Information Exchange and Use in GSS and Verbal Group Decision Making: Effects of Minority Influence Alan R. Dennis, Kelly M. Hilmer, Nolan J. Taylor, Anthony Polito Dept. of Management, Terry College of Business, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602 adennis@uga.cc.uga.edu Abstract The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of GSS use on the exchange and use of information in group decision making under two conditions: when there was and was not a majority/minority split of opinion in the group. When there was a distinct majority/minority, groups using a GSS exchanged more information, made better decisions, and took no more time than when they did not use a GSS. In the uniform treatment where there was no majority/minority, groups using a GSS exchanged more information, but made worse decisions, and took more time than when they did not use a GSS. 1. Introduction Individual managers seldom have access to all relevant information, so when truly important decisions have to be made, a group is typically formed to make the decision or to advise the individual who must make it [9]. Group discussion enables members to share information so that the group as a whole can access a larger pool of information than any one member acting alone [23]. However, information exchange in group decision making is often done poorly; much unique information known to some group members is never shared with the group [24]. Individuals, particularly low status participants, may withhold information due to apprehension about the group’s reaction to it or may feel pressured to conform to the views of the group majority [7, 9, 13]. This unique information can be important, leading to poor decisions when it is not considered [8, 25]. Information technologies such as Group Support Systems (GSS) have the potential to fundamentally change the nature of information exchange in group discussion [6, 20]. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of GSS use on the exchange and use of information in group decision making under two conditions: when there was and was not a majority-minority split of opinion in the group. 2. Information and group decision making 2.1. Information distribution In decision making situations, group members typically know a host of information about possible alternatives. This information shapes members' pre-discussion preferences and, as members communicate information during discussion, shapes the group's decision. The information that members have can be distributed in a variety of ways. It can be known to all members before group discussion (“common”), known to only one before group discussion (“unique”) or known to some but not all before discussion (“partially shared”). Unique information can be distributed so that it is unbiased; that is, each group member has a balanced set of information that indicates the optimal alternative. In this case, the group should reach the same decision with or without the unique information. However, information can be distributed so that the optimal alternative is hidden; that is, the common information indicates a non-optimal alternative, but if the group were to pool the unique information, they would be able to discover the optimal alternative. This situation is called a "hidden profile task" by Stasser [24]. In this case, if discussion does not raise all available but unique information, the group likely will make a poor decision. Here, the value of unique information is high, but empirical studies have found that much of it is not exchanged [26, 27]. In this paper, we focus on two conditions of information distribution in hidden profile tasks. One well-studied condition is when a majority/minority exists in the group [19]. In this case, a majority of group members favor one alternative (a sub-optimal one) and a minority favor a second alternative (the optimal one). Another, lessstudied, condition is when no majority/minority exists in the group; members are evenly divided among the alternatives in their opinions. We term this a “uniform” condition. We begin with the uniform condition and then consider how this may change when a majority/minority exists. 2.2. Information exchange and use In order to reach a group decision, members engage in three activities simultaneously: information recall (either from memory or notes), information exchange (either giving or receiving information), and information processing (actually using the information: assessing the cognitive and social implications of the information and storing it in memory) [2]. Humans have a limited amount of cognitive resources to spread across these three activities [1]. Most people can engage in only one activity at one time, so that engaging in any one activity limits the ability to engage in the other two. For example, actively listening to discussion blocks the processing of newly received information and the recall of related information [13]. We consider each activity in turn. 2.2.1. Information recall. Information recall of the group as a whole is biased toward common information, due to the laws of probability. Common information known by all members is more likely to be remembered by at least one individual than is unique information known only to one member [27]. Thus, groups are more likely to focus discussions on common rather than unique information, because common information is more likely to be recalled. Yet in a hidden profile task, it is the unique information that is critical to success; without adequate consideration of the unique information, group members will miss the optimal decision. 2.2.2. Information exchange. The exchange of information in verbal discussion is not perfect. Members must first decide to contribute the information, and then have the opportunity to contribute it. Individuals are often motivated to defend or support their initial preference, so the information they choose to contribute often favors their preferences or challenges an alternative [26]. Information contradicting the preferences of the majority is also less likely to be exchanged [9], because members may fear a negative evaluation of their comments [7, 13]. In verbal discussion, only one group member can speak at one time, so all other members are blocked from contributing. This "production blocking" significantly reduces the exchange of information because members who are prevented from contributing information as they choose, often forget or suppress it because it seems less relevant or less important later in the discussion [7, 13]. In a situation where a group majority favors one alternative, the tendency to support one’s own choice will result in a majority of members contributing information that supports the majority choice. Likewise, the tendency not to contribute information contradicting the group majority (e.g., information challenging it, or favoring a competing alternative) may also result in incomplete information exchange. The result may be a very biased exchange of information that favors the majority alternative, further reinforcing its apparent superiority. 2.2.3. Information processing and use. Once information is exchanged, members must actually process it before it can affect decisions. Petty and Cacioppo [21] argue that there are two “routes” by which information is processed. The first is the “central route,” in which members actively assess the information and integrate it into their overall understanding of the situation and their preferences (also called persuasive arguments or information influence). This theory argues that unique information not previously known should be more persuasive than information already known and considered [18, 32]. However, empirical evidence has found the opposite: unique information is more likely to be ignored, while common information known to all members prior to discussion has a greater influence on the groups decision [28]. There are at least two plausible reasons. First, during verbal discussions, members must constantly pay attention to other members or miss their contributions, and therefore the unique information first received during discussion is processed only superficially and not considered as fully as information studied before discussion. Second, information that supports prediscussion choices is more thoroughly processed and integrated into an individual’s cognitive schema [21], while information challenging initial decisions is discounted [14, 34]. The processing of information is also affected by the presence of a majority opinion within the group. When faced by a group majority whose preferences are different from theirs, members assume the majority to be correct, and focus on comparing their preferences to the majority's. "The world of alternatives is reduced to two" [19, p. 25]. This stimulates a comparison process of convergent thought, of viewing the issues from the majority's preference [17]. In contrast, when faced by a minority with a different preference, members are motivated to assume that minority members are incorrect, and initially dismiss them [15, 19]. However, if the minority is consistent in presenting information and opinions, and does not yield to majority pressure, members realize that the minority is convinced of its position. They make attempts to understand it -- a validation process of divergent thought [17]. Information is re-examined from many perspectives, not just that of the minority, to understand why it has led to different conclusions [15, 19]. Information from minority members may be more thoroughly processed than information from the majority because minority members are perceived to be more independent or more dissimilar from one another than are members of the majority [10]. The second route by which information is processed is the “peripheral route,” in which members’ preferences are shaped more by peripheral cues such the number of people arguing for a position or their attractiveness, rather than the quality of the information itself (also called social comparison or normative influence). This theory argues that members are motivated to conform to the opinions of others to preserve a favorable self-perception and self-presentation [18]. After obtaining information about others' preferences, members may change their preference to more closely match others, either through choice or coercion [9]. Also, publicly stating a preference may make that preference stronger; public commitment may make changing preferences more difficult without losing face [22] and may cause stronger internalization to reduce cognitive dissonance [18]. In a majority/minority situation, normative influence is usually quite strong [15]. Members may not actually change their preference, but just state a more socially acceptable preference than their true preference. They may simply comply with the majority preference to avoid further stress, without examining any information [15]. 2.3. GSS effects on information exchange and use GSS provide new opportunities for information exchange that may differ in important ways from non-GSS environments. One of these is electronic communication that can augment or replace verbal communication. This electronic communication provides a package of many different components, each of which may introduce new dynamics into the information exchange process. Three components in particular should affect information exchange and use: parallelism, group memory, and anonymity [20]. The first, parallelism, is the ability of all group members to enter information at the same time. All group members have computer workstations, which enable them to contribute information and opinions by typing ideas, which are immediately shared with all other members. Since all members can type at the same time, no participant need wait for others to finish before contributing information. This parallelism mitigates the blocking that inhibits the exchange of information in verbally interacting groups [31]. The second, group memory, means that all remarks typed into the computer are stored so that members can refer to them later in the discussion. One of the key problems in verbal discussion is that listening to group discussion blocks processing of new information, and information processing blocks the receipt of new information from discussion. A group memory enables members to enter or read information at their discretion; they can more easily pause to process information without risk of missing information. The third component is anonymity; group members make contributions without attaching their names, which may motivate them to participate differently. Anonymity may reduce the reluctance to contribute information that contradicts the dominant group preference [20]. Members are also often motivated to defend or support their initial preference, so the information they choose to contribute favors their preferences or attacks an alternative [26, 27]. To the extent that this motivation stems from the need to save face about a publicly advocated decision preference, anonymity, which can obscure both the initial public statement of preference as well as any subsequent arguments for or against it, may reduce the tendency to contribute information supporting one's initial preferences. 2.4. Summary and hypotheses 2.4.1. Information exchange. There is often an incomplete exchange of information in verbal discussions, which may lead to poor decisions. GSS use may increase the amount of information exchanged because it provides parallelism (which reduces blocking) and anonymity (which may reduce the reluctance to contribute information challenging one's own preference or that of the majority). These factors are likely to affect common and unique information equally. Therefore: H1: Members of GSS groups will exchange more common information and more unique information than members of non-GSS groups. In a majority/minority situation, there is more pressure on minority members to accede to the majority, encouraging less discussion of the available information; they may simply agree with the majority without much discussion because swaying the majority will be more difficult than swaying the few members required in the uniform situation. The shield provided by anonymity will be more strongly felt when there is a majority/minority within the group. Minority members who oppose the preference favored by the group majority will be more likely to oppose the majority viewpoint when they use a GSS. Because they will present their opinions more strongly and are more likely to persist in their opinions, they are likely to exchange more information. Therefore: H2: The difference in information exchange between GSS and non-GSS groups will be the greatest in majority/minority groups. 2.4.2. Information processing and use. As argued above, GSS should better enable group members to process information because it provides a group memory that enables members to review information at will. Previous GSS research has found GSS use to increase the use of factual information rather than members’ preferences; that is, to increase information influence rather than normative influence [5, 12]. H3: Members of GSS groups are more likely to consider the information exchanged in making their decisions than are members of non-GSS groups. There will be different effects on information processing between majority/minority groups and uniform groups. Minority members are more likely to accede to the majority without the equal time provided by parallelism and the shield of anonymity. In GSS groups, minority members are likely to present their opinions more strongly and persist longer, and therefore are more likely to trigger the intensive validation process of Moscovici [17]. Therefore: H4: The difference in information use between GSS and non-GSS groups will be the greatest in majority/minority groups. 2.4.3. Outcomes. The discussion and use of more information should increase the time required to reach a decision compared to non-GSS groups, unless the GSS enables groups to contribute and process information faster. There is evidence that GSS use takes longer, in part because GSS require participants to type, which is slower than talking. [3]. Therefore: H5: GSS groups will take longer to reach a decision than non-GSS groups. In a majority/minority situation, there is more pressure on the minority members to accede to the majority, encouraging less discussion and less consideration of information. The equal time provided by parallelism and the shield of anonymity will encourage minority members to present their opinions more strongly and to persist in their opinions, requiring more time. Therefore: H6: The difference in time taken between GSS and nonGSS groups will be the greatest in majority/minority groups. In certain situations, such as hidden profile tasks, greater use of unique information will enable groups to better identify the optimal alternative. Anonymity may also reduce the pressure to conform to others' preferences, and reduce the loss of face in backing down from previous positions, making it easier for members to change their preferences. Therefore: H7: When information distribution is biased, members of GSS groups will make better decisions than members of non-GSS groups. In a majority/minority situation, the minority members are more likely to accede to the group majority, unless the parallelism and anonymity encourages more thorough exchange and use of the available information. Therefore: H8: The difference in decision quality between GSS and non-GSS groups will be the greatest in majority/minority groups. 3. Method 3.1. Subjects One hundred and fifty sophomore, junior, and senior business students drawn from a core business course participated as members of 15 ten-person groups (5 majority/minority task groups; 10 uniform task groups). The average age was 21 years; 56 % were male. 3.2. Independent variables This study used a 2 x 2 repeated measures design, crossing GSS use with the decision situation (majority/minority or uniform). Each group was assigned into either the majority/ minority or uniform condition, and performed two tasks, one using a GSS and one without. The order of the two tasks and the GSS and verbal treatments were reversed for half the groups. In the non-GSS treatment, subjects interacted verbally. In the GSS treatment, subjects used TCBWorks, a GSS developed at the University of Georgia [30] that provided electronic communication with parallelism, anonymity, and group memory. Subjects were instructed to use the software to exchange information until their first vote. After the first vote they were instructed to use both the software and to discuss the issues verbally. The majority/minority task was designed such that seven subjects received a sub-set of information favoring the same sub-optimal alternative, while three received a sub-set of information favoring the optimal alternative. In the uniform task three subjects received a sub-set of information favoring the optimal alternative, four received a sub-set of information favoring a sub-optimal the alternative and three received information favoring a different sub-optimal alternative. Any session in which the tasks did not produce the intended majority/minority (defined as at least six members favoring the sub-optimal alternative) or an uniform split (defined as no more than five members favoring the sub-optimal choice and at least three favoring the optimal choice) for both tasks was discarded. We studied 21 groups, but only 15 met these criteria and were used in the study; i.e., the intended task split failed in one of the two tasks for six groups (a failure rate of six in 42 tasks or 14%). 3.3. Tasks There were two tasks. Subjects performed both tasks, one in the GSS condition and the other in the non-GSS condition. Subjects were informed at the start of each task that they each had only a subset of all available information and would have to share their information to make a good decision. Both tasks underwent extensive design and pilot testing for both the complete information version and the incomplete information versions of the task given to each individual subject (i.e., at least six rounds of redesign and pilot testing and as many as ten for one set of incomplete information versions that proved difficult to refine). One task asked subjects to select one student from a set of four students for admission to the university. The complete information set included positive, negative, or neutral information on 18 criteria for each alternative for a total of 72 pieces of information. Five pieces of information for all four alternatives (verbal SAT, math SAT, GPA, sex, major) were "common" -- i.e., given to all subjects. The remaining information was "unique." This task was designed with the assistance of the university Admissions Office. The complete information version of the task was independently validated by three admissions officers (the director of admissions and two associate directors) to ensure the alternative designed to be optimal, was optimal. The complete information version was also validated by 28 subjects drawn from the same subject pool but who did not participate in the study. Twenty-two (79%) chose the optimal choice; five (19%) chose next alternative; one (2%) chose the third alternative. The second task asked subjects to select a computer to be the “standard” computer required of all College of Business students from a set of four computers. The complete information set included positive, negative, or neutral information on 18 criteria for each alternative for a total of 56 pieces of information. Four pieces of information for all four alternatives (price, CPU, RAM, hard drive size) were "common." The remaining information was "unique." This task was designed in conjunction with the College’s computer lab staff. The chair of the College’s computer users committee (who was also a lab manager) and two instructors independently validated the task to ensure the alternative designed to be optimal, was optimal. The complete information version of the task was validated by 21 subjects drawn from the same subject pool but who did not participate in the study. Sixteen (76%) chose the optimal choice; four (19%) chose the second best alternative; one (5%) chose the third alternative. 3.4. Procedures The procedures followed those of Stasser and Titus [26]. Subjects first read the task and made an individual decision. Then they worked together as a group (using either a GSS or no GSS) to arrive at a unanimous group decision or until 25 minutes had elapsed. (Some subjects using the GSS may have been less experienced with computers than others, but this was not seen as a hindrance since groups using GSS were trained on the GSS before starting the group task. These subjects were shown how to add and view comments using the GSS.) Subjects then individually completed the post-session questionnaire that asked them to make another individual decision (the same as or different from the group decision) and to report several perceptions. After a five-minute break, these steps were repeated for the second treatment. 3.5. Measures 3.5.1. Information exchange. The amount of information exchanged was measured by three raters, who counted the pieces of correct information in the group discussions (video-tapes for non-GSS treatments; printed computer records and video-tapes for GSS treatments). The video camera failed during the last three sessions (it appeared to be recording but did not), so only the data for the 12 sessions for which the video tapes were available were used in these analysis). A set of six randomly selected groups (three uniform, three majority/minority) was selected to determine inter-rater reliability. The raters agreed on .94 of the ratings (measured as 1 - (number of disagreements/information exchanged)), indicating adequate agreement. 3.5.2. Information processing and use. Determining information processing and use was done in three ways. First, a perceptual measure was used. The post-session questionnaire included two scales designed to measure perceived information usage: “I-thought” (i.e., the extent to which the respondent thought about the information contributed by others, 3 items, Cronbach alpha=.84, 1=low, 7=high), and “others-thought” (i.e., the extent to which the respondent believed other group members thought about the information contributed by respondent, 3 items, alpha=.85, 1=low, 7=high). While perceptions of information usage can be unreliable, they are one way of triangulating on this concept [21]. Second, information use was considered by examining the information recalled by each group member at the end of the group discussions [4]. Information recalled at the end of the experiment must have been received and processed during discussions because information must be processed to be stored in memory [27]. Subjects were asked to list all information they remembered in a free recall question on the post-session questionnaire. Two raters coded the questionnaires and counted the number of correct information provided. Inter-rater agreement was again adequate (.93). Third, information use was considered by examining the unique information learned by each group member at the end of the group discussions. Information learned is the information subjects recalled at the end of the experiment that they did not have in their initial task descriptions [4]. This was done using the information coded above, but counting only the information that the subject did not have at the beginning of group discussion. 3.5.3. Outcome measures. Decision time was measured by the number of minutes required for the group to reach a decision. Decision quality was measured by the number of groups making the optimal decision. Three additional perceptual measures were also included on the post-session questionnaire, all with scales of 1=low, 7=high: satisfaction (4 items, alpha=.83), apprehension (4 items, alpha=.78), and information credibility (i.e., the degree of accuracy that subjects attributed to information contributed by others, 4 items, alpha=.82). 4. Results 4.1. Information exchange H1 was partially supported. Groups exchanged more unique information when using the GSS than when not but there was no difference in the amount of common information exchanged (see Tables 1 and 2 for the means and all statistical results). H2 was not supported. There was no interaction between the majority/minority status of the groups and GSS use. We used three measures of information use. The first were perceptual data. Participants perceived themselves and others to have thought about information to a lesser extent when using the GSS than when not. There were interaction effects indicating that members of the majority/minority groups perceived themselves and others to have thought less about the information than members of the uniform groups. The second measure was the recall of information. There were significant interaction effects on the recall of both common and unique information between the use of GSS and the majority/minority and uniform treatments. Participants in the uniform treatment recalled less common and less unique information when using the GSS. Members of majority/minority groups recalled more common information when using the GSS, but there were no differences in the recall of unique information. The third measure was the recall of unique information unknown at the start of the discussions (i.e., the information learned from the discussion). Participants learned less initially unknown information when working with the GSS, suggesting that they were less likely to process previously unknown unique information received during the discussion. 4.2. Information processing and use Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations Uniform GSS Verbal Group Measures mean std mean std Common information exchanged 9.40 4.84 5.88 3.18 Unique information exchanged 12.30 2.63 5.00 3.55 Common information recalled 3.91 3.67 4.58 3.61 Unique information recalled 2.12 2.10 3.78 2.86 Information learned 1.19 1.44 2.32 2.25 Time 1400 205 422 227 Decision quality 0.10 0.32 0.80 0.42 Subject Perceptions (1= low, 7=high) I thought 5.25 1.52 5.63 1.19 Others thought 4.83 1.30 5.12 1.31 Satisfaction 5.44 1.33 5.68 1.18 Apprehension 1.97 1.12 2.52 1.28 Information credibility 4.21 1.65 4.80 1.42 Majority/minority GSS Verbal mean std mean std 10.20 6.05 8.25 1.71 13.40 7.83 12.25 4.35 5.28 4.04 4.20 2.93 3.54 4.63 3.76 4.53 2.12 3.11 2.72 2.84 943 331 848 603 0.60 0.55 0.00 0.00 4.92 4.91 5.85 1.68 4.17 1.75 1.38 1.10 0.78 1.80 6.01 5.75 6.11 1.96 5.17 Table 2: Statistical Results Group Measures Common information exchanged Unique information exchanged Common information recalled Unique information recalled Information learned df 8 8 146 145 145 GSS F p 1.91 ns 7.54 .025 0.34 ns 10.12 .002 15.28 .001 Minority F p 1.17 ns 1.06 ns 0.01 ns 1.68 ns 0.28 ns GSS x Minority F p 0.03 ns 3.28 ns 6.36 .013 9.80 .001 2.46 ns 0.91 0.98 0.75 1.03 1.42 Time Decision quality Subject Perceptions I thought Others thought Satisfaction Apprehension Information credibility 11 11 16.11 0.17 .002 ns 0.22 1.41 ns ns 7.69 21.48 .018 .001 146 146 146 146 146 22.42 18.19 4.95 14.66 21.63 .001 .001 .028 .001 .001 0.19 2.48 5.00 3.72 0.30 ns ns .027 ns ns 4.99 4.83 0.08 1.15 1.05 .027 .030 ns ns ns These three measures indicate that groups in the uniform treatment were less likely to process and use the information present in their discussions when using the GSS. The perceptual and learning measures suggest that participants in majority/minority groups were less likely to process and use the information when using the GSS, but the recall measures suggest otherwise. H3 is not supported. H4 is partially supported. 4.3. Outcomes There was a main effect on time due to GSS use, but also an interaction effect. Groups took longer to reach a decision when using a GSS in the uniform condition, but there were few differences in the majority/minority condition. H5 was partially supported, but H6 was not. The effects on decision quality were rather unusual, producing an interaction effect but no main effects. In the majority/minority treatment, groups made significantly better decisions when using the GSS. The reverse was true for the uniform condition: groups made significantly better decisions when not using the GSS. H7 was not supported, but H8 was supported. Participants in the uniform treatment reported less satisfaction, less apprehension, and less credibility in the information discussed when using the GSS than when interacting verbally. Participants in the majority/minority treatments reported more satisfaction but no differences in apprehension or information credibility. 5. Discussion The objective of this study was to examine the information exchange and use processes in group decision making. The results were rather surprising. 5.1. Majority/minority groups In groups that had a pre-discussion majority favoring a sub-optimal alternative, verbal interaction without a GSS led to poor decisions: groups almost uniformly choose the sub-optimal alternative favored by the group majority. The exchange of unique information key to the optimal decision was done poorly; very little of the information needed to identify the optimal alternative was contributed. The final group decision depended more on the biased information members received before the meeting than the unique information first exchanged during the meeting. For these groups, the use of GSS added considerable value. When using the GSS, these same groups exchanged more unique information and almost uniformly made the optimal decision in no more time than when not using the GSS. Without the GSS, normative influence dominated, and the group majority convinced the minority to accede to its preference. With the GSS, information influence dominated, and the minority was able to induce the majority to listen to the information presented and to convince them to change their minds to the optimal alternative. We attribute the value of the GSS to its ability to provide parallelism, anonymity, and group memory. 5.2. Uniform groups The situation was quite different in the uniform treatment. When not using the GSS, members exchanged only a small portion of the available information, appeared to think about it more, and yet generally quickly found the optimal decision. When using the GSS, members exchanged more information, but appeared to think about it less, took a longer time, and made a sub-optimal decision or could not agree on a unanimous decision. In short, in the uniform treatment, GSS use impaired the groups’ decision making process. We believe that when there was no majority in the group, the groups tended to focus more on the information than on peripheral normative influence cues. Without the GSS, the groups quickly shared the critical unique information, discussed it, and identified the optimal decision (one group doing so in less than two minutes). Three pieces of evidence suggest that the problem with GSS use in the uniform condition was that participants were less likely to have processed and used the information they received during group discussion when using the GSS: participants perceived themselves and others to have used less information, recalled less common and less unique information, and learned less information when using the GSS than when not using it. The question, is why did this occur? 5.3. Lack of information processing The reasons for the inhibited information processing in uniform groups when using the GSS is unclear. We offer three plausible interpretations for future research: information integration, information credibility, and lack of focus. 5.3.1. Information integration. One possibility may lie in group members' abilities to integrate newly received, unique information into their existing base of information. During discussion, group members must engage in three simultaneous activities (information recall, information exchange, and information use), and because humans have a limited amount of cognitive resources, engaging in any one activity limits the ability to engage in the other two. Factual information requires more cognitive processing than information about others' preferences [21]. Streufert and Swezey [29] argue that if factual information arrives faster than one item every three minutes, individuals have difficulty integrating it with existing information. While we do not believe the volume of information in these studies was very high [27], it clearly required the processing of more than one piece of information every three minutes. In the uniform treatment, there were three competing alternatives (versus two in the majority/minority treatment), and no minority advocating its opinions to focus the analysis on just two alternatives. There was simply more to consider. When using the GSS, members generated about twice as much information as when not using it. Therefore, one plausible explanation for the lack of information processing may lie in the cognitive limitations of individuals' ability to integrate information. Participants may have suffered from information overload and an inability to focus their analysis on the dominant alternatives to a greater extent when using the GSS and therefore failed to process the information it contained as thoroughly as information received in the verbal discussions. By limiting the amount of information available, the production blocking in the non-GSS groups may actually have helped by filtering the available information [11]. Under this interpretation, there are two implications for future research. First, we need research varying the information load to determine if GSS use helps or hinders in situations of high and low information load. Second, we need to better understand how to structure the information in the GSS to minimize overload and enable better integration of information. 5.3.2. Information credibility. Another possibility for the reduced processing of unique information in GSS groups may lie in the credibility of the information. Morley and Walker [16] suggest that to cause a change in a previous decision (in this case, the individual decision made prior to group discussion), information must be important, novel (i.e., not previously processed), and credible. In this case, the unique information received during discussion was both important and novel. However, participants were less likely to find it credible when using the GSS, and unlike in the majority/minority treatment, there was no subgroup lobbying hard for a specific alternative. Thus participants may have intentionally disregarded the information in the GSS. There are at least two plausible explanations for this reduced credibility. The first is a negative anonymity effect on credibility. The credibility of the source of information can be important to the acceptance and processing of a message, particularly when the information is ambiguous or difficult to process [21]. It may be that the anonymity of the GSS used in this study made the information more suspect, because it was more difficult to verify the source's credibility. Anonymity may also conceal the number of information sources. Information perceived to be from several independent sources is seen as more credible than information from one source [10, 31]. Thus information from several distinct individual sources in the non-GSS groups may have been seen as more credible than the pool of information from an unknown number of sources in the GSS. A second reason for reduced credibility may be a reduced ability to challenge the information’s contributor. In most non-GSS groups, we observed at least one (usually several) occasions when unique information presented to the group was immediately challenged (e.g., "Really? I didn't get that."). The contributor would then confirm the validity of the information and discussion would move on. Similar challenges were seen in GSS groups, but the response would come much later or not at all. It may be that the reduced ability to provide immediate confirming feedback reduces the credibility of the information. Under this interpretation, we need additional research to determine if anonymity is useful in this situation. Anonymity has been found to be important for groups whose members have different power and status, but to have fewer benefits for peers [20]. 5.3.3. Information focus. A third possibility may be the ease of ignoring the information because its presentation is unfocused. When a person speaks verbally and contributes information it may focus the group's attention on that information [10] -- it can be difficult to ignore a person speaking right in front of you. In contrast, when using a GSS, information is contained in lines of text in a pool of information. Without a GSS, information is presented to you; with a GSS, you must actively peruse the group memory. When using the GSS, participants may not have actively searched out and reviewed all available information. Under this interpretation, we need research to discover how GSS can better engage participants and encourage them to actively seek and use the information it contains (i.e., a GSS like a video game that draws participants in). Systems," presented at INFORMS, Singapore, 1995. 5.4. Implications for practice [4] Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E. and Morris, K. “Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall, and persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (45) 1983, 805818. This study has several implications for managers striving to understand and improve the exchange and use of information in organizational problem solving. For groups that had a majority/minority, GSS use improved the exchange of information and the quality of the decision. However, for groups without a majority/minority, GSS use impaired the quality of decision making. One implication is to use a GSS only when there are strong differences of opinion within a group, where the GSS’s ability to focus the group on the information will be valuable. We believe that the key problem in using this information for both GSS and non-GSS groups was the difficulty in assessing and integrating the new information received during group discussion into their existing information base, which encouraged members to overlook this information. A second implication is therefore to structure group meetings to provide sufficient time for members to study, assess, and integrate this new information before they must make decisions. Separating information exchange from the decision making that uses it is one option. Members could hold one meeting to discuss a problem or opportunity, break for several hours or days to evaluate and integrate the information, and hold a second meeting to make the actual decision. This structure is similar to the "incubation" period recommended for idea generation. A third implication deals with knowledge of having all of the information. We informed subjects that they had incomplete information and would need to share information. In a meeting, participants do not know if they have complete information. Do they assume that they do have all of the information needed? The answer to this may change how GSS is used or not used. Clearly another alternative is to develop new group processes and GSS tools to help members better integrate information, but this will have to wait for more research. 6. References [1] Ball, S.A. and Zuckerman, M. “Sensation Seeking and Selective Attention: Focused and Divided Attention on a Dichotic Listening Task,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (63) 1992, 825-831. [2] Briggs, R.O. "Focus Theory: An Explanation of Group Productivity for Developers and Users of Electronic Meeting [3] Benbasat, I. and Lim, L.H. “The Effects of Group, Task, Context, and Technology Variables on the Usefulness of Group Support Systems: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies,” Small Group Research, (24), 1993, 430-462. [5] Clapper, D.L., McLean, E.R. and Watson, R.T. “An Experimental Investigation of the Effect of a Group Decision Support System on Normative Influence in Small Groups,” Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Information Systems, New York, 1991, 77-87. [6] DeSanctis, G.L. and Gallupe, R.B. “A Foundation for the Study of Group Decision Support Systems,” Management Science, (33) 1987, 589-609. [7] Diehl, M. and Stroebe W. “Productivity Loss in Brainstorming Groups: Toward the Solution of a Riddle,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (53), 1987, 497509. [8] Gigone, D. and Hastie, R. “The Common Knowledge Effect: Information Sharing and Group Judgment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,” (65), 1993, 959-974. [9] Hackman, J.R and Kaplan, R.E. “Interventions into Group Process: An Approach to Improving the Effectiveness of Groups,” Decision Sciences, (5), 1974, 459-480. [10] Harkins, S.G. and Petty, R.E. “Information Utility and the Multiple Source Effect,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (52), 1987, 260-268. [11] Hiltz, S.R. and Turoff, M. “Structuring ComputerMediated Communication Systems to Avoid Information Overload,” Communications of the ACM, (28:7), 1985, 680. [12] Huang, W., Raman, K.S., and Wei, K.K. “A process Study of the Effects of GSS and Task Type on Informational and Normative Influence in Small Groups,” Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Information Systems, Orlando, 1993, 77-87. [13] Lamm, H. and Trommsdorff, G. “Group versus Individual Performance on Tasks Requiring Ideational Proficiency (Brainstorming): A Review,” European Journal of Social Psychology, 1973, 361-387. [14] Lord, C.G., Ross, L., and Lepper, M.R. “Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (37), 1979, 2098-2109. [15] Maass, A. and Clark, III, R.D. “Hidden Impact of Minorities: Fifteen Years of Minority Influence Research,” Psychological Bulletin, (95), 1984. 428-450. Organizations, 1986, Academic Press, Orlando. [30] TCBWorks at http://tcbworks.mgmt.uga.edu:8080. [16] Morley, D.D. and Walker, K.B. “The Role of Importance, Novelty, and Plausibility in Producing Belief Change,” Communication Monographs, (54), 1987, 436-442. [17] Moscovici, S. “Toward a Theory of Conversion Behavior,” in L. Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Volume 13, New York: Academic Press, 1980, 209-239. [18] Myers, D.G. and Lamm, H. “The Group Polarization Phenomenon,” Psychological Bulletin, (83), 1976, 602-627. [19] Nemeth, C., “Differential Contributions of Minority and Majority Influence," Psychological Review, (93), 1986, 1-10. [20] Nunamaker, Jr., J.F., Dennis, A.R., Valacich, J.S., Vogel, D.R., and George, J.F. “Electronic Meeting Systems to Support Group Work,” Communications of the ACM, (34:7), 1991, 4061. [21] Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. Communication and Persuasion, Springer-Verlag, 1986. New York. [22] Salancik, G.R. “Commitment and the Control of Organizational Behavior and Belief,” in B.M. Staw and G.R. Salancik (Eds.), New Directions in Organizational Behavior, Chicago: St. Clair, 1977, 1-54. [23] Shaw, M. Group Dynamics: The Psychology of Small Group Behavior, (3rd ed.) 1981, New York: McGraw Hill. [24] Stasser, G. “Information Salience and the Discovery of Hidden Profiles by Decision-Making Groups: A ‘Thought Experiment,’” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, (52), 1992, 156-181. [25] Stasser, G. and Stewart, D. “Discovery of Hidden Profiles by Decision-making Groups: Solving a Problem versus making a Judgment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (63), 1992, 426-434. [26] Stasser, G. and Titus, W. “Pooling of Unshared Information in Group Decision making: Biased Information Sampling During Group Discussion” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (48), 1985, 1467-1478. [27] Stasser, G. and Titus, W. “Effects of Information Load and Percentage of Shared Information on the Dissemination of Unshared Information During Group Discussion," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (53), 1987, 81-93. [28] Stasser, G., Taylor, L.A., and Hanna, C. “Information Sampling in Structured and Unstructured Discussions of Threeand Six-Person Groups” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (57), 1989, 67-78. [29] Streutfert, S. and Swezey, R.S. Complexity, Managers, and [31] Valacich, J.S., Dennis, A.R., and Connolly, T. “Group Versus Individual Brainstorming: A New Ending to an Old Story," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, (57), 1994, 448-467. [32] Vinokur, A., Trope, Y., and Burnstein, E. “A Decision Making Analysis of Persuasive Argumentation and the ChoiceShift Effect,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, (11), 1975, 127-148. [33] Wilder, D.A. “Some Determinants of the Persuasive Power of In-Groups and Out-Groups: Organization of Information and Attribution of Independence,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (59), 1990, 1202-1213. [34] Wood, W. “Retrieval of Attitude Relevant Information from Memory: Effects on Susceptibility to Persuasion and on Intrinsic Motivation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (42), 1982, 798-810.