Memory, Learning and Amnesia Memory, Learning and Amnesia • Memory = site and/or process where knowledge and experiences are stored. • Learning = the process of committing new knowledge and experiences into (semi-) permanent storage. – Classical conditioning – Operant conditioning – Other neural mechanisms • Amnesia = the inability to form or recall memories. Memory, Learning and Amnesia • Types of memory and amnesia • Brain areas involved in memory – Sensory and working short-term memory – Procedural memories – Declarative memories • Neural mechanisms of learning History of Memory Studies • The study of memory – – – – 1885 Ebbinghaus publishes first studies on memory. 1889 Korsakoff describes severe anterograde amnesia. 1915 Karl Lashley begins a long-term study of memory. 1950 Lashley states “… the engram is represented throughout the region.” – 1953 Dr. William Scoville removes the bilateral medial temporal lobes of H. M. to stop epileptic seizures and inadvertently discovers the role of the hippocampus. Areas of Memory • Lashley was wrong. Memories are not evenly distributed over the cortex. • Memories are not all stored in the same place. Different types of memory are found in different areas, but all rely on synaptic connections. • There is no “grandma” neuron. • All parts of the nervous system can learn and remember. • Multimodal information is remembered better. Types of Memory - Data • Declarative or explicit (conscious) – Facts & events – Easily formed, and easily forgotten • Nondeclarative or implicit (unconscious) – – – – – a.k.a. procedural memory Skills, habits and conditioning Skeletal muscle practiced movements. Emotional responses Requires repetition, but rarely forgotten Types of Memory - Data Types of Memory - Time • Short-term – Only good for seconds to hours – Easily disruptable • Long-term – Lasts for days, months or years – Permanent Short-term Memory • Average capacity is 7 +/- 2 chunks, generally proportional to intelligence. – Kept in right orbital cortex (frontal lobe). • Data only remains there for a few seconds without rehearsal. Modulated by attention. • Easily disrupted. • Unrelated to long-term memory. Short-term Memory • Short-term sensory memory – The senses have independent short-term storage. – Kept in the cortical area of the sense. • Temporal lobe for audio data, etc. • The lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP) seems to hold short-term visual memories in monkeys. – If there is sufficient attention, the sensory information can be moved to short-term working memory areas. If not, the information will be lost. Types of Memory Sensory Information Attention Sensory Register Declarative Implicit Short-term Memory Consolidation Long-term Memory Loss of Memory • Amnesia = The loss of (declarative) memory – Retrograde • Can’t recall previously available information. • Sometimes very old memories are still available. – Anterograde amnesia • Can’t learn new information. • Can affect short-term, long-term, or both. • Usually accompanied by retrograde amnesia. – Specific deficits • Prosopagnosia, anomia, etc. Procedural Memory Areas • The striatum seems to be strongly involved in procedural memories and conditioning. – Huntington’s and Parkinson’s patients have difficulties learning procedural tasks because of damage to the striatum. • The cerebellum is the primary site of coordinated movement learning. Declarative Memory Areas • Amnesia, lobectomy and stimulation studies point to the temporal lobe as the primary site for declarative memories, or at least their recall. • Stimulation of the temporal cortex produces more complex memories and hallucinations than any other brain area. • Anomia and prosopagnosia tied to temporal lobe. Declarative Memory Areas – H.M. • Case study: H. M. (1953, M, 27 y.o.) – Dr. Scoville removed both medial temporal lobes to alleviate untreatable epileptic seizures. – Seizures were greatly reduced, BUT… – H. M. had severe post-op anterograde amnesia which never improved, but little retrograde or motor amnesia or short-term memory problems. – From previous understanding (distributed memory), this could not occur. – Research changed from place to process. Declarative Memory Areas • Medial temporal lobe – Removed in H.M. – Hippocampus is directly below the amygdala (highlighted in pink). Implicit Memory Areas – H.M.’s working memory is intact. – H.M. can still learn habits and trained tasks. – This shows that lack of the hippocampus impairs consolidation required for conscious recall, but not for implicit memories. • Priming – Exposure to a stimulus makes it easier to recognize that stimulus again (it is remembered). – H. M. shows very limited signs of recognizing prior stimuli without cognitively realizing it. Declarative Memory Areas • 8 other psychotic patients were examined – Only those who had a hippocampusectomy had anterograde amnesia. – They deduced the hippocampus is necessary for new memory formation, but not recall. – It is not necessary for short-term memory. – Modern procedures call for only one hippocampus to be removed, and it is now tested for functionality before the operation. Declarative Memory Areas • Alzheimer’s disease – A progressive disease causing loss of cells and deterioration in the association cortex. – Marked by anterograde amnesia and later also by retrograde amnesia. – Damage begins in medial temporal cortex and spreads to other areas. – This is evidence that anterograde amnesia is related to the medial temporal cortex. Declarative Memory Areas • Korsakoff’s Syndrome – Symptoms • Severe anterograde amnesia • Confabulation – Make up stories based on fragments of recent occurrences – Caused by thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency • Alcoholism • Malnutrition – Damages the mammillary bodies, which relay information from the hippocampus to the thalamus via the fornix. Declarative Memory Areas • Patient R. B. – Permanent anterograde amnesia caused by anoxic ischemia of the hippocampus. – On autopsy, it was found that the CA1 region of the hippocampus was gone. – The CA1 region is especially rich in NMDA receptors (involved in learning). • If only CA1 damaged: anterograde amnesia only. • Anoxia causes NMDA receptors to allow excessive Ca++ influx, damaging cells. Declarative Memory Areas • Further evidence of NMDA-Hippocampus connection: – Mice with NMDA receptor knock out learn very slowly, if at all. – Mice with excess NMDA receptor genes learn quicker than normal. Declarative Memory Areas • Neuromodulation in the hippocampus – 5-HT inhibits memory formation. – NE, E, D, cocaine enhance memory formation. – Cholinergic theta rhythms (5-8 Hz) from medial septum seem to be necessary. – In rats, theta activity is correlated with exploratory behaviors. • Info sampled into dentate gyrus and CA3 on theta. • Info moved to CA1 when theta waves subside. Declarative Memory Areas • Anatomical structures: – Thalamus, sensory relay – Amygdala, emotional memory – Hippocampus, spatial memory • Rat radial maze performance: evidence of place neurons – – – – Rhinal cortex, object & recognition memory Fornix and mammilary bodies Prefrontal cortex Surrounding limbic structures Neural Mechanisms Classical Conditioning • A form of learning where an otherwise unimportant stimulus acquires the properties of an important stimulus. • Forms an association between two stimuli, one which would normally cause a behavior and one which would not. • Implicit memory Classical Conditioning • Ex. Rabbit eye blink – A puff of air directed at a rabbit’s eye causes the rabbit to blink, an unconditioned response. – A 1000 Hz tone is played independently and causes no eye blink response. – A tone is played and shortly followed by an air puff and this sequence is repeated. – The rabbit quickly learns to blink as soon as the tone is sounded, a conditioned response. Hebb’s Rule • 1949 Donald Hebb proposes that a synaptic connection will be strengthened if a synapse repeatedly becomes active at the same time or just after the postsynaptic nerve fires (he could not verify his own theory). Operant Conditioning • Similar to classical conditioning, except that it involves an association between a learned behavior and a response (instead of an automatic behavior and another stimulus). • Permits an organism to adjust its behavior according to the consequences. • Reinforcing stimuli increase the likelihood of the response, punishing stimuli decrease it. Operant Conditioning Dr. Skinner and his famous box Operant Conditioning • Ex. Skinner Box - Training – A hungry rat is placed in a box with a lever. – It has no particular reason to press the lever. – By random interaction, the rat learns that it will get a food reward for pressing the lever. – This will increase the likelihood that the rat will press the lever to get more food (reinforcing stimulus). Operant Conditioning • Ex. Skinner Box - Extinction – Once trained, the rat is then also shocked (a punishing stimulus) when the lever is pressed, decreasing the likelihood of further lever presses. – The lever pressing behavior is extinguished. – Recent research suggests 2 mechanisms: • Immediate: The new synaptic connection destroyed. • Delayed: A separate learned inhibitory pathway forms. – Consolidation seems to be required. Neural Mechanisms • The basis of all learning is plasticity, the ability of the nervous system to change its neural connections by: – Forming or destroying neural connections. – Forming or destroying receptors. – Activating or deactivating receptors. Learning • Two major plasticity mechanisms – Long-term potentiation (LTP) • Creates associations by synaptic enhancement – Long-term depression (LTD) • Loosens associations by synaptic degradation Anatomy Review • Hippocampus (a.k.a. Ammon’s Horn = cornu ammonis) is heavily involved in new memory formation. • Neurons enter through the entorhinal cortex, relay through the granule cells of the dentate gyrus, and project to pyramidal cells of CA3 (30,000+ spines per dendrite). • Output is from CA1. Long-term Potentiation • Glutamate is the predominant interneuronal neurotransmitter in the CNS. • Two major glutamate receptor types: – AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4isoxazole propionate) • Na+ ion channels – NMDA (n-methyl-D-aspartate) • Voltage and glutamate controlled Ca++ ion channel • The channel is normally blocked by a Mg++ ion, which is expelled when the cell becomes depolarized. Long-term Potentiation • “Silent synapse” theory - new dendritic spines only contain NMDA receptors (no AMPA receptors). • If the new synapse receives stimulation at the same time as the nerve fires, AMPA receptors will be created, unsilencing the synapse. Long-term Potentiation • The NMDA receptors are assumed to be responsible for LTP. – AP5 (2-amino-5-nopentanoate) blocks NMDA channels and temporarily inhibits learning, but not recall. • Ca++ acts as a 2nd messenger, regulating the creation of new AMPA receptors. – EGTA, which binds to Ca++ and makes it insoluble, also blocks learning. Long-term Potentiation • Ca++ influx – Activates type II calcium-calmodulin kinase (CaM-KII). – Converts arginine to nitrous oxide (NO). Which signals presynaptic neuron to release Glu. • CaM-KII self-phosphorylates, allowing continued action after Ca++ influx. • CaM-KII controls synthesis of receptors, protein kinases and cytoskeleton, and phosphorylates the AMPA receptors. LTP Summary • Initially only NMDA channels. • Simultaneous presynaptic glutamate and postsynaptic depolarization let Ca++ enter NMDA channels. • AMPA receptors are synthesized and strengthen the synaptic connection. LTP • CaM-KII effects: – Self-phosphorylation – Creation of new AMPA receptors – Arginine to nitrous oxide conversion Long-term Potentiation • NO release by the postsynaptic cell has retrograde causes further presynaptic glutamate release. Long-term Potentiation • Recent evidence also shows that the presynaptic terminal button projects a finger-like extension into the postsynaptic dendritic spine. • The projection divides the spine and causes a split into two buttons and two spines. Long-term Potentiation Long-term Potentiation • Protein synthesis in LTP – Proteins (i.e. AMPA receptors) don’t last long, but memories do. – Something else must make memories permanent. – Protein synthesis inhibitors have been found to interfere with the formation of long-term memories. Long-term Potentiation • Protein synthesis experiments – Experiments with Drosophila identified two proteins involved with long term learning, cAMP Response Element Binding proteins CREB-1 and CREB-2. – CREB2 repressed memory formation. – CREB1 gave super-memory. – CREB formation is governed by protein kinases that results from varying Ca++ influx. Long-term Potentiation • CREB-2 does not permit synthesis • CREB-1 readily replaces CREB-2, but does not permit synthesis either. • Phosphorylated CREB-1 does permit synthesis. Long-term Depression • CPP, an NMDA antagonist blocks LTP but not LTD. • This suggests at least two subtypes of NMDA receptors. • AMPA receptors are dephosphorylated, decreasing their sensitivity to glutamate. • AMPA receptors also decrease in number. Long-term Potentiation Hebb’s Rule • After 50 years and many new tools (cellular recording, drugs, electron microscopy) we now have solid evidence for at least one mechanism of learning predicted by Hebb. • Other mechanisms also exist, but they are not yet well understood.