CSC 426 Team Presentation Week 8 Spring 2013 Jinghui CHENG Jean HOP Raj PARTHASARATHY RESEARCH ETHICS the rules of conduct recognized in respect to a particular class of human actions or a particular group, culture (dictionary.com) Why You Should Care 1 1. Promote the aims of research – Knowledge - Truth - Avoidance of Error 2. Promote values of collaborative work – Trust – Accountability – Mutual Respect – Fairness 3. Help build public support for research – Integrity – Quality 4. Promote important moral and social values – Social Responsibility – Health – Safety 1 “What is Ethics in Research & Why is it Important?” David B. Resnik, NIH, May 1, 2011. www.niehs.nigh.gov Why You Should Care 1. Promote the aims of research – Knowledge - Truth - Avoidance of Error 2. Promote values of collaborative work – Trust – Accountability – Mutual Respect – Fairness 3. Help build public support for research – Integrity – Quality 4. Promote important moral and social values – Social Responsibility – Health – Safety F F P: The BIG Three + One Falsification Fabrication Plagiarism Misrepresentation FALSIFICATION Outright falsification rare but exists • 2002 Bell Labs: Jan Hendrick Schon • 2005 U of VT: Eric Poehlman Falsification GREY Area • • • • Culling of “spurious” data Removal of badly behaved points Decisions on controls and statistical tests Etc FABRICATION Outright fabrication rare but exists • 2002 Bell Labs: Jan Hendrick Schon • 2005 U of VT: Eric Poehlman • 2005 Seoul University Woo Suk Hwang MISREPRESENTATION • Present results in a way that inaccurately reflects the outcome • Understate the work of others • Imply a higher confidence in results than is merited SPECIAL CASE: Online changes Why You Should Care 1. Promote the aims of research 1 – Knowledge - Truth - Avoidance of Error 2. Promote values of collaborative work 1 – Trust – Accountability – Mutual Respect – Fairness 3. Help build public support for research 1 – Integrity – Quality 4. Promote important moral and social values 1 – Social Responsibility – Health – Safety PLAGIARISM Outright plagiarism rare but exists • 2002 Historian Stephen Ambrose • 2013 Science Journalist Jonah Lehrer • 2013 German Education Minister Annette Schavan Plagiarism GREY Areas: • Self Plagiarism • Unconscious Plagiarism SELF PLAGIARISM Why can’t I reuse MY work? 1. Published or put out for publication – Copyright on your EXPRESSION of your idea 2. Theoretically ideas become tired recycleds “UNCONSCIOUS” PLAGIARISM • Paraphrase of another’s work • Unclear citation as to which parts of the work are another’s • Paraphrase of paper structure • Citing same background literature • Similar experiments AUTHORSHIP Included as authors but did not contribute • Owed a favor • Research team head RELATIONSHIPS Required for collaboration and just plain living Power Imbalances • Graduate Students • Lab heads, Department Chairs Sharing data/Confidentiality Future Competition MORE NOT ALWAYS BETTER! What about “dangerous” information? Example: 2012 Two papers published in Nature and Science due to implications for terrorism. Why You Should Care 1. Promote the aims of research 1 – Knowledge - Truth - Avoidance of Error 2. Promote values of collaborative work 1 – Trust – Accountability – Mutual Respect – Fairness 3. Help build public support for research 1 – Integrity – Quality 4. Promote important moral and social values 1 – Social Responsibility – Health – Safety PUBLIC SUPPORT Funding, funding, funding! Examples • West Nile Disease versus Valley Fever funding • Polio funding Why You Should Care 1. Promote the aims of research 1 – Knowledge - Truth - Avoidance of Error 2. Promote values of collaborative work 1 – Trust – Accountability – Mutual Respect – Fairness 3. Help build public support for research 1 – Integrity – Quality 4. Promote important moral and social values 1 – Social Responsibility – Health – Safety CONFLICT OF INTEREST Universities associated with BIG business • 1974 Monsanto and Harvard Human Testing (not us) EXAMPLES • Dr B’s Meat Example • Pharmatec and U of FL • Boots Pharmaceuticals and Betty Dong • Apotex and Nancy Olivieri (U of Toronto) ETHICS ISSUE: SORTING IT OUT 1. 2. 3. 4. What is the problem or issue? What is the relevant information? What are the different options? How do ethical codes or policies as well as legal rules apply to these different options? 5. Are there any people who can offer ethical advice? KEY IS TO BE ABLE TO JUSTIFY YOUR DECISION ETHICS CHECKLIST See pg 224 in text book for checklist MEASUREMENT Part 1: Construct Validity Part 2: Reliability Construct Validity • One facet of measurement • How do we take ideas or theories and measure experimental results? • How do we know we are measuring the right things? Construct Validity • Operationalization: eliminate subjectivity by by operationally defining concepts • Operationally – define concepts by how you will measure them • Defining variables as MEASURABLE factors OPERATIONALIZATION • Example: Variable Factor Age Number of years and months Gender Male/Female/Other Distance Number of miles • How well do we measure what we purport to be measuring? CONSTRUCT VALIDITY • Definitionalist Perspective: • Define construct so tightly as to define operationalization at the same time • Relationalist Perspective: • Everything related; Not black and white • Can’t really use operational definitions to define constructs CONSTRUCT VALIDITY • Translation Validity • Does the chosen operationalization reflect the construct? • Criterion-related Validity • How will the operationalization perform? CONSTRUCT VALIDITY: Translation • Face Validity • On its face, does it appear to be good? • Subjective judgment call therefore weak • Can strengthen by expert review/feedback • Content Validity • Check against content domain • Checklist approach CONSTRUCT VALIDITY: Criterion Given what we know theoretically and using operationalization, can we … • Predict (Predictive) • Distinguish between groups (Concurrent) • Find similar results with theoretically similar operationalizations (Convergent) • Find dissimilar results with theoretically dissimilar operationalizations (Discriminant) ALL WE NEED IS BOTH! Convergent AND Divergent Convergent, Divergent – correlation analysis How high is high? How do we decide? THREATS-NON SOCIAL •Inadequate Preoperational Explication of Constructs Concepts not well defined prior to measurement phase •Mono-operation bias Only one version of variable used limiting the breadth of the experiment •Mono-method bias Only one method used to observe or measure a key concept •Interaction of treatment and testing Testing becomes part of treatment and the interaction skews the results THREATS-NON SOCIAL •Interaction of Different Treatments Other reasons besides chosen treatment might be influencing results •Restricted Generalizability Across Constructs Might be a situation of working only in this experiment THREATS-SOCIAL •Hypothesis Guessing Participants respond based on what they think the goal of the experiment is. •Evaluator Apprehension Knowledge of experiment affects participant •Experimenter Expectancies Researcher wants experiment to go a certain way HANDLING CONSTRUCT VALIDITY • Theoretical representation: Nomological Net http://www.socialresearchm ethods.net/kb/nomonet.php HANDLING CONSTRUCT VALIDITY • Application: Multitrait Multimethod Matrix • Developed in the 1950s • Not used in practice due to multi method constraints • Too much “subjective” judgement • All about correlation HANDLING CONSTRUCT VALIDITY http://www.socialresearchm ethods.net/kb/nomonet.php Pattern Matching Correlation • Overall estimate of construct validity • Matches predicted, theoretical • Compare concept map with observed pattern http://www.socialresearchm ethods.net/kb/nomonet.php RELIABILITY • Reliability = consistency = repeatability RELIABILITY • Reliability = consistency = repeatability • True Score Theory says … Observations = True Measurement + Random Error var(X) = var(T) + var(eX) – Variability due to variability in the True and Random measures RELIABILITY • Across individuals – not related to individual measure • Can’t compute actual Reliability var(T) ------var(x) Because we do not know var(T) • So have to estimate ESTIMATING RELIABILITY • Can estimate • Always between 0 and 1 • .x means x percent attributable to true measurement and (1 - .x) is attributable to error TYPES OF RELIABILITY • Test-Retest Reliability • Administer same test twice to same group of individuals with a period of time between the tests • Parallel Forms Reliability • Administer different versions of a test tool TYPES OF RELIABILITY • InterRater Reliability • Assess the degree to which different raters agree on their assessment • Internal Consistency Reliability • Assess consistency of results across items in the test RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY • Can view as continuum ON BEING A SCIENTISTRESPONSIBLE CONDUCT IN RESEARCH Presented by: Raj Parthasarathy CSC 426 Spring 2013 May 28 ON BEING A SCIENTIST- RESPONSIBLE CONDUCT IN RESEARCH ON BEING A SCIENTIST-RESPONSIBLE CONDUCT IN RESEARCH • Free book/lengthy paper written by the ‘Committee on Science, Engineering & Public Policy’, ‘National Academy of Sciences’, ‘National Academy of Engineering’ & ‘Institute of Medicine’ (1995). • A treatise on do and don’ts in scientific research including topics such as allocation of credit, and error and negligence. • Not possible to cover every detail in the available time today. I have selected some of the most interesting and important sections for presentation today. Introduction • What keeps the researcher rooted to their research? • Pursuit of an experience so interesting that one cannot wait to get up every morning and continue to work on it • Ability to associate with colleagues that have made significant contributions to the subject matter • Knowing that their work may have a direct and impact on society • Interest of others in the findings and implications of the research SOCIAL FOUNDATIONS OF SCIENCE • Scientific research is a social enterprise. It cannot be done without drawing on the work of others or collaborating with others. • Of course, the object of research is to extend human knowledge beyond what is already known. • But such contribution enters the domain of science only after it is presented to other researchers so that they can independently judge its validity. HOW IS VALIDITY JUDGED? • Presentation at seminars and conferences, and to scientific journals (which in turn send the papers to be scrutinized by reviewers) • After a paper is published or a finding is presented, it is judged by other scientists in the context of what they already know from their research/knowledge. • After this continuum of discussion and deliberation the ideas of individuals are eventually incorporated into scientific worldview. REVIEWS ARE IMPORTANT • The process of review and revision (when a paper is submitted to a journal, as we all know) is critically important. It minimizes the influence of individual subjectivity by requiring that research results be accepted by other scientists. • Powerful inducement for researchers to be critical of their own conclusions SOME TOPICS WE HAVE DISCUSSED IN CLASS • We have already discussed the following topics in class through presentations, and at length: • Experimental techniques and treatment of data: • moral of the story: methods for the treatment of data and experimental techniques are NOT infallible. Therefore researchers must be careful in this regard, so as to assure the validity of the results. Also need to be forthcoming about the procedures used. Values in Science • Values in science: • Scientists must display curiosity, intuition, creativity • Some religious values, social beliefs and personal beliefs may distort the work of researchers. For example, the field of eugenics used the techniques of science to try to demonstrate the inferiority of certain races. • Despite such cautionary episodes, it is clear that values cannot—and should not—be separated from science. CONFLICTS OF INTEREST • Sometimes values conflict. For example, a particular circumstance might compromise—or appear to compromise— professional judgments. • One example: A scientist might receive a manuscript or proposal to review that discusses work similar to but a step ahead of that being done by the reviewer. • Virtually all institutions that conduct research and many scientific journals have policies and procedures for managing conflicts of interest. These policies and procedures are designed to protect the integrity of the scientific process CONFLICTS ON INTEREST • Interesting case: • John, a third-year graduate student, is participating in a department-wide seminar where students, postdocs, and faculty members discuss work in progress. An assistant professor prefaces her comments by saying that the work she is about to discuss is sponsored by both a federal grant and a biotechnology firm for which she consults. In the course of the talk John realizes that he has been working on a technique that could make a major contribution to the work being discussed. But his faculty advisor consults for a different, and competing, biotechnology firm. • How should John participate in this seminar? • What, if anything, should he say to his advisor-and when? • What implications does this case raise for the traditional openness and sharing of data, materials, and findings that have characterized modern science? PUBLICATION AND OPENNESS • Until the latter half of the seventeenth century. many scientists sought to keep their work secret so that others could not claim it as their own. Prominent figures of the time, including Isaac Newton, were loathe to convey news of their discoveries for fear that someone else would claim priority—a fear that was frequently realized. • Henry Oldenburg (the secretary of the Royal Society of London) won over scientists by guaranteeing rapid publication and also pioneered the practice of sending submitted manuscripts to experts who could judge their quality (today known as the practice of peer review). • First to publish a view or finding, not the first to discover it, tends to get most of the credit for the discovery. Once results are published, they can be freely used by other researchers to extend knowledge. But until the results become common knowledge, people who use them are obliged to recognize the discoverer through citations. In this way scientists are rewarded through peer recognition for making results public. PUBLICATION AND OPENNESS • During the initial stages of research, a scientist deserves a period of privacy in which data are not subject to disclosure.--allows researchers to advance their work to the point at which they have confidence both in its accuracy and its meaning. • After publication, scientists expect that data and other research materials will be shared with qualified colleagues upon request. At this stage, a scientist who is unwilling to share research materials with qualified colleagues runs the risk of not being trusted or respected. • Publication in a peer-reviewed journal remains the standard means of disseminating scientific result. Posters, abstracts, lectures at professional gatherings, and proceedings volumes are being used more often to present preliminary results before full review. • It is inappropriate for a scientist to release important and controversial results directly to the public before submitting them to the scrutiny of peers. If the researcher has made a mistake or the findings are misinterpreted by the media or the public, the scientific community and the public may react adversely. Also, dangers in application. ALLOCATION OF CREDIT • Need to follow the principle of fairness : credit is explicitly acknowledged in three places: in the list of authors, in the acknowledgments, and in the list of references or citations. Conflicts over proper attribution can arise in any of these places. • Failure to cite the work of others can give rise to more than just hard feelings. Citations are part of the reward system of science. They are connected to funding decisions and to the future careers of researchers. • MUST GIVE CREDIT WHERE AND WHEN IT IS DUE. ERROR AND NEGLIGENCE • All scientific results must be treated as susceptible to error—researchers are not perfect. An author who is willing to take credit for a paper must also bear responsibility for its contents (example, when errors are found in the paper). • Even the most responsible scientist can make an honest mistake. When such errors are discovered, they should be acknowledged, preferably in the same journal in which the mistaken information was published. Scientists who make such acknowledgments promptly and openly are rarely condemned by colleagues. • Mistakes made through negligent work are treated more harshly. If scientists cut corners for whatever reason, they are placing their reputation, the work of their colleagues, and the public's confidence in science at risk. (This explains the time it takes to translate research work into a publishable paper in a peer reviewed journal!) MISCONDUCT • Misconduct goes beyond error and negligence—involves deception. • Examples: Making up data or results (fabrication), changing or misreporting data or results (falsification), using the ideas or words of another person without giving appropriate credit (plagiarism) • Misconduct strikes at the heart of the values on which science is based. Anyone who engages in any of these practices is putting his or her scientific career at risk. Even infractions that may seem minor at the time can end up being severely punished. • Misconduct in science is unlikely to remain internal to the scientific community. Its consequences are too extreme: it can harm individuals outside of science (as when falsified results become the basis of a medical treatment), it squanders public funds, and it attracts the attention of those who would seek to criticize science. As a result, federal agencies, Congress, the media, and the courts can all get involved. RESPONDING TO VIOLATIONS OF ETHICAL STANDARDS • Violations of ethical standards must be taken very seriously. • In most cases the best thing to do is to discuss the situation with a trusted friend or advisor. In universities, faculty advisors, department chairs, and other senior faculty can be invaluable sources of advice in deciding whether to go forward with a complaint. Rash complaints without basis can break the career of a sincere researcher. • An important consideration is deciding when to put a complaint in writing. Once in writing, universities are obligated to deal with a complaint in a more formal manner than if it is made verbally. Putting a complaint in writing can have serious consequences for the career of a scientist and should be undertaken only after thorough consideration. • Many universities and other research institutions have designated an ombudsman, ethics officer, or other official who is available to discuss situations involving research ethics. Such discussions are carried out in strictest confidence whenever possible. THE SCIENTIST IN SOCIETY • In addition to everything discussed in this presentation, scientists have additional responsibilities to society. • Even scientists conducting the most fundamental research need to be aware that their work can ultimately have a great impact on society. Construction of the atomic bomb and the development of recombinant DNA two examples of how seemingly arcane areas of science can have tremendous societal consequences. GUIDELINES ON RESEARCH PRACTICE IN COMPUTER SCIENCE Compiled by Justin Zobel, Department of Computer Science, RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia MAY 1999 Presented by: Raj Parthasarathy Introduction • Developed by the major organizations responsible for research and science in Australia, such as the AVCC (The Australian Vice-Chancellor’s Committee) and NHMRC (National Health and Medical Research Council). • Principal topics: Data Storage and Retention, Authorship, Publication, Supervisor of Students, Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest, and Research Misconduct. Introduction • Some topics repeated from what I presented in the previous document presented today (viz. “on being a scientist-responsible conduct in research”). • Therefore in the interests of time I will discuss only those that are new or different and will not repeat what was already discussed. Principal Recommendations • Research workers should only participate in work which conforms to accepted ethical standards and which they are competent to perform. • The minimum requirement for authorship of a publication is participation in conceiving, executing or interpreting a significant part of the outcomes of the research reported. • Supervisors must not publish a student's work without giving appropriate credit (usually authorship) to the student. • Researchers should not referee a paper or examine a thesis where there is a real or perceived conflict of interest, or where there is some reasonable likelihood that it will be difficult for the referee to maintain objectivity. Data storage and retention • 2.1 Data (including electronic data) must be recorded in a durable and appropriately referenced form. • 2.2 The department or research unit must establish procedures for the retention of data and for the keeping of records of data held. • 2.3 Data must be held for sufficient time to allow reference. The minimum period for retention is at least 5 years from the date of publication. • 2.4 Wherever possible, original data must be retained Authorship, Publication & Misconduct • All co-authors of a publication must acknowledge their authorship in writing in a “signed statement of authorship”, which must specify that the have seen the version of the paper submitted for publication. • Publications must include information on the sources of financial support • Researchers shall report relevant negative results as well as positive results. • Interference : A researcher or reviewer shall not intentionally damage any research-related property of another (becomes scientific misconduct) ACM (Association for Computing Machinery) Policy, Code of Ethics and Professional Conduct Guidelines Presented by: Raj Parthasarathy Introduction • Relates to moral imperatives and professional responsibilities that members and researchers may face in their daily lives. • Since many are repeats from the previous two documents presented today, I will discuss only what is new or different (in the interests of time). Moral Imperatives & Professional Responsibilities • Be fair and take action not to discriminate in any manner. • Give proper credit for intellectual property (Specifically, one must not take credit for other's ideas or work, even in cases where the work has not been explicitly protected by copyright, patent, etc.) • Give comprehensive and thorough evaluations of computer systems and their impacts, including analysis of possible risks (especially, any signs of danger from systems must be reported to those who have opportunity and/or responsibility to resolve them). Moral Imperatives & Professional Responsibilities • Improve public understanding of computing and its consequences (Computing professionals have a responsibility to share technical knowledge with the public by encouraging understanding of computing, including the impacts of computer systems and their limitations. This imperative implies an obligation to counter any false views related to computing). • Access computing and communication resources only when authorized to do so (No one should enter or use another's computer system, software, or data files without permission. One must always have appropriate approval before using system resources, including communication ports, file space, other system peripherals, and computer time). Leadership Imperatives & Code Compliance • Articulate social responsibilities of members of an organizational unit and encourage full acceptance of those responsibilities (Research must serve public interest and thus fulfill social responsibility). • Design and build information systems that enhance the quality of working life (When implementing a computer system, organizations must consider the personal and professional development, physical safety, and human dignity of all workers). • Create opportunities for members of the organization to learn the principles and limitations of computer systems Summary & Concluding Comments • Being a researcher is a complex role involving ethics, moral behaviors, and legal and societal responsibilities. • The research system exerts many pressures on beginning and experienced researchers alike. Principal investigators need to raise funds and attract students. Faculty members must balance the time spent on research with the time spent on teaching. Industrial sponsorship of research introduces the possibility of conflicts of interest. • Ultimately, a successful researcher will be one that incorporates a judicious mix of all of the above elements in his/her day to day activities, while at the same time making significant contributions to advance the body of knowledge in his/her chosen field of research. • THANK YOU FOR LISTENING. Record Keeping in Experiments Jinghui Cheng 5/21/2013 • Zobel, J. (1998, February). Reliable research: Towards experimental standards for computer science. In Proceedings of the Australasian Computer Science Conference (pp. 217-229). Springer-Verlag, Perth, Western Australia. An Important Ethical Standard • The Office of Research Integrity – Integrity and Misconduct in Research: Report of the Commission on Research Integrity – http://ori.hhs.gov/images/ddblock/report_commission.pdf • NH&MRC/AVCC – Statement and Guidelines on Research Practice – http://www.nhmrc.gov.au/guidelines/publications/r0024 • American Psychological Association – Record Keeping Guidelines – http://www.apa.org/practice/guidelines/record-keeping.pdf • National Institutes of Health – Guidelines for Scientific Record Keeping in the Intramural Research Program – http://sourcebook.od.nih.gov/ethic-conduct/recordkeeping.pdf NIH Guidelines for Scientific Record Keeping • Elements of Good Laboratory Notes – – – – – – – – – – Who did it (the person making the record). What you did. When you did it (clearly stating month, date and year). Why you did it. What project the research was part of. How you did it (including the methodology). What materials were used. The findings. Your interpretation. The next step. Why Keeping Records? • Fundamental motivations: – It provides evidence of precedent. – It is the only evidence resulting from an experiment. – It can be re-used in the future. • Additional motivations: – rigor, elucidation, reproduction, verification Recordkeeping in Computer Science • Depend on the type of research – Meet the needs of keeping records – Ensure that the records themselves are verifiable • Principle: – To provide corroborating testimonies • Elements: – notebooks, code, logs Workshop • What records to keep when… Workshop • What records to keep when… – Scenario 1: – You proposed a new face recognition algorithm. It could be used in a photo organizing applications (e.g. iPhoto) to group photos by person. You plan to use 1,000 images to evaluate your algorithm against two baseline ones. You want to compare their efficiency (run time) and effectiveness (false positive and false negative error rates). Workshop • What records to keep when… – Scenario 2: – You are evaluating the usability of cellphone keyboards. You plan to ask 30 participants to input a sentence on iPhone and a Blackberry phone (counterbalanced). You want to compare the efficiency (time on task), effectiveness (error rate), and user satisfaction (using a questionnaire) of two kinds of keyboards. Special Issues with Human Subjects • The Belmont Report – Respect for Persons • “the requirement to acknowledge autonomy and the requirement to protect those with diminished autonomy” – Beneficence • “(1) do not harm and (2) maximize possible benefits and minimize possible harms.” – Justice • “equals ought to be treated equally” • http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/policy/belmont.html Special Issues with Human Subjects • When Keeping Records – Be careful about privacy and confidentiality • Informed Consent – Illinois law prohibits the recording of conversations without the consent of everyone who is a party to the conversation • Institutional Review Board (IRB) – A review committee established to help protect the rights and welfare of human research subjects – http://research.depaul.edu/IRB/IRB_Home.html Thank you!! • Questions? • Comments?