Variants of the case- control design Katharina Alpers EPIET introductory course, Menorca (Spain), 10 October, 2011 Overview Design of case-control studies • Exclusive („traditional“) • Inclusive („case-cohort“) • Concurrent (density) • Case-to-case • Case-crossover 2 Cohort study: incidence risk Exposure Total Cases Risk (%) Risk ratio Exposed 100 40 40% 4 Unexposed 100 10 10% Reference Total 200 50 25% Cumulative incidence Number of cases/population initially at risk 3 Cohort study: incidence rate Exposure Total Time Cases Rate per 100 p.y. Rate ratio Exposed 1500 p.y 40 2.7/100 p.y. 2.7 Unexposed 1000 p.y. 10 1.0/100 p.y. Reference Total 2500 p.y. 50 2.0/100 p.y. Incidence density Number of cases/sum of times at risk 4 Cohort study Exposed population (E) Initially at Risk Ne Cases exposed Ce Person years at risk of exposed (pyare) Currently at risk Still at risk Ne - Ce Unexposed population (U) Initially at Risk Nu Cases unexposed CU Person years at risk of unexposed (pyaru) Start of study Rodrigues L et al. Int J Epidemiol. 1990;19:205-13. Currently at risk Still at risk Nu - Cu End of study Time 5 Case-control studies • Efficient for rare diseases • Compare exposure in cases to sample of population – sampled from source population that gives rise to cases – representative of exposure in source population • Sampling independent of exposure status • Different control sampling schemes 6 Traditional case-control design (exclusive) Exposed population (E) Cohort study Cases exposed Cases Still at risk Unexposed population (U) Cases unexposed Sample of “non cases” Still at risk End of study 7 Traditional design • Controls sampled from population still at risk at the end of the study period • Disease odds ratio = exposure odds ratio • If disease is rare: OR good estimate of risk ratio and rate ratio 8 Inclusive design: Case cohort study Exposed population (E) Cohort study Cases exposed Cases Still at risk Unexposed population (U) Cases unexposed Sample of source population Still at risk End of study 9 Case-cohort design • Control group estimates the proportion of the total population that is exposed • Controls selected from all individuals at risk at the start of the study – sampled regardless whether or not they will fall ill • Case may also be selected as a control and vice versa -> kept in both groups • OR estimates relative risk 10 Concurrent design: density case control Exposed population (E) Cohort study Cases exposed Cases Still at risk Unexposed population (U) Cases unexposed Sample of source population Still at risk Still at risk End of study 11 Concurrent design: density case control • Controls selected from those still at risk when a case occurs • Control can later become a case • Not vice versa -> cases no longer at risk • Controls who later become cases kept in both groups • Controls represent person years at risk experience among exposed and unexposed • Matched analysis on time of selection • OR estimates the rate ratio 12 How to select controls to estimate the respective measure of association Measure Design Risk ratio Inclusive Rate ratio Concurrent Odds ratio Exclusive Formulation Alternative formulation Controls to be sampled from Ce/Ne Cu/Nu Ce/Cu Ne/Nu Total study population regardless of past or future disease status Ce/pyare Cu/pyaru Ce/Cu pyare/pyaru People currently at risk Ce/(Ne- Ce) Cu/(Nu- Cu) Ce/Cu (Ne- Ce) /(Nu- Cu) People disease-free throughout study period 13 Rodrigues L et al. Int J Epidemiol. 1990;19:205-13. What design and when? • Rare diseases: all give similar results • Non-recurrent disease with high incidence -> Case cohort design (inclusive): OR relative Risk • Recurrent common disease -> Density case control design (concurrent): OR relative Rate • Probability or effect of exposure changes along time -> Density case control design: OR relative Rate • No need to quantify -> traditional design 14 Relationship between OR and RR, according to the primary attack rate (AR) Acknowledgements: Olivier le Polain, EPIET Cohort 15 HPA London Epidemiology Unit, UK 15 Cases detected by surveillance systems • Non-random selection process: – Host factors (eg. asymptomatic infections) – Different health care seeking behaviour – Incomplete lab investigation – Incomplete reporting • Differential recall – Between reported and not reported cases – Between cases and controls 16 Case-to-case approach • Same disease, different subtypes/clones: – Serotypes – Phage types – Antibiotic resistance patterns • Controls = cases with non epidemic subtypes – – – – from same source population same susceptibility (underlying diseases) included as cases if they had the outbreak strain readily available • Reduces selection AND recall bias • Food-exposure collected before status is known 17 Two listeriosis outbreaks France, 1999-2000: two distinct PFGE patterns Cases 10 9 Outbreak 2 (32 cases) October November 1999 8 6 4 2 52 50 48 46 44 Outbreak 1 (10 cases) 42 40 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 December January February March 2000 de Valk H et al. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:944-50 18 Listeriosis outbreak cases and sporadic cases distinguished by routine PFGE, France, 1999-2000 Cases 14 Sporadic cases 12 Outbreak 2 (32 cases) 10 Outbreak 1 (10 cases) 8 6 4 2 October November December 1999 de Valk H et al. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:944-50 January 2000 8 6 4 2 52 50 48 46 44 42 40 0 February March 19 Case to case control study: controls selected among sporadic cases listeriosis outbreak, France, 1999-2000 Cases 14 12 10 Other sporadic cases Sporadic cases used as controls (N = 32) Outbreak 2 (N = 32) Outbreak 1 (N = 10) 8 6 4 2 December 8 6 4 2 52 November 1999 50 48 46 October 44 42 40 0 January February March 2000 20 de Valk H et al. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:944-50 Outbreak of listeriosis, France, December 1999 - February 2000 Results multivariable analysis (29 cases, 32 controls) Food consumed Pork tongue in jelly Cooked ham Pâté de campagne Adjusted Odds ratio* 95% CI p 75.5 7.1 8.9 4.7 – 1216.0 0.7 – 71.8 1.7 – 46.1 0.002 0.1 0.009 *adjusted for underlying condition, pregnancy status and date of interview de Valk H et al. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:944-50 21 Case-crossover design • Same person taken as its own control -> No between-persons confounding • Matched design: – Compare exposure in a risk period to one or more control periods – Only pairs of discordant periods used in the analysis • Acute diseases • Exposure – must vary over time – short induction and transient effect • sensitive to recall bias 22 Case-crossover design Reference period “Wash out” Current period period Cases Matched pairs 1 Discordant 0, 1 2 Discordant 1, 0 3 Concordant 1, 1 4 Concordant 0,0 Exposure Study Prolonged Salmonella Typhimurium outbreak, France Food exposures in the risk and control period and matched OR for 17 nosocomial cases Foods Risk period Exposed (%) Veal Pork Hamburgers Ham Pâté Chicken Turkey “Cordon bleu” Lamb sausages Poultry sausages 5 (29) 4 (23) 13 (77) 6 (35) 2 (12) 2 (12) 11 (65) 0 (0) 2 (12) 2 (12) Control period Matched OR Exposed (%) 1 (6) 6 (35) 5 (29) 5 (29) 2 (12) 3 (18) 6 (35) 2 (12) 0 (0) 0 (0) Haegebaert S et al. Epidemiol infect 2003;130,1-5 5 0,6 5 1,5 1 1 2,67 undefined undefined undefined 95% C.I. 0,6 - 236,5 0,1 - 3,1 1,1 - 46,9 0,2 - 17,9 0,01 - 78,5 0,01 - 78,5 0,7 - 15,6 24 Time trend in exposure: Between period confounding Case-time controls: ORb for the time trend Cases: ORa for the exposure and the time trend Control period Risk period onset Case-time control design ORa/ORb = OR of exposure adjusted for time trend 25 Folic acid antagonists (FAA) in pregnancy and congenital cardiovascular defects (CCD) • • • • Case-crossover approach Case: Woman who had a child with CCD (N=3870) Exposure: FAA during 2nd & 3rd month of pregnancy Control: Woman who had a child without CCD (N=8387) Delivery Cases: -2 -1 1 Control period Controls: -2 -1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2 3 9 OR=1.0 (0.5-2.0) Case-time control OR = 1/0.3 = 2.9 (1.2-7.2) Risk period 1 8 4 5 Hernandez-Diaz S. Am J Epidemiol 2003;158:385-391 6 7 8 9 OR= 0.3 (0.2-0.6) 26 Conclusions • If you do not need that OR estimates correctly the RR -> “traditional design” • Otherwise, if you need OR RR -> identify the best design for each situation • If it is difficult to find appropriate controls – Case to case – Case-crossover 27 References • • • • • • • Rodrigues L et al. Int J Epidemiol 1990;19:205-13 Rothman KJ. Epidemiology: an introduction. Oxford University Press 2002, 73-93 Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL: Modern Epidemiology. 3. ed., Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2008. Chapter 8: Case-Control Studies, 111-127 McCarthy N, Giesecke J. 1 Int J Epidemiology 1999; 28, 764-8 de Valk H et al. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:944-50 Haegebaert S et al. Epidemiol infect 2003;130,1-5 Hernandez-Diaz S et al. Am J Epidemiol 2003;158:385-391 Further Reading • • • • Suissa S. The case-time-control design. Epidemiology 1995;6:248-253. Greenland S. Epidemiology. 1996; 7231-239. Mittleman, Maclure, Robins. Am J Epidemiol 1995;142;1:91-98. Karagiannis I et.al. Epidemiol Infect 2010;138, 1726-1734 28