The Effects of Increased Maternal Education on Children’s Academic Outcomes: Evidence from the ECLS-K Katherine Magnuson & Hilary Shager University of Wisconsin-Madison This draft was prepared for the ITP Spring Training Seminar, April 11, 2008 Please do not cite or circulate without the authors’ permission Introduction By pursuing more education, do mothers set their children on better academic courses? Or do such pursuits do little to alter children’s academic trajectories? Research shows that children of more highly educated parents enter school with higher levels of academic skills and continue to perform better than other children (Entwisle & Alexander, 1993; Lee & Burkham, 2002). Social scientists often attribute this achievement gap to the mix of economic and social advantages afforded by higher levels of parental education, but debates continue on the extent to which parental education is causally associated with children’s achievement (Sobel, 1998). Researchers have studied parental education largely as if it were static; yet, it has become increasingly common for adults to accrue education in a discontinuous fashion, and to extend their schooling well into adulthood (Astone, Schoen, Ensminger & Rothert, 2000; Jacobs & Stoner-Eby, 1998). Aware of the increasing importance of a skilled labor force, policymakers have created incentives for adults to return to school; for example, by offering tuition tax credits and publicly funding adult basic education and job training programs. National estimates suggest that now over 20 percent of adult women pursue some type of education (Rich & Kim, 1999), and about 27 percent of all female college students are over age 25 (Shin, 2005). Economically disadvantaged women, and mothers in particular, are especially likely to return to school later in life. Recent studies find that close to 50 percent of low-income mothers attend school after the birth of their children (Love et al., 2002; McGroder, Zaslow, Moore & LeMenestrel, 2000; Rich & Kim, 1999). Nearly 20 years ago Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn and Morgan (1987) found that some children born to poor, urban, adolescent mothers seemed to flourish despite experiencing the same adverse early environments as peers who did not fare so well. Over 50 percent of mothers in their study had attended school after the birth of their children, and Furstenberg and colleagues 1 argued that this additional education contributed to children’s subsequent scholastic success. This was a compelling hypothesis, although with a small sample of young mothers, it was largely speculative. The authors could not conclude that it was the mothers’ education per se that mattered, rather than other characteristics that were correlated with mothers’ schooling. For example, perhaps a mother’s academic aptitude both brought her back to school and set her child on a better academic trajectory from the very beginning. For those who consider maternal educational attainment as simply a proxy for a set of individual or family advantages or socioeconomic standing, it may seem as if an incremental change in a mother’s education would have little bearing on her child’s development. From this perspective, increases in maternal education would matter only if they bring about large shifts in family resources, perhaps as a result of improvements in mothers’ job prospects and earnings. Yet for those who consider maternal education as an important determinant of the quality of the child’s learning environment, incremental improvements in maternal education may well influence children’s developmental outcomes, because they create positive changes in children’s developmental niches (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). Ecological and life course theories emphasize the interconnection between the experiences of parents and their children, and argue that proximal environments and interactions that facilitate the intergenerational transmission of achievement and behavior patterns are dynamic and changeable (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Elder, 1994; Macmillan, McMorris, & Kruttschnitt, 2004). To date, studies have primarily focused on links between changes in parents’ economic resources and children’s outcomes (e.g., Macmillan et al., 2004). Changes in their parents’ education, however, are also likely to be important (Furstenberg et al., 1987), especially because maternal education is often the component of socioeconomic status most strongly associated with children’s achievement (Bornstein, Hahn, Suwalsky, & 2 Haynes, 2003). How might additional education improve the contexts in which young children are reared? Research on how educational attainment affects children’s achievement provides some likely explanations. To the extent that a mother’s additional schooling provides her with positive learning experiences, as well as increases in basic skills, knowledge, and higher-order thinking, it may shape her expectations for her children’s education and enable her to create better home learning environments for her children (Alexander, Entwisle, & Bedinger, 1994; Corwyn & Bradley, 2003; Davis-Kean, 2005; Halle, Kurtz-Costes, & Mahoney, 1997). Parents with higher levels of education engage their children in more learning-related activities both in the home (e.g., reading books) and out of the home (e.g., music or art lessons) (Davis-Kean, 2005). More highly educated mothers are more verbally responsive to their young children and tend to use teaching strategies with their children that mimic formal instructional techniques, such as asking questions and offering feedback rather than issuing directives (Laosa, 1980; Richman, Miller, & LeVine, 1992; Tracey & Young, 2002). Opportunities for children to engage in learning activities and the quality of mother-child interactions, particularly verbal interactions, have been linked to children’s cognitive development and their academic skills (Taylor, Clayton, & Rowley, 2004). Finally, among school-age children, higher levels of parental education have also been linked to higher levels of parental involvement in school, which in turn have been linked to better school outcomes (Brody & Flor, 1998; Hill et al., 2004; Stevenson & Baker, 1987). Understanding whether changes in mothers’ educational attainment influence children’s developmental trajectories is important not only because such changes are common, but also because it may shed light on the extent to which parental education, more generally, influences 3 children. One of the key difficulties in studying the effects of maternal education on children’s development is that when it is completed before the birth of children, maternal education is likely to be confounded with other relevant mother and family characteristics that are difficult to untangle. For example, we might hope to isolate the effects of a mother’s schooling from her sense of academic motivation, efficacy, or aptitude. However, if a mother’s education is already completed, it is likely that her educational attainment reflects all of these factors, and yet it is also likely that all of these factors affect her children’s achievement independent of her attainment. Thus, researchers end up in the unenviable position of having to over-control or under-control for these and other family characteristics. When a mother’s education improves after the birth of her children, this provides a unique opportunity to estimate the effects of maternal education on children’s development, with fewer concerns about how to account for confounding factors. By focusing on the effects of changes in education, many characteristics of mothers and children measured before mothers attend school can be taken into account. Nevertheless, unobserved characteristics may differentiate mothers who return to school and those who do not. If the effects of these unobserved characteristics are constant over time, then change analyses provide a way to reduce omitted variable bias (NICHD & Duncan, 2003). Thus, the unbiased effects of maternal education can be identified more clearly than when a mother’s education is completed prior to the birth of her children, although as with all non-experimental research, concerns about over- or under-controlling for related characteristics and factors may still remain. Despite the important advantage such an approach provides, few studies have analyzed the effects of changes in parents’ education, perhaps because it requires either an experimental manipulation of parental education or longitudinal observations of both parents’ education and children’s outcomes. 4 In an effort to shed light on the relationship between a mother’s further education and her child’s school success, three experimental evaluations of large education and job training interventions for disadvantaged mothers have included a child assessment, two of which specifically targeted teen mothers (McGroder et al., 2000; Maynard, Nicholson & Rangarajan, 1993; Quint, Bos, & Polit, 1997). In all three studies, mothers assigned to the control group attended educational programs almost as frequently as those in the experimental group; thus, the interventions did not boost educational levels by much. In the absence of substantial effects on mothers’ education, it is not surprising that these programs did not appear to affect children’s outcomes. Quasi-experimental studies have proven more informative, suggesting that increases in maternal education may improve young children’s achievement. However, these studies have found that effects are concentrated among particularly disadvantaged populations. An analysis of education mandated as part of a large welfare-to-work intervention fielded during the 1990s suggests that mothers’ participation in adult basic education improved children’s school readiness even when mothers’ earnings did not increase (Magnuson, 2003). Estimates suggest that about 8 months of participation in educational activities when children were between ages 4 and 6 boosted school readiness by about a quarter of a standard deviation by age 6. Using econometric methods with data from the Maternal and Child Supplement to the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY), Rosenzweig and Wolpin (1994) found that additional maternal education completed during the first three years of a child’s life improved his or her later vocabulary and academic skills. A more recent study by Moore and Schmidt (2007), also using the NLSY data, found that a mother’s enrollment in education during the first three years of her child’s life was associated with higher math and reading achievement by age 6. 5 Just one previous study has considered whether improvements in maternal education improved achievement during middle childhood. Also using the NLSY data, Magnuson (2007) examined whether additional maternal education that was obtained when children were between the ages of 6 and 12 influenced children’s academic skills. Again, results suggest that positive effects were only evident among children with young and educationally disadvantaged parents, and were more pronounced for reading skills than for math skills. In addition, associations were concentrated among younger children. For example, an additional year of completed education was associated with a .23 increase in children’s reading skills between ages 6 and 8, whereas no discernable benefits were found for mothers’ education completed after age 10. Prior studies have considered the most basic question, whether additional education improves the achievement of disadvantaged children, with little attention to whether the effects are heterogeneous. Studies have also suggested that the most likely mechanism for explaining these effects is an improvement in the quality of children’s home environments, which includes more enriching learning opportunities and responsive and supportive parenting. It is likely, however, that not all educational experiences are likely to bring about such positive changes in a child’s environment. Attending school may be more costly and stressful for some mothers than for other mothers, and the additional resource costs may reduce the impact of such education on children’s home environments. In particular, in the current policy context, mothers with very low incomes or single parents may find meeting demands of being a student, caregiver, and provider difficult (Polakow, Butler, Deprez, & Kahn, 2004). For these families, it may be that in the short term additional education does not improve the home learning environment or the achievement of their children. 6 Although there may be heterogeneity in the effects of additional maternal education in general, the difficulty of combining parenting and student roles may have gotten more difficult in recent years by the passage of welfare reform legislation in 1996. Such legislation has greatly reduced access to cash support for mothers pursuing their education, although some support is still available for basic and vocational education (Polakow et al., 2004). This may mean that disadvantaged mothers who are pursuing their education in the post-welfare reform era are facing greater economic hardship while in school, are more likely to combine their schooling with working, attend school part-time, or are seeking out a different type of education. To date, all of the studies have used data collected from the pre-welfare reform era; thus, it is not clear if the association between maternal education and children’s achievement will be as strong in the current policy context as prior studies suggest. Although evidence is accumulating that improvements in maternal education may benefit children’s academic and language skills, the research literature is still quite sparse. The present study uses a large longitudinal study, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Cohort, to consider whether changes in maternal education are associated with concurrent improvements in children’s math and reading skills. We hypothesize that maternal education will be linked to higher levels of academic achievement and that this association will be most evident among mothers with lower levels of initial schooling. In addition, we examine whether increases in maternal education are associated with reductions in grade retention and receipt of special education services. The study explores whether there is heterogeneity in the effects of education, considering subgroups defined by income (low income vs. higher income families) and family structure (single vs. two-parent families). 7 Study Methods Data Data for this study are from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K), kindergarten through fifth grade public use files. The ECLS-K, which was designed and conducted by the National Center for Educational Statistics (NCES), provides a nationally representative sample of children who attended kindergarten beginning in the fall of 1998. Six waves of the survey are currently available, beginning in the fall of kindergarten and continuing through the spring of fifth grade. For this study, we use information collected during the fall of kindergarten (1998) through the spring of first grade (2000).1 Sample The sample consists of 10,680 children with complete data for fall kindergarten and spring first grade assessments in reading and math. In addition to non-response, missing assessment data in the fall of kindergarten occurred because children who spoke a language other than English or Spanish, or were determined to have significant developmental disabilities, were not tested (N=378). Children who spoke Spanish were given a reduced version of assessments; therefore, they are also excluded from our sample (N=866). Missing data in spring of first grade in part reflects planned attrition; only half of the students who changed schools after the fall of kindergarten were followed in subsequent waves of the study. Also excluded from the sample are children whose parents did not complete surveys in the fall of kindergarten (N=775) or the spring of first grade (N=950), because these are the sources of information about mothers’ education. Finally, an additional 1,507 cases are excluded because we do not have information 1 Current analyses are limited to the kindergarten and first grade waves due to a problem with the collection of data in round five (spring of third grade), which led to significant measurement error in the variables used to determine changes in maternal education (Elvira Hausken, NCES, personal communication, July 3, 2007). We are currently attempting to find alternative ways to identify such changes, in order to incorporate more waves of data in future iterations of this study. 8 on our key explanatory variables, mothers’ initial level of education or change in education between the fall of kindergarten and spring of first grade. The primary reason for this type of missing data was that the child’s mother was not identified as a primary caregiver.2 Characteristics of the sample of children and their families are presented in Table 1. Although the above exclusions should be noted when interpreting our findings, they create a consistent sample with which to estimate associations between changes in maternal education and children’s academic skills. Given that it is likely that we are analyzing a slightly more advantaged sample, however, we consider the possibility of differential effects for children of mothers with lower initial levels of education.3 Measures Academic Outcomes. The primary outcomes of interest in this study are reading and math skills, assessed in one-on-one testing sessions in the spring of first grade. Scores from comparable assessments conducted in the fall of kindergarten are used as control variables in most regressions. These assessments were created specifically for the ECLS-K by a team of experts, with some items adapted from existing instruments. The reading test included items measuring knowledge of letters, beginning and ending sounds, word recognition, vocabulary, and passage comprehension. The math test assessed understanding of numbers, operations, measurement, patterns, and geometry and spatial relations. NCES reports high reliabilities for all assessments (Tourangeau, Nord, Lê, Pollack, & Atkins-Burnett, 2006). 2 We impose this restriction because NCES only asked about educational changes for household members identified as primary caretakers. Thus, if mothers resided in the home, but were not listed as the first or second household member on the household roster, data was not collected on their education at later waves. 3 The weights constructed by NCES for longitudinal analyses are not appropriate given the criteria we use for sample selection. However, applying weights does not substantially change the pattern of results reported in the tables. 9 Math and reading outcomes are transformations of latent Item Response Theory (IRT) ability scores into standardized t-scores with a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10 (based on the full sample distribution).4 These scores are interpreted as children’s performance relative to their peers, and can be converted to effect sizes by dividing the regression coefficients by 10. Children in the sample analyzed scored slightly higher than the full ECLS-K sample, suggesting disproportionate attrition of less-skilled children (see Table 1). Reading and math skills were highly correlated at both kindergarten (0.77, p<.01) and first grade (0.70, p<.01). In the spring of 2000, data were collected on the child’s grade placement. If this information suggested that the child still in kindergarten, rather than progressing directly into first grade, the child was coded as having been retained. About 3.3 percent of students repeated kindergarten in the 1999-2000 school year. We do not categorize students who were repeating kindergarten in the fall of 1998 as having been retained, as they were retained before the time of this study (and their mothers’ educational increase). We do, however, use such an indicator as a control in our analyses. Data from the teacher survey in the spring of first grade also provided information about whether students received any special educational services. We use this information to create an indicator of special education receipt. About 4.5 percent of the sample received special education services in spring of first grade. Maternal Education. The key independent measure in this study is whether mothers’ educational attainment increased between the fall of their child’s kindergarten year and the spring of first grade. We identify such changes from responses to a question in the first grade spring survey that asked whether the child’s mother had completed any additional grades of 4 We use the T-scores because Reardon (2007) suggests that these scores have better properties than the unstandardized latent IRT scores. 10 school or received any diplomas or degrees. A dummy variable was created indicating the presence of additional schooling (1=yes, completed additional schooling; 0=no). This measure captures not only mothers who completed additional degrees, but also those who continued their education at a particular level (e.g., mothers who were continuing with college but had not yet finished a bachelor’s degree). Given that mothers’ educational attainment was likely to be recorded with some error, we checked the pattern of responses for inconsistencies. In particular, for mothers who indicated that they had increased their education between rounds, we compared the type of new degree or additional grade of schooling to the initial level of maternal education composite from round one of the parent survey (a categorical variable with nine options ranging from eighth grade or below to doctoral or professional degree). If the new degree information was missing, seemed unobtainable given the time between survey rounds (e.g., an advanced degree for a mother with less than a high school degree initially), or was less than the initial level of education, the variable indicating change in maternal education was set to missing (N=58). Any remaining error in the measures of mothers’ education will likely attenuate estimates of the effects of increases in mothers’ educational attainment. Using this method, we identified 519 mothers (5 percent of the sample) whose education increased between their child’s kindergarten and first grade year. The type of education mothers pursued varied. Most mothers reported that they had completed a vocational degree (30%) or some college (22%). Less common was completing an associate’s degree (16%), a bachelor’s degree (10%), some graduate or professional schooling 11 (10%), or a high school diploma or General Equivalency Degree (10%). Least common was completing a year of high school without obtaining a degree (less than 1%).5 Covariates. Mothers who pursue additional education are likely to be different from those who do not. In order to isolate the effects of additional maternal education, we include an extensive set of controls for child and family background characteristics that might be associated with both mothers’ decisions to obtain more education and their children’s academic outcomes. Measures were drawn primarily from surveys of parents in the fall of kindergarten, and include demographic and family information such as child age; gender; race/ethnicity; birth weight; height; weight; region of the country; urbanicity; family structure and size; parental education and employment; income to needs ratio; mother’s immigrant status; mother’s age at birth of child; language spoken in the home; whether the child was a first-time kindergartener; and whether the child attended center-based childcare, non-center-based childcare, or Head Start in the year before kindergarten. 6 Descriptive statistics for the demographic covariates are presented in Table 1. Most regressions include an additional set of covariates meant to proxy for the quality of home learning environment experienced by each child. These include measures of how often the family participates in activities such as singing songs, building things, and doing arts and crafts. In addition, several covariates designed to indicate early literacy practices (e.g., the number of children’s books in the home, how often the child is read to or looks at picture books) are also included. Appendix Table 1 provides details describing all of the covariates and how they were constructed. 5 Additional information about their school attendance; for example, whether they attended school full or part-time, was not collected. 6 Information regarding mother’s immigrant status was available only from the spring of kindergarten parent questionnaire. 12 Some of the covariates were missing information at the item level, although rates of missing data are quite low, less than 3 percent for most cases. To retain these cases, relevant regressors were set to zero, and dummy variables were created to indicate the presence of missing values (Allison, 2002). For example, for a case missing data on father’s initial level of education (the most common type of missing data), paternal education was coded as having a value of zero, and a dummy variable indicating missing father’s education data was created. This approach yields unbiased estimates of the key independent variables when there are low rates of missing data (Allison, 2002). Analytic Models Given the clustered structure of the data in the ECLS-K (students clustered within schools), we used hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to assess the extent to which mothers’ increases in education between fall of kindergarten and the spring of first grade are associated with children’s reading and math skills at the spring of first grade. HLM is a generalization of linear regression that accounts for the fact that observations of students’ outcomes within a school may not be statistically independent (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).7 In our HLM models, fixed effects, which are analogous to standard regression coefficients, are directly estimated at the individual child level, while allowing for a random intercept term at the school level.8 For our dichotomous outcomes, grade retention and receipt of special education services, we used probit regression models, and for ease interpretation, report marginal effects. To account for the non-independence among students within a school in the probit models, we used clustered standard errors. 7 In our sample, the intraclass correlation (i.e., the proportion of variation existing between schools) for first grade spring reading scores was 0.21, and for math scores was 0.19, suggesting that HLM is an appropriate analytic strategy. The 10,680 students in the sample attended 910 different schools. 8 As a robustness check, we also ran OLS models, clustering standard errors at the school level. These models produced estimates that were very similar to the HLM results. 13 The primary challenge of our regression approaches is to ensure that we account for the variety of ways in which mothers who go back to school after the birth of their children may differ from mothers who do not. First, descriptive statistics were used to explore the extent to which mothers whose education increased differed from mothers whose education did not change. If our statistical models did not account for these possible differences, we may risk attributing to mothers’ improved education benefits that reflect differing child or family background characteristics. Next, we regressed children’s academic skills on an indicator of whether mothers’ education had increased. We adjusted for initial level of skill achievement in reading and math as well as background differences using the rich data collected in the ECLS-K study. These data enabled us to statistically control for many preexisting differences that may explain the association between maternal education and children’s achievement. Unobserved characteristics may still result in omitted variable biases. Four separate estimation models with differing levels of controls were conducted for each outcome. The first regression model included only the indicator for whether a mother had increased her education. In the second model, children’s achievement in reading and math during the fall of kindergarten was entered. In the third model, a set of child, mother, and family controls were included. In the fourth model, measures of the child’s home learning environment were added. By parsing out any association between mothers’ education and initial levels of achievement and family characteristics, this fourth regression model is particularly powerful in controlling for earlier differences in family contexts and children’s development. We do not wish to control for co-occurring changes that result from an improvement in mothers’ education, as these may reflect the mechanism by which maternal 14 education influences children’s development. If, however, there are spurious and simultaneous changes that are not due to mothers’ education, these may bias our results. First, we estimated regressions among the full sample of children. Second, because previous research has suggested that the effects of incremental changes in maternal education are larger for mothers who have initially low levels of education, we also estimated these models for the subgroup of mothers who reported having a vocational degree or less education (N=4,640) and for those who had completed at least some college (N=6,040). We expected that the associations would be larger for mothers with lower levels of education. Finally, among the lesseducated families, we explored whether some subgroups of children were more or less likely to benefit from their mothers’ increased schooling. We considered low-income families (those with household incomes <=1.5 of the federal poverty threshold) and single parent families. Even holding constant a large set of observed characteristics may fail to appropriately estimate maternal education effects if the children whose mothers complete additional schooling differ greatly from comparison children. For example, regression estimates may be biased if there is insufficient overlap in distribution of observed characteristics, and the models are forced to extrapolate beyond the data. Our HLM models also impose assumptions about the linearity and additivity of regressors that are difficult to test with many covariates. Selecting an appropriate comparison group through propensity score matching offers an alternative way to obtain comparable samples and requires fewer assumptions about the “correct” functional form than regression approaches. Our propensity score analysis proceeds in two steps. First, we estimate a propensity score for each individual as the conditional probability (from a probit model) of having a mother whose education increases given the full set of covariates. 15 The propensity score is next used to create a matched control group of children whose mothers did not complete additional schooling. 9 We use the nearest neighbor matching technique and limit the sample to children for whom there is sufficient overlap in propensity scores between the maternal education and comparison group (the area of common support).10 If the matching process proceeds correctly, the treatment and control children will have similar measured characteristics, and the effects of increased maternal education can be estimated by regression methods, which again include controls for all covariates. 11 Because propensity score techniques match cases on measured characteristics, unobserved differences between children whose mothers completed additional schooling and other children remain a possible source of bias in these analyses. Results We begin by considering how mothers who pursue additional education after the birth of their children differ from mothers who do not. Prior research has indicated that mothers who attend school have more to gain from it than mothers who do not, and this appears to be the case in these data as well (Table 1). Mothers who completed additional education have lower household incomes, are more likely to have been teen parents, and less likely to live with the biological father of their children compared with other mothers. They also have partners with lower levels of education. These differences suggest that mothers who pursued education after the birth of their children are disadvantaged compared with mothers who do not attend school. 9 We include a continuous measure of family income-to-needs. We use STATA8’s shareware psmatch2 program to conduct the propensity score analysis, specify a caliper width of .01, and allow for “replacement” (or the ability for a comparison case to serve as a control more than once). 11 We use OLS regressions for achievement, because with the reduced sample size children are not sufficiently clustered within schools to estimate a random effect at the school level. We use probit models for grade retention and special education placement regressions. All regressions include clustered standard errors to account for nonindependence within the school. 10 16 Given these disparities, it is not surprising that their children also have lower levels of achievement. Splitting the sample by prior levels of education reduces the disparities, but it is notable that mothers who attend school are still significantly more disadvantaged than those who do not. Understanding how mothers who increase their education differ from those who do not provides some insight into how we might expect their children to fare. Among the full sample, if mothers who complete additional schooling are relatively disadvantaged, then it is likely that their children may not fare as well as other children. Means presented in Table 1 indicate that on, average, children who have mothers whose education increased between the fall of kindergarten and the spring of first grade performed significantly less well on both the kindergarten and the first grade achievement tests. Although children of mothers’ whose education increased had lower levels of special education and grade retention, this difference did not reach statistical significance. Results from the HLM regression models, which include a school level random error, however, suggest no difference in the reading and math skills of children whose mothers completed additional schooling compared with other children (Table 2). Adding in controls for children’s earlier achievement scores results in positive coefficients for maternal education, although the coefficients are small and not significant. Additional controls for family background differences further increase the maternal education coefficients, to the point of marginal significance for reading only (columns 3 and 4 of Table 2). Results of the propensity score analysis are displayed in column 5 of Table 2. The goal of this approach is to construct a comparison sample that is matched to the treatment group on the predicted likelihood of having a mother who returns to school, and also on all regressors. 17 Consequently, one key step is to ensure that the observable characteristics of the comparison group (created in the first step) do not differ from those of the maternal education group. Our check for such balance confirmed that there were no differences in the mean level of covariates across the two groups.12 Satisfied that our matching resulted in an appropriate comparison group, we continued to the second stage of the propensity score analysis. The resulting regression based propensity score coefficient estimates are very similar to those from the HLM analyses. This suggests that our HLM results were not biased by using an inappropriate comparison group. The results from our probit models, which estimate marginal changes in the probability of grade retention and receipt of special education services, are presented in Table 3. Although the negative coefficients suggest a potential association between maternal education and reductions in grade retention and receipt of special education services, they do not reach levels of statistical significance.13 We next consider whether the effects differ by mothers’ prior level of education, by looking at the association between increased maternal education and children’s achievement within more and less-educated mother subgroups. First, taking children of mothers with a vocational degree or less education (top panel of Table 4), we find that without controls there appears to be no benefit of increases in maternal education. However, after controls are included for both prior achievement and family characteristics, there is a positive effect of increases in mother’s education on children’s reading achievement (effect size of .10). Moreover, the 12 We used a Hotelling T2 test for the joint equality of covariate means, conducted for 5 groups stratified on the propensity scores, which further verified balance on the covariates across the additional maternal education and comparison group. Balance was achieved for the full sample, as well as each of the education subgroups we consider. 13 We did not estimate propensity score models for these outcomes, because the reduced sample size and low frequency of grade retention and special education placement limited the ability of these models to converge. 18 propensity score model, which also includes a full set of covariates, yields a somewhat larger estimate than the HLM models (effect size of .15). In the case of math achievement, the estimated effects are again positive after controls are added, but do not reach statistical significance in the HLM and propensity score models (effect sizes of .07 and .10, respectively). Among children of mothers who have more than a vocational degree in the fall of kindergarten, children whose mothers complete additional education have considerably lower achievement than their peers whose mothers did not attend school (Table 4, panel 2). Although including controls reduces these disparities, both the HLM and propensity score results suggest there is no apparent association between mothers’ continued schooling and children’s reading or math achievement. The associations between additional maternal schooling and children’s grade retention and special education placement also differ by mothers’ prior level of education. For both outcomes, coefficients for children of mothers with lower initial levels of education are consistently negative and larger, compared to the coefficients for children whose mothers had higher initial levels of education (Table 5). Again, however, most coefficients fail to reach statistical significance. Next, we look at variation within the subgroup of mothers with low levels of education.14 The results presented in Table 6 come from HLM models with a full set of covariates (Model 4 in Tables 2 through 5). Results for all of the low-educated mothers are repeated in column 1 of Table 6 for comparative purposes. First, we consider the association between increased maternal education among families that differ in terms of their household incomes (low income vs. higher income) as well as their family structure (single parent vs. two parent families). Ideally, we 14 We have also considered these subgroups among the more highly educated mothers (mothers with at least some college) and find that there is no discernable association among any of these groups. 19 would distinguish between subgroups based on characteristics of families before mothers attended school. Given that family structure is measured in the fall of kindergarten, it is likely exogenous to mothers’ schooling. However, the income measure was not collected until the spring of kindergarten, and thus it is possible that household incomes may have been affected by mothers’ school attendance. For this reason, this subgroup split provides only a rough guideline about how the effects of maternal education may differ across families with differing levels of economic resources. With respect to reading, the pattern of associations is remarkably consistent in suggesting that the association between maternal education and achievement appear to be more pronounced among more advantaged families with low levels of education. Among those families who have low incomes (income-to-needs ratio less than or equal to 1.5 of the federal poverty threshold), there is little detectable effect of increased maternal education; whereas among those with higher income-to-needs ratios, the association appears to be more substantial (.04 versus .16 effect sizes). Likewise, the association is negative (-.06 effect size), albeit imprecisely estimated, among single parent families, but positive and larger among two parent families (.16 effect size). In terms of math, we find that there is less consistent evidence of a differential pattern of effects by family income. However, it seems that a positive association emerges among twoparent families (effect size of .13), whereas the association is negative (although again imprecisely estimated) among single parent families. With respect to grade retention and special education, coefficient patterns for groups defined by income and family structure are less consistent (Table 7). Negative coefficients, suggesting an association between increased maternal education and reductions in grade retention and special education receipt, are present for both the low- and higher-income sub- 20 groups, although they do not reach statistical significance. The results for the single and not single parent sub-groups are more consistent with the achievement findings. For non-single parent families, increased maternal education is associated with a marginally significant 0.8 percentage point reduction in the probability of being retained. Discussion Consistent with prior research, these preliminary results suggest that children of mothers with lower levels of education appear to have improved reading skills when their mothers’ education improves. The findings indicate no such association among more highly educated mothers. In addition, there is less improvement in children’s math skills. These results appear to be robust across HLM and propensity score methods. We also find some evidence of a possible trend linking increases in mothers’ education to reductions in the probability of receipt of special education services and grade retention among children of mothers with lower initial levels of education. The stronger association between mothers’ increased education and children’s reading skills compared with math skills replicates findings from prior studies. The explanation for such a pattern of effects, however, is elusive. There are several possible reasons for this difference. Young children’s calculation skills may not be sensitive to variations in their social background including parental education, once differences in verbal skills are taken into account (Entwisle & Alexander, 1990; Ginsberg & Russell, 1981; Jordan et al., 1992). Some research has suggested that social class differences in verbal and literacy learning opportunities may be greater than differences in math learning opportunities (Jordan et al., 1992; Kellaghan, 1997; Saxe, Guberman & Gearhart, 1987), and that math skills and concepts are primarily learned through formal instruction in school (Entwisle & Alexander, 1992). Alternatively, if mothers are more 21 likely to undertake educational activities that bolster their language and literacy skills rather than mathematical skills, then this might also explain this pattern of effects. The magnitude of the association between improvements in maternal education and children’s reading skills is quite modest, with estimated effect sizes ranging from .10 to .16. These estimates correspond remarkably well to the effect of an additional year of parental education on children’s achievement suggested by prior rigorous cross sectional studies. These findings, however, are just slightly smaller than estimates derived from prior longitudinal studies of changes in mothers’ educational attainment, which have typically estimated effect sizes of about 0.20 for children of a similar age (Magnuson, 2007; Magnuson et al., 2007). One explanation for the slightly lower estimates in this study compared with prior studies is that the policy context may differ. The current study is conducted after welfare reform, which may have limited poor women’s access to welfare benefits while they attend school (Covington & Spriggs, 2004; Jacobs & Winslow, 2003). Prior studies draw their data from the pre-welfare reform era in which economic supports were available to mothers pursuing additional education and less emphasis was placed on quickly transitioning mothers into the labor market. Such policies may have reduced the benefit of maternal education by making it more difficult and costly for low-income mothers to attend school. This, in turn, may have changed the type of education that mothers pursued, reducing attendance in higher educational institutions in particular, and increasing vocational education (Polakow et al., 2004). Also, it may have increased the likelihood that families faced financial hardship, or mothers were combining school and work. A unique contribution of this study is its investigation of the relationship between increases in maternal education and other important school outcomes. Evidence suggests that for 22 children of non-single mothers with lower initial levels of education, increases in maternal schooling are associated with a reduction in the probability of grade retention. The low frequency of both grade retention and special education makes changes in these outcomes difficult to detect, yet many of the results hovered near marginal significance. We will continue our work with these outcomes, and if these analyses hold up in additional work, the findings are promising, given the mounting evidence of long-term negative impacts of retention on children (Hauser, 2000), as well as the societal financial benefits to be realized from reductions in remediation and unnecessary special education service provision (Reynolds et al., 2001). Unlike prior studies, this study considered whether effects were homogenous across the sample of less educated mothers, and found that the pattern of associations appeared to differ by household income and family structure. However, with information on family income collected in the spring of kindergarten, we caution that family income may in part be endogenous. Nevertheless, in both cases, the estimated association between increased maternal education and achievement appear to be slightly stronger among the more advantaged families (non-low income and two parent families). Why? We speculate that the answer hinges on the processes that are operating to improve children’s achievement. The main theoretical explanation for improved achievement is that additional maternal education results in improved home learning environments, including enhanced literacy and learning opportunities and activities. Certainly, parents with lower levels of education are more likely to have more to gain from additional education, and yet for parents with few supports or resources, additional education may be less likely to translate into changes in their children’s home environments. Without support or assistance, low-income and single mothers may find combining school and family responsibilities financially difficult and psychologically stressful. Thus, we hypothesize that in 23 the current policy context, for low-income and single parents, the costs of schooling may eclipse the benefits, at least in the short term, and may do little to promote their children’s achievement. Another explanation may be the type of schooling that mothers in these two groups experience. Low-income and single mothers were more likely to complete a high school degree or GED than their more advantaged peers. Recent research has suggested that completing a GED may do little to promote skills, and thus has little payoff in the labor market (Heckman & LaFontaine, 2006). If this type of education is also less beneficial than other types of schooling, then this might explain why there is less overall association between increases in maternal education and children’s achievement among the more disadvantaged mothers. We intend to explore these possible explanations in future work. There are several important limitations to this preliminary work. First, it is important to recognize that these analyses are non-experimental and may not identify causal associations. The analytic approaches employed attempt to reduce omitted and third variable bias by controlling not only for important demographic and family characteristics, but also for children’s prior achievement. Nevertheless, it is possible that bias remains. Second, although this study goes beyond the usual investigation of children’s academic achievement and includes alternative outcome measures such as grade retention and receipt of special education services, other indicators of school success should be considered. For example, future research should explore whether increases in maternal education predict disciplinary actions and engagement with school. Third, our analyses only consider children’s achievement at the spring of first grade. We very much would like to know whether any positive benefits persist beyond first grade. With ECLS-K data on children’s later achievement, we hope to answer this question in future work. Fourth, we plan to explore whether particular types of 24 education are more or less strongly associated with children’s achievement outcomes. Finally, states vary considerably in the extent to which they allow or encourage mothers to pursue education through their welfare policies. We hope to exploit such state variation to estimate instrumental variable models of the effects of additional education on children’s achievement. Such an approach, if it can be well implemented, may improve our confidence in inferring causality. In summary, these findings replicate prior studies that have found that improvements in mothers’ education appear to be linked to better reading achievement among their children. In addition, increasing maternal education may result in reduced probabilities of grade retention and receipt of special education services. These associations, however, are modest and limited to a sample of less educated mothers. 25 References Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Bedinger, S. D. (1994). When expectations work: Race and socioeconomic differences in school performance. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57(4), 283299. Allison, P. (2002). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Astone, N. M., Schoeni, R., Ensminger, M., & Rothert, K. (2000). School reentry in early adulthood: The case of inner-city African Americans. Sociology of Education, 73, 133-154. Bornstein, M.H., Hahn, C.S., Suwalsky, J..T.D., & Haynes, O. M. (2003). Socioeconomic status, parenting and child development: The Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status and the Socioeconomic Index of Occupations. In M. H. Bornstein, & R.H.Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development (pp. 29-82). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bracken, B. A. (1984). Bracken Basic Concepts Scale. San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation. Bradburn, E. M., Moen, P., & Dempster-McClain, D. (1995). Women’s return to school following the transition to motherhood. Social Forces, 73, 1517-1551. Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., Burchinal, M., McAdoo, H. P., & Garcia Coll, C. (2001). The home environments of children in the United States Part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development, 72, 1868-1886. Brody, G. H., & Flor, D. L. (1998). Maternal resources, parenting practices, and child competence in rural, single-parent African American families. Child Development, 69, 803816. 26 Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723-742. Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME). Little Rock: University of Arkansas at Little Rock. Corwyn, R. F., & Bradley, R. H. (2003). Socioeconomic status, poverty status and childhood externalizing behaviors: Theoretical and methodological considerations within a Structural Equation Modeling framework. In V. Bengtson, A. Acock, K Allen, P. Dilworth-Anderson & D. Klein (Eds.), Sourcebook of Family Theories and Methods: An Interactive Approach. Minneapolis: National Council on Family Relations. Covington, K. L. & Spriggs, W. E. (2004). Negative effects of state welfare policies on recipient college enrollment. Review of Black Political Economy 32(2): 7-26. Davis-Kean, P. (2005).The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: The indirect role of parent expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 294-304. Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. A. (2001). Change in family income matters more for children with less. Child Development, 72, 1779-1793. Elder, G. Jr. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4-15. Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1990). Beginning school math competence: Minority and majority comparisons. Child Development, 61, 454-471. Entwisle, D., & Alexander, K. L. (1992). Summer setback: Race, poverty, school composition and mathematics achievement in the first two years of school. American Sociological Review, 57, 72-84. 27 Entwisle, D., & Alexander, K. L. (1993). Entry into school: The beginning school transition and educational stratification in the United States. Annual Review of Sociology, 19, 401-423. Furstenberg, F. F., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Morgan, S. P. (1987). Adolescent mothers in later life. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Ginsberg, H. P., & Russell, R. L. (1981). Social class and racial influences on early mathematical thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46 (2, serial No. 193). Halle, T. G., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Mahoney, J. L. (1997). Family influences on school achievement in low-income, African American children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 527-537. Harris, Y., R., Terrel, D., & Allen, G. (1999). The influence of education context and beliefs on the teaching behavior of African American mothers. Journal of Black Psychology, 25, 490-503. Hauser, R. (2000). Should we end social promotion? Truth and consequences. Available on-line at: http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_01/0000019b/80/16/74/a4.pd f. Haveman, R. & Wolfe, B. (1995).The determinants of children’s attainments: A review of methods and findings. Journal of Economic Literature, 23, 1829-1878. Heckman, J. J., & LaFontaine, P. A. (2006). Bias-corrected estimates of GED returns. Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 661-700. Hill, N., Castellino, D. R., Lansford, J. E., Nowlin, P., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E. et al. (2004). Parent academic involvement as related to school behavior, achievement, and aspirations: Demographic variations across adolescence. Child Development, 75, 1491-1509. Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development, Developmental 28 Review, 26, 55-88. Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1378. Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 603-629. Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input at home and at school: Relation to child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337-374. Jacobs, J. & Stoner-Eby, S. (1998). Adult enrollment in high education and cumulative educational attainment, 1970-1990. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 559, 91-108. Jacobs, J. A. & Winslow, S. (2003). Welfare reform and enrollment in postsecondary education. The Annals, AAPSS, 58: 194-217. Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (1992). Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle and low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 28, 644-653. Kellaghan, T. (1997). Relationship between home environment and scholastic behavior in a disadvantaged population. Journal of Educational Psychology, 33, 107-119. Laosa, L. (1980). Maternal teaching strategies in Chicano and Anglo-American families: The influence of culture and education on maternal behavior. Child Development, 51, 759-765. Lee, V., & Burkham, D. (2002). Inequality at the starting gate: Social background differences in achievement as children begin school. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute. Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi-Schwartz, A., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A.,et al. (2002). Making a difference in the lives of infants and toddlers and their families: The 29 impacts of Early Head Start. Princeton, NJ: Mathematica Policy Research. Macmillan, R., McMorris, B. J., Kruttschnitt, C. (2004). Linked lives: Stability and change in maternal circumstances and trajectories of antisocial behavior in children. Child Development, 75, 205-220. Magnuson, K. (2007). The effect of increases in maternal education on children’s academic trajectories: Evidence from the NLSY. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin, Institute for Research on Poverty. Magnuson, K. (2003).The effect of increases in welfare mothers’ education on their young children’s academic and behavioral outcomes. University of Wisconsin, Institute for Research on Poverty Discussion Paper, 1274-03. Maynard, R., Nicholson, W., & Rangarajan, A. (1993). Breaking the cycle of poverty: The effectiveness of mandatory services for welfare-dependent teenage parents. Princeton, NJ: Mathematica Policy Research, Inc. McGroder, S. M., ., Zaslow, M. J., Moore, K. A., & LeMenestrel, S. M. (2000). National evaluation of Welfare-to-Work Strategies impacts on young children and their families two years after enrollment: Findings from the child outcomes study. Washington, DC: United States Department of Health and Human. Moore, Q., & Schmidt, L. (2007). Do maternal investments in human capital affect children’s cognitive development? Unpublished manuscript, Williams College, Department of Economics. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41, 428-442. 30 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network & Duncan, G. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children’s preschool cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 1454-1475. Polakow, V., Butler, S., Deprez, L. S., & Kahn, P (Eds). (2004). Shut out: Low income mothers and higher education in post-welfare America. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Quint, J., Bos, J., & Polit, D. (1997). New Chance: Final report on a comprehensive program for young mothers in poverty and their children. New York, NY: MDRC. Raudenbush, S.J. and A.S. Bryk. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Raviv, T., Kessenich, M, & Morrison, F.J. (2004). A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 528-547. Reardon, S. (2007). Thirteen ways of looking at the black-white test score gap. Unpublished manuscript. Reynell, J. (1991). Reynell Developmental Language Scales (U.S. Edition). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service. Reynolds, A.J., Temple, J.A., Robertson, D.L., & Mann, E.A. (2001). Long-term effects of an early childhood intervention on educational achievement and juvenile arrest. JAMA, 285, 2339-2346. Rich, L. M., & Kim, S. (1999). Patterns of later life education among teenage mothers. Gender and Society, 13, 798-817. 31 Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., & LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28, 614-621. Rosenzweig, M. R., & Wolpin, K. I. (1994). Are there increasing returns to intergenerational production of human capital? Journal of Human Resources, 29, 670-693. Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R., & Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52 (2, Serial No. 216). Shin, H. B (2005). School enrollment—social and economic characteristics of students: October 2003. (Current Population Report P20-554). Washington, DC: US Census Bureau. Sobel, M. E. (1998). Causal inference in statistical models of the process of socioeconomic achievement. Sociological Methods and Research, 27, 318-348. Stevenson, D. L., & Baker, D. P. (1987). The family-school relation and the child's school performance. Child Development, 58 (5), 1348-1357. Taylor, L. C., Clayton, J. D., & Rowley, S. J. (2004). Academic socialization: Understanding parental influences on children’s school-related development in the early years. Review of General Psychology, 8, 163-178. Tourangeau, K., Nord, C., Lê T., Pollack, J.M., and Atkins-Burnett, S. (2006). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS-K), Combined user’s manual for the ECLS-K fifth-grade data files and electronic codebooks (NCES 2006–032). U.S. Department of Education. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. Tracey, D. H., & Young, J. W. (2002). Mothers’ helping behaviours during children’s at-home oral-reading practice: Effects of children’s reading ability, children’s gender and mothers’ educational level. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 729-737. 32 Votruba-Drzal, E. (2003). Income changes and cognitive stimulation in young children's home learning environments. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 341-355. 33 Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for Demographic Control Variables and Child Academic Outcomes Min Child Gender Male (1=Male, 0=Female) Race/Ethnicity White, Non-Hispanic (Referent) African American Hispanic Asian American Indian Other race/ethnicity Child Age, First Grade Spring (years) First Time Kindergartener Census Region West (Referent) Northeast Midwest South Urbanicity Rural (Referent) Urban Fringe or Large Town Central City Low Birth Weight (<=2500 grams) Number of Children in Household Family Structure 2 Biological Parents (Referent) 1 Biological, 1 Step Parent Single Parent Adoptive or Foster Parents Non-English Speaking Household Mother Not Born in US Mother <20 Years Old at Child's Birth Received WIC Benefits All (N=10,680) Max Mean SD Change in Maternal Education (N=519) Mean SD No Change in Maternal Education (N=10,161) Mean SD 0 1 .50 .50 .52 .50 .50 .50 0 0 0 0 0 0 6 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 8 1 .66 .13 .12 .04 .02 .04 7.25 .96 .47 .33 .32 .19 .13 .20 .35 .20 .54 .19 .14 .03 .04 .06 7.22 .96 .50 .40 .35 .17 .20 .23 .33 .19 .67 .12 .12 .04 .02 .04 7.25 .96 .47 .33 .32 .19 .13 .20 .35 .20 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 .20 .20 .28 .32 .40 .40 .45 .47 .18 .18 .29 .35 .39 .38 .46 .48 .20 .20 .28 .32 .40 .40 .45 .47 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 11 .24 .40 .37 .07 2.45 .42 .49 .48 .26 1.08 .25 .36 .40 .10 2.45 .43 .48 .49 .30 1.19 .23 .40 .37 .07 2.45 .42 .49 .48 .26 1.07 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 .71 .07 .19 .02 .06 .11 .10 .32 .45 .26 .39 .15 .24 .31 .31 .47 .60 .11 .29 .01 .08 .13 .18 .49 .49 .31 .45 .11 .28 .33 .39 .50 .72 .07 .19 .02 .06 .11 .10 .31 .45 .26 .39 .15 .23 .31 .30 .46 Sig. Diff. a, b, c a, b, c a,c c a, b a, b a, b, c a, c a, b, c a, c c a, b, c a, b, c Table 1 Continued: Means and Standard Deviations for Demographic Control Variables and Child Academic Outcomes Min Mother's Employment Not Employed (Referent) Employed Full-time Employed Part-time Father's Employment Not Employed (Referent) Employed Full-time Employed Part-time Mother's Education Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Some College or More Father's Education Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Some College or More Income to Needs Ratio All (N=10,680) Max Mean SD Change in Maternal Education (N=519) Mean SD No Change in Maternal Education (N=10,161) Mean SD 0 0 0 1 1 1 .30 .46 .24 .46 .50 .43 .29 .46 .25 .45 .50 .44 .30 .46 .24 .46 .50 .43 0 0 0 1 1 1 .04 .93 .03 .20 .26 .17 .05 .92 .03 .23 .27 .16 .04 .93 .03 .20 .26 .17 0 0 1 1 .43 .57 .50 .50 .41 .59 .49 .49 .44 .56 .50 .50 0 0 0 1 1 59.88 .37 .45 3.30 .48 .50 3.21 .41 .33 2.45 .49 .47 2.13 .37 .46 3.35 .48 .50 3.25 Sig. Diff. c a, b, c a, b, c Child Academic Outcomes Reading, Kindergarten Fall 22.09 95.14 51.39 10.03 49.86 9.29 51.46 10.06 a, c Math, Kindergarten Fall 18.62 96.25 52.14 9.71 50.57 9.09 52.22 9.74 a, b, c Reading, First Grade Spring 6.63 80.29 51.72 9.16 50.84 8.87 51.76 9.17 a, c Math, First Grade Spring 2.68 78.1 51.74 9.24 50.57 8.90 51.78 9.25 a, c Child Retained btwn. K and 1st Grade 0 1 .03 .18 .03 .16 .03 .18 Child Received Special Ed Services 0 1 .05 .21 .04 .19 .05 .21 NOTE: Significant difference between mothers who change education and those who do not is indicated by the following letters: a=difference among whole sample; b=difference among mothers with vocational/technical degree or less education; c=difference among mothers with some college or more education. For continuous outcomes, mean differences between mothers with changes and education and those with no change were determined through t-tests (p<0.05). For categorical (dichotomous) outcomes, differences between frequencies for the two groups were determined through chisquare tests (p<0.05). Table 2: Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Reading and Math Skills in Spring of First Grade, Full Sample Reading Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) Model 1 HLM Model 2 HLM Model 3 HLM Model 4 HLM Model 5 Propensity 0.07 (0.38) 0.37 (0.28) 0.43 (0.27) 0.46t (0.27) 0.67 t (0.41) X X X X X X X X X Model 4 HLM Model 5 Propensity K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables Model 1 HLM Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) -0.17 (0.39) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables t Math Model 2 Model 3 HLM HLM 0.18 (0.28) 0.32 (0.28) 0.32 (0.28) .32 (0.41) X X X X X X X X X NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05; p<0.10; Standard errors in parentheses. Missing data dummies included in models 3 and 4. The sample size for the HLM models is 10,680 and for the propensity models is 1,023. Table 3: Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Grade Retention and Receipt of Special Education Services in Spring of First Grade, Full Sample Grade Retention Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) Model 1 Probit Model 2 Probit Model 3 Probit Model 4 Probit -0.007 (0.007) -0.008 (0.005) -0.004 (0.003) -0.005 (0.003) X X X X X X 10,634 10,626 10,570 K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables N Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables 10,634 Model 1 Probit Special Education Model 2 Model 3 Probit Probit Model 4 Probit -0.009 (0.008) -0.011t (0.006) -0.006 (0.004) -0.005 (0.005) X X X X X X N 10,554 10,554 10,554 10,504 t NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05; p<0.10; Robust standard errors in parentheses, clustered by school. Missing data dummies included in models 3 and 4. Table 4: Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Reading and Math Skills in Spring of First Grade, by Initial Level of Maternal Education Mothers with a Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Education Model 1 HLM Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) 0.57 (0.64) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Reading Model 2 Model 4 HLM HLM Model 5 Propensity Model 1 HLM -0.04 (0.65) 0.87t (0.47) 0.97* (0.46) 1.45* (0.68) X X X X X X X Home Learning Variables Math Model 2 Model 4 HLM HLM Model 5 Propensity 0.41 (0.47) 0.73 (0.47) 0.99 (.70) X X X X X X X Mothers with Some College or More Education Model 1 HLM Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables -1.07 (0.47) Reading Model 2 Model 4 HLM HLM Math Model 4 HLM Model 5 Propensity Model 1 HLM Model 2 HLM -1.06 (0.47) -0.16 (0.34) -0.03 (0.34) 0.24 (.52) X X X X X X X -0.17 (0.34) -0.05 (0.34) 0.24 (.52) X X X X X X X Model 5 Propensity NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05, t p<0.10; Standard errors in parentheses. Missing data dummies included in model 4. The sample size for the first panel (mothers with a vocational degree or less education) for the HLM models is 4,640 and for the propensity score is 412. The sample size in the second panel (mothers with some college or more education) for the HLM models is 6,040 and for the propensity score models is 603. Table 5: Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Grade Retention and Receipt of Special Education Services in Spring of First Grade, by Initial Level of Maternal Education Mothers with a Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Education Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) Grade Retention Model 1 Model 2 Model 4 Probit Probit Probit Special Education Model 1 Model 2 Model 4 Probit Probit Probit -0.012 (0.011) -0.028 (0.015) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables N 4,617 -0.010 (0.007) -0.007 (0.003) X X X X 4,617 4,493 4,595 -0.027t (0.011) -0.015 (0.008) X X X X 4,595 4,566 Mothers with Some College or More Education Model 1 Probit Change in Maternal Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) -0.003 (0.009) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Grade Retention Model 2 Model 4 Probit Probit -0.007 (0.006) -0.002 (0.003) X X X Home Learning Variables N Special Education Model 1 Model 2 Model 4 Probit Probit Probit 0.006 (0.011) -0.001 (0.007) 0.002 (0.005) X X X X 6,017 6,017 5,964 X 5,959 5,959 NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05, t p<0.10; Standard errors in parentheses, clustered by school. Missing data dummies included in model 4. 5,870 Table 6: Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Reading and Math Skills in Spring of First Grade, for Mothers with a Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Education Reading All Mothers with Vo/Tech Degree or Less Educ. (N=4,640) Inc. to Needs <=1.5 (N=1,945) Inc. to Needs >1.5 (N=2,695) Single Parent (N=1,185) Not Single Parent (N=3,455) Model 4 HLM Model 4 HLM Model 4 HLM Model 4 HLM Model 4 HLM .97* (0.46) 0.37 (0.67) 1.64* (0.67) -0.59 (0.87) 1.64** (0.56) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables X X X X X X X X X X Home Learning Variables X X X X X All Mothers with Vo/Tech Degree or Less Educ. (N=4,640) Model 4 HLM Inc. to Needs <=1.5 (N=1,945) Model 4 HLM Inc. to Needs >1.5 (N=2,695) Model 4 HLM Single Parent (N=1,185) Model 4 HLM Not Single Parent (N=3,455) Model 4 HLM 0.73 (0.47) 0.61 (0.67) 0.85 (0.68) -0.37 (0.85) 1.25* (0.57) X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Change in Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) Math Change in Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05, t p<0.10; Standard errors in parentheses. Missing data dummies included in model 4. Table 7 : Summary of Results from Regressions of Changes in Maternal Education on Children's Grade Retention and Receipt of Special Education Services in Spring of First Grade, for Mothers with a Vocational/Technical Degree or Less Education Grade Retention Change in Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) K-Fall Test Scores Demographic Variables Home Learning Variables N Change in Education (K-Fall to 1st Grade-Spring) All Mothers with Vo/Tech Degree or Less Educ. (Low Ed.) Model 4 Probit Low Ed Inc. to Needs <=1.5 Model 4 Probit Low Ed Inc. to Needs >1.5 Model 4 Probit Low Ed Single Parent Model 4 Probit Low Ed Not Single Parent Model 4 Probit -0.007 (0.003) -0.009 (0.005) -0.003 (0.002) 0.001 (0.005) -0.008t (0.003) X X X 4,493 X X X X X X X X X 1,841 2,422 1,026 Receipt of Special Education Services X X X 3,330 All Mothers with Vo/Tech Degree or Less Educ. (Low Ed.) Model 4 Probit Low Ed. Inc. to Needs <=1.5 Model 4 Probit Low Ed. Inc. to Needs >1.5 Model 4 Probit Low Ed. Single Parent Model 4 Probit Low Ed. Not Single Parent Model 4 Probit -0.015 (0.008) -0.026 (0.012) -0.006 (0.011) -0.003 (0.008) -0.020 (0.009) K-Fall Test Scores X X X X X Demographic Variables X X X X X Home Learning Variables X X X X X N 4,566 1,820 2,630 1,049 3,354 NOTE: ** p<0.01, * p<0.05, t p<0.10; Robust standard errors in parentheses, clustered by school. Missing data dummies included in Model 4. Appendix Table 1: Definitions, Additional Details, and notes about Covariates Used in Analyses Constructs and Variables Definition, Details, and Notes Child Characteristics Continuous variable. Child age in years, 1st grade spring. Dummy variable (boy=1). Two dummy variables for: <1500 grams, 1500-2500 grams. Average of two interviewer assessed measurements in lbs. Average of two interviewer assessed measurements in inches. Black, Hispanic, Native American, Asian, Other race. Omitted reference group is White, non-Hispanic. Dummy variable Child age Child gender Birth weight Child weight Child height Race and ethnicity First time kindergartener Parental Reports of Family Characteristics Number of children in household Family structure Urbanicity Region of country Early maternal employment Father’s and mother’s education Non-English speaking household Teen mother Mother’s immigrant status Father’s and mother’s employment WIC participation Childcare the year before kindergarten Household income-to-needs ratio Ordinal variable. Ranges from 1 to 11. Single parent (one biological parent), blended family (one biological and one non-biological parent), adopted or foster parents. Three dummy variables. Omitted reference group is two biological parents. Residence in central city or large town. Two dummy variables. Omitted reference group is rural residence. Northeast, Midwest, South. Three dummy variables. Omitted reference group is West. Mother ever employed between child’s birth and entry into kindergarten. Dummy variable. Less than high school degree through advanced post-graduate degree. Five dummy variables for each parent. Omitted reference group is some college. Home language of child non-English. Dummy variable. Mother’s age-child’s age <20. Dummy variable. Mother not born in the United States. Dummy variable measured at kindergarten spring. Full-time (35 or more hours per week), part-time work (fewer than 35 hours per week). Two dummy variables for each parent. Omitted reference group is no work. Mother or child ever participated in Women, Infants, and Children nutritional supplement program. Dummy variable measured at kindergarten fall. Child received center-based care, non-center based care (from relative or non-relative), or participated in Head Start. Three dummy variables. Household income compared to federal poverty threshold. Nine dummy variables. Omitted reference group is household income-to-needs ratio greater than 4.5. Parental Reports of Home Learning Activities Home learning activities Number of children’s books in home Reading outside of school Frequency of parent engaging the child in 7 activities: building things, teaching about nature, playing sports, doing art, doing chores, singing songs, playing games. Responses range from 1 (not at all) to 4 (every day). Ordinal variable. Ranges from 0 to 200. Frequency of child looking at picture books and reading outside of school. Two variables with responses ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (every day).