1 Spider mimicry in prey and predator visual systems Marc Théry & Jérôme Casas Manuscript reference number TX7018A Details of modelling calculations Determination of colour loci in the hexagon of Hymenoptera colour vision We proceeded as Chittka et al.1. To determine photoreceptor excitations for each measured spectra, we used spectral sensitivity functions of standard photoreceptors for trichromatic Hymenoptera2. The sensitivity factor R for each photoreceptor is determined by the equation R 1 I B λ S λ Dλ dλ 700 300 (1) Where I B λ is the spectral reflection function of a large sample of green foliage background to which the receptors are adapted; S λ is the spectral sensitivity function of the receptor in question and Dλ is the illuminating daylight spectrum (CIE D65). The effective quantum flux P for a given spectra in the respective photoreceptor is computed as P R I Sλ S λ Dλ dλ 300 700 (2) Where I S λ is the spectral reflection function of spiders or flowers. 2 It is assumed that the photoreceptors display half their maximum response when stimulated by the light reflected from the adaptation background. When the maximum excitation E max of the photoreceptor is normalised to unity, the physiological receptor voltage signals E UV , E B and E G are computed for each photoreceptor as E P /( P 1) (3) The two dimensions of the colour hexagon are calculated using receptor excitations as x = sin 60°( E G - E UV ) (4) y = E B - 0.5( E UV + E G ) (5) Euclidean distances St between stimuli are calculated as St x y 2 2 (6) Determination of colour loci in the tetrahedron of bird colour vision To compute colour loci of spiders and flowers seen by a tetrachromat, we use spectral sensitivities of single cones determined in the passeriform bird Leiothrix lutea3, and optical models derived from microspectrophotometry of cone pigments with oil droplets4. The sensitivity factor R, the effective quantum flux P and the physiological receptor voltage signals E UV , E B , E G and E R were determined respectively from equations (1), (2) and (3). 3 The three dimensions of the tetrahedron5 are calculated using receptor excitations as x = 2 2 cos30° ( E G - E R ) 3 (7) y = E UV - 1 ( E B + E G + E R ) 3 (8) z = 2 2 [sin30°( E G + E R ) - E B ] 3 (9) Euclidean distances between stimuli are calculated as St x y z 2 2 2 (10) Quantification and test of colour contrast for Hymenoptera and birds The null hypothesis is verified if Euclidean distances are randomly distributed above and below a threshold of colour contrast discrimination for both predators and preys. For a honeybee, colour discrimination is a function of wavelength with an optimal resolution of 5 nm around 500 nm 6. We shifted 81 twin reflectance spectra from 300 to 700 nm in 5 nm steps to compute the mean Euclidean distance of colour contrast discrimination, which was measured as 0.05. A colour contrast of 0.1 is considered as equivalent to about 70% discriminability7,8. For each pair of spider and flower, colour contrasts were compared to the 0.05 discrimination value providing a measure of individual colour mimicry. We tested the frequencies of colour contrasts below the discrimination threshold against the null hypothesis using chi-square tests. We proceeded similarly for birds by computing the minimal colour distance allowing to discriminate two object spectra differing by 4 nm 9. In the colour tetrahedron, two spider or flower reflectance spectra shifted between 300 4 and 700 nm in 4 nm steps were separated by a mean Euclidean distance of 0.06. As for Hymenoptera, frequencies of colour distances below the discrimination value were contrasted against the null hypothesis of random distribution using chi-square tests. Quantification and test of achromatic contrast for Hymenoptera and birds Instead of colour contrast, honeybees are known to use the signal of green receptors for detection of small targets10. Green receptor signals were computed for each spider and flower spectra, and compared using repeatedmeasures ANOVAs after controlling data normality. Birds are known to use double cones to detect achromatic contrast 11,12. The spectral sensitivity of double-cones was computed by combining absorbance spectra of the medium- and long-wavelengths sensitive photoreceptors13. Double cone signals produced by each spider and flower spectra were compared using repeated-measures ANOVAs after controlling data normality. 1. Chittka, L., Schimda, A., Troje, N. & Menzel, R. Ultraviolet as a component of flower reflections, and the colour perception of Hymenoptera. Vis. Res. 34, 1489-1508 (1994). 2. Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel H., de Souza, J., Ventura, D. F. & Menzel, R. The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 23-40 (1992). 5 3. Bowmaker, J. K., Heath, L. A., Wilkie, S. E. & Hunt, D. M. Visual pigments and oil droplets from six classes of photoreceptor in the retina of birds. Vis. Res. 37, 2183-2194 (1997). 4. Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T. D., Marshall, N. J. & Cuthill, I. C. Tetracromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A 183, 621-633 (1998). 5. Goldsmith, T. H. Optimization, constraint, and history in the evolution of eyes. Quart. Rev. Biol. 65, 281-322 (1990). 6. von Helversen, O. Zur spectrale unterschiedsempfindlichkeit der honigbiene. J. Comp. Physiol. 80, 439-472 (1972). 7. Chittka, L. Optimal sets of colour receptors and opponent processes for coding of natural objects in insect vision. J. Theor. Biol. 181, 179-196 (1996). 8. Chittka, L. Camouflage of predatory crab spiders on flowers and the colour perception of bees (Aranida: Thomisidae / Hymenoptera: Apidae). Entomol. Gener. 25, 181-187 (2001). 9. Neumeyer, C. Evolution of color vision. In Vision and Visual Dysfunction (eds. Cronley-Dillon, J. R. & Gregory, R. L.) 284-305 (Macmillan, London, 1991). 6 10. Spaethe, J., Tautz, J. & Chittka, L. Visual constraints in foraging bumblebees: flower size and color affect search time and flight behavior. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 3898-3903 (2001). 11. Osorio, D., Miklósi, A. & Gonda, Zs. Visual ecology and perception of coloration patterns by domestic chicks. Evol. Ecol. 13, 673-689 (1999). 12. Osorio, D., Vorobyev, M. & Jones, C. D. Colour vision of domestic chicks. J. Exp. Biol. 202, 2951-2959 (1999). 13. Hart, N. S., Partridge, J. C., Cuthill, I. C. & Bennett, A. T. D. Visual pigments, oil droplets, ocular media and cone photoreceptor distribution in two species of passerine bird: the blue tit (Parus caeruleus L.) and the blackbird (Turdus merula L.). J. Comp. Physiol. A 186, 375-387 (2000).