Final Report for 08SM-Hudson “Product placement and its influence on children” One page Summary Recent marketing literature has called for more research that focuses on the influence of new media on children and specifically the use of product placement. The objective of this study was to analyze the impact of food and beverage product placements on children of different ages. Using an experimental approach, groups of children viewed the same television program, but with either healthy products or unhealthy brands digitally inserted. Children were exposed to a 20-minute section of a popular television show that typically contains a number of product placements. Respondents were divided into groups and each group saw the same television clip but with different (digitally inserted) placements of various types of food and beverages. Unaided recall was measured by asking children after exposure if they could recall any brands, sponsors or advertising messages in the program. Aided recall was measured by asking children if they recognized any brands shown to them (a list was provided). The influence of product placement on behavior was measured by offering children a selection of beverages and snacks. Children then completed a questionnaire with the help of the researchers asking specific questions about their recollection, observation and impressions of the show. In total 225 children from two schools took part in the experiment – 163 Grade 7 students (aged 10-12) and 62 students from Grades 3 and 4 (aged 7-9). The children exposed to product placements clearly recalled the products placed in the show. For example, of the children who saw the unhealthy segment, 90 percent remembered seeing Pepsi, 70 percent saw the Fruit Gushers, 75 percent saw the Cheetos, and 67 percent recalled seeing Reese’s Pieces. Further analysis revealed that a significantly higher percentage of older children recalled the product placements. However, the product placements had very little influence on immediate behavior for respondents as a whole. The majority of children chose Pepsi or Coke along with a packet of Fruit Gushers, regardless of the segment they watched. The only placement that possibly had an influence on behavior was Cheetos, as twice as many children who had been exposed to Cheetos chose that snack. The study has significant implications for public policy officials. The limited available research suggests that product placements are especially potent in their effects upon children and adolescents and the results of this study support that contention, even though placement had very little influence on immediate behaviour. While this study does not suggest a direct “media effect,” it draws attention to the need for considering both the ethical dimensions of this type of marketing and the ways that product placement might impact preferences over time. In Canada, policies related to product placement are currently quite vague, although the Broadcast Code for Advertising to Children states that “no children’s advertising may employ any device or technique that attempts to transmit messages below the threshold of normal awareness” (Advertising Standards Canada, 2004). Perhaps these policies should be re-visited. Finally, public health field officials who have the health of children a priority may be concerned that even though children were exposed to healthier product placements and given healthy snack options, the majority chose foods of poor nutritional quality. 1 Executive Summary This study examines advertising to children, one of the most important topics worthy of academic research in the marketing field (Hyman, Tansey and Clark, 1994). In particular there have been calls for more research that focuses on the influence of new media on children and specifically the use of product placement (Bachman, 2008). Product placement has been defined as the planned entry of products into movies or television shows that may influence viewers’ product beliefs and/or behaviors favorably (Balasubramanian, 1994). The practice has emerged as one of the fastest growing segments of the advertising market (Elliott, 2005; Hudson and Hudson, 2006) and it is predicted that up to 75% of all scripted, prime-time network shows in the US will soon star products or services paid for by advertisers (George, 2005). Companies like Walmart and Procter & Gamble are even creating their own television programs in order to have more control of the placement of their products (Vranica and Brown, 2010). Global paid product placement was estimated to be worth $4.38 billion in 2007—a growth of 30% from 2006—and product placement in films, TV shows, video games and even song lyrics is set to triple by 2010 (PQ Media, 2006). Alongside this growth and increasing sophistication have been rising concerns about the ethics of product placement. Research has confirmed that consumers are concerned about the ‘subliminal’ effect of product placement (Tiwsakul, Hackley and Szmigin, 2005). Others fear that product placement’s influence on the content of movies and television will seep into news magazines, where editorial content is seen by many as inviolate. Critics also claim that the trend of embedding products into songs is an invasion of music lovers’ privacy, and critics have articulated concerns over loss of artistic freedom resulting from the increased use of brands in video games (Neslon, 2002). Concern has been cited for product placements of ethically charged products (guns, alcohol and tobacco for example), while others question the appropriateness of allowing brands to be associated with video games that exhibit excessive violence (Gupta and Lord; Gibson and Maurer, 2000). Particularly contentious is the use of product placement in children’s programming (Hudson, Hudson and Peloza, 2008; Avery and Ferraro, 2000). Such tactics are becoming more and more common. Consequently, there have been calls for more research that focuses on the influence of product placement on children (American Psychological Association, 2004; Moore, 2004; Tiwsakul et al. 2005). This is because children have not yet developed sensitivity to this type of promotional tool (Avery and Ferraro, 2000) and yet its practice is growing. Furthermore, some researchers have suggested that advertising and product placement practices geared to children – which largely consists of ads for sugary cereals, candy and fast-food restaurants – may be contributing to the increase in childhood obesity by promoting unhealthy foods (Halford et al., 2007; Datamonitor, 2002; Story and French, 2004; Jeffery, 2008). In the past 35 years, the percentage of obese children in Canada has more than doubled, swelling to over 500,000 children according to Statistics Canada (Deveau, 2006). According to the latest Standing 2 Committee on Health Report, titled Healthy Weights for Healthy Kids, over 26 per cent of Canadian children are currently overweight or obese (House of Commons Canada, 2007). Although the advertising industry acknowledges the potentially harmful effects of advertising on child audiences, it generally balks at calls for it to take responsibility for such unintentional effects (Haefner, 1991). Product placement practitioners argue that the placement of brands in children’s programming is very limited (and certainly there is less product placement in television than film) (Beck, 2001). However, they know that children watch programs intended for other target audiences and are thus exposed to advertising intended for those audiences. It has been argued that unintended exposure to these advertisements may have powerful extended effects on child viewers’ role expectations, values and world views (Haefner, 1991). The general objective of this study was to fill a research gap in the literature when it comes to understanding the impact of product placement on children. Specific objectives were: to analyze the placement of food and beverage products on children of various ages; to measure and assess the cognitive and behavioural response to these food/ beverage product placements; and to provide relevant policy recommendations pertaining to product placement in terms of children’s preferences and awareness of advertising intent in relation to age. This study employed experimental methods, as they have been used successfully in previous product placement research (Russell, 2002; McKechnie and Zhou, 2003; Gupta and Lord, 1998). Based on Auty and Lewis (2004), children of different ages were exposed to a 20-minute section of a popular television show that typically contains a number of product placements. Child-respondents were divided into groups and each group saw the same television clip but with different (digitally inserted) placements of various types of food and beverages. All groups were shown a clip from Pop Idol – the British equivalent of American Idol. Pop Idol was selected because the children are familiar with the format of the show (in light of the popularity and audience base of American Idol), but do not know the exact players or have preconceived expectations about the “proper” product placement (i.e., the ubiquitous and prominent Coke promotions in American Idol, etc.). American Idol has millions of child viewers, and in 2008 branded content in the show jumped 19 percent. The first 28 episodes of 2008 included 4,151 product placements (Sullivan, 2008). In the experimental design, Groups 1 (7-8 years) and 2 (11-12 years) viewed the same 20minute segment in which various foods and drinks were digitally inserted into the program. In the first sitting, Groups 1a and 2a viewed a 20-minute segment in which various “healthier” choices (Milk 2 Go, Black Diamond cheese strings, Yoplait tubes, and Dole Diced Peaches) were digitally inserted. In separate sittings, Groups 1 and 2 viewed the same 20-minute segment. However, in this segment, different “unhealthy” products were digitally inserted. Digital inserts of Pepsi, Betty Crocker’s Fruit Gushers, Reese’s Pieces and Frito-Lay Cheetos were strategically “placed” throughout. Finally, Groups 1b and 2b, the control groups, viewed the same 20-minute segment but with no product placements. Unaided recall was measured by asking children after exposure if 3 they could recall any brands, sponsors or advertising messages in the program. Aided recall was measured by asking children if they recognized any brands shown to them (a list was provided). Although not strictly subliminal, product placements may not be consciously encoded even by those who are cognitively capable of doing so (Auty and Lewis, 2004). This makes it important to measure the influence of product placement on behavior independent of recall measures. All of the children were therefore offered a selection of beverages (small cans of Pepsi and Coke, water, and bottles of Milk 2 Go) and a selection of snacks (cheese strings, Yoplait tubes, Dole Peaches, Fruit Gushers, Reese’s Pieces, Cheetos, Smarties and a granola bar). Children then completed a questionnaire with the help of the researchers asking specific questions about their recollection, observation and impressions of the show. In total 225 children from two schools took part in the experiment – 163 Grade 7 students (aged 10-12) and 62 students from Grades 3 and 4 (aged 7-9). The gender split was fairly even, with 107 girls and 118 boys. Fifty-one percent of the sample viewed the segment with unhealthy product placements, 32% saw the show with healthy products inserted, and the remainder were the control group, watching a segment with no placements at all. Data is currently being analyzed, but initial results show that the children exposed to product placements clearly recalled the products placed in the show. For example, of the children who saw the unhealthy segment, 90% remembered seeing Pepsi, 70% saw the Fruit Gushers, 75% saw the Cheetos, and 67% recalled seeing Reese’s Pieces. Of those that saw the healthy segment, memory of placements was high for the drink placement (90% for Milk 2 Go), but lower for the snacks (26% remembered Dole Fruit Cups, 27% Cheese Strings and 38% Yoplait tubes). Further analysis revealed that a significantly higher percentage of older children recalled the product placements. The choice of snacks and drinks the children selected after viewing the program suggests that product placement had very little influence on immediate behavior for respondents as a whole. The majority of children chose Pepsi or Coke along with a packet of Fruit Gushers, regardless of the segment they watched. The only placement that possibly had an influence on behavior was Cheetos, as twice as many children who had been exposed to Cheetos chose that snack. The study has significant implications for public policy officials. The limited available research suggests that product placements are especially potent in their effects upon children and adolescents and the results of this study support that contention, even though placement had very little influence on immediate behaviour. While this study does not suggest a direct ‘media effect’, it draws attention to the need for considering both the ethical dimensions of this type of marketing and the ways that product placement might impact preferences over time. Public health field officials who have the health of children a priority may also be concerned that even though children were exposed to healthier product placements and given healthy snack options, the majority chose foods of poor nutritional quality. 4 Scientific Report Product placement Product placement has been defined as the planned entry of products into movies or television shows that may influence viewers’ product beliefs and/or behaviors favorably (Balasubramanian, 1994). The practice has emerged as one of the fastest growing segments of the advertising market (Elliott, 2005; Hudson and Hudson, 2006) and it is predicted that up to 75 percent of all scripted, prime-time network shows in the US will soon star products or services paid for by advertisers (George, 2005). Global paid product placement was estimated to be worth $4.38 billion in 2007—a growth of 30 percent from 2006—and product placement in films, TV shows, video games and even song lyrics is set to triple by 2010 (PQ Media, 2006). In the last few decades product placement has also become very sophisticated. Products no longer merely appear; they are placed or embedded, woven into storylines of shows, films, songs, novels and games, making a stronger emotional connection with the consumer than traditional product placement. In the past, advertisers sought to place products in shows as soon as they became hits. Now, advertising deals are happening alongside the creative development (Hudson and Hudson, 2006; Duffy, 2005). Product placement is now commonplace in reality shows and is seeping into scripted shows, where products are woven in during series development (Romano, 2004). Companies like Walmart and Procter & Gamble are even creating their own television programs in order to have more control of the placement of their products (Vranica and Brown, 2010). Alongside this growth and increasing sophistication have been rising concerns about the ethics of product placement. Research has confirmed that consumers are concerned about the ‘subliminal’ effect of product placement (Tiwsakul, Hackley and Szmigin, 2005). Others fear that product placement’s influence on the content of movies and television will seep into news magazines, where editorial content is seen by many as inviolate. Critics also claim that the trend of embedding products into songs is an invasion of music lovers’ privacy, and critics have articulated concerns over loss of artistic freedom resulting from the increased use of brands in video games (Neslon, 2002). Concern has been cited for product placements of ethically charged products (guns, alcohol and tobacco for example), while others question the appropriateness of allowing brands to be associated with video games that exhibit excessive violence (Gupta and Lord; Gibson and Maurer, 2000). Particularly contentious is the use of product placement in children’s programming (Hudson, Hudson and Peloza, 2008; Avery and Ferraro, 2000). Such tactics are becoming more and more common. Examples of products aimed at children that are placed in films include Zero Skateboards in A Cinderella Story, Etch-a-Sketch in Elf, Beanie Babies in Agent Cody Banks 2, and Sony products in Freaky Friday. Recently, PepsiCo put $3 million into the soccer movie Gracie, which is why the film opens with a shot of PepsiCos sports drink, Gatorade, sitting on the hood of a car. Consequently, there have been calls for more research that focuses on the influence of product placement on 5 children (American Psychological Association, 2004; Moore, 2004; Tiwsakul et al. 2005). This is because children have not yet developed sensitivity to this type of promotional tool (Avery and Ferraro, 2000) and yet its practice is growing. Furthermore, some researchers have suggested that advertising geared to children – which largely consists of ads for sugary cereals, candy and fast-food restaurants – may be contributing to the increase in childhood obesity by promoting unhealthy foods (Halford et al., 2007; Datamonitor, 2002; Story and French, 2004; Jeffery, 2008). In the past 35 years, the percentage of obese children in Canada has more than doubled, swelling to over 500,000 children according to Statistics Canada (Deveau, 2006). According to the latest Standing Committee on Health Report, titled Healthy Weights for Healthy Kids, over 26 per cent of Canadian children are currently overweight or obese (House of Commons Canada, 2007). The evolution of product placement The origins of product placement can be found in the 1930s, when US tobacco companies paid movie stars and sporting heroes to endorse their brands. In the 50s in the first days of television, Bing Crosby would open his radio show by literally singing the praises of Chesterfield cigarettes; TV shows had names like Texaco Star Theater and The Colgate Comedy Hour. A second ‘wave’ of product placement emerged in 1982, when Reese’s Pieces were used to lure a lumbering little alien out of hiding in the film E.T. This placement proved profitable for the candy’s manufacturers. Hershey saw a 65 percent rise in sales following the film’s release. Ever since, the placement of products in movies and television has become an important element of consumer marketing programs. Spurred by the diminishing effectiveness of television advertising and other traditional techniques, product placement has seen considerable growth in the last 10 years (Kaikati and Kaikati, 2004). For advertisers, it is one way of combating unprecedented audience fragmentation and new technologies that allow people to zap through commercials. Consumers are increasingly refusing to abide commercial interruptions and demanding a choice in the timing, placement and quantity of advertising they are exposed to. Studies show that people with Personal Video Recorders (PVRs) like TiVo use them to skip 92 percent of the ads (George, 2005). However, these same people recall strategic product placements four times as much as paid commercial messages, suggesting that product placement can be far more effective than 30-second TV commercials. Another reason for the development of product placement is the growth of the entertainment industry. Traditionally advertising was placed adjacent to entertainment in order to capitalize on the audiences the entertainment attracted. However, over the last decade, the entertainment industry has proliferated and entertainment is now distributed and consumed online, by mobile, on television or in cinemas. These changes have opened the door to integrated advertising. Marketers realize that communications via branded entertainment can be more sophisticated, more targeted and more widely seen than traditional advertising methods. Ethical attitudes towards product placement 6 Product placement’s ‘evolution’ has not been without hitches. From the late 1980s to the early 1990s, the movie industry encountered much controversy as consumer advocate groups voiced concerns that product placement was a deceptive practice and not in the best interest of the public. In 1991, in response to the dispute, the product placement industry formed the Entertainment Resources and Marketing Association (ERMA) as a way to self-regulate the industry and to pre-empt any government regulation. Interestingly, previous research indicates that consumers have a positive view towards product placement (Nebanzhal and Secunda, 1993), and it seems to increase brand loyalty by validating the purchase decisions of the consumer (Hart, 2003). However, as previously mentioned, there are ethical concerns over its use as a marketing tool—and the main concern centers on the issue of deception. Product placements are not labeled as advertisements and therefore may be viewed as “hidden but paid” messages (Balasubramanian, 1994). Research confirms that consumers are generally concerned about the ‘subliminal’ effect of product placement (Tiwsakul et al., 2005). Commercial Alert, an Oregon advocacy group, calls embedded advertising “an affront to basic honesty,” and petitioned the US Federal Trade Commission (unsuccessfully) to regulate product placement, suggesting that shows be required to insert pop-up notifications to alert viewers to prearranged product placements. The issue of subliminal marketing is extremely controversial. The idea of advertising which affects people below their level of conscious awareness, so that they are not able to exercise conscious control over their acceptance or rejection of the message creates ethical issues for both marketers and consumers (Morton and Friedman, 2002). New technology also fuels ethical dilemmas by blurring the line between commercial messages and entertainment. Products are now being added to scenes in order to target a particular market. Sony Pictures, for example, re-edited an existing series episode of The King of Queens to add a product placement, incorporating a plug for Dr. Scholl’s Massaging Gel Insoles (Lafayette, 2004). Critics believe this takes advantage of unsuspecting viewers because they might not realize that the products were not originally part of the scene during filming, but were added to target a specific viewing audience Morton and Friedman, 2002). Particularly troubling is the use of product placement in children’s programming (Avery and Ferraro, 2000). Such tactics are becoming increasingly common; consequently there have been calls for more research that focuses on the influence of product placement on children Tiwsakul et al., 2005; Bachman, 2008). The influence of product placement on children “Given the significant role played by media in the lives of the nation’s children, it is time to move forward with new academic research initiatives in this realm” (American Psychological Association, 2004) Children worldwide are exposed to an ever-growing number of advertising messages. Children in North America spend more time watching television than any other activity 7 in their lives, except sleeping (Walsh and Gentile, 2002). Advertisers are increasingly targeting children and youth for three core reasons. First is the potential exposure - the typical North American child takes in some 38 hours of commercial media every week and sees between 20 and 30 ads per hour (Laczniak and Palan, 2004). The second reason is economic. Children represent an important consumer segment with an estimated $80 billion in aggregate income and the ability to influence an additional $225 billion in spending by their parents (Berg, 2008). Finally, there is the race to establish brand loyalty (Walsh and Gentile, 2002). Companies realize that brand loyalty established at an early age may reap significant economic rewards over a child’s lifetime (McNeal, 1987). Growth in marketing efforts directed towards the young has been accompanied by an upsurge in the use of psychological knowledge and research to more effectively market products to children. Specialist conferences now advise industry players on how to develop sections of the children’s market, and how to manipulate the child’s influence over parents – known in marketing circles as either ‘pester power’ or the ‘nag factor’. This, in turn, has prompted concerns over the ethics of such research. Children are viewed as a special market segment because of their lack of experience and stilldeveloping cognitive skills. With some exceptions, the primary goals of marketing and advertising to children exclude notions of child welfare. For example, the tobacco industry has, historically, employed the techniques of persuasion to influence children to adopt an addictive, and dangerous, habit. Food marketers, too, are routinely chastised for promoting foods of poor nutritional value to children. Manufacturers know that children are particularly susceptible to advertising’s persuasion. The Supreme Court of Canada recognized this in 1979, when it held that the Quebec Consumer Protection Act’s protection of children under the age of 13 did not unduly limit constitutionally protected free expression. The Court stated that “advertising directed at young children is per se manipulative. Such advertising aims to promote products by convincing those who will always believe” (Consumer Protection Act, 1989) As a consequence, the province of Quebec has, since 1980, banned all television advertising to children under the age of 13 on the grounds that such advertising is inherently deceptive. Advocacy groups such as the Canadian arm of the Center for Science in the Public Interest (among others) continue to lobby to extend it to the rest of Canada. In March 2007, Bill C-414—carrying the subhead “child protection against advertising exploitation”—had its first reading in the House of Commons (Bill C-414, 2007). A fundamental criticism of advertising to children revolves around the issue of fairness (Treise et al., 1994) and the fact that children are less able to evaluate commercial persuasion (Kumkel, 1988; Jeffery, 2006). Numerous studies have documented that children under eight years of age especially are developmentally unable to understand the intent of advertisements and accept advertising claims as factual (American Academy of Pediatrics, 1995). Even after that age, youngsters may recognize that commercials intend to sell, but not necessarily understand that they are biased messages which warrant some degree of skepticism (American Psychological Association, 2004; Jeffery, 2006). 8 Thomas Barry (1980) was one of the first to focus on deception in children’s advertising, using theory from psychology to support his contention that children are simply more susceptible to deceptive messages. He referred to the work of Piaget (1970), who claimed that children do not have the formal operational skills to logically test principles. They are therefore more susceptible to deceptive messages from all sources. Moore comments that to evaluate advertising, children must be able to distinguish between commercial and non-commercial content (Moore, 2004). However, with the advent of product placements, she says discriminating between an advertisement and entertainment may be a much more difficult task for a child. Work that has focused on children’s responses to ads has found that the appearance of a character with a product favorably affects children’s evaluations of a product. Further, advertising directed at children can lead to conflict between children and parents. Laczniak and Palan (2004) investigated children’s responses to ads and their influence on parents. Somewhat surprisingly, they found that older children (ages 8-9) may be more at risk from the negative effects of television advertising messages than those under eight. One study found that 40 percent of children aged 8 to 18 say they try to buy products they have seen on TV or in the movies, with those aged eight to 12 most likely to agree (53% of them) (Harris, 2006). A recent report from the American Psychological Association (2004) recommended a restriction on all advertising primarily directed to audiences of children below the age of 7-8 years. This was based mainly on the long-standing principle in communication law that for advertising to be considered fair, it must be readily identifiable as such to its intended audience. As far as the task force is concerned, compelling evidence suggests that children do not have the defense mechanisms to identify the source of an advertising message, making it unfair and deceptive to advertise to them. They also suggest that it is equally unfair to use empirical research to identify the most effective strategies to persuade child viewers, at least in relation to product marketing. Although the advertising industry acknowledges the potentially harmful effects of advertising on child audiences, it generally balks at calls for it to take responsibility for such unintentional effects (Haefner, 1991). Product placement practitioners argue that the placement of brands in children’s programming is very limited (and certainly there is less product placement in television than film) (Beck, 2001). However, they know that children watch programs intended for other target audiences and are thus exposed to advertising intended for those audiences. It has been argued that unintended exposure to these advertisements may have powerful extended effects on child viewers’ role expectations, values and world views (Haefner, 1991). Under a new Canadian Children’s Food & Beverage Advertising Initiative, 15 Canadian companies have agreed to devote at least 50 percent of their television, radio, print and Internet advertising directed primarily to children under 12 years of age to further the goal of promoting healthy dietary choices and/or healthy active living. The social marketing initiative was developed by Concerned Children’s Advertisers (CAA), a nonprofit organization of 22 member companies, supported by over 40 partner companies 9 and governments, issue experts and NGOs, that work together to contribute positively to the media and life issues that effect children. Canada’s Broadcast Code for Advertising to Children also recently introduced new guidelines for food advertising to children, to help ensure that “advertising to children in all media encourages responsible product use and that the amount of food shown being consumed in an advertisement does not exceed an appropriate single serving size” (see Elliott, 2008). Unlike Canada’s move toward industry self-regulation, in the UK, junk food adverts during TV shows aimed at under16s have been banned. The crackdown affects television commercials for all food and drink products high in fat, salt and sugar (HFSS). Some researchers have suggested that advertising geared to children – which largely consists of ads for sugary cereals, candy and fast-food restaurants – may contribute to the increase in childhood obesity by promoting unhealthy foods (Halford et al., 2007; Datamonitor, 2002; Story and French, 2004; Jeffery, 2008). Others have documented how child-oriented food marketing reorients children’s perception of what ‘healthy’ food entails (Elliott, 2009). In the past 35 years, the percentage of obese children in Canada has more than doubled, swelling to over 500,000 children according to Statistics Canada (Deveau, 2006). According to the latest Standing Committee on Health Report, Healthy Weights for Healthy Kids, over 26 percent of Canadian children are currently overweight or obese (House of Commons Canada, 2007). A fundamental criticism of advertising to children revolves around the issue of fairness (Treise et al., 1994) and the fact that children are less able to evaluate commercial persuasion (Kunkel, 1988; Jeffery, 2006). Numerous studies have documented that children under age eight are cognitively unable to understand the intent of advertisements and accept advertising claims as factual (AAP, 1995; APA, 2004; Jeffery, 2006). Objectives The general objective of this study is to fill a research gap in the literature when it comes to understanding the impact of product placement on children. Specific objectives are: to analyze the placement of food and beverage products on children of various ages; to measure and assess the cognitive and behavioural response to these food/ beverage product placements; and to provide relevant policy recommendations pertaining to product placement in terms of children’s preferences and awareness of advertising intent in relation to age. Method How to “measure” product placement has been the subject of much discussion, as many marketers want to know the return on investment for money spent on placements. Just like advertising, the effectiveness of product placement as a communications strategy must be gauged against the specific objectives of decision-makers (d’Astous and Chartier, 2000). Certain academics suggest that message impact should be assessed at recall, persuasion, and behavioral levels (Balasubramanian, 1994). Amongst product placement practitioners, unaided recall and brand recognition are the two most popular means of assessing placements, although the tracking of subsequent related sales or the measurement of trade or general press coverage are methods growing in use (Karrh, McKee and Pardun, 2003). 10 This study employed experimental methods, as they have been used successfully in previous product placement research (Russell, 2002; McKechnie and Zhou, 2003; Gupta and Lord, 1998). Based on Auty and Lewis (2004), children of different ages were exposed to a 20-minute section of a popular television show that typically contains a number of product placements. Child-respondents were divided into groups and each group saw the same television clip but with different (digitally inserted) placements of various types of food and beverages. All groups were shown a clip from Pop Idol – the British equivalent of American Idol. Pop Idol was selected because the children are familiar with the format of the show (in light of the popularity and audience base of American Idol), but do not know the exact players or have preconceived expectations about the “proper” product placement (i.e., the ubiquitous and prominent Coke promotions in American Idol, etc.). American Idol has millions of child viewers, and in 2008 branded content in the show jumped 19 percent. The first 28 episodes of 2008 included 4,151 product placements (Sullivan, 2008). In the experimental design, Groups 1 (7-8 years) and 2 (11-12 years) viewed the same 20minute segment in which various foods and drinks were digitally inserted into the program. In the first sitting, Groups 1a and 2a viewed a 20-minute segment in which various “healthier” choices (Milk 2 Go, Black Diamond cheese strings, Yoplait tubes, and Dole Diced Peaches) were digitally inserted. In separate sittings, Groups 1 and 2 viewed the same 20-minute segment. However, in this segment, different “unhealthy” products were digitally inserted. Digital inserts of Pepsi, Betty Crocker’s Fruit Gushers, Reese’s Pieces and Frito-Lay Cheetos were strategically “placed” throughout. Finally, Groups 1b and 2b, the control groups, viewed the same 20-minute segment but with no product placements. Unaided recall was measured by asking children after exposure if they could recall any brands, sponsors or advertising messages in the program. Aided recall was measured by asking children if they recognized any brands shown to them (a list was provided). The methodological design allows the children to select snacks from the exact same range of products. However, Group 1 and 2 viewed a television sequence with healthier “branded” products (milk, cheese, yogurt, fruit slices) digitally inserted, whereas Group 1a and 2a viewed the same television sequence—but with nutritionally poor selections (Pepsi, Fruit Gushers, Cheetos, Reese Pieces) digitally inserted. These different branded product placements allowed the study to reveal the degree to which children of varying ages both recognized and recalled product placements. It also provided insight to the degree to which such placements impacted children’s immediate snack choices (for instance, children from Group 1 and 2 might be more likely to select milk and Yoplait as a snack, while children from Group 1a and 2a might opt for Pepsi and Cheetos.) The inclusion of Coke—a popular, competing cola product—as one of the snack selections ensured that the study was not simply measuring children’s preference for sugary soda over water or juice. Similarly, the study design to digitally insert include three “healthy” options (for Groups 1 and 2) and three “junk” options (for Groups 1a and 2a) serves not merely to reveal the impact of product placement on healthy and less healthy choices. The three options allows the researchers to measure the degree to which product 11 placement recall actually impacts children’s immediate snack choices. For instance, if a child can only recall a Reese’s Pieces product placement and then selects it as a snack, then this, too, is particularly revealing. The option of selecting healthier choices for Groups 1a and 2a and “junk” options for Groups 1 and 2 (i.e., the opposite snacks of what they viewed in the television sequence) also works to sidestep the possibility of confounding (due to too narrow of a selection of snack choices). Although not strictly subliminal, product placements may not be consciously encoded even by those who are cognitively capable of doing so (Auty and Lewis, 2004). This makes it important to measure the influence of product placement on behavior independent of recall measures. All of the children were therefore offered a selection of beverages (small cans of Pepsi and Coke, water, and bottles of Milk 2 Go) and a selection of snacks (cheese strings, Yoplait tubes, Dole Peaches, Fruit Gushers, Reese’s Pieces, Cheetos, Smarties and a granola bar). Children then completed a questionnaire with the help of the researchers asking specific questions about their recollection, observation and impressions of the show. Multivariate analysis (logistic regressions) will be used to analyze the data. It is a more advanced statistical method, where one can ‘predict’ the outcome of a dependent variable - in this case what drink and snack did the child choose - with not one, but many independent variables. In this case, the independent variables would include the demographics (these are standard in regression analysis), and a whole range of questions about the snack: whether they have it regularly, how often, whether they think it is a treat, whether they like the packaging, their attitudes towards the particular snack, attitudes towards the show, attitudes towards the T.V., and, of course, whether or not they remember seeing the drink/snack during the show. The advantage of applying regression analysis here is that we will be able to see what independent variables have a significant impact on the dependent variable, as well as the ‘strength’ of that impact while accounting for, or isolating the effects of the other independent variables (i.e., holding them constant). Thus, if our research hypothesis is that remembering seeing the snack/drink during the show will impact the immediate choice of that snack/drink, we should witness the ‘recalling seeing the snack/drink’ having a strong and significant impact on ‘immediate choice of snack/drink’. It is necessary to do this type of analysis because a simple cross tabulation or a correlation between ‘recalling seeing the snack/drink’ and ‘immediate choice of snack/drink’ only tells us part of the story; for example, a significant relationship between recalling seeing coke and choosing it might be explained by a third factor of liking the package or thinking that the drink is yummy, which we will not be able to examine in a crosstab or correlation. Regression analysis will give us those answers. Results In total 225 children from two schools took part in the experiment – 163 Grade 7 students (aged 10-12) and 62 students from Grades 3 and 4 (aged 7-9). The gender split was fairly even, with 107 girls and 118 boys. Fifty-one percent of the sample viewed the segment with unhealthy product placements, 32 percent saw the show with healthy products inserted, and the remainder were the control group, watching a segment with no 12 placements at all. Data is currently being analyzed, but Table 1 indicates that the children exposed to product placements clearly recalled the products placed in the show. For example, of the children who saw the unhealthy segment, 90 percent remembered seeing Pepsi, 70 percent saw the Fruit Gushers, 75 percent saw the Cheetos, and 67 percent recalled seeing Reese’s Pieces. Of those that saw the healthy segment, memory of placements was high for the drink placement (90% for Milk 2 Go), but lower for the snacks (26% remembered Dole Fruit Cups, 27% Cheese Strings and 38% Yoplait tubes). Further analysis revealed that a significantly higher percentage of older children recalled the product placements. On average, 96 percent of them recalled placement of the drinks, 74 percent remembered the unhealthy snacks and 31 percent the healthier snacks. For the younger children, memory was weaker with 72 percent recalling placement of the drinks, 58 percent remembering the unhealthy snacks and 29 percent the healthier snacks. Table 1 – Product Placement Recall Group NPP/No Product Placement Did you see this product during the show? Milk 2 Go Coca Cola Water Pepsi Gushers Granola bar Cheetos Dole Reese's Smarties Cheese Strings Yoplait tubes Yes (#) % (2) 5.1 (3) 7.9 (7) 18.9 (3) 7.9 (1) 2.6 -(1) 2.6 (2) 5.3 (2) 5.1 (2) 5.6 --- UPP/Unhealthy Product Placement No Yes (#) % (#) % (37) 94.9 (26) 23.6 (35) 92.1 (49) 45.4 (30) 81.1 (19) 17.3 (35) 92.1 (102) 90.3 (37) 97.4 (75) 69.4 (38) 100.0 (14) 12.4 (37) 97.4 (83) 74.8 (36) 94.7 (7) 6.3 (37) 94.9 (76) 67.3 (34) 94.4 (32) 28.6 (38) 100.0 (20) 18.3 (38) 100.0 (11) 10.2 HPP/Healthy Product Placement No Yes No (#) % (84) 76.4 (59) 54.6 (91) 82.7 (11) 9.7 (33) 30.6 (99) 87.6 (28) 25.2 (104) 93.7 (37) 32.7 (80) 71.4 (89) 81.7 97 (89.8) (#) % (62) 89.9 (8) 11.6 (18) 26.1 (7) 10.9 (11) 16.7 (7) 10.3 (18) 25.4 (17) 26.3 (3) 4.3 (4) 5.7 (18) 27.3 (25) 37.9 (#) % (7) 10.1 (61) 88.4 (51) 73.9 (57) 89.1 (55) 83.3 (61) 89.7 (53) 74.6 (48) 73.8 (67) 95.7 (66) 94.3 (48) 72.7 (41) 62.1 The choice of snacks and drinks the children selected after viewing the program suggests that product placement had very little influence on immediate behavior for respondents as a whole (see Table 2). Table 2 – Choice of Snacks and Drinks Group Fruit Gushers Reese's Pieces Smarties Yoplait tubes NPP/No Product Placement UPP/Unhealthy Product Placement (17) 44.7 (1) 2.6 (2) 5.3 (3) 7.9 (63) 56.2 (10) 8.9 (7) 6.2 (3) 2.7 HPP/Healthy Product Placement (48) 69.6 (2) 2.9 (3) 4.3 (2) 2.9 13 Dole Peaches Cheese Strings Cheetos Granola Bar (1) 2.6 (3) 7.9 (11) 28.2 -- (5) 4.5 (23) 20.5 (23) 20.5 (1) 0.9 (3) 4.3 (2) 2.9 (8) 11.6 (1) 1.4 Coca Cola Pure Life Water Pepsi Milk 2 Go (8) 20.5 (11) 28.2 (13) 33.3 (7) 17.9 (38) 33.3 (13) 11.4 (37) 32.5 (26) 22.8 (28) 40.0 (7) 10.0 (23) 32.9 (12) 17.1 The majority of children chose Pepsi or Coke along with a packet of Fruit Gushers, regardless of the segment they watched. The only placement that possibly had an influence on behavior was Cheetos, as twice as many children who had been exposed to Cheetos chose that snack. Further breakdown of results between the two age groups showed a few significant differences. Of the younger children, 64 percent of those exposed to Fruit Gushers chose that snack as opposed to only 46 percent who had not seen the snack in the show. Further, 33 percent of that age group who had viewed the unhealthy segment chose Pepsi as their drink compared to only 17 percent who saw the healthy segment. Children who viewed the healthy segment were more likely to choose Milk 2 Go, 2% white milk. Of the older groups who had viewed the unhealthy products, 12% chose Reese’s Pieces compared to only 4 percent who had not been exposed to the product. Similarly 21 percent of the older children viewing Cheetos chose that snack whereas only 9 percent of viewers of the healthier segment chose that snack. Implications Initial findings from the study have significant implications for both advertisers and public policy officials. Advertisers will be interested to see that recall of product placement was strong, and was highest amongst older children and highest for the unhealthy snacks. The limited available research suggests that product placements are especially potent in their effects upon children and adolescents and the results of this study support that contention. However, contrary to Auty and Lewis’s findings, placement had very little influence on immediate behaviour. There are several possible explanations for this. Children could have viewed the post-television segment snack selection as a “treating” opportunity, choosing products that they rarely get at home. Further, while the children were invited over to the snack table one at a time to select their beverage and food product (so that the choices were individual choices), it remains possible that the snack choices of the first children ‘invited’ to the table influenced the choices of subsequent children. The results of the study may have important implications for policy makers. In Canada, policies related to product placement are currently quite vague, although the Broadcast Code for Advertising to Children states that “no children’s advertising may employ any device or technique that attempts to transmit messages below the threshold of normal awareness” (Advertising Standards Canada, 2004). While this study does not suggest a direct “media effect,” it draws attention to the need for considering both the ethical dimensions of this type of marketing and the ways that product placement might impact preferences over time. Public health field officials who have the health of children a priority may be concerned that even though children were exposed to healthier product 14 placements and given healthy snack options, the majority chose foods of poor nutritional quality. Results will be disseminated through University and local media channels and will also be presented at the following conferences: Association of Marketing Theory and Practice Conference USA, March 2010; Academy of Marketing Conference UK, July, 2010 International Child and Teen Consumption (CTC) conference Linköping, Sweden, June 2010 References Advertising Standards Canada (2004), “The Broadcast Code for Advertising to Children”, The Canadian Association of Broadcasters, April. American Academy of Pediatrics (1995), “Children, adolescents, and advertising: Committee on Communications, American Academy of Pediatrics”, Pediatrics, 95, 295297. American Psychological Association (2004), Report of the APA Task Force on Advertising and Children. d’Astous, A and Chartier, F (2000), “A study of factors affecting consumer evaluations and memory of product placements in movies”, Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 22 (2), 31-40. Auty, S and Lewis, C (2004), “Exploring children’s choice: the reminder effect of product placement”, Psychology & Marketing, 21 (9), 697. Avery, R J and Ferraro, R (2000), “Verisimilitude or advertising? brand appearances on prime-time television,” Journal of Consumer Affairs, 34 (2), 217-245. Bachman, K (2008, June 27), “FCC to scrutinize TV, radio product placement”, Mediaweek, available at: http://www.mediaweek.com/mw/content_display/news/mediaagencies-research/e3if0790e8b4c2f2907182c5ffc5ac52d0d Balasubramanian, S K (1994), “Beyond advertising and publicity: hybrid messages and public policy issues”, Journal of Advertising, 23 (4), 29-47. Barry, T E (1980), “A Framework for Ascertaining Deception in Children’s Advertising,” Journal of Advertising, 9 (1), 11-18. Beck, J (2001, March 1), “Product Placement Gaining Promo Weight,” Kidscreen Magazine, 13. C. Elliott (2008). “Taste rules!: Food marketing, food law and childhood obesity in Canada,” Cuizine: The Journal of Canadian Food Cultures, 1(1). 15 Berg, C (2008), “Kids and consumerism”, Western Parent, 13 (4), 5. Bill C-414. “An Act to amend the Competition Act and the Food and Drugs Act (child protection against advertising exploitation)” http://www2.parl.gc.ca/HousePublications/Publication.aspx?Docid=2786680&file=4 Consumer Protection Act. (1989) 40.1. Datamonitor. (2002, September 30). “Childhood obesity: how obesity is shaping the US food and beverage markets”, Market Research Report, York: Datamonitor. DeBerry-Spence, B (2008), “Consumer creations of product meaning in the context of African-style clothing”, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 36 (3), 395-408. Deveau, S (2006, June 2), “Mars drops ads aimed at very young”, National Post, FP2. Duffy, J (2005, March 30), “Well placed: Rappers are being invited by McDonald's to drop the words "Big Mac" into songs, while some British TV looks set to follow cinema by allowing brands to feature in shows,” BBC News Magazine. Available at: http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/uk_news/magazine/4391955.stm Elliott, C (2009), “‘Healthy food looks serious’: How children interpret packaged food products”, Canadian Journal of Communication, 34 (3), 359-380. Elliott, S (2005, March 29), “More products get roles in shows, and marketers wonder if they’re getting their money’s worth”, The New York Times, C3. George, L (2005), “Is Kiefer Sutherland trying to sell you something?”, Macleans, 118 (8), 30-36. Gibson, B and Maurer, J (2000), “Cigarette Smoking in Movies: The Influence of Product Placement on Attitudes Toward Smoking and Smokers,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1457-1473. Gupta, P B, and Lord, K R (1998), “Product placement in movies: the effect of prominence and mode on audience recall”, Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 20, 47-59. Haefner, M J (1991), “Ethical Problems of Advertising to Children,” Journal of Mass Media Ethics, 6 (2), 83-92. Halford, J C G, Boyland, E J, Highes, G, Oliveira, L P, Dovey, T M (2007), “Beyondbrand effect of television (TV) food advertisements/commercials on caloric intake and food choice of 5-7-year-old children”, Appetite, 49, 263-267. Harris, M (2006, August 25), Marketers, friends have ear of youth, National Post, FP7. 16 Hart, P J (2003), “Product Placement for Dummies,” Marketing Magazine, 108 (17), 17. House of Commons Canada (2007), “Healthy weights for healthy kids”, Report of the Standing Committee on Health. Rob Merrifield, MP (Chair). Ottawa. Available at: http://cmte.parl.gc.ca/cmte/CommitteePublication.aspx?SourceId=199309 Hudson, S and Hudson, D (2006), “Branded entertainment: a new advertising technique, or product placement in disguise?”, Journal of Marketing Management, 22 (5-6), 489504. Hudson, S, Hudson, D and Peloza, J (2008), “Meet the parents: a parents’ perspective on product placement in children’s programming”, Journal of Business Ethics, 80 (2), 289304. Hyman, M R, Tansey, R and Clark J W (1994), “Research on advertising ethics: past, present and future”, Journal of Advertising, 23 (3), 5-15. Jeffery, B (2006), “The Supreme Court of Canada’s appraisal of the 1980 ban on advertising to children in Québec: implications for “misleading” advertising elsewhere”, Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 39 (1), 321–360. Jeffery, B (2008), “Exhorting Gen-XS to XL, cheating at child’s play: their health, our laws”, in J. Greenberg and C. Elliott (eds.), Communication in Question: Competing Perspectives on Controversial Issues in Communication Studies. Thomson-Nelson. Kaikati, A M and Kaikati, J G (2004), “Stealth Marketing: How to Reach Consumers Surreptitiously,” California Management Review, 46 (4), 6-22. Karrh, J A, McKee, K B, and Pardun, C J (2003), “Practitioners’ evolving views on product placement effectiveness”, Journal of Advertising Research, 43 (2), 138-149. Kunkel, D (1988), “The evolution of children’s television regulatory policy”, Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 31(Fall), 367-389. Laczniak, R N, and Palan, K M (2004), “Under the influence”, Marketing Research, (Spring), 35-39. Lafayette, J (2004), “Reopening the Door to Product Placement,” TelevisionWeek, 23 (37), 2. McKechnie, S A, and Zhou, J (2003), “Product placement in movies: a comparison of Chinese and American consumers’ attitudes”, International Journal of Advertising, 22, 349-74. 17 McNeal, J J (1987), Children as Consumers: Insights and Implications, Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Moore, E S (2004), “Children and the changing world of advertising”, Journal of Business Ethics, 52 (2), 161-167. Morton, C R and Friedman, M (2002), “I Saw it in The Movies: Exploring the Link Between Product Placement Beliefs and Reported Usage Behaviour,” Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 24 (2), 33-40. Nebenzahl, I D and Secunda, E (1993), “Consumers’ Attitudes Toward Product Placement in Movies,” International Journal of Advertising, 12 (1), 1-11. Nelson, M R (2002), “Recall of Brand Placements in Computer/Video Games,” Journal of Advertising Research, 42 (2), 80-92. Piaget, J (1970), “The Stages of the Intellectual Development of the Child,” in P.H. Mussen (ed.), Readings in Child Development and Psychology. New York: Harper and Row. PQ Media Global Product Placement Forecast Series 2006-2010: Country-by-Country Analysis, Stamford, CT: PQ Media. Romano, A (2004), “The New Script for Product Placement,” Broadcasting & Cable, 134 (4), 13. Russell, C A (2002), “Investigating the effectiveness of product placement in television shows: the role of modality and plot connection congruence on brand memory and attitude”, Journal of Consumer Research, 29 (December), 306-318. Story, M and French, S (2004), “Food Advertising and marketing directed at children and adolescents in the US”, International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 1 (3), 1-17. Sullivan, L (2008, June 20), “FCC is urged to clamp down on product placement”, Marketing Daily. Tiwsakul, R, Hackley, C and Szmigin, I (2005), “Explicit, non-integrated product placement in British television programmes”, International Journal of Advertising, 24 (1), 95-111. Treise, D, Weigold, M F, Conna, J and Garrison H (1994), “Ethics in advertising: ideological correlates of consumer perceptions”, Journal of Advertising, 23 (3), 695-743. Vranica, S., and Brown, E. (2010, February 11), “Giants Ally to Promote Familty TV”, Wall Street Journal, B5. 18 Walsh, D, and Gentile D A (2002), “Slipping under the radar: advertising and the mind”, in L. Riley and I. Obot (eds.), Drinking It In: Alcohol Marketing and Young People, Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. 19