Communication in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs): An inquiry, by means of a case study, into how a preschool specialist provision for children with ASDs interpets theoretical models of practice. Dimitra Tavulari Supervisor: Nick S. Hodge MA Education of Children and Young People with Autism School of Education, Sheffield Hallam University March 2004 Dimitra Tavulari Supervisor: N. S. Hodge Abstract: The aims of the study were to identify through a literature review recommended interventions for supporting the development of communication skills in children with autism; understand how staff, within a specific pre-school establishment, select, interpret and apply these methods and to evaluate how successful these are in contributing to the development of communication skills. The findings of the study show that a number of structured intervention strategies are used by the staff with apparent positive affect on the development of children’s communication and social skills. Also additional strategies are used by the staff inuitively but are not recognised by them as formal intervention methods of the nursery. The findings also show that the children appeared to be higly responsive to and motivated by these strategies suggesting they were received positively by them. TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction 1 Autism: Definition, Symptoms, Characteristics 4 Communication Impairments in Autism 9 Literature Review 15 The Case Study 41 Methodology 42 Data Analysis 50 Findings 51 Discussion of Outcomes 58 Conclusion 60 References 62 Appendix 1: Transcript of Interview 67 Appendix 2: Notes from Video Tapes 70 Appendix 3: IEPs 72 Aknowledgement I would like to take this opportunity and thank my supervisor Nick Hodge for his valuable help and advice in improving my work. I would also like to thank all the staff at the pre-school setting for supporting me and helping me throughout this project. Furthermore, I want to thank my close friend for being present at difficult time, and above all, my parents for supporting me all these years. Communication in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs): An inquiry, by means of a case study, into how a pre-school specialist provision for children with ASDs interpets theoretical models of practice INTRODUCTION Since the first identification of the condition of autism by Kanner (1943), many researchers (e.g. Koegel and Koegel, 1994 and Valdez-Menchaca, 1994) have suggested that the hallmarks of the condition, which lie at the heart of the autistic spectrum, are the communication and the social interaction deficit. Therefore, many educational interventions have focused on these two areas of development in particular, based upon the belief that almost all the other difficulties frequently observed in children with ASDs are a result of these communication-based difficulties. Different approaches have different principles and use different techniques and teaching methods to encourage the development of communication and social skills. Each one of them works differently for different individuals and therefore it is suggested that not one ''best'' approach that works well for everyone (NAS, 2001). However, the acquisition of effective communicative and social skills for children with ASDs is of huge importance. The significance of structure, early and intense intervention and collaborative working in educational settings, as well as the need to address core deficits of social emotional development, pragmatic aspects of communication, play skills and comprehension cannot be emphasised enough. Therefore the contribution of educational settings and especially pre-school educational settings to the communication, cognition and learning development of children with autism, is of particular value. They provide all the above features such as structure, communication and play skills, essential for the further development of children. The present study will identify and evaluate what contribution a pre-school establishment for children with ASDs, makes to the development of the communication and social skills of children with ASDs. Detailed data will be collected on two pupils. The aims of the study are to: Identify approaches, for the development of communication skills in children with ASDs, reccomended by the body of the literature identify the communication intervention procedures that are used in one particular setting understand how staff are applying these strategies understand the way in which these interventions might contribute to the communication development of children The mode of enquiry is a case study research design, involving staff and two children of the nursery as research participants. There were eight pupils at the nursery and the staff consisted of two nursery nurses and two speech therapy assistants. The role of the researcher was to observe and analyse as a participant observer. The findings of the study establish that: a number of structured intervention strategies are used by the staff; deliberately selected and that these appear to have an apparent positive effect on the development of children’s communication and social skills; that additional strategies were used by the staff intuitively but were not recognised by them as strategies and that the children appeared to be highly responsive to and motivated by these strategies suggesting they were received positively by them. The case study took place over a period of three months. It involved eight visits; one interview with the staff’s leader; several observations; analysis of video tapes of the early days of the children in the nursery, recorded by the staff and analysis of the Individual Education Plans (IEPs). The parents of the two selected children were aware of the study and gave their full permission. Furthermore, the names of the children were changed in order to protect their privacy. They will be named as Andy and Stewart. AUTISM: DEFINITION, SYMPTOMS, CHARACTERISTICS Autism has been defined as a condition since 1943 when Leo Kanner first described a number of features which identify children with this disorder. He described the defining features of autism as: - A profound autistic withdrawal - An obsessive desire for the preservation of sameness - A good rote memory - An intelligent and pensive expression - Mutism, or language without real communicative intent - Over-sensitivity to stimuli - A skilful relationship to objects (Jordan, 1999) Kanner explained that he referred to the work of Bleuler (1911) when choosing the word “autism” to describe these symptoms. Bleuler used the term to describe a withdrawal from previous participation, whereas the children Kanner described had never engaged in social interaction. As the information provided by Kanner’s observations on the nature of autism became more widely known, more children were referred for diagnosis. However, as more children were seen, it became clear that Kanner had only recognised one small group of children who fitted his particular criteria. The list of Kanner’s defining features was felt to be too limiting by those who were involved with diagnosing the syndrome. There were children for whom it was felt that a diagnosis of autism would be a useful descriptor yet who did not fit exactly into the criteria outlined by Kanner (Wing and Gould 1979). At about the same time, 1943, an Austrian psychiatrist, Hans Asperger, independently recognised a pattern of abnormal behaviour in a group of adolescents, which he chose to call “autistic psychopathy”. In 1971, Van Krevelin compared the two disorders and found that there were many similarities with Kanner’s subjects, but also significant differences. Unlike the children described by Kanner, those reported by Asperger had an adequate spoken vocabulary, with other abnormalities such as an obsessional interest in factual matters. Like the children described by Kanner, they had great difficulty in situations requiring two-way social interaction and communication. Essentially, they did not seem to be interested in or to understand the responses of other people. “Autistic” was also the term used by Asperger to describe his group. This condition bears his name now and is called Asperger syndrome. Since that time, other researchers have listed their own criteria, and for a number of years it was usual to diagnose autism by counting up a requisite number of points (Creak, 1964; Rendle-Short, 1971 cited in Newson, 1979; Rutter et al, 1971; Newson, 1979). Wing and Gould (1979) conducted research in Camberwell looking at all the children referred for psychiatric help. They were able to select a group of these children who had severe learning difficulties and were socially impaired, compared to others who had equally severe learning difficulties but without social impairment. They also found that three areas of development were associated with this social impairment, forming a cluster of features that provide diagnostic criteria for autism. These are known as the “Autism Triad of Impairments”: Impairment of social development this refers to impaired, developmentvariation deviant especially may be from and extremely interpersonal delayed social development. The “autistic aloofness” to “active but odd” characteristics Impairment of social communication refers to impaired and deviant language and communication, both verbal and non-verbal Impairment of social understanding and imagination refers to rigidity of thought and behaviour and impoverished social imagination. Also, ritualistic behaviour, reliance on routines and extreme delay or absence of “pretend play” Wing and Gould (1979) Wing and Gould identified a number of children with what they described as “Kanner’s syndrome” and a larger number of children who showed similarities to this group but did not fit Kanner’s criteria exactly. Thus in 1988, Wing went on to use the broader term “Autistic Spectrum”. The term spectrum has been useful in allowing for a broad definition of autism based on the triad of impairments and in capturing the idea of a range of manifestations of the same fundamental disorder. The manifestations of autism vary considerably among children and within an individual child over time. There is no single behaviour that is always typical of autism and no behaviour that would automatically exclude an individual child from a diagnosis of autism, even though there are strong commonalities, especially in social deficits. In general terms, children within the autistic spectrum display a highly unusual pattern of deficits and skills. Often, they exhibit difficulties in areas other than these of the Triad. Unusual responses to stimuli for example, such as oversensitivity to certain sounds, heat or cold, fascination with bright lights or objects and indifference to pain, may all occur in young children with autism (Jordan, 1999) and research has indicated that the majority of children with autism show hypersensitivities or atypical responses to sensory stimulation (O’Neill and Jones, 1997). Problems with sensory processing can cause distractibility, disorganization and discomfort, and the result might be a tendency to hyperfocus or be drawn to the same repetitive stimulus. Repetition is viewed, in part, as a means to create order amid chaos (Gradin, 1995; Williams, 1992). Bizarre behaviours such as abnormal body movements like repetitive arm flapping, rocking, spinning or jumping frequently observed in children within the spectrum may then be ways of creating order or self-stimulation. Additionally, research has suggested that children with autism exhibit overselectivity, which means that it is difficult for them to attend to the multiple features inherent in all stimuli (Lovaas, Koegel and Schreibman, 1979). They also have difficulty determining the most meaningful feature of a given stimulus (Frith and Baron-Cohen, 1987). Other studies report that children with ASDs have difficulty shifting attention between visual and auditory stimuli (Ciesielski, Courchesne and Elmasian, 1990; Courchesne, 1991). This impairs their ability to follow the rapid pace and complex features of social interaction. Children appear to struggle to attend to rapidly occurring sensory, language and social events. Children with autism also appear to suffer a severe difficulty in attributing mental states-such as intents, feelings and thoughts to themselves and to others in order to understand social behaviour- they seem to lack what is called a Theory of Mind (ToM) (Baron-Cohen, Leslie and Frith, 1985). The development of a ToM is of huge importance, because, according to TagerFlusberg (1997), it enables the child to interpret human behaviour within a casual-explanatory framework, and affords understanding that the mind does not simply copy reality but provides a representation of the world (p.135). Thus, it should be obvious that a representational view of the world is essential to the establishment of meaning and to effective communication overall. On the other hand, some children with autism may also display unusually high skills in some areas. Some of them appear to have truly savant abilities, especially in music, in art and in mathematical calculations. Their rote memory can be remarkably good and some of them may have extremely good reading skills. Some of them may develop huge encyclopaedic knowledge on a topic of their interest. These children, however, are in the minority. Usually, those who are most able may pursue a very narrow interest which may be very context-dependent and thus may have very limited value because the individual is unable exercise conscious control and adapt and apply his or her skill. Autism is in fact found at all IQ levels but is often accompanied by general learning difficulties and, the more profound the learning difficulties, the more likely that the individual will have autism. Such individuals may have a characteristic unevenness in their skill development but they seldom have abilities that are remarkable in themselves when compared to a normally developing population (Jordan, 1999). COMMUNICATION IMPAIRMENTS ASSOCIATED WITH AUTISM Communication difficulties are universally present in individuals with autism and problems in social communication are an early sign that a disability is present and may persist (Mundy, Sigman and Kasari, 1990). This has led many researchers (e.g. Koegel and Koegel, Valdez-Menchaca, 1994), to hypothesize that deficit in social communication or social interaction might be the primary underlying deficit of autism and that other behavioural problems are secondary as a result of these underlying communication-based difficulties. Over the past two decades, there have been major advances in understanding the communication and language deficits in children with ASDs. Until about 1980, peculiar speech patterns were emphasized such as echolalia, pronoun reversal, and unusual intonation (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). Nowadays, verbal and non-verbal communication is considered a core deficit in the diagnostic criteria for autistic spectrum disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1987; 1994). This change highlights the recognition of the fact that children with autism not only have difficulty in the acquisition of speech and language, but also in understanding and using non-verbal behaviour in communicative interactions. The level of communicative competence attained by children with autism has been found to be an important predictor of more positive outcomes (Garfin and Lord, 1986; McEachin, Smith and Lovaas, 1993). The presence of fluent speech, defined as using multiword combinations spontaneously, communicatively and regularly, before the age of 5 is a good prognostic indicator of subsequent IQ scores, language measures, adaptive skills and academic achievement in adolescence (Lord and Paul, 1997). There is much heterogeneity in the speech, language and communication characteristics of children within the autistic spectrum. There is a huge variation in their language and communication abilities, ranging from children who are non-verbal to children who have developed fluent speech but who find difficulty with using their language to communicate effectively. For those children who develop speech, echolalia is a frequent phenomenon. Echolalia refers to the repetition of utterances heard in the immediate or distant past. Whereas imitative repetition is a natural part of typically developing children’s language, its persistence past the first few years of language learning is used as one of the diagnostic criteria for autism. In addition, in contrast to neurotypical language learners, children with autism lack clear evidence of communicative intent in their echolalic utterances (Koegel, 1995). Echolalia can be used to initiate communicative acts (e.g. requesting, calling or protesting) or maintain communicative exchanges (e.g. turn-taking or providing information) (Quill, 2000). It often appears to be produced as a method of avoiding appropriate language. For example, if an adult asks a child “Do you want milk?” and the child responds “milk” s/he may appear to be using appropriate language but in reality the child may be simply repeating the last portion of the previous utterance. It might be that the child is doing their best to do what is expected, thus, just that repeating the last thing they have heard is the best that they can do at that instance. Lovaas, Varni, Koegel and Lorsch, (1977) have also suggetsted that delayed echolalia, is usually emitted as self-stimulatory behaviour, apparently to provide some sort of sensory input. Another aspect of the language ofchildren with ASDs that appears to be impaired is the use of pronouns. Many children with ASDs make pronoun reversal errors, referring to themselves as “you” and to other people as “I” or “me” (Sigman, Ungerer, Mundy and Sherman, 1987). When Kanner originally observed pronoun reversals in his patients, he attributed it to a form of echolalia. He assumed that children with ASD repeated what they heard last and thus did not always changed the pronouns appropriately. However, psychoanalytic explanations (Despert, 1951, Bettelheim, 1967) saw the pronoun reversal errors as a child’s inability to separate himself/herself from others. According to Baron-Cohen (1989), the pronoun errors are directly linked to the children’s lack of Theory of Mind (ToM). He argues that if children with autism lack a ToM they confuse discourse roles because they cannot understand that individuals might have different conceptual perspectives. Another frequent characteristic of the communication of children within the autistic spectrum is the over-literalness of their language. They often display great difficulties with the understanding of intentions behind speech. Thus they have huge problems with understanding metaphors, similes, humour, sarcasm or irony. A characteristic example of such over-literalness is that of responding to questions like “can you pass the salt?” by simply “yes” or the little boy who runs to the window and is looking for cats and dogs when his mother says “It’s raining cats and dogs”. According to Baron-Cohen (1989) this again is directly linked to their lack of a ToM. Since they lack of ToM they are unable to understand the hidden meaning behind speech and therefore get very confused and upset. Paralinguistic features such as vocal quality, intonation, pitch, and stress patterns are other often deviant speech characteristics of verbal children with autism (Tager-Flusberg, 1990). Children with ASDs are frequently described as speaking in a “monotone”, and their speech may sound unnatural, or “robot like”. They lack the ability to express the emotional intensity such as surprise, emphasis, anger, or excitement when communicating a message, and they also lack the ability to recognise such cues in a speaker’s voice. All these intonational phenomena however, are also used to mark topic/comment relations that aid listeners in processing discourse elements. For example, they can signal that sentences marked as assertions are in fact questions (i.e. “You’re going home now?”); and they help to segment longer strips of discourse into processable units (Paul, 1987). Thus, failure to use and appreciate intonational cues, will not only affect the emotional tone of an utterance but it will also hamper its comprehensibility. Moreover, children with ASDs, may also exhibit repetitive stereotyped, inflexible and often idiosyncratic use of words and phrases, immaturity of grammatical structure of spontaneous speech and problems in sequencing and in understanding meaning (Paul, 1987). Communication impairments in children with ASDs are also present in the nonverbal domain. The understanding, as well as the use of non-verbal cues such as gestures, facial expressions, eye contact or bodily posture can be severely affected in children with ASDs. Some of them predominantly use primitive motoric gestures to communicate, for example leading, pulling or manipulating an adult’s hand; and some also lack the use of many conventional gestures such as waving, nodding and symbolic gestures depicting actions (Loveland and Laundry, 1986; Stone and Caro-Martinez, 1990; Stone et al, 1997). Eye contact can often be inappropriate, being too fixed at some times and averted at other times when it would be socially appropriate to make contact (Atwood, 1987). Facial expressions as well are often an area of difficulty for children with ASDs. Some of them appear not to recognize facial expressions that declare happiness, fear, anger etc. In addition, their use of facial expressions can sometimes be impaired. According to Koegel (1995), the communicative difficulties, both in the verbal and in the non-verbal domain, that are common in children with autism, as well as the lack of sufficient language development, can lead to a significant disability in the area of pragmatic competence. Pragmatics is a component of language central to effective communication. It includes ways of using language to convey different speech acts such as requests, assertions, or promises; the appropriate use of language in specific contexts such as dialogue, monologue, or narrative; and also structuring utterances to distinguish between already given and new information (Sperber and Wilson, 1986). Moreover, the area of pragmatics include several aspects related to topic, such as selection, introduction, maintenance and change (Prutting and Kirchner, 1987) and turn-taking, including initiation, response, pause time, interruption/overlap, feedback to speakers, contingency and politeness (Koegel, 1995). Children with autism very often violate the unwritten rules of pragmatics. They can, for example, introduce their favourite topic and talk excessively about it without taking into account their listener’s needs. They may ignore the rules of turn-taking, be unable to understand how long it is appropriate to talk about the same topic or that their listener may be bored or might need additional information in order to understand. Non-verbal aspects may include such behaviours as eye gaze or proximity between the communicative partners. These rules can also be corrupted by children with autism. Furthermore, tantrums, aggression and other avoidance or attention-seeking behaviours often displayed by children with ASDs are also included in the pragmatics area if they are used for communicative purposes (Koegel, 1995). In general terms, research since the 1980s has identified communication deficits in children with autism that fall into two major areas: the capacity for joint attention, which reflects difficulty coordinating attention between people and objects, and the capacity for symbol use, which reflects difficulty learning conventional or shared meanings for symbols and is evident in acquiring language as well as symbolic play (Wetherby, Prizant and Schuler, 2000). From all the above, it will be apparent that what is distinctive about the communication of children with autism is that it reflects the cognitive and social impairments associated with the disorder (Aarons and Gittens, 1992). For both verbal and non-verbal individuals, impairments in social aspects of language and related cognitive skills are the most salient (Lord and Paul, 1997; Wetherby, Schuler and Prizant, 1997). Non-autistic children with a language disorder will certainly show difficulties with the understanding as well as the expression of language but, although probably immature, their social development is not deviant. They are likely to find alternative means of expressing themselves and are capable of engaging in the sequence of attracting attention, and sharing their varied interests. Children with autism on the other hand, may even not be aware of the valuable role of communication in expressing their needs and wants and in being an active part of a social world. This emphasizes the importance of communication intervention as a primary goal in the education of children with ASDs in order to improve their quality of life as much as possible. COMMUNICATION INTERVENTIONS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM The complexities of social and communication development and the significant diversity that is observed in children with a diagnosis of ASD led researchers (e.g. Prizant, Wetherby and Rydell, 2000; Quill, 2000) to conceptualize approaches to enhancing language and communication abilities along a continuum, with traditional behavioural approaches such as discrete trialtraditional behavioural approaches at one end (Lovaas, 1977, 1981) and developmental social-pragmatic approaches at the other end, which include “relationship-based” approaches that are individualized and grounded in a transactional model (Greenspan, 1992; 1997; Greenspan and Wieder, 1998; MacDonald, 1989; Prizant, Schuler, Wetherby and Rydell, 1997). Contemporary behavioural approaches fall between these two ends of the continuum, incorporating aspects of each (Warren, 1993). Traditional behavioural approaches in the 1970’s used teaching procedures which were characterized by a one-to-one massed trial drill format to train early “readiness” skills in eye contact, attention, and sitting followed by more advanced skills in matching, verbal imitation, receptive and expressive language, play and so forth. These approaches received much attention, mostly because they were the first to objectively demonstrate that children with autism were capable of acquiring a variety of skills through systematic teaching methods (Quill, 2000). According to Lovaas (1981), the justification for a highly repetitive one-to-one approach was the belief that children with autism were not able to learn in more natural environments because of their extreme learning and attentional difficulties and the lack of practice opportunities and systematic reinforcement in more natural events. However, in the 1980s, the movement of developmental pragmatics (i.e. the study of language and communication development in social contexts) (Bricker, 1993), engendered a number of principles, that guided clinical and educational practice with children with ASDs and other severe communication disabilities, which appeared to be antithetical to traditional behaviour approaches in theory and in practice (Prizant, Wetherby and Rydell, 2000). For example, the social context of naturally occurring interactions was considered to be of primary importance for communication and language development. Also, the child was viewed as an active learner and social participant rather as a learner primarily under the control of the teacher. Furthermore, the developmental pragmatics movement emphasized the importance of deriving individualized goals and strategies in communication abilities as well as learning strengths and needs (Prizant and Wetherby, 1989; 1990b; Wetherby and Prizant, 1992). This was in contrast to programmes using the same sequence of goals and teaching curriculum, as was common in some traditional behavioural approaches, especially in early stages of training (Bricker, 1993). Finally, the pragmatics movement has emphasized the need to focus on meaningful preverbal and verbal language and functional communication abilities at the onset, rather than to build repertoires of speech sounds, words and sentences largely devoid of conceptual understanding and social impact (Prizant, Wetherby and Rydell, 2000). Nowadays, several viewpoints exist among the professional community about the social and communication skills intervention. Dawson and Osterling (1997) reviewed eight early intervention programmes for preschool children with autism, ranging from intensive, one-to-one discrete trial approaches, to programmes in inclusive educational settings using naturalistic procedures. They concluded that the level of success achieved across these programmes was fairly similar. The level of success was determined based on changes in measures such as IQ scores and academic achievement. These conclusions provide important implications for intervention programmes and directions for future research. Firstly, there is no evidence indicating that one approach works better than others, and therefore, caution is reasonable in drawing conclusions about intervention efficacy. Secondly, given that impaired social and communication development is the hallmark of autism, a true understanding of the “best” means to support social and communication development in children with ASDs has not been empirically studied, and thus, is unknown. There is a need to recognise which specific intervention methods work best to accomplish which goals, for which children. According to Quill (2000), an understanding of best practices to enhance social skills and communication in children with autism, it seems is just beginning. Moreover, the combination of beneficial components of behavioural technology with pragmatic developmental principles is likely to be the logical way to enhance social and communicative competence in children with ASDs. Approaches that combine behavioural and developmental principles incorporate specificity of goals and objectives, promote the child’s level of motivation and interest, use developmental activities, use instructional cues and prompts in a systematic manner, and emphasize meaningful interactions within the context of adult structured, organized learning environments (Quill, 2000). This blend of behavioural and developmental principles to augment social and communication skills is described in many popular approaches used for children with various developmental disabilities and/or children with autism. This study will describe some communication intervention approaches that come from traditional ABA procedures, however modified to include developmental, more child-centred principles, and some more developmental social-pragmatic approaches which however include elements of traditional ABA techniques. The selection of these approaches was made in order to show the contrast between more behavioural to more developmental programmes and because the pre-school establishment participating in this research, uses most of the more developmental social-pragmatics ones. Incidental Teaching (Hart and Risley, 1982) Pivotal Response Training (Koegel and Schreibman, 1991) TEACCH (Treatment and Education of Autistic and related Communication Handicapped Children; Schopler and Mesibov, 1985, 1986) Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS, Bondy and Frost, 1994) Proximal Communication Approach (Whittaker, 1996) Musical Interaction (Prevezer and Chandler, 1998) FROM ABA TO INCIDENTAL TEACHING Behavioural methods have been used in the education of children with autism and severe learning difficulties for many years, and as such, applied behavioural analysis (ABA) is not autism specific (Wallin, 2000). The primary teaching method of ABA is discrete trial. A discrete trial is a single cycle of behaviourally based instruction routine. Every skill the child with autism does not demonstrate-from relatively simple responses like looking at others, to complex acts like social interaction-is broken down into small steps. Each step is taught (usually into on-to-one teaching situations, to begin with) by presenting a specific cue or instruction (Green, 1996). Sometimes, a prompt may be added to help the child respond correctly. Appropriate responses are followed by consequences that have been found to function effectively as reinforcers-that is, when those consequences have consistently followed the child’s response it has been shown that the response was likely to occur again. Problematic responses, such as tantrums, stereotypies, self-injury or withdrawal, are explicitly not reinforced, which often requires systematic analyses to determine exactly what events function as reinforcers for those responses (Green, 1996). Discrete trial is so widely known in the treatment of autistic spectrum disorders, that many regard it as synonymous with ABA. However, ABA is not defined by any single intervention procedure (Fenske, Krantz and McClannahan, 2001). Although discrete trial training has been found effective in building language and other skills in children with autism (Lovaas, 1977, 1981; Wolf, Risley and Mees, 1964), it is only one of the procedures that may be used to promote verbal and other behaviour and it has certain limitations. For example, the structured learning environment that occurs typically with discrete trial training, may fail to promote generalization of skills across situations (Anderson, Taras and Cannon, 1996). Incidental Teaching, although based on traditional ABA, uses techniques that promote generalization and spontaneous use of emerging skills, (Hart and Risley, 1968, McGee, Krantz, Mason and McClannahan, 1983). According to Hart and Risley (1982), incidental teaching is used to get elaborated language by waiting for another person to initiate conversation about a topic and then responding in ways that ask for more language for that person (p.5). There are four steps involved in incidental teaching, which include: arranging a setting that contains materials of interest to the child, waiting for the child to initiate an interaction about an object of interest, asking for more elaborate language or approximations to speech and providing the object for which the child initiated the interaction. Children’s initiations of interactions are reflective of their current language abilities. Children who have acquired productive language may initiate for items or activities with phrases, labels or word approximations (i.e. “m” to signify “more”). However, even non verbal children can be candidates for incidental teaching. They can initiate interactions by reaching for, pointing to or gesturing towards activities or objects of interest (Fenske, Krantz and McClannahan, 2001). When selecting target skills to teach, it is important to consider the child’s current language repertoire. Incidental teaching helps children take the next steps, but must not request skills that are presently out of their reach. Furthermore, teaching is more effective and children progress more rapidly if teachers select only one or a few language targets at a time, instead of trying to teach multiple responses (Fenske, Krantz and McClannahan, 2001). For example, a teacher might initially target the pronouns “I” and “you” and reserve instruction on the use of other pronouns until the child masters these. The procedure of incidental teaching is effective for shaping new language skills even before children learn to imitate verbal models or follow adults’ directions. A child, for example, who initiates interaction by pulling an adult towards a preferred item, presents opportunity for incidental teaching. An initial instructional goal could be “Point to the ………” or “Do you want ……..?” Moreover, opportunities to promote initiations of interactions for children, can be maximized by arranging the children’s usual environments in such ways that they encourage initiations; favourite snacks, toys, books can be put on visible places but out of reach of children, so that they are likely to evoke initiations. Fenske, Krantz, and McClannahan (2001), have suggested several ways other than controlling access to materials for adults to increase children’s initiations: play with toys that are of special interest, set up repetitive play situations, withhold materials needed to pursue activities, display photographs of preferred activities, begin favourite activities and then pause, glance at the materials and then look expectantly at the child, and move the materials closer to the child. After the child initiates an interaction, the adult’s request for a more elaborate response should cue the child to display the language skill that has been identified as the target for instruction. For example, when a child who is learning to imitate words reaches for a cracker, the teacher moves the cracker beyond his/her reach, models the word “cracker” and provides access to it only when the child imitates the target word. Later, words can be expanded to phrases (e.g. “Say cracker please”) and phrases can be expanded to sentences or questions (e.g. “Say can I have a cracker?”) It is important to note here that requests for elaboration are prompts. Some prompts however can be quite general like “what do you want?” and do not indicate the correct responses. If the child does not respond or respond incorrectly, the adult may provide a more specific prompt such as “say I want cracker”. The decision about the type of prompt to be used depends on the child’s language repertoire and on data based on his/her performance (Fenske, Krantz and McClannahan, 2001). After a teacher requests an elaboration, a child may respond appropriately, may respond incorrectly or incompletely or may not respond at all. If the child provides a correct elaboration, the adult confirms that s/he is correct and immediately provides the object or activity of interest. If the child does not respond correctly or does not respond at all the adult models the correct response and delivers the item only if the child imitates the verbal model. Incidental teaching occurs only if the adult provides the child with the requested object or activity. There is also a possibility for the child to display problem behaviours during incidental teaching. In that case, the adult should direct the child to another activity or should use other procedures that have been proven effective for reducing challenging behaviours for the particular child; but should not provide the item for which the child initiated. In this way, the danger for the child to learn that crying and screaming are effective ways of communicating with others is avoided. To conclude, incidental teaching has been proven to be an effective behavioural educational intervention for children with ASDs (Hart and Risley, 1968, 1982; McGee, Krantz, Mason and McClannahan, 1983; Anderson, Taras and Cannon, 1996; Fenske, Krantz and McClannahan, 2001). In contrast to discrete trial training it is conducted on the child’s natural environment and this helps to the generalization of skills learned in several situations. Furthermore, incidental teaching typically involves child-directed activities which are likely to be more meaningful and interesting for the child than adult-directed activities, thus motivating the child to interact more frequently. In addition, the materials in incidental teaching are chosen by the children and the rewards for correct responses are the same materials for which the children initiated the interaction. This may help children understand the valuable meaning of communication with others, since they receive what they communicated for, contrary to discrete trial where children may receive totally irrelevant with the learning activity rewards. Nevertheless, both discrete trial and incidental teaching are effective ABA programmes and should be used at different times depending upon what specific skill is being taught as well as the child’s ability. For example, it may be necessary to teach some vocabulary to a distractible child who needs the highly structured environment and the frequent repetition that discrete trial provides. Then, once a small vocabulary has been acquired, incidental teaching should be used to make the skill functional. According to Reichle and Keogh (1985), and Sundberg and Partington (1999), both are effective for teaching receptive and expressive language skills to children with ASDs and both are important components of a language curriculum. However, it seems that incidental teaching is more appropriate for verbal children with ASDs, although researchers support the view that non-verbal children can also initiate interactions. It seems though that non verbal children will need much more time to reach the point where more elaborated language is requested from them. PIVOTAL RESPONSE TRAINING Another educational intervention programme for children with autism, which comes from a discrete trial applied behavioural analysis approach, is the Pivotal Response Training Model. It started in 1979 at the University of California at Santa Barbara with the ultimate goal to provide children with ASDs with the social and educational proficiency to participate in inclusive settings. In early stages this model used discrete trial procedures, but there has been a shift towards use of more naturalistic behavioural interventions (National Research Council, 2001). The overriding strategy of the Pivotal Response Model is to aim at change in certain pivotal areas, such as responsiveness to multiple cues and motivation (Koegel et al 1999). According to Koegel and Frea (1993), change in these pivotal behaviours may lead to important collateral changes in a broader range of atypical behaviours within the syndrome of autism. Since these behaviours are central to a wide area of functioning, positive changes in these behaviours should have widespread effects on other behaviours. For example, research suggests that if one can affect joint attention development in children, then this change may be expected to contribute to the development of symbolic abilities (Hobson, 1993; Mundy et al, 1993), the development of language abilities (Baldwin, 1995; Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni and Volterra, 1979) and the development of general social-cognitive processes (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Mundy, 1995; Tomasello, 1995). According to Stone (1997), and to Rogers and Pennington (1991), the idea of pivotal skills to be targeted as goals may also hold for behaviours such as imitation, maintaining proximity to peers (Hanson and Odom, 1999) and learning to delay gratification. Similarly to incidental teaching, pivotal response training uses systematic teaching trials that are initiated by the child, and focus on the child’s interests; they are interspersed and embedded in the natural environment and they use natural reinforcers that follow what the child is trying to communicate. Furthermore, pivotal response training uses multiple components in an interaction. For example, during language training, a teacher can teach the same verb but use two different objects (i.e. “roll car” and then “roll ball”). Multiple components also mean using adjectives to describe objects like “red car” or “big ball” versus just “car” and “ball”. This is done in order to increase children’s responsiveness to multiple cues (www.spectrumcentre.org) Generally, the pivotal response training like the incidental teaching and others contemporary ABA approaches, such as Natural Language Paradigm (Koegel and Johnson, 1989; Schreibman and Pierce, 1993) and enhanced milieu teaching approaches (Kaiser, Yoder and Keetz, 1992) were developed as methods of achieving a more naturalistic approach to enhancing language and communication development for children with autism and other childhood communication disabilities. The focus and ultimate goal of all of these approaches are to facilitate spontaneous communication and interaction. Childpreferred and child-selected activities provide the primary contexts and topics for communicative exchange (Schreibman and Pierce, 1993), with the adult being highly responsive and following the child’s lead, so that interactions are more natural and loosely structured than in traditional ABA discrete trial programmes. TREATMENT AND EDUCATION OF AUTISTIC AND OTHER COMMUNICATION RELATED HANDICAPPED CHILDREN (TEACCH) A developmental social-pragmatic intervention for children with ASDs, which also uses elements of behavioural methods, is TEACCH. TEACCH is a whole life approach in helping people with autism, which aims to equip children for a productive life in the community. It sets out to provide visual information, structure and predictability. It was devised in 1966 by Eric Schopler and his colleagues. The approach requires that adaptations must occur in the three major areas of the child’s life: home, school and community. The major priorities include centering on the individual, understanding autism, adopting appropriate behaviours, and a broadly-based intervention strategy building on existing skills and interests (Mesibov, 1999). Structured teaching is an important priority of the TEACCH approach. Organizing the physical environment, developing schedules and work systems, making expectations clear and explicit, and using visual materials have been effective ways of developing skills and allowing children with autism to use these skills independently of direct adult prompting and cueing (Mesibov, 1999). Cultivating strengths and interests is another important priority. TEACCH suggests that capitalising the children’s interests, helps increase their motivation and understanding of what they are doing. An important part of any TEACCH curriculum is developing communication skills, pursuing social and leisure interests, and encouraging people with autism to pursue more of these opportunities (Schopler and Mesibov, 1994). The TEACCH programme employs a number of educational techniques, such as physical organisation, schedules, visual structure and routines, which have been proven to help develop skills and minimise behavioural problems (Mesibov, 1994). Providing structure and organisation in the classroom or any other learning environment on a students’ level of understanding can help to alleviate or moderate these problems and the resulted ineffective learning situations. The first component of structured teaching that the TEACCH approach adopts is the structure of the physical environment. This refers to the way in which the environment is organised. The physical layout of the classroom is an important consideration when planning learning experiences for autistic students. Even the arrangement of the classroom furniture can help or hinder a student’s independent functioning and his/her recognition and compliance with rules and limits (Marcus, 2002). Many students with ASDs have organisational problems not knowing where to be and how to get there by the most direct route. Structuring the environment gives them visual cues to help them understand. Physical organisation might also involve structuring the environment so it is not distracting. Lighting, noise, size of the room and distracting wall spaces are some of the features that attention is given to (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). Having specific areas for learning specific tasks, marking clear boundaries and making materials easily accessible, helps children independently know where they are supposed to be and where to get their own materials. Establishing areas in the classroom begins with the natural setting. For example, work areas are avoided to be set up near distracting windows or mirrors. It is beneficial to have work areas near shelves or storage, so that materials are easily accessible (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). Blank walls are also good to build a work area around. Students’ tables face the blank walls and some distractions are thus eliminated. Additionally, several materials of the classroom such as rugs, bookshelves, partitions, tape on the floor or arrangement of the tables are used to mark clear boundaries. For example, a work area may be outlined by shelves and work tables; or the carpeted area may indicate the leisure area; a small rug in front of the sink may be used to show students where to stand when they are washing their hands or when washing dishes. Materials are also clearly marked. Pictures, colour coding, or numbers are used to help students label and obtain or put away materials by themselves. However, along with the physical structure of the environment, TEACCH considers the individuals needs of students as well. For example three work tables could be used to provide the structure needs of different students. One table could be placed in the middle of the class, for students who are not easily distracted by other people’s activities; another table could be placed facing a blank wall and pieces of tape on the floor can be used to show where chairs should be while working. This could be for students who are more easily distracted and tend to wonder around when they are not busy working. The third work table could be placed facing a blank wall and is parted on two sides with dividers, and would be for students who are easily distracted from what others are doing and who might exhibit behaviours that are disturbing to others while working (www.teacch.com/structur.htm) Another component of structured teaching is scheduling. This tells the students visually what activities will occur and in which order. Using objects, photos, pictures or numbers, depending on children’s developmental level, children are helped to understand a sequence of events (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). Schedules can help children organise and predict daily and weekly events. Besides knowing what activity will happen during a time period, a schedule lets them know where they should go next. Also, children with low initiative may be more motivated to complete a difficult or dreaded task if they see on their schedule that it will be followed by a more enjoyable task or activity. There are usually two types of schedules being used simultaneously in classrooms. The first type is the general overall classroom schedule. The second type is the individual student schedule. The overall classroom schedule outlines the events of the day. It does not specify work activities for students but does show general work times, break times, lunch time etc. The general classroom schedule should be posted somewhere in the classroom for all to be able to see and use. The format may be written or for students who do not comprehend written language can be made by pictures or drawings. During the work times of the general schedule, teachers and students might be involved in a variety of activities. These are reflected in the individual student schedules. These, can take a variety of forms but must be individually oriented; i.e. age appropriate, balanced with difficult and enjoyable activities, based on the child’s level of comprehension and based on a student’s endurance level; in terms of how often reinforcement or change in activity is needed (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). As students learn to comprehend and follow schedules, they develop good independent functioning skills both of which are very important skills to have for successful functioning in future placements like vocationally and residentially (Marcus, 2002). Another way structure is used to help children function successfully is in the setting up of teaching tasks. Direction of all tasks and the use of prompts and reinforcers are organised and systematic in order to build success experiences for students. This makes learning situations more predictable for students and help to overcome distractibility, resistance to change, and lack of motivation (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). Directions can be given to children verbally or non-verbally according to the child’s level of understanding. For verbal directions, this means using the minimum amount of language needed. Furthermore, they are also accompanied with gestures to help children understand better. Non-verbal directions are given with contextual visual cues, like systematically presenting and positioning materials, and using jigs and written instructions. Setting up a students work from left to right for example, gives an organised and systematic base for the student to work, completing tasks more easily without as many verbal instructions. Placing materials in the setting where they will be used can also help a student complete directions and complete tasks more successfully. Jigs and written instructions also help a student to get and stay organised while working. Samples of pictures of finished products are used to show students what needs to be done. Moreover, pictures and written instructions are used to help a student complete a sequential task in the right order (www.teacch.com/structur.htm). Prompts are also used to help children be successful in what they are learning and doing. The TEACCH approach also takes into account that students with ASDs are usually not motivated by social reinforcements. Thus, teachers need to discover reinforcements that the children understand and enjoy. However, in order for the teacher to use reinforcement as an effective teaching tool, s/he must be systematic in the use of it. The type and frequency of reinforcement for individual students are planned prior to activities. The teacher also needs to make sure the reinforcing consequence immediately follows the skill been learned or increased so that the relationship between the two is clear to the student (www.teacch.com/structur.htm) Significant improvement in appropriate behaviour and communication are the main reported benefits of TEACCH (Cumine, Leach and Stevenson, 2000). Some may feel that TEACCH over-structures children, limiting their decision making and creativity. However, flexibility is encouraged within the structured framework, particularly in developing problem-solving skills. PICTURE EXCHANGE COMMUNICATION SYSTEM (PECS) The Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) is an augmentative communication system developed to help children and adults with autism and other developmental disabilities, to rapidly acquire functional communication skills (Bondy and Frost, 1994a). It is one of the intervention approaches for ASDs which integrates theoretical and practical perspectives from the fields of ABA and speech and language pathology. Several techniques developed within ABA are used as the fundamental teaching strategies. For example, to implement PECS, the teacher must first identify powerful reinforcers for which a child will be motivated to communicate. Teaching strategies use a variety of prompting, shaping, and fading techniques to gradually improve and modify the way children use the system. Techniques are drawn from discrete-trial and sequential formats, as well as incidental teaching formats (Bondy and Frost, 1994). PECS is an appropriate programme for children who do not use speech or who may speak with limited communication effectiveness. Children using PECS are taught to give a picture of a desired item to a communicative partner in exchange of the item. By doing so, the child initiates a communicative act for a concrete outcome within a social context (Wallin, 2002). There are typically six phases involved in the teaching of PECS use. While these phases should be approached and taught sequentially, there may be times when a child is working on two or more phases simultaneously (Wallin, 2002). During phase one, the lesson taught is to spontaneously request items and activities. To do this, the teacher first needs to identify what items the child persistently wants, those things for which s/he would be willing to make a request. When preferences emerge, two adults start working with the child at this initial stage. The first adult entices the child with an object s/he really likes. The role of the second adult is to stay behind the child and wait for him/her to reach for the item and then to physically assist the child to pick up the picture of that item and hand it to the first adult. When the first adult receives the picture s/he immediately gives the child the reward, along with an appropriate comment (e.g. “Oh, you want the candy!”). As soon as possible the physical assistance from the second adult should be faded out until the child is exchanging a picture for the item independently with the first adult. Because the goal for the child is to initiate communication, rather than to simply respond to requests, it is very important not to ask the child what s/he wants or to use other verbal prompts. The goal is for the child to seek a communicative partner, from the start (www.polyxo.com:PECS) After the child can reliably exchange a picture for a requesting object, the move is made into the second phase. The child is now encouraged to use greater spontaneity and persistence, and to generalise the skill s/he has acquired. The child continues to request motivating items and activities, but now is required to move a longer distance to get to the communication partner or to get to the picture. S/he also begins to make requests in settings different from those in which s/he was taught the initial phase (i.e. different rooms, at the local store, at the grandparents’ house etc) and with a variety of different people. Furthermore, the child begins to expand his/her vocabulary of symbols, requesting different reinforcing objects or activities. According to Bondy and Frost (1994), the successful completion of phase two is often vital if the child is to be a spontaneous communicator. In the third phase of PECS, the child is beginning to be asked to discriminate between a number of items on a board, making choices as to what items s/he may want, to try. The child begins by answering questions like “what do you want?” but these are faded quickly so the child will make choices spontaneously as well as in response to questions. If discrimination is a new skill for the child, the teacher uses just two items and as the child becomes more comfortable with discriminations more items can be added. At this point of the training, several technical decisions are to be made, such as whether to reduce the size of the pictures, change the format of the board and so forth (Ryan, 1990). Once the child is able to discriminating and making requests for a variety of items, to a variety of people and in a variety of environments, phase four begins, which focuses on sentence structure. The child will start combining a picture for “I want” with the picture of the requested item or activity. The two pictures are attached to a sentence strip and the entire strip is exchanged with the adult for the item or activity (www.polyxo.com:PECS) The fifth and sixth phases occur at the same time, focusing on different extensions of the child’s skill with picture exchange. The fifth phase extends the sentence structure begun in phase four. Adjectives and other words can be added to the child’s repertoire to encourage him/her further refine his/her requests. The sixth phase is considered to be a fundamental shift in the child’s communication and the expected outcome from the teachers or peers. Through the use of pictures for “I see”, “I hear”, “I feel” etc the child is able to comment on elements of his/her environment. The PECS system has gained widespread use internationally and is appealing for several reasons (Siegel, 2000; Yamall, 2000). Each exchange is clearly intentional and readily understood. From the start, communication is initiated by the child and is always meaningful and highly motivating for the child. Also, the materials used are cheap, easy to prepare and portable. Furthermore, several informational reports have suggested that a large number of children who learn PECS, also develop spoken language (Bondy and Frost, 1994; Shaartz Garfinkle and Bauer, 1998). Moreover, anecdotal reports have indicated that the use of PECS may result in a decrease in problem behaviour and improved social behaviour (Bondy and Frost, 1994; Peterson, Bondy, Vincent and Finnegan, 1995). PROXIMAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH According to Whittaker (1996), the term Proximal Communication is applied to a range of non-verbal techniques that adults can use to engage children with autism in social interactions. It is an autism specific interactional approach based upon the social strengths of children. Whittaker (1996) and Whittaker and Reynolds (2000), identified children's strengths in non-verbal communication which include: approaching adults who were passive and did not talk, maintaining proximity to adults in situations where no toys or objects were being used, reaching out to adults after being tickled to ask for more, and so forth. The aim of Proximal Communication is to engage the children in playful and pleasurable non-verbal interaction to develop their early social skills (Potter and Whittaker, 2001). One of the major strategies used in the proximal communication approach is the burst/pause activity. The adult does something the child appears to enjoy-the active, burst phase. The adult then stops and waits for the child to communicate that s/he wants the activity to start again-the passive, pause phase. If the child is not exhibiting intentional communication, the adult treats a chosen aspect of the child's behaviour as if it was intentional. For example, the adult may tickle the child and then stop and wait for the child to communicate that s/he wants more. As soon as the child looks at the adult, s/he immediately tickles again so that the child realizes that it was the looking at the adult that caused him/her to repeat the tickling. Once the child has realized this, then s/he begins to look at the adult, expecting that the look will have particular effect and finally s/he looks in order to achieve that effect. Based on their knowledge of the child, the adults must decide which behaviour they want to shape as communication. Potter and Whittaker (2001) have identified several such behaviours like hand signalling for example. Another major strategy that the proximal communication approach adopts is the minimal speech approach. This means that adults should consistently use one or two relevant concrete words when interacting with children who understand little speech. According to Potter and Whittaker (2001), such an approach can be highly effective and is very important in the creation of more communicationenabling environments for children with autism. Furthermore, such an approach, in conjunction with strategies such as the visual cues, and adult use of pointing and multipointing, is likely to enable children to progress in their understanding of individual words (Potter and Whittaker, 2001). Appropriate rough and tumble play is also a strategy frequently used in proximal communication. Rough and tumble is a term that covers a broad range of playful interaction behaviours generally involving large body movements and some form of physical contact. Observation of the child prior to proximal communication sessions will enable the adult to decide about how much physical activity the child would enjoy. The important thing here is not the actual running or chasing but rather, a slow motion chase. Exaggerated movements and facial expressions from the adult, help on the building up of the anticipation of the child for the final tickle. Potter and Whittaker (2001) also suggest that adults should make use of nonspeech vocalizations in imitation of the child's sounds. They point out that this is generally effective in engaging children in proximal communication sessions. By using bursts of vocal imitation, followed by pauses, some children could be engaged in a non-speech vocal dialogue, with the amount of vocalization used by the children increasing significantly compared to when the adult was silent. However, they emphasize that the main purpose of this is to engage children with the adults and not to teach different sound patterns. Proximal Communication is about engaging children in social interactions that they understand and enjoy, and teaching children the meaning of communication-the cause and effect relationship between them and their communicative partner. MUSICAL INTERACTION Musical interaction is another educational intervention for children with ASDs which aims to engage each child in the process of interaction, to build up the desire for communication. It aims to enable students to develop social and communication skills, including the pragmatics, by interacting through songs, games, and musical ‘conversations’ (Prevezer and Chandler, 1998). At the beginning, the adult respond to the child’s spontaneous sounds and movements as if they were communicative intents-this may encourage the child to start using them intentionally. Prevezer (1990) suggests ways of ‘tuning in’ to the child. She begins by creating shared attention, by joining in with or imitating the child. She then gives a simple running commentary to the child’s actions, which is chanted, sung or played on an instrument. Use of songs, rhymes and play routines as frameworks for communication, creating pauses for anticipation, helps to underscore the interaction. Using mainly keyboard and voice, the adult helps the interaction to flow and develop played improvised music, accompanying songs, initiating activities and supporting or prompting in various ways. Activities, as well as ways of responding are based on the child’s individual age and circumstances. Whether in lap play with younger students or instrumental play with older ones, skills such as turn-taking, social timing, eye contact and imitation are made easier and more meaningful with music. According to Prevezer and Chandler (1998), gaining these fundamental conversation skills gives a framework for further communication development. Musical interaction is another intervention which recognises that children with ASDs appear to lack the basic motivation to interact, and by teaching the skills of communication in an interactive way, it facilitates an enjoyment of being in an interactive process. It is again a child-centred method, with the adult following the lead of the child, and interacting with the child at the developmental level, rather than the age level. ESSENTIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF EFFECTIVE INTERVENTIONS As already mentioned above, there is at least some evidence (i.e. Dawson and Osterling, 1997) for each of these approaches which suggests that all of these communication intervention programmes have been proven effective for different children with autism and their level of success, on the development of social and communicative skills of children, has been determined by changes in measures such as IQ scores and academic achievement. However, according to Lord (1999), it is very time consuming to implement a thorough evaluation of social and communicative areas especially because communication varies in different contexts, among various communicative partners and over time. However, there is a large and growing corpus of rigorous single-subject experimental studies and some group comparisons documenting individual differences in outcomes that contribute to an understanding of current research opportunities and gaps (Koegel, 2000). In general, there is consistent agreement across intervention programmes about a number of features, through practical and, sometimes ethical considerations have made well-controlled studies with random assignment very difficult to conduct without direct evaluation (National Research Council, 2001). Characteristics of the most appropriate intervention for a particular child must be tied to that child’s and family’s needs. However, without direct evaluation it is difficult to know which features are of greatest importance in a programme. Across primarily pre-school programmes, there is a very strong consensus that the following features are critical: Entry into intervention programmes as soon as an ASD diagnosis is seriously considered Active engagement in intensive instructional programming for a minimum of the equivalent of a school day, 5 days a week, with full year programming varied according to the child’s chronological age and developmental level Repeated, planned teaching opportunities generally organized around relatively brief periods of time for the youngest children, including sufficient amounts of adult attention in one-to-one and very small group instruction to meet individualized goals Inclusion of a family component, including parental training Low student/teacher ratios Mechanisms for ongoing programme evaluation and assessments of individual children’s progress, with results translated into adjustments in programming (National Research Council, 2001) All of the above programmes share these key elements. They are all embedded into the above approaches as important factors for the effectiveness of the programmes. Overall, many of the programmes are more similar than different in terms of levels of organization, ongoing monitoring and the use of certain techniques, such as discrete trials, incidental teaching and structured teaching. Furthermore, they all share the view that family involvement is a vital component of an effective intervention programme, and moreover, they all suggest that early and intensive intervention is likely to lead to huge progress. However, curricula across these programmes differ in several ways. They include the ways in which goals are prioritized, affecting the relative time spent on verbal and non-verbal communication, social activities, behavioural, academic, motor and other domains. They also differ in the teaching methods they use, with others adopting one set of procedures, and others adopting combinations of approaches. There is no outcome study published that supports comparative statements of the superiority of one programme or approach over another. Rather, with few exceptions, much of the current outcome information is in the form of programme evaluation data or measures of children’s progress when comparisons are made before and after intervention. The available research strongly suggests that a substantial subset of children with ASDs are able to make marked progress during the period that they receive intensive early intervention, and nearly all the children appear to show some benefit. The fact that there are real differences in philosophy and practice of different approaches, provide a range of alternatives for parents and school systems considering various approaches. The key to any child’s educational programme lies in the objectives specified in the IEPs and the ways they are addressed. Thus, effective intervention programmes, are and should vary considerably across individual children, depending on a child’s age, cognitive and language levels, behavioural needs and family priorities. THE CASE STUDY The present study aimed to identify how practitioners in a particular pre-school setting for children with ASDs are interpreting and applying strategies, proposed by theorists as methods of good practice in the teaching of communication skills. The literature review identified the various strategies recommended while the practical investigation, the actual case study, looks at how practitioners are interpreting and applying these strategies in their everyday contact with the children. Although much has been written about the implementation and the evaluation of specific intervention strategies for children with ASDs, usually by those proposing a specific approach (e.g. Schopler and Mesibov, 1985; Bondy and Frost, 1994), there has been very little investigation into how practitioners are interpreting and applying this varying and often conflicting advice. This current study therefore, by making detailed observation of the implementation of the strategies in a real life setting, informs the debate on how children with ASDs are being taught by evaluating how theorists are impacting upon classroom practice. The particular pre-school establishment is a specialist provision for children with ASDs and it accommodates children from 2 to 4 years of age. The nursery is staffed by four members, two nursery nurses and two speech therapy assistants. There were eight children in the group studied, attending the nursery everyday from 9.30 to 11.30 am. The findings of the study identified the particular intervention methods used in the nursery, identified the way that practitioners interpret and apply these methods and showed that they indeed positively contribute to the communication development of the children. METHODOLOGY The research questions which formed the focus of the study were: To identify through a literature review recommended interventions for supporting the development of communication skills in children with autism understand how staff, within a specific pre-school establishment, select, interpret and apply these methods and to evaluate how successful these are in contributing to the development of communication skills A qualitative method of research was selected for this study. This was felt to be the most appropriate methodology because qualitative perspectives commonly focus upon the constructed nature of social reality (Gillham, 2000). The present study was conducted in a real-life setting and it aimed at a detailed description of it. It took the form of a group case study. A case study is one which investigates an individual or a group of individuals, in order to answer specific research questions and which seeks a range of different kind of evidence; evidence which is there in the case setting and, which has to be abstracted and collated to get the best possible answers to the research questions (Gillham, 2000). Nisbet and Watt (1984) suggest that a case study is a specific instance that is frequently designed to illustrate a more general principle; it is the study of an instance in action. The single instance is of a bounded system, for example, a child, a clique, a class, a school or a community. It provides a unique example of real people in real-life situations, enabling readers to understand ideas more clearly than simply by presenting them with abstract theories or principles. In the present study, it was felt particularly appropriate to use a group case study approach. Since I wanted to investigate a particular group of children in a specific context, a case study approach allowed the selection of two children of the nursery, and the collection and presentation of detailed information about this group of children. In depth and detailed observation of these children was essential, because I wanted to look at all the aspects of their everyday communication within the nursery. The most effective methods of identifying what communication strategies are being used within a particular setting are observation and interview, traditionally the tools of qualitative research. Two boys aged from 2 to 3 years old were randomly selected to take part in the study. For the limited time span that this study was conducted, it was felt that for in depth and detailed observation and data collection, two children would be most appropriate. The researcher enquired as to whether there might be a cause and effect relationship, between the communication strategies used in the nursery and the development of communication skills of children. According to Cohen, Manion and Morrison (2000), case studies can also establish cause and effect, and indeed one of their strengths is that they observe effects in real contexts, recognizing that a context is a powerful determinant of both causes and effects. Case studies are thought to have several strengths and weaknesses. Nisbet and Watt (1984), point out, that strengths of case studies are: The results are more easily understood by a wide audience as they are frequently written in everyday, non professional language. They can be undertaken by a single researcher without needing a full research team They can embrace and build in unanticipated events and uncontrolled variables. However, there are also weaknesses, which involve: The results may not be generalizable except where other readers/researchers see their application They are not easily open to cross-checking; hence they may be selective, biased, personal and subjective. They are prone to problems of observer bias, despite attempts made to address reflexitivity. (page 184) Yin (1989), describes a case study as being one of three types in terms of outcomes: exploratory, descriptive and/or explanatory. Exploratory cases are sometimes considered as a prelude to social research. Explanatory case studies may be used for doing casual investigations. Finally, descriptive cases require a descriptive theory to be developed before starting the project. This case study could be described as “Descriptive” as it relies heavily on the theories of several communication interventions analysed in the literature review. In order to enhance objectivity, triangulation of methods was used. Triangulation refers to the use of different methodological standpoints, in order to aid reliability and objectivity (Stake, 1995). In the present study, several methods for data collection were used. The major data collection instruments were: Review of literature Direct observation Interviews Video analysis IEPs analysis These tools for data collection were chosen in order to provide the researcher with information about the children, the staff, the environment of the setting, the intervention methods, the application of the methods as well as the possible progress of the children. Evaluation of the tools used Review of the literature The review of the relevant literature on the communication-based interventions for children with ASDs was felt to be very useful to the present study. The selection of the specific intervention programmes analysed in the literature review was made in order to firstly show the difference between behavioural and more developmental approaches, and secondly, in order to identify and describe the approaches mostly used in the nursery-it provided a framework by which the researcher could identify what might be termed “strategies” used in the nursery. Thus, the Incidental Teaching and the Pivotal Response Training approaches were described in order to show the strong behavioural elements involved in them, as well as the combination of behavioural and developmental techniques they use. TEACCH, PECS, Proximal Communication approach, and Musical Interaction were described as they are the main approaches used in the nursery although some of them are not being “formally” adopted in the nursery, but many elements of them are frequently used by the staff. The collection of information about all the approaches was made using books, journals and the Internet. Observation Observation is a very important method of data collection in every case study. There are two principle kinds of observation known as “participant” and “nonparticipant” or “detached/structured observation” (Gillham, 2000). Participant observation occurs when the researcher is actively involved in what goes on, and is mainly descriptive. Non-participant observation occurs when the researcher is watching from “outside” in a carefully planned and specified way-counting and classifying what s/he sees. I adopted the role of a participant observer since I was actively involved in the everyday activities of the children in the nursery. Being actively involved in all the activities and tasks in the nursery I gained a more in-depth view of the practitioners’ suggestions and way of work. The overpowering validity of observation is that it is the most direct way of collecting data. It is not what people have written on a topic, or what people say they do, but it is what they actually do. Because the present study involved very young children with severe communication difficulties, observation seemed to be the strongest way of collecting data about their everyday interactions. However, from the point of view of positivist “objectivity” a major objection to observation is the effect of the researcher’s presence on those who s/he is observing. This is known as the “observer effect” (Stake, 1995). According to Robson (1993) this can be overcome-for example by seeking to ensure that the observed are unaware of being observed, at one extreme; or by them being so accustomed to the presence of the observer that they carry on as if the observer was not there, at the other extreme (page 191). For the particular case study, it was felt that the children were unaware of being observed. Of course, it is very difficult to ascertain whether this is a true fact. However, according to staff’s comments nothing seemed to have changed in their usual behaviour with the presence of the researcher. Morrison (1993), points out that observation enables the researcher to gather data on: The physical setting-physical environment and its organisation The human setting-organisation of people The interactional setting-interactions that are taking place-formal, informal etc The programme setting- e.g. resources and their organisation The present study provides an analytical description of the above data that is analysed in the Findings section. Field notes were collected from the observation of the environment of the nursery, the staff, the children and the resources and techniques used to enhance communication development of children. The observation data were collected in eight visits to the nursery. Specifically: Visit 1 (27/05/03) Observation of the physical environment and the everyday routine of the nursery Observation of Andy in threading, computer and painting activities Observation of Stewart in painting, water tray and jigsaw activities. Observation of both Andy and Stewart during snack time. Visit 2 (29/05/03) Observation of Andy throughout his day at the nursery. Visit 3 (05/06/03) Observation of Stewart throughout his day at the nursery. Visit 4 (10/06/03) Viewing and analysis of videos of both Andy and Stewart during their first months at the nursery Visit 5 and 6 (18/06/03-02/07/03) Observation of both Andy and Stewart throughout the day Visit 7 (23/07/03) Interview with the leading nursery nurse Visit 8 (17/09/03) Observation of the whole group in the nursery Interview Interviews are one of the most commonly recognised forms of qualitative research method. The overwhelming strength of a face-to-face interview is the richness of the communication that is possible. For the purposes of the present study, the lead teacher of the nursery was interviewed, after several weeks of the researcher’s observation of the children. A 20 minute semi-structured interview which was tape-recorded took place in the nursery, in order for the researcher to clarify issues she had observed and to gain a more in-depth view of the communication strategies used in the nursery. Furthermore, the interview was used as a method of triangulation for the present study. In addition to the observation, which was the main data gathering tool, the interview as well as the video and the IEP’s analysis aimed to serve as different kind of sources (or data) to prove the same kind of evidence-that was to investigate how the nursery actively encourages the development of communication skills of children. According to Gillham (2000), there are several strengths in interview techniques. For the present study, where a semi-structured interview was used these are the most relevant: Small number of people are involved (just the leader of the team of staff) They are accessible The questions are mainly open and require an extended response with prompts and probes from the researcher to clarify the answers (page 62) The leading teacher of the nursery was selected to be interviewed, due to the fact that she has been working in the nursery longer and had the most experience with children with ASDs. A transcript of the interview is included as Appendix 1. Video Tape Analysis Video analysis was used as another tool to give evidence of the communication development of the children of the case study. Andy and Stewart were videotaped by the staff on their first day at the nursery as well as on later sessions. In order for the researcher to discover progress in the communication skills of these children, she observed their behaviour on these first days and compared it to the communicative behaviour they displayed when she visited the nursery-that was three months later. Individual Educational Plans (IEPs) Analysis Every child receives an IEP that focuses on the development of goals and objectives for him/her to achieve. The first IEPs of the children involved in the study were compared with the latest ones in order to find evidence of progress especially on their communication development. The change of the goals and targets on each IEP as well as the remarks of the staff on whether goals and targets were achieved were carefully examined and analyzed. DATA ANALYSIS All the data collected, were categorised according to the source which they were provided from. For each specific aim of the study, data were collected from the literature review, from the observation, the interview, the video tapes and the IEPs. Data from the literature review can be found in the according section. Data from observation can be found in the appendices section along with data from the IEPs. A transcript of the interview can also be found in the appendices section, as well as data from the video tapes in the form of notes. Each aim of the study was addressed using different sources of data or combinations of sources of data. For example, several points or arguments were supported by data from one source, i.e. observation, while others were supported by combinations of sources i.e. interview and observation, or literature review and observation. Furthermore, the appendices section can provide the reader with detailed information about the data collected and used to support arguments made in the study. FINDINGS OF THE STUDY One of the main aims of this study was: to identify the particular communication intervention procedures used in the nursery. From the first visit of the researcher to the nursery, it was identified that the TEACCH and the PECS methods were the interventions mostly used in the nursery. Children are communicating with adults using the PECS system and the environment follows the TEACCH principles of physical structure, and visual clarity. Everything is labelled, there are clear visual boundaries segmenting the space into recognisable parts and the visual schedule tells the children which activities will occur and in which order. Tasks and activities are presented visually so as to make the expectations clear and highlight the important information. The interview with the leader teacher of the nursery, I will name her Dawn, also confirmed that these two methods were the main approaches used in the nursery; and were used deliberately and knowningly in the knowledge that they are recognised strategies. Dawn: “PECS is the first communication strategy introduced to the children when they first arrive at the nursery. After we find out what motivates the child and what does not, we introduce the first phase of PECS”. .......... The other way we encourage communication is with the TEACCH timetable that we introduce. The staff also make home visits to the children, twice a week in order to teach them to use PECS in places other than the nursery, and in order for the parents to practise the use of it at home. According to the interviewee, it is the main method used within the nursery for the encouragement of the communication development in children. Furthermore, TEACCH is also introduced at children’s homes earlier or at the same time that they join the nursery. However, from the researcher’s observation of the staff and the children in the nursery, it appears that, in addition, there is use of many elements of other communication interventions, such as proximal communication and musical interaction. Although these two methods are not part of the formal interventions used in the nursery, many strategies of them are frequently used by the staff, embedded in their overall attitude to the children. The “minimal speech approach” for example, a basic strategy of the Proximal Communication was observed to be adopted by the staff, especially towards children who are not vocal and who seem to not comprehend speech. Staff were observed to give quite a lot verbal requests to Stewart for example, whose verbal comprehension level is quite high, while they keep a minimal speech approach with only keywords involved towards Ben who does not understand speech. The “burst/pause” activity of Proximal Communication’s strategies was also observed to be involved in many non-directive activities like chasing games or tickling games or blowing bubbles games. Dawn identifies that staff may intuitively use these strategies to encourage more initiations of communication by the children. Dawn: “we try to create as many “communication temptations” as possible with the children, in structured as well as in non-structured activities. Depending on the level of ability of each child and on what they are working on at the time, we create opportunities for the children to practice the use of the communication means each one has and initiate the interaction him/herself”. For example, in visit 2 of the researcher, Andy is observed blowing bubbles with the speech therapist in a structured activity. When she stops, she prompts Andy to use PECS in order to request for more bubbles. In a similar game with Stewart however, who is more vocal than Andy, she encourages him to say “bubble” or “again” when she stops. In visit 6 of the researcher, Andy is observed in the multisensory room, in a nonstructured activity. He and one of the teachers are engaged in a tickling game. When the teacher stops tickling, she turns her back and stops looking at him. Andy is approaching her and touches her on the shoulder to indicate he wants more. These observations support the interviewees comments that staff differentiate their approach towards different children according to their level of ability and the target they are working on, as well as the staff efforts to make the children initiate interactions and practise their communication skills frequently. Furthermore, staff was also observed using another strategy of the Proximal Communication approach which involves the adult’s imitations of the children’s vocalisations. This strategy was observed to be used regularly by the staff with one specific boy, who is not vocal. In visit 8 for example, the teacher is observed approaching the boy while he is playing alone with car toys and she joins in. She imitates the vocal sounds he is making and he seems to take notice, and enjoys it. They engage in a “babbling conversation” where turn taking and eye-contact are used. The staff have also adopted techniques of the Musical Interaction approach. In several occasions for example, staff were observed singing their words to children or giving chanted commentaries of the children’s actions. Furthermore, several singing games are part of the everyday activities in the nursery in order to encourage interaction and turn taking between the children. According to Dawn's comments, although all these strategies are not part of the formal communication interventions used in the nursery, the staff feel that “some of them may encourage development of communication skills for some children”. Therefore, they use elements of these approaches embedded in the methods formally used at the nursery, according to each child’s abilities and needs, in order to further encourage their communication and social interaction skills. The second and third aim of this study were to: identify the communication intervention procedures that are used in the nursery and understand how staff are applying these strategies The literature review provides an analytical description of the principles and the aims of each communication intervention, as well as the way each one of them might contribute to the communication development of different children. Different communication approaches, take into account the different strengths and weaknesses as well as the different needs of children and their families, and modifying their principles upon them contribute to the social and communicative development of children. The interview with the leading teacher of the nursery also gives evidence that the methods used actually help children to develop their communication skills. According to her, the fact that children learn to use PECS gives them a means of communication that they did not have before their entry into the nursery. Moreover, the “communication temptations” that the staff create for the children, give them the opportunity to use and practice their means of communication and improve their communication skills everyday. She very strongly believes that the strategies used in the nursery actively contribute to the communication development of the children. “Take Andy for example! When he first attended the nursery we couldn’t get him out of his mother’s arms. He would not do nothing but scream and cry. Now he can communicate his requests and dislikes effectively and he definitely is more independent and happier!” The fourth aim of this study was to: understand the way in which these interventions might contribute to the communication development of children The progress in the development of communication skills of the children was recorded through observation, video-tapes and IEPs. The researcher, on her fourth visit, watched a video-tape of Andy and of Stewart, on their early days at the nursery. From the comparisons of her previous observations of the communication skills of children with those recorded on the tapes, it appeared that there was significant progress achieved by the children. However, this progress is different for each child depending on their communication abilities before they joined the nursery. Notes of the children’s performance during their early days at the nursery are included in Appendix 2. Stewart: Stewart already had some experience of PECS before his entrance at the nursery. He used to go to playgroups of the same nursery and he also had home visits from the speech therapist assistant once a week. From the video tape of his early days at the nursery, it is obvious that Stewart has made great progress with his communication skills. He reached the 6th and final phase of PECS in very little time; his spoken vocabulary has increased as well as his intention to communicate with the adults. The children’s IEPs can also provide evidence of their communication and social development during their attendance at the nursery. The detailed IEPs can be found in the appendices section. From the comparison of Stewart’s January IEP with his April one, it is obvious that he has achieved great progress with his communication skills during his attendance to the nursery. By achieving the targets in his January IEP he has moved towards working on new ones and he gradually improves his communication skills. His spoken language has significantly increased as well as his comprehension of spoken language. Moreover, Stewart has become more social. He tolerates adults and children in his play for longer periods and he appears to be enjoying his day at the nursery. Andy: Andy seems to have made great progress as well. Unlike Stewart, Andy had no experience of PECS before attending the nursery; he had no spoken language and no effective means of communication. From the video tape of his recorded performance during his early days at the nursery, it appears that Andy very quickly adjusted to the nursery routine and learned to use PECS. He seems to have grasped the concept of communication, although he still needs quite a lot of prompts to use it. Progress in the social and communication development of Andy is also obvious from the analysis of his IEP. Comparison between Andy’s IEP of January 2003 with his IEP of April 2003, shows that he has achieved certain goals and targets in that period, and he continues to develop communicatively. In general terms, after a period of five months in the nursery, Andy seems to have made great progress on his communication and social skills. The TEACCH visual aids and labels have helped him to make sense of what is expected from him in the nursery. With the use of PECS he is now able to communicate what he wants. Having moved to the sixth phase of PECS he can even comment about the environment around him. With the everyday practise of these communication interventions at the nursery as well as at home, Andy seems to have found ways to interact with the world and most significantly to initiate these interactions himself. Furthermore, he can now tolerate other people close to him and he even seems to enjoy it in certain situations. DISCUSSION OF OUTCOMES The present study attempted to identify the possible ways in which a specific preschool establishment might contribute to the development of communication and social skills of two children with ASDs. The PECS and TEACCH are the main intervention approaches used in the nursery and have already been proven to work effectively for different children with ASDs as well as the several elements of the other communication interventions used by the staff. The findings showed that both the children involved in the case study made significant progress during their attendance at the nursery. According to staff comments and to the researcher’s observations both Stewart and Andy showed substantial increases in their intentional communication and seemed to be highly responsive to the interventions used in the nursery. They developed effective ways of communicating their needs, making choices, commenting about their environment and enjoying their everyday routine at the nursery. Furthermore the introduction of PECS and TEACCH at their homes gave them the opportunity to practise further their acquired communication skills in places outside the nursery and with a range of different people. The contribution of the nursery then to their communication and social development can be characterised as highly important. The high ratio of adults and children in the classroom allows teachers and speech therapists to differentiate and modify the philosophy and principles of several communication interventions according to each child’s strengths and needs. Moreover, the repeated communication opportunities that the staff creates for children help them to practice their communication skills frequently and for several different occasions. Stewart and Andy had very different abilities and skills when they first joined the nursery. By working on these different strengths staff enabled them to develop effective means of communication and to adjust at the nursery with the best possible way. According to staff’s comments, the development of communication and social skills made both Andy and Stewart more independent, more social and happier. However, the interventions used in the nursery are not the only factor of success. It is also the enthusiastic and committed staff, who treat each child as an individual and always differentiate and modify their approach according to the particular child, and of course the parental involvement; parents are a key factor to the success of any intervention programme and the nursery is working closely with them in order for the children and the families to have their needs met. Prizant and Wetherby (1998) suggest that in measuring efficacy of intervention, researchers need to go beyond traditional measures of communicative and language skills such as improvement on standardised tests and include broader characteristics, such as degree of success in communicative exchange, related dimensions of emotional expression and regulation, sociocommunicative motivation, social competence, peer relationships, and the child’s competence in natural environments. Future research then, should strive to document meaningful changes that reflect the core domains associated with ASDs as well as measures of family functioning. CONCLUSION The present study attempted to identify and evaluate what contribution a preschool establishment for children with ASDs, makes to the development of the communication and social skills of the children. The three main aims of the study were to: identify the particular communication intervention procedures that were used in a particular educational setting for children with ASDs understand the way in which these interventions might contribute to the communication development of children identify any potential relationship between the development of communication skills and the strategies used The findings of the study showed that all three of these aims were addressed effectively with the use of several methodological tools. However, there are several questions arisen which, due to the limited time span of the study, could not be addressed, and which could be characterised as methodological weaknesses of the study. For example, the parent’s view on the interventions of the nursery as well as on their children’s progress would be of valuable importance. An interview with the parents of the two children studied would have provided the researcher with data that would have added greatly to the arguments made about the contribution of the nursery to the children’s communication development. Additionally, a visit to the children’s house would have been an opportunity for the researcher to see the practice of the children’s communication skills in places outside the nursery. Moreover, another important question that also arises is what would be the communication abilities of the children if they did not attend the nursery, and did not have access to the particular intervention methods. Although there is great progress recorded for both the children studied, one can not be certain on whether their communication development is due to the strategies used in the nursery. If the children received different intervention strategies or even if they did not receive any intervention at all, would their communication abilities be different or would they exist at all? This is a very important issue concerning intervention methods for children with ASDs which was not addressed in the present study. Another important issue that arises from the present study is that of the way that children might experience the intervention approaches used in the nursery. It would have been very interesting to know the children’s views on the everyday routine of the nursery, what do they find helpful and what not, what they like or dislike. Again, this is an issue of great importance which however is quite difficult to address since it has to do not only with very young children but also with children with communication difficulties. Nevertheless, all of the above issues that have risen from the present study can be future issues for further research. Moreover, the present study can be replicated with a longer time span, which will allow the longer systematic observation of more children, in different places (nursery, home environment), with interviews of parents, siblings or more staff members. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association (1980): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association American Psychiatric Association (1987): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition (revised), Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association Bleuler E. (1911): Dementia praecox oder gruppe der schizophrenien, (Zinken translation, 1950), New York, International University Press. Cited in Cumine, Leach and Stevenson (2000): Autism in the Early Years, David Fulton Publishers, London Ciesielski K., Courchesne E. and Elmasian R. (1990): Effects of focused, selective attention tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and normal individuals, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 7.Cited in: in Quill (2000): Do-Watch-Listen-Say, Social and Communication Intervention for Children with Autism, Paul Brookes Publishing Frith U. and Baron-Cohen S. (1987): Perception in autistic children, in Cohen and Donellan (Eds), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, New York, John Wiley and Sons. Cited in: in Quill (2000): Do-Watch-Listen-Say, Social and Communication Intervention for Children with Autism, Paul Brookes Publishing Gillham B., (2000): Case Study, Research Methods, Continuum Publishing, London and New York Grandin T. (1995): Thinking in pictures and other reports of my life with autism, New York, Doubleday Jordan R. (1999): Autistic Spectrum Disorders: An Introductory Handbook for Practitioners, David Fulton Publishers, London Koegel R., (1995): Communication and Language Intervention, in Koegel and Koegel (Eds) (1996), Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies for Initiating Positive Interactions and Improving Learning Opportunities, Paul Brookes Publishing Koegel R., Valdez-Menchaca M. and Koegel L.(1994): Autism: Social communication difficulties and related behaviours, in Van Hasslet and Hersen (Eds): Advanced Abnormal Psychology, New York, Plenum Press. Cited in Koegel: “Communication and Language Intervention”, in Koegel and Koegel (Eds) (1996), Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies for Initiating Positive Interactions and Improving Learning Opportunities, Paul Brookes Publishing Lord C. and Paul P. (1997): Language and Communication in Autism, in Cohen and Volkmar (Eds), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, New York, John Wiley and Sons. Cited in: Educating Children with Autism, Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism, National Research Council, National Academy Press, Washington DC Lovaas I. , Varni J., Koegel R. and Lorsch J.(1977): in Koegel (1995): Communication and Language Intervention, in Koegel and Koegel (Eds) (1996), Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies for Initiating Positive Interactions and Improving Learning Opportunities, Paul Brookes Publishing Lovaas I. , Koegel R. and Schreibman L,. (1979): Stimulus Overselectivity in Autism: A review of Research, Psychological Bulletin, 86. Cited in: in Quill (2000): Do-Watch-Listen-Say, Social and Communication Intervention for Children with Autism, Paul Brookes Publishing Loveland K. and Laundry S. (1986): Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16 Morrison K. (1993): Planning and Accomplishing School-centred Evaluation, Norfolk, Peter Francis Publishers Mundy P. , Sigman M. and Kasari C. (1990): A Longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20. Cited in Koegel : “Communication and Language Intervention”, in Koegel and Koegel (Eds) (1996), Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies for Initiating Positive Interactions and Improving Learning Opportunities, Paul Brookes Publishing National Autistic Society (NAS) (2001): Approaches to Autism, An easy to use guide to many and varied approaches to autism, revised edition National Research Council (2001): Educating Children with Autism, Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism, Division of Behavioural and Social Sciences and Education, Washington DC: National Academy Press, 2001 Nisbet J.. and Watt H. (1984): “Case Study” in Bell, Bush, Fox, Goodey and Goulding (Eds), Conducting small case investigations in educational management, London, Harper and Row O’Neill J. and Jones G. (1997): Sensory perception abnormalities in autism: A case for more research? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27. Cited in Quill (2000): Do-Watch-Listen-Say, Social and Communication Intervention for Children with Autism, Paul Brookes Publishing Prevezer W. (1990): Strategies for tuning into autism, Therapy Weekly, 4 in Cumine, Leach and Stevenson (2000): Autism in the Early Years, David Fulton Publishers, London Prevezer W. and Chandler S. (1998): “Musical Interaction” in Approaches to Autism- An easy guide to many and varied approaches to autism, National Autistic Society, 2001, London Prutting C. and Kirchner D. (1987): A clinical appraisal of the pragmatics aspects of language, Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52. Cited in Koegel and Koegel (Eds) (1996), Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies for Initiating Positive Interactions and Improving Learning Opportunities, Paul Brookes Publishing Ricks D. and Wing L. (1976): Language, Communication and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children, Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5. In Paul (1987): “Communication”, cited in Cohen and Donellan (Eds) (1987): Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, John Wiley and Sons Publishing Sigman M., Ungerer J., Mundy P. and Sherman T. (1987): in Paul (1987): “Communication”. Cited in Cohen and Donellan (Eds) (1987): Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, John Wiley and Sons Publishing Stake R. (1995): The Art of Case Study Research, Sage Publications, London, New Delhi Stone W. and Caro-Martinez L. (1990): Naturalistic observations of spontaneous communication in autistic children, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20. Cited in Educating Children with Autism, Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism, National Research Council, National Academy Press, Washington DC Stone W. et al (1997): Non-verbal communication in 2- and 3- year old children with autism, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27. Cited in Educating Children with Autism, Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism, National Research Council, National Academy Press, Washington DC Williams (1992): Nobody, Nowhere, New York, Times Books Wing L. and Gould J. (1979): Severe Impairments of Social Interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and Classification, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9. Cited in Jordan (1999): Autistic Spectrum Disorders: An Introductory Handbook for Practitioners, David Fulton Publishers, London Yin R. (1989): Case study research; design and methods, 2nd edition, Newbury Park and London, Sage Publications www.teacch.com/structur.htm: accessed on 02/06/03 www.polyxo.com:PECS: accessed on 13/03/03 APPENDIX 1 Transcript of Interview Researcher: What are the intervention methods you use to encourage communication development of the children? Teacher: The first communication method we introduce at the nursery is PECS. The first few weeks we are actually getting to know the children, we find out what motivates them, what they like, what they dislike and then we start them of with phase 1. The other way we encourage communication is with the TEACCH timetable that we introduce. All the visual cues and aids that TEACCH involves are very helpful for the children to know what is going on at any time and what is expected of them. Also, the speech therapist makes home visits usually twice a week so that children learn to use PECS at home and the parents have the chance to see how it works so that they can practice it as well… The TEACCH approach is also introduced at home sometimes even earlier than the children’s attendance at the nursery. So what we do at the school environment can then be reinforced at the home environment…. R: I have noticed that there some other ways that you try to engage children in interactions, other than TEACCH and PECS. In several unstructured activities for example, you use techniques which are not part of TEACCH or PECS… to make children initiate interactions. T: We do what we call “communication temptations” with the children. Depending on what are we working on and on what level the children are we try to get them to engage in communication we think it is meaningful for them…We know what motivates them and we try to get them to communicate about it… R: For example the fact that you use minimal speech with specific children or that you use singing utterances… T: Yes, there are some things that we find they help specific children. We use little speech with a child that does not comprehend speech and we try to get him to communicate with PECS; with a child that is more vocal we use greater amount of speech and we try to get him to communicate with a sound for example to show us that he wants something. Stewart for example…. He is in a level that can understand language and can use it, so we try to get him to speak more than using PECS. R: Do you encourage communication between the children at all or do you only focus in the communication between children and adults? T: It depends on what stage they are ready for. We always try to encourage communication between them like in snack time, or singing time and outside play. But to be honest it never really gets to the stage that children are actually aware of even their peers names….What we do here is to get them ready for nursery and hope that they will move on a lot more…. R: So, do you think that the strategies you use in the nursery contribute to the communication development of children and in what way? T: Yes, of course! I very strongly believe that both TEACCH and PECS actually help children to develop communication skills! We can see their progress day by day…Take Andy for example! When he first attended the nursery we couldn’t get him out of his mother’s arms. He would not do nothing but scream and cry. Now he can communicate his requests and dislikes effectively and he definitely is more independent and happier! The great thing is that they acquire a means of communication that they did not have before… they can express themselves… R: So you think that children respond positively to the communication strategies you use? T: Well, all children are very different… but in general…it always seems to be a very positive response from them…Sometimes it might take quite long…but definitely the TEACCH and PECS are helping children and there is definitely progress… APPENDIX 2 Notes from the video tapes Stewart On his first day at the nursery, 09/01/03, although no communication skills are recorded on the tape, Stewart investigates the new environment and seems interested in the toys as well as the activities that are going on. He approaches the water tray where an adult and a boy are playing and he imitates them. He tries several toys and plays quietly. A week after, Stewart is recorded using the third phase of PECS. When the speech therapist stops blowing bubbles, he gives her the sentence strip “I want..bubbles” and he articulates the word “bubbles”. He uses PECS spontaneously. On the 18/02/03, he seems totally adjusted to the routines of the nursery. With the TEACCH visual timetable he knows what he has to do and when. He also uses spontaneously key words that the staff is using with him, like “finished”, or “painting”, or “bye”. Three months later, Stewart is recorded making attempts to speak. While playing with an adult close to the window, he points out. The adult says “tree”, Stewart repeats it. He points to the light; the adult says “light” he repeats it. He then says “again” and gives the adult the toy car to continue playing with him. On singing time, he joins in happily, trying to pronounce all the words he can. Andy On his first day, 09/01/03, there is no evidence of communication skills recorded on the tape. Andy seems to investigate the new environment, the children and the toys. However, he does not tolerate people and especially children close to him. Most of the time he runs up and down the rooms screaming. Two weeks later, 23/01/03, he has started using the first phase of PECS. He still hesitates to initiate interaction with staff but he seems to have grasped the concept of picture exchanging. When he is interested and motivated by the activity he spontaneously uses PECS. When however he is not interested in the activity, he refuses to use PECS even with prompts. On the 18/02/03, he seems familiar with the environment and the routines. He uses the third phase of PECS-he discriminates between different pictures for different objects and activities. He is able to follow the visual timetable, but still needs prompt to start an activity even though he knows what he has to do. On the 16/05/03, Andy has moved to the fourth phase of PECS. He starts combining a picture for ''I want'' with the picture of the requested item or activity. He also seems to tolerate staff and children around him for a significant amount of time, in snack time or singing time as well as in the multi-sensory room. APPENDIX 3 IEP OF STEWART January 2003 Communication development targets: To continue to develop use of PECS, functionally and independently. Although Stewart could use the PECS he seemed to have a preference to certain members of staff. Thus, staff prompted him to use it with different people. To identify and use his own timetable without prompts from adults. Language development targets: To develop language skills through labelling 10 familiar objects. To encourage use of animal noises. Social interaction targets: Develop ability to play cooperatively with adults and to share toy with one other child for 3 minutes. To join in group singing sessions for 5 minutes. April 2003 . Communication development targets: To introduce “I hear”, using animal and transport noises. To encourage Stewart to build more sentences with “I want”. Language development targets: To develop language skills through labelling action words. To continue to encourage imitation of animal noises. Social interaction targets: To develop ability to play cooperatively with other children for longer periods (5 minutes). To share toy or simple turn taking activity with another child for 3-5 minutes. IEP FOR ANDY January 2003 Communication development targets: To develop the use of PECS to make choices between two snacks or toys. To introduce Andy to some visual structure to help him begin to follow the routine of the group. Language development targets: To develop Andy’s tolerance of adults in his play. To develop some imitation skills through gross motor actions e.g. clap hands. Social interaction targets: To increase Andy’s use of eye contact with adults during one-to-one sessions and at group or snack time. April 2003 Communication development targets: Introduction of the fourth phase of PECS. Introduction of the “I want” strip to make choices for activities and toys. To get Andy to travel further for PECS. Language development targets: To continue to develop Andy’s tolerance of adults and children in his play. Social interaction targets: Andy to join in group sessions for 5 minutes