at least a difference in the way baby B communicates. Due to the lack in quality of amplitude, crying may be harder to interpret which can be a crucial part of the communicating (Corwin, Lester & Golub, 1996). Baby J also had higher A values for pitch. The mean A values for pitch was higher for baby J (0.022) than for baby B (0.019). However, the difference was not confirmed as a significant one (t=1.29, df=6, p= >0.05).. The lower quality of amplitude may mean they have difficulty in communicating to their parents; they may not be fully attuned to each other and therefore, not reach communicative musicality (Malloch, 1999). T value (Frequency/Pitch) This value considers the pitch and frequency changes parameters controlling. Pitch explains the way in which a vocalisation is controlled. Regarding the Tau coupling percentages for the pitch glides (T), baby B (who later developed autism) had a lower T value Tau coupling percentage overall (84%). Baby J (who developed typically) had a slightly higher Tau coupling percentage overall (85%). This suggests that baby J’s pitch was much higher than baby B’s. This higher percentage represents more simultaneous closure of motion gaps and therefore, more successful vocal movements. These results support the hypothesis that baby J has more prospective control of her vocal movements than baby B. However; neither of the babies Tau coupling percentages for T values reached the significant level of 90%. While considering the T values for amplitude it was clear that the mean T value of amplitude is higher for baby B (201) than for baby J (180). This was proven a significant difference between the twins (F=0.176, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). This indicates that the intensity of 34 the pitch was significantly higher for baby B. Pitch represents the fundamental frequency of a sound. Regarding the T values for timbre it is clear that baby B (0.40) had slightly higher mean T values for timbre than baby J (0.37). This suggests that baby B presents a more dynamic vocal pitch within their sounds. However, this difference was not a significant one (t= 0.57, df= 6, p= >0.05). Baby B had a higher mean of T values for pitch than baby J did (0.012). This difference was proven significant (F= 2.94, df= 1,6, p= <0.05). This again suggests that baby B controlled their vocalisations differently from baby J. This difference will relate to the differences in timbre, amplitude and pitch presented in this study. It also confirms that baby B has more pitch changes and frequency within her sound glides. From the results presented it is clear that the twins do have a different prospective control of movement. Baby B has significantly higher K glides for timbre and amplitude as well as significantly higher T glides for timbre and amplitude. Baby J has significantly higher A glides for amplitude. In general, both twins presented the same Tau coupling % of 86%. However, in specific areas the babies presented different strengths. Baby J had higher tau percentage couplings for timbre and pitch glides however, baby B had higher Tau coupling percentage for amplitude glides. These results suggest that both twins control their vocal movements differently and present different vocalisations. The question remains regarding what accounts for this difference. The tau coupling percentages represent the amount of simultaneous closure within the motion gaps. A higher Tau coupling percentage represents a more successful movement with more prospective control. The babies clearly vary in prospective control depending on the variable. A prospective of control is very important in perceiving the world around us and gives us the ability to act ahead of time. Baby J has broader ability of prospective control given that she has higher tau coupling percentages for both timbre and pitch. This may suggest that baby B has a weaker prospective control of her movements. This would support findings that autistic children, such as baby B, have less control of their movements, especially when they were self-generated (Glazebrook, Elliot & Szatmari, 2008). It was also found that autistic children struggle to understand their own motor intentions which may explain the lack of ability to control those actions (Pierno et al. 1996). Lee et al. (1999) previously found that people who suffer from Parkinson’s disease also have difficulty with self-generated movement. This problem was traced to a lack of intrinsic Tau guide (Lee et al. 1999). Their movements were a problem as they had nothing to 35 couple their movements to. This question whether there is a core deficit in such a system as the intrinsic Tau guide (Lee, 1998) or the intrinsic motive pulse (Trevarthen, 1999). It certainly appears that there is something missing in these diseases regarding movement and this explanation currently seems a valid one. Given that the twins vary on the glides that they have significant differences in, it would be unfair to assume that either has no intrinsic Tau guide at all. Instead it could be said that Tau guides are present but more available to some people than others. Further research would be useful to determine which neural system is affected by these difficulties. The basal ganglia produce the kind of consistent, patterned firing that would be required for an intrinsic timing system and is also disrupted in Parkinson’s disease (Lee et al., 1999). This may also explain the differences between the twins. Further research could consider movement and prospective control as the same or at least part of the same perceptual-motor system. This study stands in contrast to the claims made by Askhoomoff et al. (2002). They claim that the interplay of genes and environment is the root of the autistic disorder. It is suggested the impairment of language is biological but it can be aggravated by other difficulties such as hearing or visual impairments, motor difficulties or physical disabilities (Boucher, 2003). In the case presented here, there were no physical differences in the twins known at the time of writing, after the first twelve years life. We can conclude that the differences found here in their vocalisations were not due to physical causes, since both of the subjects were monozygotic twins there are clearly no biological differences between the two. The differences found in the study between the twins’ vocalisations are clearly not in any way physically or biologically related in this case. Although this cannot be generalised to all cases of autism, it does indicate that genetic or physical attributes are not the whole reasoning behind language impairments or autism disorder in general. This case could support the theory of mind hypothesis or at least lead to further investigation about the nature of the theory (Siegal & Blades, 2003). Pitch, as with timbre, relates to Malloch’s (1999) term of ‘pulse’. The changes in pitch are considered differences in pulse. Again, the affect pitch changes have on the pulse element of communicative musicality can reflect how well the child communicates to their parent. In this case baby B had higher frequency of pitch changes and we could assume from this that her vocalisations were less consistent and more erratic. Having so many pitch changes and inconsistent sounds can cause problems in the rhythm of communication 36 between the parent and baby B. If pulse is affected then there is not strong attunement between the parent and child. This supports the notion that baby B’s vocalisations were weaker than baby J’s. In many of the results for tau percentage couplings, timbre, amplitude and pitch glides, the percentage do not reach the 90% significant level. However, it does reach very close in many cases and further, more in depth analysis shows that areas of that variable are significant. It could be considered within the analysis to lower the percentage value for significance perhaps to 85% which would enable much more of the tau coupling to be significant and perhaps then more accurate. It was expected there would be a significant difference in the data to show a difference in the amount of intentional control performed by each twin during vocalising. The twin who later developed autism exhibited less acoustic control regarding pitch, timbre and amplitude than the normally developing twin in all variables which supported previous research (Trevarthen and Daniel, 2005; Malloch, 1999). It was suspected that the child who is later diagnosed with autism would present lower tau coupling percentage. However, there was no significant difference between the twins in terms of their general intentional control of their vocalisations. This may be because it is still too early to detect. It may relate to the methods used which may not be as effective as initially thought with such early language capabilities. This study is just simply trying to establish a new way of analysis that is not intrusive and allows us to determine whether or not there are earlier signs of autism that are informative, worth looking at and have been missed or are difficult to find by pre-existing techniques of analysing or autism diagnosis techniques. This study aims to establish the acoustic analysis programme as a reliable and valid means for determining vocal actions which can be widely used among other studies. This study aims to aid further the research into early diagnosis of autism by establishing a new approach of how to analysis children at an earlier age. It is also hoped this study will further establish home videos a useful tool in analysing the early signs of children with autism. Further research would be beneficial in this area of study. Perhaps comparing the timbre, amplitude and pitch findings with the different sub types of autism current being researched (Tager-Flusberg, 2006). The findings from timbre, amplitude and pitch could also be compared to the research that focuses on the different brain regions affected by language 37 abilities using MRI scanning (Siegal & Blades, 2003). Previous research highlights the cerebellum and sub cortical white matter as the key areas that mediate motor control and ongoing work continues in this area (Bauman & Kemper, 2005; Courchehene, 2002). There is also a lot research into cry analysis of children with medical and social problems that could be included in this further research. Studies found that brain damaged children had higher pitch, more dynamic sound and more unstable fundamental frequency (Corwin, Lester, & Golub, 1996). The research could focus on whether the children with differences in timbre, amplitude and pitch findings also display differences in their brain scans. It would also be relative to compare the timbre, amplitude and pitch findings with those same children later on in their development to determine who later develops language impairments, such as specific language impairment, and what their original differences for variables were compared to normally developing children (Tager-Flusberg, 2006). It is important to clarify that this study serves only as an indicator for further study rather than trying to claim substantial findings on its own. The sample size in this study is extremely small and the results serve only as an insight into early language development between autistic children and non autistic children. The basis of autism is still very unclear and there is still a vast amount of research to be conducted. This study simply aims to highlight an area of interest that may provide further information into autism if more research is followed up. This study hopes to provide the foundations to further research in this area. If these vocal differences are apparent from such an early age, autism could be detected earlier and therapies are likely to be more effective. It is important to note however, that not all children who go on to develop autism will display such language differences in early development. Vocal difficulties in children may only manifest or become an issue much later in development for many children and it may prove difficult to attempt to detect autism at such an early stage. It is also important to note that neither verbal difficulties, nor physical movement explains the whole extent of autism. Autism cannot be diagnosed simply from these symptoms therefore; children who display similar movement and vocal difficulties may not be diagnosed with autism. This study highlights home videos as a reliable method of research. It allows data to be accessed at any given time and to be considered or compared at a much later date that 38 recorded. The setting is observational and the subjects are in their own environment so the behaviour is more natural than it would be in a laboratory. As this is the case there are no variables to control for. There can be problems with home video tapes however. For instance they can produce very poor sound quality which would be a problem for sound analysis. Not having the ability to control for certain factors can discourage many researchers from using this method of research. Generally, the language of autistic sufferers varies greatly from person to person. Each case can be taken as an individual one as every child develops differently. It has been difficult for researchers so to even categorise types of autistics (Tager-Flusberg, 2006). It is therefore, difficult to generalise the difference in vocalisations from this study to all autistic sufferers. It is important to note that further studies of this kind may find a varying degree of difference between autistic and non autistic sufferers in terms of timbre, amplitude and pitch. This study does not aim to do so and merely serve as an indicator of what kind of controlling parameters to consider for when analysing a child who possibly has autism. 39 References Akshoomoff, N., Lord, C., Lincoln, A.J., Courchesne, R.Y., Carper, R.A., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., (2004). Outcome classification of preschool children with autism spectrum disorders using MRI brain measures. Journal of American Academics Children and Adolescent Psychiatry 43: 349-357 Baron-Cohen, S., (1995). Mindblindess: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., & Frith, U, (1985). Does the autistic child have a `theory of mind'? Cognition, 21, 37-46. Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F.M., (1994). Representation and Internalization in Infancy: Three Principles of Salience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11, p127-165. Bauman, M.L., & Kemper, T.L. (2005). Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions. International journal of developmental neuroscience, 23, p183187. Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H., (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of speech-language pathology, 12, (3), p349-358. Corwin, M.J., Lester, B.M., & Golub, H.L., (1996). The Infant Cry: What can it tell us? Current problems in paediatrics, 26, p325-334. Courchehesne, E., (2002). Abnormal early brain development in autism. Molecular Psychology, 7, p21-23. Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A.N., & Kuhn, P., (2000). Case study of the development of an infant with autism from birth to two years of age. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 299-313. 40 Dunn, M., Vaughan, H., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D., (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autism and normal children. Developmental Neuropsycholgy, 16, (1), p79-99. D’Entremont, B.D., & Yazbek, A. (2006). Imitation of Intention and Accidental Actions by Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, (9), p1665-1678. Freeman, J.S., Cody, F.W.J. & Schady, W. (1993). The influence of external timing cues upon the rhythm of voluntary movements in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 56, 1078-1084. Glazebrook, C.M., Elliott, D., and Szatmari, P., (2008). How do individuals with autism plan their movements? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, p114-126. Gowen, E., Stanley, J., & Miall, R.C., (2008). Movement interference in autism-spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46, (4), p1060-1068. Happe, F.G., (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, (3), p843-855. Hauge, T.S., & Hallan Tensberg, G.E. (1996). The musical nature of prelinguistic interaction. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 5, (2), p63-75. Kanner L (1943). "Autistic disturbances of affective contact". Nervous Child, 2: 217–50. Reprint (1968). Acta Paedopsychiatr, 35 (4): 100–36. Kjelgaard, M.M., Tager-Flusberg, H., (2001). An investigation of language impairment in Autism: Implications for genetic subgroups, 16, (2-3), p287-308. Leary, M., & Hill, D., (1996). Moving on: autism and movement disturbance. Mental Retardation, 14, p39-53. Lee, D. N. (1998) Guiding movement by coupling Taus. Ecological Psychology 10(3-4), 221250. 41 Lee, D. N. (2005). Tau in Action in Development. In: Rieser, J.J., Lockman, J. J., and Nelson, C.A. Eds. Action, Perception & Cognition in Learning and Development. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Lee, D. N., Craig, C. M. & Grealy, M. A. (1999). Sensory and intrinsic coordination of movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 266, 2029-2035. Lord, C. & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive development disorders, 2nd edition. New York: John Wiley. Malloch, S.N., (1999). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, 29-57. Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., & Wagner, G.C., (2007). Prevalence of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Development, 29, p565-570. Notordaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F.,& Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with specific speech and language disorder. European Children and Adolescent Psychology, 11, p219-225. Osteling, J., & Dawson, G., (2004). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, p247-257. Ostwald, P., Peltzman, P., Greenberg, M., & Meyer, J. (1970). Cries of a trisomy 13-15 infant. Development Medical Child neurology, 12, p472-477. Pierno, A.C., Mari, M., Glover, S., Georgiou, I., & Castiello, U., (2006). Failure to read motor intentions from gaze in children with autism. Neuropsychologia, 44, (8), p1483-1488. Schogler, B., Pepping, G-J., & Lee, D., (2008). TauG-guidance of transients in expressive musical performance. Experimental Brain Research, in print. 42 Siegal, M., & Blades, M., (2003). Language and auditory processing in autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7, (9), p378-380. Trevarthen, C., & Daniel, S., (2005). Disorganized rhythm and sychrony: Early signs of autism and Rett syndrome. Brain and Development, 27, p25-34. Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of autism developmental disorders, 30, p157-162. 43 Appendices 44 Appendix 1 Detailed description of the protocol used in the analysis stages. Video edit and audio transfer Review all clips and identify worthy sections. Take notes on any utterances and/or interactions. Edit out each clip tagging each file name as ex 1, ex 2, ex 3 (excerpt) using final cut pro. Export audio section using the same labelling protocol and save as mono.wav file 44100kHz. Wave processing Open wav. file using praat (read, read from file) Save file as a short text file (write, write to short text file (save in corresponding ex folder)). Right click open with ‘text edit’. Delete first 13 lines (header information). The first and last lines should be the start and end of the data, nothing else. Save the changes and close the text file. TauGUI Open Matlab, open the TauGUI folder in the workspace and type: TauGUI in the command window. TauGUI will then open in a new window. Go to file open Select the text file created from the wave file. Sample rate; 44000Hz Smoothing filter- 4, run the section seeker 45 Enter criteria: max duration- 0.5, min duration- 0.00005 Max amplitude- 1.5, min amplitude- 0.001 Run test Click select all Click analyse When finished it will come up ‘done’ at the bottom of the screen box. Go to file, export as text file TKA contour- extraction Copy and paste the appropriate columns into the wave worker spreadsheet. A, B, C, D, E, F, and G- and K, L, and N (not including the headers) Do not include direction or number of gaps or the velocity information. (make sure there are no blank rows at the bottom of the spreadsheet). Save excel sheet as ‘name’ waves.excel DynK- % criteria (enable macro’s) from this spreadsheet copy column (L) timesinglemotion- and (I) N_Percentage and (H) R2. Paste into Kaliedograph. Smooth the data: Go to Macros, Gauss Plot a scatterplot to examine % criteria: Go to Gallery, Linear, Plot Realtime TKA Copy the following columns from the waves spreadsheet: BWFcomposite (N), Time (O), Mean UPDOWN by BWF (S), MEAN AMP (T). Paste (paste special, values, move time column to A column) them into the delete balnk rows spreadsheet. 46 Ensure you only have numeric data (no headers or N/A data, delete whole rows if N/As come up) Go to tools- macros, macros, then click select delete blank rows, and run. Then save data as a TKA file (e.g. test01.tka.xls) There are now 4 columns of continous numeric information: Time, 1/T (waveduration/ frequency), dynK (k), and AMP (amplitude). Save TKA file. Copy and paste these into Kaleidagraph Smooth the data: Go to Macros, Gauss Sigma value of 4 (column 1=4, 2=5, 3=6, add in headings: 1/t_g4, k_g4, A_g4) Save Kaleidagraph file (click on file name and add ‘_g4’ to it and save). Create plots of 1/T, dynK and A and save these with the data (gallery, linear, scatter). Make sure appropriate scaling is applied. This is a good chance to compare the plots of non and smoothed data. Delete non-smoothed rows and columns. Go to save as format (at the bottom) and tab delimited to save as a text file. Open text file in text edit and remove any header information to ensure only numeruc information remains in the file (top and bottom). In Matlab, clear the workspace (make sure it is clear!) Import data (under the workspace) Open resample.m (in current directory, my folder) Edit lines 1,2,3,4 so they have the same name as the txt.file you are resampling (e.g. Line 1 “exact file name except extension”) 47 In line 8 (nsteps) you need to enter the number of new samples Which is a calculation: end time-start time/ 0.002= number of steps (for 500, 1000 would be= 0.001) Round the result to the nearest whole number. Click the save and run script button of the editor window (green play button) In the workspace you notice a bunch of new files The data has been resampled but you must now convert these into .txt files that can be analysed in the TauGUI. Go to File, close editor. In the command window execute the following commands replacing the name of your file where you see name. (e.g., Test01_TKA_g4T=[Dur2] Now export these individual files (one for T, one for K, and one for A) as .txt files using the following command line in the Matlab command window (e.g., save test01_TKA_g4T.txt test_TKA_g4T_ASCII). Save. You now have T, K, and A- contours for the appropriate section at the sample rate of 500Hz. Open each in the TauGUI- select 500Hz. And enter the appropriate values in the min and max these will be dictated by the data you have- i.e. if a vocalisation is very loud you will need a larger range in the gap size criteria. Suggested sigma 4. Use 5% velocity And min duration 0.02 (you should not go lower than 0.02 as for the recursive analysis 0.02 will provide 10 data points at 500Hz- if you wish to analyse smaller units then you must resample at a higher frequency). 48 Select the motion gaps you wish to analyse. Click analyse- the export as text. It is recommended that motion gaps identified are marked on the kaleidagraph plots produced earlier to facilitate interpretation. 49