Sound Advertising 1 Sound Advertising: A Review of the Experimental Evidence on the Effects of Music in Commercials on Attention, Memory, Attitudes, and Purchase Intention David Allan, Ph.D. Department of Marketing Saint Joseph's University Online Publication Date: October 23, 2007 Journal of Media Psychology, Volume 12, No. 3, Fall, 2007 Sound Advertising 2 Abstract This article reviews the empirical studies on the interaction of music and the hierarchy of advertising effects, or specifically attention, memory, attitudes and purchase intention. The most relevant literature is analyzed through the formation of two comprehensive tables of theories and experiments. Music variables such as appeal, fit, melody, mood, tempo, texture, tonality, and valence are shown to influence consumer attitude toward the ad and the brand, recall, pleasure and arousal, and purchase intention. This review provides a summary of the results and the foundation for future research into sound advertising. Sound Advertising 3 Introduction It is almost impossible to turn on the radio or the television, or walk into a retail establishment and not witness the marriage of art and commerce. Even before the days of media and malls, music was a major force in consumer marketing. Without exception, music plays a vital role in the interactive process of consumer behavior. The commercial uses of music in marketing account for billions of dollars nationwide. Not surprisingly, this area of study has received considerable attention primarily focused on the impact of music on consumer responses to commercial advertising. There are many stimuli, or environmental cues, that retailers use to affect consumer behavior including music, color, scents, etc. Music is considered to be the most commonly studied stimulus variable (Turley & Milliman, 2000). Most retailers would agree that music is one of their most important considerations and expenses (Yalch & Spangenberg, 1993). Billions of dollars are spent worldwide on music in the retail environment (North & Hargreaves, 1998). Past reviews of experimental evidence in this area have included music as part of a larger review of atmospheric effects (Lam, 2001; Turley & Milliman, 2000), and more narrowly focused on just its effect on shopping behavior (Allan, in press). There are also many stimuli, or executional cues, that advertisers use to affect consumer response to commercials including music, spokespersons, animation, etc. Music is also considered to be the most used executional cue in commercials (Yalch, 1991). Dunbar (1990, p. 200) argued that “music makes you watch or listen [to advertising] in a different way” than commercials without music and adds an emotional dimension to the consumer response to the brand. Sound Advertising 4 While it should not be surprising that the effect of music on advertising has been extensively researched, it should be surprising that a current, comprehensive, and critical review of the literature has not been completed. Bruner (1990) provided an early collection of relevant research involving music and advertising as part of the literature review for his “Music, Mood and Marketing” but that is now more than a decade old. North and Hargreaves (1997) updated the list as part of a larger chapter (“Music and Consumer Behaviour”) on the commercial and industrial uses of music (advertising, shops and the music industry) in The Sociology of Music. T This article then, has three purposes. First, it is a review of the most important studies involving music and advertising beginning with the most relevant definitions (Table 1). Second, it is a synthesis and comparison of variables and results. Third, based on what has been done and how it has been executed, it is a foundation and facilitation for future research. Sound Advertising 5 Table 1 A Summary of Relevant Definitions_______________________________________ Definition________________________________________ Citation__________________ Fit The music’s relevance or appropriateness to the central ad message. MacInnis & Park (1991) Indexicality The extent to which the music arouses emotion-laden memories. MacInnis & Park (1991) Jingle Unique, novel lyrics written for a particular advertisement. Wallace (1991) Modality A pitch-related variable that is the configuration of intervals between notes in the scale such as major and minor modes. Kellaris & Kent (1991) Mood A fleeting, temporary feeling state, usually not intense and not tied to a specifiable behavior. Gardner (1985) Music A complex chemistry of three main controllable elements (time, pitch and texture). Bruner (1990) Needledrop Music that is prefabricated, multipurpose, and highly conventional. Scott (1990) Placement The position of the music in the advertisement. Brooker & Wheatley (1994) Popular Music Music that is “well-liked” by “ordinary people” (Shuker, 1994) that has had wide exposure and appeal but usually for a fixed period of time. Shuker (1994) Tempo A time-related variable that controls pace. Kellaris & Kent (1991) Texture Comprised of timbre and orchestra. Kellaris & Kent (1994) Tonality The configuration of intervals between pitches on a scale. Kellaris & Kent (1994) Sound Advertising 6 Literature Review The review of literature is divided into two parts. The first part is a discussion of the most cited theories and models including the terms and concepts. The second part is an analysis of the most relevant empirical studies of the effects of music on advertising. A summary table follows each section. Theories and Models Advertising and music have been investigated through many variables with a wide range of outcomes. An analysis of these studies begins with a discussion of the most relevant theories and models. These theories and models provide the foundation of music in advertising experimentation and include attitude theory, classical conditioning theory, involvement theory especially the elaboration likelihood model (ELM), and music theory. Attitude Theory. Fishbein’s (1963) attitude theory, that a person’s attitude is a function of his salient beliefs activated from memory at a point in time in a given situation, is the primary consideration with all research dealing with attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the brand. As will be apparent in the results section, many researchers have studied music’s effect on attitude toward the brand in regard to product preference (Allen & Madden, 1985; Gorn, 1982; Kellaris & Cox, 1989; Middlestadt et al., 1994; Park & Young, 1986; Pitt & Abratt, 1988; Zhu, 2005) and purchase intention (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Morris & Boone, 1998). Others have also considered attitude toward the ad and product preference (Macklin, 1988; Shen et al., 2006) and purchase intention (Morris & Boone, 1998; North et al., 2004). The music variables with regard to attitude toward the brand and the ad that have been most studied are indexicality, or “the extent to which the music arouses emotion-laden memories,” and fit, or “the music’s Sound Advertising 7 relevance or appropriateness to the central ad message,” and its effect on the processing of the commercial (MacInnis & Park, 1991). Classical Conditioning Theory. Pavlov’s classical conditioning, as it relates to advertising, suggests that positive attitudes towards an advertised product or a conditioned stimulus, usually the brand, might develop through its association in a commercial with stimuli that are reacted to positively, such as music, celebrities, or color. But, research has produced conflicting results. Gorn (1982) concluded that positive attitudes towards an advertised product, in this case colored pens, might develop through its association in a commercial with other stimuli like music (he used the theme from “Grease” and classical Indian music). The results of two experiments supported the notion that the simple association between a product and another stimulus such as music can affect product preferences as measured by product choice. Furthermore, an individual who is in a decision-making mode when exposed to a commercial is more affected by the information therein than an individual who is not in a decision-making mode. Many researchers have attempted to extend Gorn’s study but have been unable to replicate his findings (Allen & Madden, 1985; Alpert & Alpert, 1990, Kellaris & Cox, 1989; Pitt & Abratt, 1988). As it stands, classical conditioning appears to occur unreliably (Kellaris & Cox, 1989) and then only in case of low involvement consumers. Involvement Theory. Involvement Theory in general, and the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) in particular, have been suggested as keys to understanding how music affects responses to advertising. Krugman (1965) defined involvement as “the number of conscious bridging experiences, connections, or personal references per minute that a viewer makes between his or her own life and a stimulus” (p. 356). Salmon (1986) added that “involvement, in any form, seems to mediate both the acquisition and processing of information through activating a Sound Advertising 8 heightened state of arousal and/or greater cognitive activity in an interaction between an individual and a stimulus” (p. 264). Petty and Cacioppo’s (1986) concept of elaboration likelihood refers to “the likelihood one engages in issue-relevant thinking with the aim of determining the merits of the arguments rather than the total amount of thinking per se in which a person engages” (p. 674). ELM assumes that once an individual receives a message, processing begins. Depending on the personal relevance of this information, the receiver will follow one of two “routes” to persuasion: “central” and “peripheral.” When the consumer gives the message a high degree of attention, there is high involvement and thus a central (active) processing route. When the consumer gives the message a low degree of attention, there is low involvement and a peripheral (passive) processing route. Petty and Cacioppo suggested that high involvement was the result of a message with high personal relevance. Researchers that have studied involvement with regard to advertising and music have found that it can positively affect message processing in low involvement conditions (MacInnis & Park, 1991; Park & Young, 1986). Music theory. The idea that music has the potential to enhance attention (stimulate awareness) and memory (recall) has been widely speculated and researched (Adorno 1941, 1976; Bartlett & Snelus, 1980; Bower & Bolton, 1969; Galizio & Hendrick, 1972; Hecker, 1984; Macklin, 1988; Rothschild, 1987; Rubin, 1977; Schulkind et al., 1999; Wallace, 1994). Macklin (1988) found that messages that were sung in a produced, original jingle that sounded like a nursery rhyme produced the same recall from children as spoken messages. Bartlett and Snelus (1980) found that cued recall of lyrics of popular songs from 1921 (“When Francis Dances With Me”) to 1974 (“Morning Has Broken”) was higher in response to melodies than in response to titles. Schulkind, Hennis, and Rubin (1999) observed a correlation between music, emotion and autobiographical, long-term memory of older adults and songs from their Sound Advertising 9 youth when testing the Top 20 from 1935 (“On Treasure Island”) to 1994 (“That’s The Way Love Goes”). Some researchers have also observed the enhancement of recall by music. Rubin (1977) found that recall of information is improved when cued with the melody of a well known song (“Star Spangled Banner”). Wallace (1994) found that the melody of a song (using three ballads from “The Frank C. Brown Collection of North Carolina Folklore) could facilitate recall of text by providing it with musical structure for learning and remembering. Serafine, Crowder, and Repp (1984), Serafine, Davidson, Crowder, and Repp (1986), and Crowder, Serafine and Repp (1990) suggested an integration effect where the melody or text of a song (using folksongs from Erdei) is better recalled with original text than with different text. These results suggest that music in ads has the potential to stimulate emotion, attention and recall but not all studies support this theory. Galizio and Hendrick (1972) did not observe that memory for verbal information was enhanced by presenting the information in the form of a song (the musical accompaniment of a guitar). A summary of the most cited theories and models including terms and concepts can be seen in Table 2. Sound Advertising 10 Table 2 A Summary of Relevant Theories and Models ___________________________ Theory____________________________________________________ __Citation________________ Attitude Theory Suggests that beliefs are the only mediators of attitude formation and change Fishbein and that a person’s attitude is a function of salient beliefs at a particular (1963) moment. Classical Conditioning The process of behavior modification by which a subject (dog) comes to respond in a desired manner to a previously neutral stimulus (bell) that has been repeatedly presented along with an unconditioned stimulus (food) that elicits the desired response. Pavlov (1927) Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) Assumes that once an individual receives a message, processing begins. When the consumer gives the message a high degree of attention because it is relevant there is high involvement and thus a central (active) processing route. When the consumer gives the message a low degree of attention because it is not relevant, there is low involvement and a peripheral (passive) processing route. Petty & Cacioppo (1986) Hierarchy of Advertising Effects The processes of attending to a commercial, learning and remembering its content, developing attitudes, and generating conative [tendency to move towards] responses occur in a sequential causal chain. Thorson et al. (1992) Involvement The number of conscious bridging experiences, connections, or personal references per minute that a viewer makes between his or her own life and a stimulus. Krugman (1965) Music Theory The recall of information is improved when cued with the melody of a well known song. The melody of a song can facilitate recall in certain environments. There is a correlation between music, emotion and memory. Rubin (1977) Wallace (1994) Schulkind et al. (1999) Sound Advertising 11 Variables While the amount of consumer behavior theories and models used in the investigation and explanation of music’s effect on advertising may be relatively few, the variables have been many and varied. The following is a summary of the most widely observed independent and dependent variables including a continuum, followed by a comprehensive review of the results of the interaction of these variables. Independent Variables. The impact of music on advertising has been observed with a variety of behaviors when mediated either individually or through the interaction of certain variables. Music appeal (like or dislike) has been observed in relation to product preferences (Allen & Madden, 1985; Gorn, 1982; Kellaris & Cox, 1989; Pitt & Abratt, 1988). The effect of music arousal or mood (a temporary feeling or state) was studied with purchase intention (Alpert & Alpert, 1990; Alpert et al., 2005; Kellaris & Mantel, 1996). Music fit has been observed in relation to message processing (MacInnis & Park, 1991; North et al., 2004; Shen et al., 2006). Different types of music placements/treatments (vocals, instrumentals, jingles, etc.) and recall have been researched (Allan, 2006a; Roehm, 2001; Wallace, 1991, 1994; Yalch, 1991). Music presence has positively affected product preference and purchase attention when interacting with attitude (Macklin, 1988; Morris & Boone, 1998; Middlestadt et al., 1994; Park & Young, 1986; Wheatley & Brooker, 1994) and recall (Macklin, 1988; Olsen, 1995; Wheatley & Brooker, 1994). Finally, music tempo (speed or pace), texture (timbre and orchestration), modality (intervals between notes), and tonality (intervals between pitches in a scale) have been shown to have the potential to enhance pleasure and arousal resulting in a greater purchase intention (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Kellaris & Kent, 1991, 1994; Kellaris & Rice, 1993). Sound Advertising 12 Dependent Variables. When mediated with the above variables, the effect of music in advertising on a variety of consumer responses has also been observed. Attitude toward the ad can be positively influenced by the presence of music (Macklin, 1988; Morris & Boone, 1998; North et. al., 2004; Shen et al., 2006). Perception of ad time can be decreased by arousing music (Kellaris & Mantel, 1996). Attitude toward the brand can be improved by appealing music (Allen & Madden, 1985; Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Gorn, 1982; Kellaris & Cox, 1989; Kellaris & Rice, 1993; Middlestadt et al., 1994; Morris & Boone, 1998; Park & Young, 1986; Pitt & Abratt, 1988; Zhu, 2005). Brand recall can be increased by personally relevant and significant music (Allan, 2006a; Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Kellaris et. al., 1993; Macklin, 1988; MacInnis & Park, 1991; North et al., 2004; Olsen, 1995; Roehm, 2001; Shen et al., 2006; Wallace, 1991, 1994; Yalch, 1991; Wheatley & Brooker, 1994). Music attitude can be positively affected by tempo (Kellaris & Rice, 1993; Kellaris & Kent, 1994). Pleasure/arousal can be affected by not only the tempo but the tonality and texture of the music (Kellaris & Kent, 1994). Finally, purchase intention can be affected by interaction of music and mood (Alpert & Alpert, 1990; Alpert et al., 2005; Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Kellaris & Kent, 1991; Morris & Boone, 1998; North et al., 2004). A continuum of dependent variables and the corresponding independent variables that have been observed can be seen in Figure 1. Sound Advertising 13 Figure 1 DV/IV Continuum ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Attitude Toward →→ → the Ad Ad→ →→ Time Attitude Toward→→ the Brand Brand→→→ Recall Pleasure/→ → → Arousal Purchase Intention Music fit Music arousal Music appeal Music presence Music fit Music melody Music presence Music tempo Music tempo Music texture Music tonality Music fit Music melody Music modality Music mood Music placement Music presence Music tempo ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Sound Advertising 14 Results Much has been learned about music and advertising since it was first used in commercials in the early days of radio. Through a survey and analysis of experimental literature and a content analyses of experimental results and their theoretical underpinnings, a better understanding of the interaction of independent and dependent variables or more conversationally, how music affects the processing of the ad, and how much it is being used in advertising, has been achieved. What follows is a discussion of the results. Attitude Toward the Ad Shimp (1981) argued that attitude toward the ad (ATTA) is an important mediator when a consumer makes a choice. Since then, music’s potential to affect the consumer’s ad attitude has received some attention with conflicting results. The presence of music was shown to affect how a viewer feels when looking at print ads (Morris & Boone, 1998). Musical fit resulted in a better attitude toward the ad (North et al., 2004; Shen & Chen, 2006). Brooker and Wheatley (1994), however, reported no effect of music placement on attitude toward the ad. Likewise, Macklin (1988) reported no effect of music presence on attitude toward the ad with children. Ad Time It has been argued that, under some circumstances, there is a disparity between objective time and perceived time of the ad and that the latter is affected by external stimuli like music (Kellaris & Mantel, 1994; 1996). While not a lot of attention has been given to perceived ad time, it is still worth mentioning. Arousal was found to moderate the influence of stimulus congruity on perceived time such that congruity contributed positively to retrospective duration estimates among subjects exposed to soothing (versus arousing) music (Kellaris & Mantel, 1996). The implications of objective time versus perceived time involve the potential benefits to the Sound Advertising 15 advertiser of increasing memory for the ad while reducing its length (60-second versus 30second commercial). Attitude Toward the Brand Mitchell and Olson (1981) argued that a consumer’s attitude or “internal evaluation” of a brand has always been an important consideration in marketing research. It is not surprising then, that a considerable amount of investigation into music’s effect on attitude toward the brand (ATTB) has been undertaken with a variety of variables and results. Gorn (1982) observed that hearing liked or disliked music can affect product preferences but his results were never replicated (Allen & Madden, 1985; Kellaris & Cox, 1989; Pitt & Abratt, 1988). The presence of music (Blondie’s “Tide Is High”) had a facilitative effect on brand attitude (shampoo and their functional performance) in the low involvement condition and a distracting effect for those in the cognitive involvement condition (Park & Young, 1986). Music presence was also shown to have either no effects (Morris & Boone, 1998) or negative effects (Shen et al., 2006) on attitude depending on its fit. As to how the process of attitude change toward the brand occurs, it has been suggested that it might be a belief-based rather than an affect-based change (Middlestadt et al., 1994). Brooker and Wheatley (1994) reported no effect of placement on attitude toward the brand. Macklin (1988) reported no effect of the presence of music on attitude toward the brand with children. Brand Recall Recall of the brand is obviously a primary consideration in the evaluation of the effectiveness of music in advertising. When the music fit, the message processing of the ad was enhanced (MacInnis & Park, 1991; North et al., 2004). Different music treatments (original and altered vocals, instrumentals, jingles, silence) did affect recall differently under varied conditions (Allan, Sound Advertising 16 2006a; Olsen, 1995: Roehm, 2001; Wallace, 1991, 1994; Yalch, 1991). The presence or absence of music was shown to be both attention-getting (Park & Young, 1988) and distracting (Wheatley & Brooker, 1994). The placement (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994) or the presence of music (Macklin, 1988) was not observed to affect recall. Musical fit, however, was shown to stimulate better recall of brands (North et al., 2004; Shen et al., 2006). Pleasure and Arousal It has been argued that music is an especially powerful stimulus for affecting moods (Bruner, 1990). Thus, it is maybe a bit surprising that music’s potential to affect moods primarily through pleasure and arousal garnered a significant amount of attention in the 1990’s but not much since. During that time, it was observed that arousing music was found to produce greater degrees of mood enhancement thus positively affecting purchase intention (Alpert & Alpert, 1990; Alpert et al., 2005; Kellaris & Mantel, 1996; Morris & Boone, 1998). Music tempo (fast) was shown to have positive effects on behavioral intent (Kellaris & Kent, 1991). Purchase Intention Since purchase intention or conation was first defined as “behavior directed toward action” (Shanteau & Ptacek, 1983, p. 149), it has been one of the most difficult advertising effects to research but arguably the most important. With regard to purchase intention and music, the results varied with some observing significant effects (Alpert & Alpert, 1990; Alpert et al., 2005; Kellaris & Kent, 1991) and some observing no significant effects (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Morris & Boone, 1998). The placement of music was shown to invite attention to the message and motivating consumers to process the message and facilitate the potential to purchase (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994). The combination of music with silence also has been shown to be attention-getting resulting in the enhancement of purchase intention (Olsen, 1995). The effect of Sound Advertising 17 music tempo was shown to have had contradictory results with some reporting a passive effect on purchase intention (Kellaris & Kent, 1991), and some reporting no effect (Brooker & Wheatley, 1994; Morris & Boone, 1998). Content Analyses Only a few studies have dealt with the amount of commercials on television and radio with music. Stewart and Furse (1986, p. 160) found music featured in slightly more than 40% of 1000 television commercials they studied, but that only 12% of those used lyrics to directly convey the advertising message. Similar frequencies were obtained in a follow-up study (Stewart & Koslow, 1989, p. 29). Appelbaum and Halliburton (1993) analyzed international commercials and found music in 89% of their sample (p. 237). Allan (2006b) analyzed commercials in prime-time television and reported that 86% of the unique ads contained some type of music. A summary of the most relevant qualitative and quantitative studies on the effects of music on advertising can be seen in Table 3. Sound Advertising Citation 18 Table 3 Summary of Relevant Effectual Research Involving Advertising and Music___________________ Sample Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results______________________ Gorn 1982 244 undergraduates Music appeal Brand attitude Hearing liked or disliked music while being exposed to a product can directly affect product preferences. Allen & Madden (1985) 60 undergraduates Music appeal Brand attitude Hearing liked or disliked music while being exposed to a product did not directly affect product preferences. Park & Young 120 women (1986) Music presence/absence, Involvement (high/low) (TV ads) Brand attitude/information Music had a facilitative effect on brand attitude for subjects in the low involvement condition and a distracting effect for those in the cognitive involvement condition. Sewall & Sarel (1986) 200 mall shoppers/ 832 radio ads Music background Brand recall Background music had no significant effect Pitt & Abratt (1988) 172 undergraduate Music appeal students Brand attitude Hearing liked or disliked music while being exposed to a product did not directly affect product preferences. Sound Advertising Citation Sample 19 Table 3 (continued)________________________________________________________ Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results___________________________ Macklin (1988) 75 preschoolers Music background Presence Ad attitude Brand attitude Brand recall Music did not enhance outcomes. Stout & Leckenby (1988) 1498 mall shoppers/ 50 TV ads Music tempo; mode; volume; presence Cognitive/ Affective responses Music had only minor effects. Kellaris & Cox 302 (1989) undergraduates Music appeal Brand attitude No evidence that product preferences can be conditioned through a single exposure to appealing or unappealing music. (Classical Conditioning) Alpert & Alpert (1990) 48 undergraduate students Music Mood Purchase intention Music had a significant effect on moods and purchase intention. Kellaris & Kent (1991) 180 undergraduates Music tempo/ modality Music evaluation Purchase intention Tempo and Modality influenced arousal and intent. MacInnis & Park (1991) 178 undergraduate women Music fit/ indexicality (TV ads) Message processing Indexicality and fit affect the processing of both high- and low-involvement consumers, influencing message- and non-messagebased processing. Sound Advertising 20 Table 3 (continued)____________________________________________________ Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results_______________________ Citation Sample Wallace (1991) 120 subjects Music placementSung/ spoken words (jingles/ ballads) Brand recall Music provides a retrieval cue. Music acts as a frame which the text is tightly fit. Yalch (1991) 103 undergraduates Music placementslogans with and without music (jingles) Brand recall Music enhances memory for advertising slogans when the slogans were incorporated into an advertisement in the form of a jingle or song. Kellaris & Rice (1993) 52 undergraduates Music tempo, Loudness, Gender Music responses Gender moderates the influence of loudness resulting in females responding more positively to music at lower volumes Kellaris et. al. (1993) 231 undergraduates Music-message fit, Attention-gaining value Brand recall/ Recognition of brand name and messages Increasing audience attention to music enhances message reception when the music evokes message-congruent thoughts. Sound Advertising 21 Table 3 _________________________________________ (continued)__________________________________________________ Citation Sample Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results_____________________ Brooker & Wheatley (1994) 100 participants Music tempo/ placement (radio ads) Ad attitudes Brand attitudes Purchase intention Brand recall Tempo had effects on perception of music but no effect on DV’s. Placement had a stronger effect on DV’s. Kellaris & Kent (1994) 288 undergraduates Music tempo/ Tonality/ Texture Pleasure/Arousal Tempo affected pleasure and arousal. Tonality affected pleasure and surprise. Texture moderated tempo and tonality on pleasure. Middlestadt et al. (1994) 97 undergraduates Music presence Brand attitude Belief-based change Wallace (1994) 64 undergraduates Music melody Brand recall Text is better recalled when it is heard as a song rather than as speech, provided the music repeats so that it is easily learned. Wheatley & Brooker (1994) 144 undergraduate Music students and their presence/absence parents Spokespersons (radio ads) Brand recall Cognitive response Music hindered message recall and did not enhance attention. Sound Advertising 22 Table 3 (continued)_________________________________________________ Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results______________________ Citation Sample Olsen (1995) 144 undergraduate students Music presence/absence (music/silence) Brand recall/ attribute importance Silence effectively increases listener retention of ad information especially when the highlighted information was the last item of a series. Kellaris & Mantel (1996) 85 undergraduate students Music arousal/ congruity (radio ads) Ad time Arousal was found to moderate the influence of stimulus congruity on perceived time such that congruity contributed positively to retrospective duration estimates among subjects exposed to soothing (versus arousing) music. Morris & Boone (1998) 90 undergraduates Music presence/absence (print ads) Emotional response Brand attitude Purchase Intention Music affected emotional response of print ads. No effect on brand attitude or purchase intention. Roehm (2001) 48 MBA students/44 community people Music placement versus vocals (radio ads) Brand recall Instrumentals produced greater recall of the message if the individuals knew the song. Vocals produced greater recall of the lyrics if the individuals did not know the song. Sound Advertising 23 Citation Sample Table 3 (continued)____________________________________________________ Independent Variables Dependent Variables Results______________________ North et. al. (2004) 162 participants Music/voice fit Attitude toward the ad Brand recall Purchase Intention Musical fit resulted in better recall of products, brands, and claims, attitude toward the ad, and purchase intention. Voice fit resulted in better recall of claims, attitude toward the ad, and purchase intention. Alpert et. al. (2005) 75 undergraduate students Music mood Purchase intention When music is used to evoke emotions congruent with the symbolic meaning of product purchase, the likelihood of purchasing is enhanced. Zhu, Rui & Meyers-Levy, Joan (2005) 77/109 undergraduates Music meanings Brand attitude (embodied/referential) Intensive processors are sensitive to music meanings. Shen & Chen (2006) 130 students Music fit When the music does not fit (music incongruity) it can have an adverse effect on attitudes toward the ad. Ad attitude Sound Advertising 24 Future Research It’s been over twenty years since Hecker (1984, p. 7) called music “the catalyst of advertising.” He suggested that “researchers can better understand and use this magic if they understand that music is too important to be wasted on amorphous, irrelevant, or inappropriate goals.” While much has been done, there is still great “potential for real research into music’s effectiveness” (Dunbar, 1990). Bruner (1990, p. 102) reinforced in his review of literature that the relevant body of research was indeed still “meager” and North and Hargreaves (1997, p. 282) concluded that the “field is [still] under-investigated.” A number of the studies reviewed have suggested additional research that either has not been undertaken or needs additional investigation. These include more collaboration between the private and academic sectors, a longitudinal study on the amount of music in advertising, more attention to the effect of music on brand image, the use of different experimental environments, and finally a richer investigation of the effect of music fit and indexicality on advertising. Advertisers and Academics Alpert and Alpert (1991, p. 236) called for more “cooperative efforts between academic researchers and industry practitioners applying musical theories to advertising executioners.” Too often the efforts remain separate with the industry findings unavailable. Joint efforts would benefit both parties to better understand “when and why music works in advertising” (p. 236). Many advertisers use popular music in their advertising especially The Gap and Old Navy. The Gap primarily uses original lyrics with Old Navy utilizing altered lyrics. Since they are both owned by the same parent company, it would be beneficial to not only conduct an academicallybased effectual study as well as to correlate the findings with sales results during the campaign. Sound Advertising 25 At the very least, private research should be made available to the academic community to verify the results and provide a practical basis for the use of music in advertising. Advertising and Popular Music It is clear that there is a need to more consistently quantify and trend the use of music, especially popular music, in advertising. Stewart and Furse (1986, p. 160) lamented the absence of its systematic measurement and set out to provide such documentation. In a later report, Stewart, Farmer, and Stannard (1990) recommended that additional research was needed in this area. Nonetheless, only Appelbaum and Halliburton (1993) and Allan (2006b) heeded this recommendation by analyzing international commercials. A longitudinal investigation of the amount of music in advertising, especially popular music, should be conducted to track its proliferation and progression. Since the use of popular music in advertising continues to prosper, more needs to also be known about the effects. In fact, Kellaris and Kent (1991, p. 248) suggested further study into the effects of musical components on the processing of verbal material (e.g., song lyrics, advertising messages). More needs to be done with popular music and altered lyrics (e.g. eBay, Old Navy). This could include more diverse demographics and “the role of listener characteristics in shaping responses to music (Kellaris & Kent, 1994, p. 397). Additionally, while comparisons have been made between some placements/treatments (vocals/instrumentals, etc.), a larger study needs to be done comparing all possible treatments (vocals, instrumentals, jingles, altered vocals, and a control treatment of silence). Also apparent is the need for further research into the role of background music (Alpert & Alpert, 1990, p. 130). It is also clear that we need further “investigations of advertisements with songs that represent varied styles of popular music” (Roehm, 2001, p. 57). Sound Advertising 26 Finally, McChesney (2001) called popular music and advertising the “bankruptcy of culture” and Burns (1996) called it “disturbing and even shocking.” Advertisers who use it consider it a “marriage of art and commerce” and Allan (2005) suggested that the combination of popular music and advertising creates “a new cultural product.” Clearly, more attention needs to be given to the social and ethical implications of the use of popular music and advertising. This can be done as part of a longitudinal study. Brand Image Stout and Lechenby (1988, p. 223) called for more research into “the contribution made by music to brand image.” It is clear that many advertisers are using music to create an image (e.g., iPod). What is not so clear is music’s effectiveness or the implications for the music and/or artists (e.g., U2). A related area of consideration is the use of music in the imaging of a brand as a distribution channel (e.g., Starbucks). Each of these areas of research is certainly underdeveloped and warrants further investigation. Additionally, some popular music and artists are actually being exposed and “branded” through advertising (e.g., Modest Mouse) and placement in television shows (e.g., Grey’s Anatomy). The effects of advertising and media on popular music could also be investigated. Environments Scott (1990, p. 234) argued that “we must not let our methods drive our theories but must instead design our methods in a way that can encompass whatever theory seems articulate enough to fully describe the phenomenon.” Studies need to be conducted under more realistic viewing and listening conditions. Bruner (1990) agreed and called for the raising of “level of experimental sophistication” (p. 100). Far too many studies continue to be done in isolated lab environments. One possible idea would be to observe and survey customers in The Gap or Old Sound Advertising 27 Navy when their respective advertisements are played in the store. Additionally, due to their growing distribution of music, Starbucks provides a potentially effective field experiment location. Fit and Indexicality More studies need to further investigate fit (a person’s perception of the music’s relevance) and indexicality (a person’s emotion-laden memories). This research can be expanded to look at various genres and eras of music with special attention given to the personal relevance of the music/effect of processing (ELM). Additionally, more focus needs to be given to “the study of executional cues and their processing implications for high- and low-involvement consumers’ ad processing” (MacInnis & Park, 1991, p. 172). It may be that music can influence a person’s involvement with advertising due to some conceptualization of involvement (Zaichkowsky, 1986) such as personal, object, or situational characteristics. A great deal of contemporary advertising utilizes classic rock music evidently targeted towards 25-54 males, yet some teenagers are also listening to this genre of music from the 1970s. It may certainly be involving to both demographics with much different indexicality, not to mention fit. Further investigation must be conducted to determine the consequences. Conclusion So is your advertising sound if you use sound in advertising? The research suggests that music is more likely to positively than negatively affect the consumer’s response to your advertising. That’s the easy part but not very helpful to an agency trying to advise an advertiser on whether or not to use music and what music to use. The use of music in general, and the type of music in particular, must be carefully chosen with the target audience and the desired outcome driving the selection. It is clear from this review that music has been shown to both positively and negatively Sound Advertising 28 stimulate a variety of responses including attention, mood, attitude, and purchase intention. The stimulation of these responses is different based on various characteristics of the music itself including its appeal, presence, tempo, background or foreground, etc. In the case of its appeal, different genres of music from different eras affect different demographics of consumers differently. So it’s complicated. What we do know from this overview of research on music and advertising is that, when used effectively, it can be effective. That “sounds” rather trite but it’s true. An advertiser should not just use any music in a commercial just for the sake of using music. It should be carefully chosen and tested to predict its potential to stimulate a positive response for the brand and/or the ad with the ultimate goal of branding and purchase. There is a substantial amount of research out there to guide the advertiser and this review provides the foundation for that process. There is also much research that needs to be done and this review also provides direction and motivation. REFERENCES Adorno, T. W. (1941). On popular music. Studies in Philosophy and Social Sciences, 9, 17-48. Adorno, T. W. (1976). Introduction to the sociology of music. New York: The Seabury Press. Allan, D. (2005). An essay on popular music in advertising and popular music: Bankruptcy of culture or marriage of art and commerce. Advertising & Society, 6(1). Allan, D. (2006a). Effects of popular music on attention and memory in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 46(4), 1-11. Allan, D. (2006b). Popular music placement in prime-time television commercials. Paper presented at the Association for the Education of Journalism and Mass Communication (AEJMC), San Francisco, CA. Sound Advertising 29 Allan, D. (in press). Sound retailing: A review of experimental evidence on the effects of music on shopping behavior. In T. M. Lowrey (Ed.), Brick & Mortar Shopping in the 21st Century. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Allen, C. T., & Madden, T. J. (1985). A closer look at classical conditioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 301-315. Alpert, J. I., & Alpert, M. I. (1990). Music influences on mood and purchase intentions. Psychology & Marketing, 7(2), 109-133. Alpert, J. I., & Alpert, M. I. (1991). Contributions from a musical perspective on advertising and consumer behavior. Advances in Consumer Research, 18, 232-237. Alpert, M. I., Alpert, J. I., & Maltz, E. N. (2005). Purchase occasion influence on the role of music in advertising. Journal of Business Research, 58(3), 369-376. Appelbaum, U., & Halliburton, C. (1993). How to develop international advertising campaigns that work: the example of the European food and beverage sector. International Journal of Advertising, 12, 223-241. Bartlett, J. C., & Snelus, P. (1980). Lifespan memory for popular songs. American Journal of Psychology, 93(3), 551-560. Bower, G. H., & Bolton, L. S. (1969). Why are rhymes easy to learn? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82(3), 453-461. Brooker, G., & Wheatley, J. J. (1994). Music and radio advertising: effects of tempo and placement. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 286-290. Bruner, G. C. (1990). Music, mood, and marketing. Journal of Marketing, 54, 94-104. Burns, G. (1996). Popular music, television, and generational identity. Journal of Popular Culture, 30(3), 129-141. Sound Advertising 30 Crowder, R. G., Serafine, M. L., & Repp, B. (1990). Physical interaction and association by contiguity in memory for the words and melodies of songs. Memory & Cognition, 18(5), 469476. Dunbar, D. S. (1990). Music and advertising. International Journal of Advertising, 9(3), 197203. Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationship between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward the object. Human Relations, 16, 233-240. Galizio, M., & Hendrick, C. (1972). Effect of musical accompaniment on attitude: The guitar as a prop for persuasion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 2, 350-359. Gardner, M. P. (1985). Mood states and consumer behavior: A critical review. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 281-300. Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46, 94-101. Hecker, S. (1984). Music for advertising effect. Psychology & Marketing, 1(3/4), 3-8. Kellaris, J. J., & Cox, A. D. (1989). The effects of background music in advertising: A reassessment. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 113-118. Kellaris, J. J., Cox, A. D., & Cox, D. (1993). The effect of background music on ad processing: A contingency explanation. Journal of Marketing, 57, 114-125. Kellaris, J. J., & Kent, R. J. (1991). Exploring tempo and modality effects, on consumer responses to music. Advances in Consumer Research, 18, 243-248. Kellaris, J. J., & Kent, R. J. (1994). An exploratory investigation of responses elicited by music varying in tempo, tonality, and texture. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(4), 381401. Sound Advertising 31 Kellaris, J. J., & Mantel, S. P. (1994). The influence of mood and gender on consumer's time perceptions. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 514-518. Kellaris, J. J., & Mantel, S. P. (1996). Shaping time perceptions with background music: The effect of congruity and arousal on estimates of ad durations. Psychology & Marketing, 13(5), 501-515. Kellaris, J. J., & Rice, R. C. (1993). The influence of tempo, loudness, and gender on responses to music. Psychology & Marketing, 10(1), 15-29. Krugman, H. E. (1965). The impact of television advertising: Learning without involvement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 29, 349-356. Lam, S. Y. (2001). The effects of store environment on shopping behavior: A critical review. Advances in Consumer Research, 28, 190-197. MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W. (1991). The differential role of characteristics of music on high- and low-involvement consumers' processing of ads. Journal of Consumer Research, 18, 161-173. Macklin, M. C. (1988). The relationship between music in advertising and children's responses: an experimental investigation. In S. Hecker & D. W. Stewart (Eds.), Nonverbal Communication in Advertising (pp. 225-245). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. McChesney, R. (2001). Money for nothing: The Media Education Foundation. [video]. Middlestadt, S. E., Fishbein, M., & Chan, D. K.-S. (1994). The effect of music on brand attitudes: Affect- or belief-based change? In E. M. Clark & T. C. Brock & D. W. Stewart (Eds.), Attention, Attitude, and Affect in Response to Advertising (pp. 149-167). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sound Advertising 32 Mitchell, A. A., & Olson, J. C. (1981). Are product attribute beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand attitude? Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 318-332. Morris, J. D., & Boone, M. A. (1998). The effects of music on emotional response, brand attitude, and purchase intent in an emotional advertising condition. Advances in Consumer Research, 25, 518-526. North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1997). Music and consumer behavior. In D. J. Hargreaves & A. C. North (Eds.), The Sociology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1998). The effect of music on atmosphere and purchase intention in a cafeteria. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(24), 2254-2273. North, A. C., MacKenzie, L. C., & Law, R. M. (2004). The effects of musical and voice "fit" on responses to advertisements. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(8), 1675-1708. Olsen, G. D. (1995). Creating the contrast: The influence of silence and background music on recall and attribute importance. Journal of Advertising, 14(4), 29-44. Park, C. W., & Young, S. M. (1986). Consumer response to television commercials: The impact of involvement and background music on brand attitude formation. Journal of Marketing Research, 23, 11-24. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitudes change. New York: Springer-Verlag. Pitt, L. F., & Abratt, R. (1988). Music in advertisements for unmentionable products - a classical conditioning experiment. International Journal of Advertising, 7(2), 130-137. Roehm, M. L. (2001). Instrumental vs. vocal versions of popular music in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 41(3), 49-58. Sound Advertising 33 Rothschild, M. L. (1987). Advertising: From fundamentals to strategies. Lexington, MA: Heath. Rubin, D. C. (1977). Very long-term memory for prose and verse. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(5), 611-621. Salmon, C. (1986). Perspectives on involvement in consumer and communication research. In B. Dervin (Ed.), Progress in Communication Sciences (Vol. 7, pp. 243-268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Schulkind, M. D., Hennis, L. K., & Rubin, D. C. (1999). Music emotion, and autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 27(6), 948-955. Scott, L. M. (1990). Understanding jingles and needledrop: A rhetorical approach to music in advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 223-236. Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration of melody and text in memory for song. Cognition, 16(3), 285-303. Serafine, M. L., Davidson, J., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1986). On the nature of melodytext integration in memory for songs. Journal of Memory & Language, 25(2), 123-135. Sewall, M. A., & Sarel, D. (1986). Characteristics of radio commercials and their recall effectiveness. Journal of Marketing, 50, 52-60. Shanteau, J., & Ptacek, C. H. (1983). Role and implications of averaging processes in advertising. In L. Percy & A. G. Woodside (Eds.), Advertising and consumer psychology (pp. 149-170). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Shen, Y.-C., & Chen, T.-C. (2006). When east meets west: The effect of cultural tone congruity in ad music and message on consumer ad memory and attitude. International Journal of Advertising, 25(1), 51-70. Sound Advertising 34 Shimp, T. A. (1981). Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of consumer brand choice. Journal of Advertising, 10, 9-15. Shuker, R. (1994). Understanding Popular Music. New York: Routledge. Stewart, D. W., Farmer, K. M., & Stannard, C. I. (1990). Music as a recognition cue in advertising-tracking studies. Journal of Advertising Research, 30(4), 39-48. Stewart, D. W., & Furse, D. H. (1986). Effective television advertising: A study of 1000 commercials. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Stewart, D. W., & Koslow, S. (1989). Executional factors and advertising effectiveness: A replication. Journal of Advertising, 18(3), 21-32. Stout, P., & Leckenby, J. D. (1988). Let the music play: Music as a nonverbal element in TV commercials. In S. Hecker & D. W. Stewart (Eds.), Nonverbal Communication in Advertising. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Thorson, E., Chi, A., & Leavitt, C. (1992). Attention, memory, attitude, and conation: A test of advertising hierarchy. Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 366-379. Turley, L. W., & Milliman, R. E. (2000). Atmospheric effects on shopping behavior: A review of experimental evidence. Journal of Business Research, 49, 193-211. Wallace, W. T. (1991). Jingles in advertisements: Can they improve recall? Advances in Consumer Research, 18, 239-242. Wallace, W. T. (1994). Memory for music: effect of melody on recall of text. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(6), 1471-1485. Sound Advertising 35 Wheatley, J. J., & Brooker, G. (1994). Music and spokesperson effects on recall and cognitive response to a radio advertisement. In E. M. Clark & T. C. Brock & D. W. Stewart (Eds.), Attention, Attitude, and Affect in Response to Advertising (pp. 189-204). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Yalch, R. F. (1991). Memory in a jingle jungle: Music as a mnemonic device in communicating advertising slogans. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(2), 268-275. Yalch, R. F., & Spangenberg, E. (1993). Using store music for retail zoning. Paper presented at the Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT. Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1986). Conceptualizing involvement. Journal of Advertising, 15(2), 4-14. Zhu, R., & Meyers-Levy. (2005). Distinguishing between meanings of music: When background music affects product perceptions. Journal of Marketing Research, 42, 333-345. Correspondence should be sent to: David Allan, Ph.D. Department of Marketing Saint Joseph's University 5600 City Avenue Philadelphia, PA 19131 phone: 610-660-1637 e-mail: dallan@sju.edu