Populations and samples

advertisement
Populations and Samples
Anthony Sealey
University of Toronto
This material is distributed under an Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 3.0
Unported Creative Commons License, the full details of which may be found online
here: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/. You may re-use, edit, or
redistribute the content provided that the original source is cited, it is for noncommercial purposes, and provided it is distributed under a similar license.
Populations and Samples
• Often researchers are interested in making
general claims about relationships between
particular political concepts.
• The complete set of all things to which the
specified relationship is thought to apply is
referred to as the ‘population’ of the
analysis.
e.g. What was the population being analyzed
when we investigated the relationship
between gender and attitudes towards
same-sex marriage?
• While the population is what is being
analyzed, it is often impractical to gather
information on the complete set of things
included in the population. For this reason,
researchers often gather information about
a subset of the population – referred to as
a sample – and try to draw inferences
about the population based on the
information gathered from the sample.
• In many respects, the best possible type of
sample is a ‘random’ sample, because
randomization generally ensures that
samples are representative and allows us to
determine the likelihood that a given
sample is unrepresentative. In many
instances, however, non-random sampling
techniques are more convenient and
sometimes even preferable.
Non-Random Sampling Techniques
1)
2)
3)
4)
5)
6)
Systematic Sampling
Stratified Sampling
Cluster Sampling
Purposive Sampling
Deviant Case Sampling
Snowball Sampling
Measurement, Sampling and Error
• Notice that we now have two possible
sources of error from the process of
operationalizing our concepts.
• The first source of error comes from the
measurement process (measurement error).
• The second source of error comes from
the sampling process (sampling error).
• However, it is possible (although
potentially dangerous) to think of sampling
error as a type of measurement error.
• It is also worth drawing attention to the
fact that in quantitative analysis, the
availability of measures often drives the
selection of measures.
e.g. Measuring attitudes towards feminism in
the World Values Survey:
Compare outlooks on the statement:
D059 – “On the whole, men make better
political leaders than women do.”
with outlooks on this statement:
D062 – “A job is alright but what most women
really want is a home and children.”
Now let’s compare data availability:
Now let’s compare data availability:
little data is missing
for ‘femism1’
Now let’s compare data availability:
all the data is missing
for ‘femism2’
• So what do we do? We use ‘femism1’
(D059) not because it’s a more valid
measure than ‘femism2’ (D062), but
because ‘femism2’ isn’t available.
• Finally, it is important to note that in many
instances, the operationalization of
measures is often highly controversial and
affected by the values and beliefs that
scholars bring to their research.
e.g. ‘Relative’ vs. ‘absolute’ measures of
poverty.
‘Measurement’ clip from:
The Gapminder Foundation
http://www.gapminder.org/videos/humanrights-democracy-statistics/
Credibility, Transferability
and Validity
• Validity is a concept most easily identifiable
with quantitative research.
• The term has a wide range of possible
meanings in the field of research methods,
but the central idea revolves around
notions of accuracy and truthfulness.
• First, we can think of ‘measurement
validity’. For a measure to be valid, it must
accurately represent the concept that it is
intended to operationalize.
• One aspect of measurement validity is ‘face
validity’. A measure has face validity if it is
an appropriate operationalization of the
concept.
e.g. Which has greater face validity as a
measure of ‘animal rights activism’:
whether someone owns a pet or whether
an individual donates to animal shelters?
• The text also discusses the ideas of
‘convergent’ and ‘divergent validity’. These
notions of validity can be applied to
indicators. Indicators are said to have
convergent validity if the variables are
thought to be indicators of the same
measure and they yield similar results for
most cases.
e.g. The indicators ‘opposition to same-sex
marriage’ and ‘opposition to abortion
rights’ are said to have ‘convergent validity’
if they are thought to be indicators of a
measure of ‘moral traditionalism’ and they
yield similar results for most cases.
• Indicators are said to be ‘divergently valid’
if the variables are thought to be indicators
of the same measure – but have reverse
directionalities – and they yield opposing
results for most cases.
e.g. The indicators ‘support for same-sex
marriage’ and ‘opposition to abortion rights’
are said to have ‘divergent validity’ if they
are thought to be indicators of a measure
of ‘moral traditionalism’ but have reverse
directionalities and they yield opposing
results for most cases.
• We can also apply the notion of validity to
studies themselves. One such application is
the idea of ‘external validity’. An analysis is
said to have external validity if its findings
can be generalized from the sample
included in the analysis to cases outside the
sample.
• Credibility and transferability are concepts
that have been developed by qualitative
researchers in as parallels to the notions of
measurement and external validity in
quantitative research.
• Qualitative research is said to be ‘credible’
if the data used in the qualitative account
fits the world being described; the
qualitative account must be believable.
• Qualitative research is said to be
‘transferable’ if the findings can be applied
to other contexts.
Dependability and Reliability
• Another important characteristic of
quantitative measures is that they should be
reliable. A measure is said to be reliable if it
consistently obtains comparable results in a
variety of instances of measurement.
• In qualitative research, the analogous
attribute is often described as ‘dependability’,
but again refers to the idea of a consistency
between the collected data and the
conclusions drawn (the results).
• Another way of thinking about this is to ask:
would the results be consistent if the analysis
of the collected data is repeated by other
researchers?
Confirmability and Replicability
• Some qualitative researchers also draw a
distinction between the ideas of
confirmability and replicability. Such a
distinction is quite subtle, however, and
probably exaggerates the extent to which
quantitative analyses are actually replicable.
• The key idea for both is to ask: if we were
to redo the study again, would the
conclusions drawn be the same again?
Terminological Summary
Quantitative Research Qualitative Research
Measurement Validity
External Validity
Reliability
Replicability
Credibility
Transferability
Dependability
Confirmability
Validity, Reliability and Bias
• As we have seen, the concept of ‘validity’
has a broad range of possible applications.
• However, two important criteria by which
to conceptualize validity involve reliability
and biasedness. Valid measures should be
both reliable and unbiased.
• A reliable or consistent estimator is one
that tends to produce estimates that do not
differ significantly from each other (i.e. the
variance of the estimates is low).
• An unbiased estimator is one for which the
average of all possible sample statistics is
equal to the population parameter that it is
estimating.
e.g. #1:
Reliable
but
Biased



e.g. #2:
Unbiased
but
Unreliable





e.g. #3:
Biased
and
Unreliable





e.g. #4:
Reliable
and
Unbiased



A Schematic Representation
of Some Aspects of the
Concept of Validity
Validity
Internal
Validity
Measurement
Validity
External
Validity
Face
Validity
Convergent
Validity
Unbiasedness
Reliability
Download