Non-Word Repetition 37-924-01 Theoretical Approaches to Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University What is a Non-Word Repetition task? In a non-word repetition task the child in asked to repeat pseudo words and/or nonwords of 1-5 syllables, with or without a cluster at the onset, middle or coda. Pseudo word: target-like Non-word: not target-like GAPS - Grammar and Phonology Screening Test Gardner, H., Froud, K., McClelland, A. & van der Lely, H. (2006) The development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language difficulties in young children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders dep, pif, dremp, klest, tobilf, difimp, bademper, fakester, padrepper, difrimple What contributes to the complexity of these words? Are they English like? Could they be used as is in a Hebrew test? Which considerations should apply? The processing involved in NWR NW >> Auditory processing >> Phonological Analysis >> Phonological storage (and learning for real words) >> Speech motor planning >> Output These can be influenced by: Hearing loss Phonotactic frequency Language dominance Stimulus length STM deficit Complexity Articulatory deficit What is NWR testing then? Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208. ANNA 13/12 Phonological working memory (phonological loop) “Working memory (WM) according to Baddeley (1986) is a multicomponent, capacity-limited system that comprises a controlling ‘‘central executive’’ and that includes an articulatory loop system. The central executive ... is thought to regulate information flow within WM, retrieval of information from other memory systems, and the processing and storage of information. The articulatory loop … includes a capacity-limited phonological short-term store and an articulatory control process … that acts to refresh and maintain speech material in the store for a brief period. The articulatory loop’s function is to store verbal input temporarily, especially novel phonological input (Baddeley, Gathercole, & Papagno, 1998), while other cognitive tasks such as auditory comprehension take place. The ability to temporarily store novel material also allows the listener the opportunity to create long-term phonological representations of that material (Baddeley et al1998).” (Montgomery 2003, p. 222) Digit Span Gray, S. 2003. Diagnostic accuracy and test-retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication disorders 36, 129-151 SLI (4-5;11) NL (age) Digit Span Mean S.D. 1.86 0.99 3.73 1.78 Min 0.00 1.00 Max 4.00 9.00 NWR [N=20] Mean S.D. 8.68 3.39 16.41 1.79 Min 3.00 12.00 Max 18.00 19.00 Linguistic Knowledge Knowledge of lexical composition Size of vocabulary Abstract representation / Poor representational system Less efficient mechanism for using lexical knowledge Speech output Non-word repetition & word learning – TLD, L1 At the age of 4 (Gathercole, Service, Hitch, Adams and Martin 1999) At the age of 5 & 13 (Gathercole, Service, Hitch, Adams and Martin 1999) L2 Acquisition (Masoura and Gathercole 1999) Success on NWR reflects vocabulary size in childhood Success on NWR correlates with the rate of learning new words even to adulthood Chiat, S. (2006). The developmental trajectory of nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 552-556. ODELYA 13/12 NWR & SLI NRT - Nonword Repetition Test Dollaghan, C., and Campbell, T. F. 1998. Nonword Repetition and Child Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol.41 1136-1146 What contributes to the complexity of these words? Are they English like? Could they be used as is in a Hebrew test? Which considerations should apply? CNRep - Children's Test of Nonword Repetition Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., and H. Emslie H. (1994) The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory 2(2):103-27. (Tables from Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. (2006)) How are they different? # of words # of Syllable Clusters Weak syllable with reduced vowels (hampent) Lexical components (morphemes) Prosodic pattern Score Test + NRT 16 1-4 - (CV structure, no late acquired phonemes) - (acoustically salient only) + - Natural for English Online (correct/incorrect) Morpho/Lexical abilities + …. Equal stress on each syllable From transcription (percentage of correct phoneme) Basic phonological processing or memory skills CNRep 40 2-5 + Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. (2006). Nonword repetition: A Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing .comparison of tests .Research, 49, 970–983 Subjects: 36 native speakers: 12 SLI (9;8), 12 TD age (9;9), 12 TD language (6;1). Same SES Scores on standardized test: Which one is easier? Within group: CNRep is easier than NRT Across group: The gap is more significant on CNRep What influences these results? How does NWR fairs with other tasks? Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1029–1037. Children with SLI are significantly worse on all four markers NWR and past tense marking yield the best results (sensitivity & specificity). Adding past tense to NWR raises the predictive ability (from 81% to 87%) While verb morphology is useful for 4-5 y.o. in English, NWR might be more promising for other languages Sentence Repetition 37-924-01 Theoretical Approaches to Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University Child: Want other one spoon, Daddy 2. Father: You mean, you want THE OTHER SPOON 3. Child: Yes, I want other one spoon, please. 4. Father: Can you say “the other spoon”? 5. Child: Other … one … spoon. 6. Father: Say … “other”. 7. Child: Other. 8. Father: “Spoon”. 9. Child: Spoon. 10. Father: “Other … spoon.” 11. Child: Other … Spoon. Now give me other one spoon? 12. (From Pinker1994, p. 281) 1. Elicited imitation as an experimental technique Filling the gap Word order (e.g. in relative clauses) Filling the gap (Slobin & Welsh 1973, from Lust et al. p. 58) Adult: The red beads and the brown beads are here Child: Brown beads here and red beads here & Relative clauses (Friedmann )Lavi, 2006 נסיין :זו הילדה שסבתא נשקה ילד :זו הילדה שנשקה את סבתא Sentence repetition as a linguistic evaluation tool How does SR work? (Bley-Vroman and Chaudron, 1994): The subject hears the input, processes it, and forms a representation. The representation includes information at various levels. The representation is kept in STM The subject formulates (and produces) a sentence based on the representation, comparing it to the model. What influences success on SR? What does it check? Verbal memory Word length Sentence length Syntactic complexity Predictability Verbal memory span Devesovi, A. & Caselli, M. C. 2007. Sentence repetition as a measure of early grammatical development in Italian. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 2 187-208. Subjects: 100 Italian preschoolers. Method: SR, spontaneous speech, verbal memory span Findings: MLU, articles omission and use of the verbs in the sentence imitation task correlated with the same measures of their free speech. Positive correlations between verbal memory span and performance of both the imitation task and the free speech . Word length Willis, C.S. & Gathercole, S.E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children . Memory.349-363 ,9 , Subjects: 30 children, 4-5 (Mean 4;6, SD, 4.28 months) Material: SR followed by picture selection with sentences containing either short or longer words (different in number of syllables), and varied in syntactic structure. Short the prepositions in and on 6.50 the prepositions above and below 7.00 reversible sentences (e.g., The fox is chased by the horse) 7.00 sentences containing a relative clause (e.g., The book is on the box 8.00 that is red) sentences of an X-but-not-Y construction (e.g., The box but not 8.50 the chair is red) embedded sentences (e.g., The shoe the comb is on is blue 9.00 Long 9.75 10.00 9.00 10.75 12.75 12.75 Repetition but not comprehension of the sentences was significantly influenced by word length. Sentence length (Armon-Lotem et al. under revisions) Frequency of errors by type of error 45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 CI om om TD CI-sub LI-S sub LI-L Substitution with code interference: The baby laughed on the clown. Substitution with no code interference: The baby laughed to the clown. Omission with code interference: The elephant pulled *(down) the zebra's pants. Omission with no code interference: The baby laughed *(at) the clown. Syntactic complexity Friedmann, N., & Lavi, H. (2006). On the order of acquisition of A-movement, Wh-movement and V-C movement. In A. Belletti, E. Bennati, C. Chesi, E. Di Domenico, & I. Ferrari (Eds.), Language acquisition and development (pp. 211-217). Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars Press/CSP. Subjects: 60 Hebrew speaking children aged 2;2-3;10: 21 children aged 2;2-2;9, 19 children aged 2;10-3;2, and 20 children aged 3;3-3;10. Task: SR - 80 sentences (8 categories by syntactic complexity), 4 words. Basic SV order: A movement: Wh movement: V-C movement: A-S-V unergative-PP A-S-V transitive-O A-S-V unaccusative-PP Topicalization O-S-V-A Subject relatives Object relatives A-V unergative-S-PP A-V transitive-S-O yesterday the-boy jumped in-the-garden yesterday the-boy built tower yesterday the-girl fell in-the-garden ACC-the-tower the-boy built yesterday (I)-saw ACC-the-girl that-kissed ACC-grandma (I)-saw ACC-the-girl that-grandma kissed yesterday jumped the-boy in-the-garden yesterday built the-boy tower "No correlation was found between repetition of any of the movement types and age (Rpb < 0.22 for all the sentences with movement), and no significant difference in repetition was detected between the three age groups: For example, a 2;3 year old girl succeeded in repeating all the V-C sentences, whereas a 3;10 boy failed in them. Two girls aged 2;5 succeeded in repeating Wh sentences, whereas 4 children aged 3;7 failed in them." (p. 214) Predictability Valian, V. Prasada, S. & Scarpa, J. 2006. Direct object predictability: effects on young children's imitation of sentences. Journal of Child Language, 33, 247-269. Predictability- It is easier to repeat sentences with highly predictable objects than sentences with less predictable objects. a. The dog chews a bone. b. The dog chews a crayon Subjects: 24/23 two-year-olds (mean 28 month, range 25-32) with TLD Task 1: Sentence repetition 6-8 morphemes. Task 2: Sentence repetition 6-8 morphemes + a stickers game SR & SLI Gardner,H., Froud,K., McClelland,A., van der Lely,H. K. J. (2006). The development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language difficulties in young children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 41(5), 513-540. A significant effect of age group: F(4, 618)547.53, p,0.001