Teacher Expectations Running Head: TEACHER EXPECTATIONS AND STUDENT SELF-PERCEPTIONS Teacher expectations and student self-perceptions: Exploring relationships Christine M. Rubie-Davies, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand This paper is based on a doctoral dissertation by the author completed at the University of Auckland, New Zealand, in April 2004. Submission Date: 10 May 2005 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christine Rubie-Davies, Faculty of Education, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland, New Zealand. Electronic mail may be sent via the internet to: c.rubie@auckland.ac.nz 1 Teacher Expectations 2 Abstract For forty years researchers have been exploring the teacher expectation phenomenon. Few have examined the possibility that teacher expectations may be class-centered rather than individually-centered. The current study aimed to track the self-perception outcomes of students (N = 256) whose teachers had high or low class-level expectations. Students completed the reading, mathematics, physical abilities and peer relations subscales of the SDQ-1 (Marsh, 1990) at the beginning and end of one year. A subscale related to student perception of how the teacher viewed their abilities was added. An overall self-perception score was computed for each subscale. At the beginning of the year there were no statistically significant differences between the expectation groups in any of the academic, teacher opinion or total self-perception scales. By the end of the year statistically significant differences were found mainly due to a decline in the self-perceptions of students with low expectation teachers. Implications for teacher practice are discussed. Teacher Expectations 3 Teacher expectations and student self-perceptions: Exploring relationships For over forty years researchers have been exploring the phenomenon of teacher expectations. A plethora of studies have been conducted which have clearly shown the existence of differential teachers’ expectations for individuals within their classrooms (e.g. Good & Brophy, 2003; Eccles & Wigfield, 1985; Jussim, Smith, Madon & Palumbo, 1998; Pellegrini & Blatchford, 2000) The vast majority of such studies, however, have concentrated on teachers and the ways in which they interact directly or more subtly with individual children for whom they have correspondingly high or low expectations (e.g. Brophy, 1985; Cooper & Good, 1983; Rosenthal, 1991). Very few have empirically examined the possibility that expectations may be teacher-centered, i.e. that there may be teachers who have high or low expectations for all the students in their classrooms (see Rubie-Davies, in review-a, for a recent example). Similarly there have been proportionately few studies that have examined the responses of students in the expectancy construct (e.g. Babad, 1998; Kuklinski & Weinstein, 2001). Students in the Expectancy Construct Weinstein and her colleagues (Weinstein, 1993; Weinstein, Marshall, Brattesani & Middlestadt, 1982; Weinstein, Marshall, Sharp & Botkin, 1987; Weinstein & McKown, 1998) and Babad and his colleagues (Babad, 1993; Babad, Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1989; Babad, Inbar & Rosenthal, 1982), however, have extensively investigated whether students are able to discern teachers’ high or low expectations and how they might do this. In five separate studies (Brattesani, Weinstein & Marshall, 1984; Kuklinski & Weinstein, 2000; Weinstein & Middlestadt, 1979; Weinstein et al., 1982; Weinstein et al., 1987) Weinstein and her colleagues used the Teacher Treatment Inventory with first to fifth graders to determine whether students perceived differential treatment by teachers, of high and low achievers. Teacher Expectations 4 They found that in comparison to low achievers students felt that teachers interacted more positively with high achieving students, that they had higher expectations of them, and that they offered them more leadership opportunities and more choice in their learning experiences. In contrast to the high achievers, students reported that teachers were more negative towards low achievers and directed their learning far more frequently. In further studies (Weinstein, 1986; 1989; 1993) Weinstein interviewed 133 fourth grade children about how they knew whether their teacher considered them ‘smart’ or not. She reported that students determined this from what their teachers told them, from the marks and grades that they received, from various instructional practices such as ability grouping, from the types of learning experiences students were given and from aspects of the affective climate of the classroom. The students showed a sophistication in their interpretation of teacher behaviors that belied their age. They could provide numerous explicit examples of comparative feedback, of single critical incidents where students had been publicly humiliated, of meaningful facial expressions by their teachers, of the vocal tone used with particular students. In response Weinstein (2002) has called for greater responsiveness by teachers to children’s perceptive illustrations, which provide clear examples of how expectations can be communicated to children. Much of Babad’s work, too, has concentrated on exploring the subtle verbal and nonverbal cues that teachers provide which lead students to interpret these as expectations for their achievement (Babad, 1993; 1998). His investigations have shown that whereas teachers reported providing more emotional support to low ability students, the children perceived the opposite (Babad, 1990; 1995). Babad reported that the teachers did endeavor to display warmth and emotional support to low expectation students but that the students were able to determine that such displays were not genuine because they were exaggerated. The teachers’ natural affection for the high expectation students was interpretable by students despite Teacher Expectations 5 teacher attempts to control this (Babad, 1998). Students were resentful of this differential emotional support and this was particularly so in the classrooms where teachers reported having ‘pets’. Babad has further investigated student interpretation of teacher behaviors using small video clips (Babad, Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1987; 1989a, 1989b; 1991). Israeli teachers were filmed interacting with either low or high expectation students. Students were then shown the video clips. Children as young as fourth graders were able to tell when teachers were talking to or about high as opposed to low expectation students even though the students involved in the interactions were not seen and the video clips were only ten seconds in length. When the video clips were shown to students in New Zealand in a separate study (Babad & Taylor, 1992) ten-year old students were able to signal whether the unseen child the teacher was interacting with was a student for whom the teacher had either high or low expectations. This was despite the fact that none of the students in this particular study understood Hebrew and hence their interpretation was gleaned from facial expressions, tone and body language. Babad’s studies showed not only the strength of the nonverbal channels in transmitting teachers’ expectations towards the student but also showed that such nonverbal affective responses were cross-cultural. It would seem clear from the extensive work of both Weinstein and Babad that students are able to perceive their teachers’ expectations for their performance. How might students respond in light of this awareness? One of the debates in the expectancy literature has been that of the direction of the expectancy effect. Do teacher behaviors shape students’ (re)actions in line with teachers’ expectations or do student behaviors influence the expectancies of teachers? While the relationship is more likely dynamic than dichotomous some of the more recent evidence has provided support for a stronger relationship from teacher to student than the alternative. Gill and Reynolds (1999) surveyed fourth-grade Teacher Expectations 6 teachers to determine their expectations for the urban African American children that they taught. The researchers reported that one-fifth of the teachers had high expectations for their students’ future achievement while approximately one-seventh had limited expectations. They found that the students’ perceptions only mediated the teacher’s expectations to a limited extent while the expectations of the teachers had a large direct effect on both the reading ( = .45) and mathematics ( = .42) achievement of the students. Kuklinski and Weinstein (2001) found similar results. They reported that teacher expectations had significant effects on Grade 5 students’ self-expectations particularly in classes where the messages that teachers gave about student ability were salient. This was not true, however, in the Grade 1 and Grade 3 classrooms included in their study. Teacher expectations, however, did have a significant effect on the achievement outcomes of students at both the earlier grades and at Grade 5. The authors concluded that at the earlier levels teacher expectations had more direct effects on students’ achievement outcomes and were not mediated to the same extent by student self-expectations. This was particularly so in classrooms where students perceived much differential treatment towards high and low expectation students. Teacher Propensities and Student Outcomes Teachers vary in their propensities to interact differently with high and low achievers. It appears that teachers who make more discrimination between their high and low achievers may affect their students’ achievement outcomes and possibly self-perceptions to a greater degree than those teachers who make less distinction in their interactions with students of different ability. Brattesani, Weinstein and Marshall (1984) defined such teachers as high or low differentiating teachers. They showed that children’s academic performance differed substantially according to their teachers’ proclivity. In classrooms where the students reported clearly differential treatment of high and low achievers, teachers’ expectations Teacher Expectations 7 explained 14% of the variance in children’s end-of-year achievement after controlling for prior achievement. On the other hand in the classrooms of low differentiating teachers only 3% of the variance could be explained by teachers’ expectations. Babad et al., (1982) also provided evidence that teachers who placed more credence in biasing information, such as prior achievement, differentiated to a greater extent in their subsequent treatment of students. This led to greater self-fulfilling prophecy effects for the students. They called such teachers high and low differentiating. Indeed it was when using video clips of high differentiating teachers that the students participating in the experiments of Babad and his colleagues (cited above) could discern differential teacher expectations and student treatment. The identification of teachers who differentiated more or less in their behaviour towards high and low expectation students led Babad (1998) to suggest that preferential affect was at the heart of the teacher expectation issue. It appears that students placed in the classrooms of particular types of teachers (high or low differentiating, high or low bias) may become more or less vulnerable to self-fulfilling prophecy effects dependent on the classroom environment in which they find themselves. Recently Rubie-Davies (in review-a) has identified teachers having correspondingly high or low expectations for all the students in their classrooms, i.e. that their expectations are at the class rather than the individual level. These were teachers whose expectations for their students were significantly above or below the students’ attainment. Similarly to the students in the classrooms of high and low differentiating teachers (Brattesani et al, 1984), the students in the classrooms of high and low expectation teachers showed contrasting patterns of academic achievement, with those in the classes of high expectation teachers making large gains over one year in reading (d > 0.5 in all classes) while those in the classes of low expectation teachers made fewer gains (d < 0.2 in all classes). Hence there are some studies which show variable student achievement according to particular teacher characteristics. It Teacher Expectations 8 would appear possible that students’ self-perception may also come to vary in classrooms where teacher propensities differ. This does not, however, appear to have been measured in empirical research. Since student self-perception has been shown by some researchers to relate to academic achievement (Assor & Connell, 1992; Hay, Ashman, & Van Kraayenoord, 1998; Lynch, 2002) then the measurement of student self-perception when placed in the classrooms of teachers with varying characteristics would appear to be important. If student self-perception and academic achievement were found to vary with teacher-type this would add further weight to the argument that the direction of the teacher expectancy effect is stronger from the teacher to the students than the opposite. Hence the current study aims to measure the self-perceptions of students placed in the classrooms of high and low expectation teachers. It is proposed that over one year students’ self-perceptions may alter in line with teachers’ class-centered expectations. Method Participants The participants in this study were 256 students from 12 different classrooms attending eight elementary schools in the Auckland area of New Zealand. Their teachers were identified in a previous paper (Rubie-Davies, in review-a) as having expectations for their students’ learning that were either significantly above or significantly below the children’s level of achievement. The teachers formed three groups. The first consisted of six teachers whose expectations for end of year performance were significantly above their students’ level of achievement at the beginning of the academic year and whose students made statistically significant achievement gains over the year in reading. This group will be referred to as the students with the high expectation teachers (HiEx Group). The second group consisted of three teachers whose expectations were significantly above their students’ level of Teacher Expectations 9 achievement at the beginning of the academic year but whose students did not make statistically significant gains over the year in reading and called the average progress teachers (AvPr Group). The final group was comprised of three teachers whose expectations were significantly below their students’ academic achievement at the beginning of the academic year and whose students made no or small gains over the year in reading. This group is called the low expectation teachers (LoEx Group). Of the 256 students included in the current study 132 were in classrooms with high expectation teachers, 65 had average progress teachers and 59 had low expectation teachers. The demographic makeup of the students in each of these groups may be found in Table 1. This table includes numbers of students by gender within each expectation group. It also includes numbers by socioeconomic level of the school. In New Zealand every school is given a decile ranking based on census data which provides an indication of the socioeconomic status of the school. For the purposes of this study deciles 15 are defined as low and deciles 6-10 are defined as high. Table 1 also includes the class level of the students where Year One and Two students are classified as junior and Year Five and Six students are classed as senior. The ethnicity of the students in each group is included. Materials The self-perception scale used to survey the students’ perceptions of themselves in various areas, academic and non-academic, was adapted from the Self Description Questionnaire-1 (SDQ-1) with the permission of the author (Marsh, 1990). The SDQ-1 consists of eight scales each containing eight statements designed to illustrate a child’s selfperceptions in various areas of self-concept. Of these the reading scale, the mathematics scale, the physical abilities scale, and the peer relations scale were of interest in the current study. The SDQ-1 includes statements such as ‘I am good at reading’ and ‘Most other kids like me’ and students respond on a five-point Likert scale ranging from false (1) to true (5). As well as these scales two additional items were added. These were: ‘The teacher thinks I’m Teacher Expectations 10 good at reading’ and ‘The teacher thinks I’m good at math’. These items were added in order to determine whether there were any changes over the year in the students’ perceptions of how their teachers viewed their abilities in two academic areas. The Cronbach alpha coefficient across the reading, mathematics, physical abilities, peer relations and teacher opinion subscales was .71; if the teacher item had been removed from the overall scale the Cronbach alpha coefficient would have remained at .71. Marsh (1990) reported much lower correlations among the factors (.05 for mathematics and reading, and .32 for peer relations and physical abilities) but argued that these low correlations provided support for the notion of self-concept as being multifaceted with distinct and measurable domains. The internal consistency coefficients across the normative sample (n = 3562) were acceptable (.89 for reading and mathematics, .83 for physical abilities and .85 for peer relations). Marsh reported (1990) that the questionnaire was suitable for students in Grades Two to Six. In a subsequent study Marsh, Craven and Debus (1991) found that students aged from kindergarten to grade two exhibited differentiated perceptions of themselves across the eight self-concept domains in the SDQ-1. Procedure All students completed the self-perception scale in March and November of one academic year. (In New Zealand the academic year in elementary schools runs from February to December.) The classroom teacher was not present for the administration of the questionnaire. Six trainee teachers completing the final year of their pre-service teaching qualification assisted with the administration of the scale. In the junior classrooms the students were divided into six or seven small groups, depending on class size. The adults then read each item aloud to their respective groups and supervised the completion of the questionnaires closely to ensure that any missed items could be detected and that student engagement could be enhanced. The author supervised the administration in smaller Teacher Expectations 11 classrooms. In the Year Five and/or Six classrooms the author read all items aloud and the trainee teachers monitored the students to again ensure high levels of completion. Data Collection and Analysis In order to score each separate subscale of the self-perception scale, the sum of the raw scores was found. Marsh (1990) suggested that the raw score allowed ‘absolute’ differences to be determined and that these may be lost when scales were normalized or standardized. Only students who were present at both testing sessions were included in the analyses. The major analyses in this study involved conducting one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) across the three groups at the beginning and end of the year in order to determine any statistically significant differences between the groups. The Tukey HSD test was used for post-hoc comparisons. A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to determine within group changes across time. Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988) was calculated to determine the effect size of any statistically significant changes for groups across the year. Results Overall Self-Perception Overall self-perception was a composite of the students’ scores for the reading, mathematics, physical abilities, peer relations and teacher perception subscales. The means and standard deviations for this aggregation of scores are presented in Table 2. A one-way between groups analysis of variance was conducted to explore any differences in overall student self-perception for beginning of year scores. There was a statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = 3.09, p < .05). Despite reaching statistical significance post-hoc comparisons using the Tukey HSD test indicated no statistically significant differences between any of the groups. When the same analysis was completed for end of year scores for overall student self-perception, a statistically significant difference was Teacher Expectations 12 found between the three groups (F (1,2) = 8.44, p < .001). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the LoEx Group was significantly lower than that of the HiEx Group (p < .001) and that of the AvPr Group (p < .01). A repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time showed a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 4.88, p < .008). This statistically significant difference was as a result of a decline in mean for the LoEx Group across time (d = - .24) and an increase in the means for both the HiEx Group (d = 0.11) and the AvPr Group (d = .33). Self-Perception of Reading The means and standard deviations for the beginning and end of year scores of the three expectation groups for the reading subscale may be found in Table 3. A one-way between groups analysis of variance was conducted to explore any differences in reading self-perception for beginning of year scores. There was no statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = .27, p < .76). When the same analysis was completed for end of year scores for self-perception in reading, a statistically significant difference was found between the three groups (F (1,2) = 6.36, p < .002). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the LoEx Group was significantly lower than that of the HiEx Group (p < .002) and of the AvPr Group (p < .01). A repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time showed a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 5.97, p < .003). This statistically significant difference was as a result of a decline in mean for the LoEx Group across time (d = - .35) and an increase in the means for both the HiEx Group (d = 0.11) and the AvPr Group (d = .24). Self-Perception of Mathematics The means and standard deviations for the beginning and end of year scores of the three expectation groups for the mathematics subscale are shown in Table 4. A one-way between groups analysis of variance to explore any differences in mathematics self- Teacher Expectations 13 perception for beginning of year scores showed no statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = 1.39, p < .25). When the same analysis was completed for end of year scores for self-perception in mathematics, a statistically significant difference was found between the three groups (F (1,2) = 5.21, p < .006). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the LoEx Group was significantly lower from that of the HiEx Group (p < .005) and of the AvPr (p < .05). A repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time did not show a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 2.82, p < .06). Self-Perception of Physical Abilities The means and standard deviations for the beginning and end of year scores of the three expectation groups for the physical abilities subscale are shown in Table 5. A one-way between groups analysis of variance for beginning of year scores showed a statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = 5.27, p < .006). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the HiEx Group was significantly greater than that of the AvPr Group (p < .01). When the same analysis was completed for end of year scores for self-perception of physical abilities, no statistically significant differences were found between the three groups (F (1,2) = 2.66, p < .07). Similarly a repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time did not show a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 2.43, p < .09). Self-Perception of Peer Relations The means and standard deviations for the beginning and end of year scores of the three expectation groups for the peer relations subscale are shown in Table 6. A one-way between groups analysis of variance was conducted to explore any differences in selfperception of peer relations for beginning of year scores. There was a statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = 3.37, p < .04). Post-hoc comparisons indicated Teacher Expectations 14 that the mean score for the HiEx Group was significantly greater than that of the AvPr Group (p < .04). When the same analysis was completed for end of year scores for self-perception of peer relations, no statistically significant differences were found between the three groups (F (1,2) = .16, p < .85). A repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time, however did show a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 3.51, p < .03). This statistically significant difference was as a result of an increase in the means for both the AvPr Group (d = 0.21) and the LoEx Group (d = .19). Self-Perception of Teacher Opinion The means and standard deviations for the beginning and end of year scores of the three expectation groups for the teacher opinion subscale are shown in Table 7. A one-way between groups analysis of variance was conducted to explore any differences in selfperception of teacher opinion for beginning of year scores. There was a statistically significant difference between the three groups (F (1,2) = 4.11, p < .02). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean score for the HiEx Group was significantly greater than that of the AvPr Group (p < .02). When the same analyses were completed for end of year scores for self-perception of teacher opinion, again a statistically significant difference was found between the three groups (F (1,2) = 6.69, p < .001). The beginning of year statistically significant difference between the HiEx and AvPr Groups had disappeared but by the end of the year the mean for the HiEx Group was statistically significantly greater than that of the LoEx Group (p < .001). A repeated measures ANOVA to compare differences within the groups over time, however, did not show a statistically significant group by time interaction (F (1,2) = 2.87, p < .06). Discussion Teacher Expectations 15 Teacher Expectations and Student Academic Self-Perceptions This study builds the argument that teacher expectations may influence student selfperceptions. In the academic areas in particular student self-perception appeared to change over the year of the research to correspond with teachers’ expectations. In reading, mathematics and overall self-perception while there were no differences between the three groups at the beginning of the year, by the end of the year the self-perception of the high expectation and average progress groups had increased while that of the low expectation group had declined. Moreover students appeared to show some awareness of their teachers’ expectations since while there were no differences between the high and low expectation groups at the beginning of the year in how students viewed their teachers’ opinions by the end of the year there was a statistically significant difference between the groups. This was due to a decline in the perception of the students in the low expectation group that their teachers favored their abilities in reading and mathematics, rather than to an increase in the perception of the high expectation group. In both reading and overall self-perception there were also statistically significant changes within the groups over the year of the research. These changes were mainly due to declines in the self-perception of the low expectation groups and increases in the self-perception of the average progress group. Although not statistically significant over the year, the changes in mathematics self-perception and perception of teacher opinion mirrored those of reading and overall self-perception. Student Self-Perception and Class-Centered Expectations It is possible, of course, that the decline in self-perceptions for the students with lowexpectation teachers was related to achievement since these students’ academic progress over the year of the research was limited. The students in the classes of the average progress teachers, however, also made only small performance gains over the year and yet their selfperceptions increased. Their teachers had high expectations for their achievement. It would Teacher Expectations 16 seem possible then that the changes in the students’ self-perceptions were teacher-related rather than achievement related. The changes may relate not only to the teachers’ expectations but also to the ways these are operationalized in the classroom. These findings support those of Kuklinski and Weinstein (2001) who reported greater effects of teacher expectations for students’ self-expectations in classrooms where ability differences were more salient, particularly at Grade Five. In the current study greater effects have also been found for particular groups of teachers. Age Effects of Teacher Expectations and Student Self-Perceptions Moreover, Kuklinski and Weinstein (2001) further reported that student selfexpectations altered in line with teacher expectations for Grade Five students but not for Grades One and Three students. This may provide one explanation for the finding in the current study that student self-perceptions mostly altered across the year for students in the classes of average progress and low expectation teachers while those of the students with high expectation teachers changed to a lesser extent. The classes of both average progress and low expectation teachers contained more than double the numbers of senior elementary age students compared with junior school students while within the high expectation group there were more junior elementary school age students than there were senior level students. Class level was not used as a controlling variable in the current study, however, due to the small numbers in each teacher expectation group and so any possible conclusions must be tentative. Future research with larger numbers of students in each teacher expectation group would enable age group factors to be identified and could lend further weight to the current study. Non-Academic Student Self-Perceptions The patterns of change between and within groups across the year of the study were not evident for the subscales of physical abilities and peer relations. In an investigation of teacher beliefs about students and student self-perceptions of ability Wigfield and Harold Teacher Expectations 17 (1992) reported that the relationship between teachers’ beliefs and students’ perceptions were greater for mathematics, reading and sports than they were in other domains. They suggested that teacher beliefs about students might influence student perceptions in some curriculum areas more than in others. In the current investigation change in student self-perceptions of physical abilities across the three groups was not found whereas changes were found in the academic areas. It is possible that students more frequently receive salient feedback from teachers about their performance in academic areas rather than in non-academic ones. In New Zealand the emphasis in both sport and physical education in elementary schools tends to be on children’s enjoyment rather than on the sequential development of skills (Graham, 2001). Students are rarely tested or provided with feedback about their learning and hence are less likely to be subjected to comparative comments. For these reasons the students may be less aware of their teachers’ expectations and may also be less likely to alter their own selfperceptions. At the end of the year there were no differences between the groups in their selfperceptions of their peer relations. There was, however, a within-groups change for both the students with the average progress teachers and those with the low expectation teachers; for both these groups their self-perceptions of their peer relations increased across the year. Again it is seems less likely that student self-perception in an area such as peer relations would be influenced by teachers’ expectations. There are various plausible explanations for why student self-perception of peer relations may have improved over the year in these groups. One possibility is that, as explained earlier, the vast majority of students in these two groups were in Grades Five and Six. By this age students are becoming less reliant on teachers for friendship and support and are beginning to place more emphasis on their peer group relationships (Kerns & Richardson, 2005). Hence over one year they may have fostered improved relationships and friendships with the peers in their respective classes. In Teacher Expectations 18 contrast the students with the high expectation teachers who tended to be younger may have been more reliant on their teachers for support and hence their peer relationships remained more static over the year. A further explanation is simply that the students with high expectation teachers enjoyed caring relationships with their teachers. Rubie-Davies (in review-b) has provided evidence that the socioemotional climate in the classrooms of high expectation teachers was more positive than that enjoyed by the other two groups of students. In such classrooms students may have felt less need to develop their peer relationships than in classrooms where the students’ experience with their teachers was less positive. The Direction of the Teacher Expectation Effect The current study adds weight to those of other researchers who argue that the direction of the expectation effect appears to be more salient from the teacher to the students than the converse (Gill & Reynolds, 1999; Kuklinski & Weinstein, 2001; Muller, Katz & Dance, 1999; Weinstein, 2002). Particularly in the academic areas there were no differences in student self-perceptions at the beginning of the year and yet by the end of the year while those of the students with high expectation teachers increased somewhat, the academic selfperceptions of the students with low expectation teachers decreased dramatically. This change in self-perception came to match the teachers’ expectations over the year. Perhaps more importantly the students in the current study appeared to be aware of their teachers’ expectations for the class since the students’ perceptions of their teachers’ opinions of their abilities also altered in line with the teachers’ expectations. While caution about assuming causality must be made the results are worthy of future investigations where the impact on student self-perception of teachers who have correspondingly high or low expectations for their students might be more thoroughly examined. Teacher Expectations 19 Future Research This study has some limitations. One mentioned earlier is that junior and senior level elementary school students were combined in the current investigation. Future studies could either concentrate on investigating any teacher expectation effects for one particular age group or could include larger numbers of students with each teacher group which would enable the results to be examined in relation to both teacher expectation group and age effects. A further limitation of the current study is that in the earlier study of Rubie-Davies (in review-a) the teachers were categorized according to their expectations of their students in reading, yet student self-perceptions were measured across a range of areas. It may be that teachers’ expectations for their class vary across different academic and non-academic areas. This may be a further explanation for the differing results found for the various curriculum and non-curriculum subscales. It remains for future research to further unravel the nested relationships of teachers’ expectations for their class across curriculum areas and student selfperceptions across a range of school related criteria. Conclusions The finding that student self-perception in academic areas did alter across the year in accordance with teachers’ expectations for their classes is an important one and has implications for practicing teachers as well as for preservice teacher education programmes. The increasing evidence for the effect of teachers’ expectations on their students in both academic and social spheres demands that teacher beliefs about student capabilities are examined. It is important that teachers and teacher education programmes place additional emphasis on the role of positive self-fulfilling prophecies for students and high expectations for teachers. Teacher education institutions, practicing teachers and families need to work collaboratively to ensure that students remain optimistic, motivated and successful within the schooling environment. In this way every child will have the opportunity to thrive and reach Teacher Expectations 20 his or her potential in a classroom environment that is supportive, constructive, encouraging and caring. Teacher Expectations 21 References Assor, A., & Connell, J. P. (1992). The validity of students' self-reports as measures of performance affecting self-appraisals. In D. H. Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 25 - 47). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Babad, E. (1990). Measuring and changing teachers' differential behavior as perceived by students and teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 683-690. Babad, E. (1993). Teachers' differential behavior. Educational Psychology Review, 5, 347376. Babad, E. (1995). The "teacher's pet" phenomenon, teachers' differential behavior, and students' morale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 361-374. Babad, E. (1998). Preferential affect: The crux of the teacher expectancy issue. In J. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research on teaching: Expectations in the Classroom (Vol. 7, pp. 183-214). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Babad, E., Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1987). Nonverbal and verbal behavior of preschool, remedial, and elementary school teachers. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 405-415. Babad, E., Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1989a). Nonverbal communication and leakage in the behavior of biased and unbiased teachers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 89-94. Babad, E., Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1989b). When less information is more informative: Diagnosing teacher expectations from brief samples of behaviour. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 281-295. Teacher Expectations 22 Babad, E. Y., Inbar, J., & Rosenthal, R. (1982). Pygmalion, Galatea and the Golem: Investigations of biased and unbiased teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 459-474. Babad, E., Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Students as judges of teachers' verbal and nonverbal behavior. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 211-234. Babad, E., & Taylor, P. B. (1992). Transparency of teacher expectancies across language, cultural boundaries. Journal of Educational Research, 86, 120-125. Brattesani, K. A., Weinstein, R. S., & Marshall, H. H. (1984). Student perceptions of differential teacher treatment as moderators of teacher expectation effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 236-247. Brophy, J. E. (1985). Teacher-student interaction. In J. B. Dusek (Ed.), Teacher expectancies (pp. 303 - 328). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cooper, H., & Good, T. (1983). Pygmalion grows up: Studies in the expectation communication process. NY: Longman. Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (1985). Teacher expectations and student motivation. In J. B. Dusek (Ed.), Teacher expectancies (pp. 185-226). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gill, S., & Reynolds, A. J. (1999). Educational expectations and school achievement of urban African American children. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 403-424. Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2003). Looking in classrooms (9th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Graham, J. (2001). Getting set for an active nation: Report of the Sport, Fitness and Leisure Ministerial Taskforce. Wellington: Sport, Fitness and Leisure Ministerial Taskforce. Teacher Expectations 23 Hay, I., Ashman, A. F., & Van Kraayenoord, C. E. (1998). Educational characteristics of students with high or low self-concept. Psychology in the Schools, 35, 391-400. Lynch, J. (2002). Parents' self-efficacy beliefs, parents' gender, children's reader selfperceptions, reading achievement and gender. Journal of Research in Reading, 25, 5467. Jussim, L., Smith, A., Madon, S., & Palumbo, P. (1998). Teacher expectations. In J. E. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research on teaching: Expectations in the classroom (Vol. 7, pp. 1-48). Greenwich, Connecticut: JAI Press. Kerns, K. A. & Richardson, R. A. (2005). Attachment in middle childhood. NY: Guildford Press. Kuklinski, M. R., & Weinstein, R. S. (2000) Classroom and grade level differences in the stability of teacher expectations and perceived differential treatment. Learning Environments Research, 3, 1-34. Kuklinski, M. R., & Weinstein, R. S. (2001). Classroom and developmental differences in a path model of teacher expectancy effects. Child Development, 72, 1554-1578. Marsh, H. W. (1990). Self description questionnaire – 1: Manual. Campbelltown, N.S.W.: University of Western Sydney. Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Debus, R. (1991). Self-concept of young children 5 to 8 years of age: Measurement and multidimensional structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 377-392. Muller, C., Katz, S. R., & Dance, J. (1999). Investing in teaching and learning. Urban Education, 34, 292-337. Pellegrini, A. D., & Blatchford, P. (2000). The child at school: Interactions with peers and teachers. London: Arnold. Teacher Expectations 24 Rosenthal, R. (1991). Teacher expectancy effects: A brief update 25 years after the Pygmalion experiment. Journal of Research in Education, 1, 3-12. Rubie-Davies (2005) Identifying high and low expectation teachers: Exploring academic outcomes for students. Manuscript submitted for publication. Rubie-Davies (2005) Classroom Interactions: Exploring The Practices Of High And Low Expectation Teachers. Manuscript submitted for publication. Weinstein, R. S. (1986). The teaching of reading and children’s awareness of teacher expectations. In T. Raphael (Ed.), The contexts of school-based literacy. NY: Random House. Weinstein, R. S. (1989). Perceptions of classroom processes and student motivation: Children's views of self-fulfilling prophecies. In A. R. & A. C. (Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 187-221). NY: Academic Press. Weinstein, R. S. 1993. Children’s knowledge of differential treatment in school: Implications for motivation. In T. M. Tomlinson (Ed.), Motivating students to learn: Overcoming barriers to high achievement. Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Reaching higher: The power of expectations in schooling Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Weinstein, R. S., Marshall, H. H., Brattesani, K. A., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1982). Student perceptions of differential teacher treatment in open and traditional classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 678-692. Weinstein, R., Marshall, H., Sharp, L., & Botkin, M. (1987). Pygmalion and the student: Age and classroom differences in children's awareness of teacher expectations. Child Development, 58, 1079-1093. Teacher Expectations 25 Weinstein, R. S., & McKown, C. (1998). Expectancy effects in "context": Listening to the voices of students and teachers. In J. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research on teaching: Expectations in the classroom (Vol. 7, pp. 215-242). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Weinstein, R. S., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1979). Student perceptions of teacher interactions with male high and low achievers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 421-431. Wigfield, A., & Harold, R. D. (1992). Teacher beliefs and children's achievement selfperceptions: A developmental perspective. In D. H. Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 95-121). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Teacher Expectations 26 Table 1 Demographic Data by Teacher Group for Students Participating in the Current Study HiEx Group AvPr Group LoEx Group Total No. of Students 132 65 59 Boys 71 35 28 Girls 61 30 31 High Decile 94 19 44 Low Decile 38 46 15 Junior Level 83 20 18 Senior Level 49 45 41 NZ European 87 14 37 Maori 12 17 16 Pacific Island 18 31 0 Asian 15 3 6 Teacher Expectations 27 Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations for Overall Self-Perception at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Beginning of Year Group End of Year M SD n M SD HiEx 132 106.96 15.70 132 108.72 15.70 AvPr 65 101.06 20.64 65 106.97 15.09 LoEx 59 102.51 15.59 59 98.73 16.32 Teacher Expectations 28 Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations for Self-Perception of Reading at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Group Beginning of Year End of Year n M SD n M SD HiEx 132 32.96 7.01 132 33.73 6.76 AvPr 65 32.22 6.92 65 33.69 5.14 LoEx 59 32.59 6.06 59 30.32 6.89 Teacher Expectations 29 Table 4 Means and Standard Deviations for Self-Perception of Mathematics at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Group Beginning of Year End of Year n M SD n M SD HiEx 132 32.09 7.13 132 33.20 6.97 AvPr 65 30.45 8.06 65 32.66 6.04 LoEx 59 30.69 7.07 59 29.80 7.31 Teacher Expectations 30 Table 5 Means and Standard Deviations for Self-Perception of Physical Abilities at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Group Beginning of Year End of Year n M SD n M SD HiEx 132 33.32 5.71 132 33.22 5.83 AvPr 65 30.60 6.63 65 32.42 5.78 LoEx 59 31.19 6.32 59 31.08 6.32 Teacher Expectations 31 Table 6 Means and Standard Deviations for Self-Perception of Peer Relations at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Group Beginning of Year End of Year n M SD n M SD HiEx 132 31.53 7.06 132 30.40 5.83 AvPr 65 28.86 7.54 65 30.35 5.78 LoEx 59 29.69 7.21 59 31.00 6.32 Teacher Expectations 32 Table 7 Means and Standard Deviations for Student Perception of Teacher Opinion at Beginning and End of Year for Students With High Expectation, Average Progress and Low Expectation Teachers Group Beginning of Year End of Year n M SD n M SD HiEx 132 8.59 1.73 132 8.56 1.71 AvPr 65 7.80 2.27 65 8.20 1.80 LoEx 59 8.03 2.03 59 7.53 2.01