ExpDes-1
Experimental design (for an experimental study): the manner in which the experimental units are arranged or grouped, and how the treatments are assigned to them.
To call in the statistician after the experiment is done may be no more than asking him to perform a post-mortem examination: he may be able to say what the experiment died of.
Sir R.A. Fisher (1938)
Fisher compared a dataset to a sample of gold ore. The finest analysis could only extract the proportion of gold contained in the ore. But a good design could yield a sample with more gold!
[ data = ore ; information = gold.]
ExpDes-2
Experimental error is the variation in the responses among experimental units (e.u.’s) which are assigned the same treatment, and are observed under the same experimental conditions. It is measured by SSE (or MSE).
Ideally, we would like experimental error to be zero ! This is impossible because of (at least) one or more of the following reasons:
1.
There are inherent differences in the e.u.’s before they receive treatments.
2.
There is variation in the devices that record the measurements.
3.
There is variation in applying or setting the treatments.
4.
There are extraneous factors other than the treatments which affect the response.
ExpDes-3
Control over the magnitude of experimental error can be achieved by:
• Careful choice of e.u.’s. Homogeneity of e.u.’s decreases MSE.
• Taking care with experimental procedures and recording of measurements. Smaller errors result in smaller MSE.
• Blocking of e.u.’s. Blocking decreases MSE.
• Choice of experimental design used. Designs with more factors generally lead to smaller MSE.
• Using explanatory variables or covariates (variables that are thought to affect the response; related to it). Models with more variables generally lead to smaller MSE.
ExpDes-4
• CRD in one-way AOV
(pooled variances).
• Experimental units are
• Experiment will use very
.
.
• Treatments are assigned to experimental units at
.
• Each treatment is replicated the same number of times
(
may be appropriate time.
).
• No accommodation made for
(extraneous sources of variation).
• High probability that a large fraction of the experimental units set out at the beginning of the study
at the
ExpDes-5
Completely Randomized Design
• Experimental Design - Completely randomized design (CRD)
• Sampling Design - One-way classification design
Assumptions:
• Independent random samples (results of one sample do not effect other samples).
• Samples from normal population(s).
• Mean and variance for population i are respectively, m i and s
2 .
Model: y ij
m i
ij
E ( y ij
)
m i
AOV model overall mean random error ~ N(0, s
2 ) effect due to population i
H
0
:
H a
:
1
At
2 least
one
t of
0 the
differ from 0
Requirement for m to be the overall mean: i t
1
i
0
ExpDes-6
Reference Group Model
Model: y tj y ij
m t m t
tj
, i
i
ij
, i
t
1 , 2 , , t
1 random error ~ N(0, s
2 ) reference group mean effect due to population i
Mean for the last group (i=t) is m t
.
Mean for the first group (i=1) is m t
Thus,
1
+ b
1 is the difference between the mean of the reference group (cell) and the target group mean. Any group can be the reference group.
H
0
H a
:
1
2
: At least one of
t
1
the
0
0
This is the model
SAS
,
SPSS and most other packages use.
ExpDes-7
CRD Practical Problems/Limitations
In many situations, the researcher:
• Does not have sufficient homogeneous experimental material in one group (location, batch, etc.) to effectively use the CRD
(i.e. resource constraints )
• The study objectives require examining treatments over a broad range of experimental units in order that results can be extended to more situations (i.e. breadth of study objectives ).
• The experimental material must be grouped for administrative or implementation purposes (i.e. implementation constraints ).
If the researcher knows something about the characteristics of the experimental material, it is often possible to group experimental units into sets of relatively homogenous material ( blocks ), and then compare treatment level means within these groups .
ExpDes-8
Example
A scientist was interested in the use of three chemicals and water on their effectiveness in extracting sulfur from Florida soils. The chemicals of interest are:
• Calcium Chloride
• Ammonium Acetate
• Mono Calcium Phosphate
• Water
CaCl
NH
4
Ca(H
H
2
2
OAc
2
PO
4
O
)
3
Five soils were chosen for this experiment:
• Troup Jackson Co. Paleudults soil
• Lakeland Walton Co. Quartzipsamments soil
• Leon Duval Co. Haplaquads soil
• Chipley
• Norfolk
Jackson Co. Quartzipsamments soil
Alachua Co. Paleudults soil
ExpDes-9
Blocking and Control of Extraneous Variation
The main interest in the experiment is the comparison of the four extraction methods.
• The variation imposed on the extraction procedure by the five different soil types represents a source of extraneous variation .
• Unless controlled for in the experiment, this variation has the potential to “swamp” or overwhelm the differences among the extraction procedure.
• High probability of concluding there are no treatment effects when treatment effects are in fact present.
Fair comparisons only occur among extractions within a soil type.
We wish to use the combined experience across soil types to make a stronger statement about the extraction procedures.
ExpDes-10
Graphical View
Note: The pattern of responses to treatments is consistent within a given soil type (a block); but responses vary across soil types.
ExpDes-11
Randomized Block Design (RBD)
Any experimental design in which the randomization of treatments is restricted to groups of experimental units within a predefined block of units assumed to be internally homogeneous is called a randomized block design . Blocks of units are created to control known sources of variation in expected (mean) response among experimental units.
There are two classifications or factors in an RBD: block “effects” and treatment “effects”.
•
•
Rules for blocking:
Carefully examine the situation at hand and identify those factors which are known to affect the proposed response.
Choose one or two of these factors as the basis for creating blocks.
Blocking factors are sometimes referred to as disturbing factors .
ExpDes-12
Examples of Typical Blocking Factors
Disturbing Variable
Nutrient gradient
Water moisture gradient
Slope differences
Soil composition
Orientation to sun
Flow of air
Distribution of heat
Age
Local density
Gender
Age
Socio-demographics
Experimental Unit
Field Plot
Location in Greenhouse
Tree
Person
ExpDes-13
Blocking Importance
How blocks are formed is critical to the effectiveness of the analysis .
• With field plots, blocks are laid out so that they are perpendicular to the maximum direction of change in the disturbing factor to be controlled.
• Wide border (discard) areas are used to overcome interference between neighboring plots (i.e. to maintain independence of responses) within blocks and between blocks.
• Time blocks may need discard times between “replications”.
This approach maximizes within block homogeneity while simultaneously maximizing among block heterogeneity .
ExpDes-14
Blocking Example
T2
T1
T5
T3
T4
T1
T2
T3
T5
T4
T1
T5
T2
T4
T3
T3
T4
T1
T2
T5
Treatment effects confounded with moisture effect!
ExpDes-15
Blocking Example
T1
T5
T3
T4
T3
T2
T5 T4
T3 T1
T2
T5
T4
T1
T5
T2
T2
T4
T1
T3
Block effect now removes moisture effect, fair comparisons among treatments.
ExpDes-16
Advantages and Disadvantages
Advantages of a Blocked Design
• Controls a single extraneous source of variation and removes its effect from the estimate of experimental error.
• Allows more flexibility in experimental layout .
• Allows more flexibility in experimental implementation and administration.
Disadvantages of a Blocked Design
• Generally unsuited when there is a large number of treatments because of possible loss of within block homogeneity .
• Serious problem with the analysis if a block factor by treatment interaction effect actually exists and no replication within blocks has been included. (solution: use replication within blocks when possible).
ExpDes-17
Complete or Incomplete Designs
Can all treatments be accommodated in each block?
Complete Block Design : Every treatment occurs in each block.
Incomplete Block Design: Not every treatment occurs in each block.
Complete Incomplete
A B C D A B C
B D C A
D C A B
B
D
D
C A
A
ExpDes-18
Balance in Designs
Balancing refers to the specific assignment of treatments to experimental units such that comparisons of treatment effects are done with equal precision . This is usually accomplished by equally replicating each treatment.
Balanced Block Design: The variance of the difference between two treatment means is the same regardless of which two treatments are compared. This usually implies that the overall replication (disregarding which blocks they are in) for the comparison of two treatments is the same for all pairs of treatments.
Partially Balanced Design : The variance of the difference between two treatments depends on which two treatments are being considered. This usually implies different replication for different treatments.
Unbalanced Designs: Unequal replication in each block - usually what one ends up with.
ExpDes-19