Recent Developments in Assessment Mid South Conference on Psychology in the Schools, Huntsville, AL October 23, 2012 JEROME M. SATTLER Copyright © 2012 Jerome M. Sattler, Publisher, Inc. There was no respect for youth when I was young, and now that I am old, there is no respect for age - I missed it coming and going. ~J.B. Priestly Old age is fifteen years older than I am. ~Oliver Wendell Holmes Men do not quit playing because they grow old; they grow old because they quit playing. ~Oliver Wendell Holmes Question: What are three examples of Marine life? Answer: 1. Marching 2. Barracks inspection 3. Running the obstacle course Question: Where is the Milky Way located? Answer: In the checkout aisle next to the rest of the candy bars Question: What is a terminal illness? Answer: When you are sick at the airport. Question: Give three geological names for rocks. Answer: Classic rock, hard rock, and acid rock, and I also know fourth one, soft rock. Question: What is water composed of? Answer: Two gins—Oxygin and hydrogin. Oxygin is pure gin, while hydrogen is gin and water. Proof Reading and Spelling Gafs Kids Make Nutritious Snacks Juvenile Court to Try Shooting Defendant New Study of Obesity Looks for Larger Test Group Proof Reading and Spelling Gafs Panda Mating Fails; Veterinarian Takes Over Hospitals are Sued by Seven Foot Doctors Proof Reading and Spelling Gafs Please excuse ray friday from school. He has very loose vowels. Please excuse Jason for being absent yesterday. He had a cold and could not breed well. Proof Reading and Spelling Gafs Please excuse mary for being absent yesterday. She was in bed with gramps. Please exkuce lisa for being absent she was sick and i had her shot. My son is under a doctor's care and should not take PE today. Please execute him. America’s Children, 2009-2010 [1] Demographic Background N (Millions) Percent Euro American 39.6 53.5 African American 10.4 14.0 Hispanic American 17.1 23.1 Asian American 3.2 4.3 Other 3.9 5.2 America’s Children, 2009-2010 [2] N Economic Well-Being (Millions) Percent Children in poverty 16.3 22.0 Parents unemployed 24.4 33.0 High cost of housing 30.4 41.0 -- 9.0 Teens not in school and not working America’s Children, 2009-2010 [3] Education Not attending preschool Percent 53.0 Not proficient in reading, 4th graders 68.0 Not proficient in math, 8th graders 66.0 High school graduates not graduating on time 24.0 America’s Children, 2009-2010 [4] Health N Percent Low birthweight babies -- 8.2 Without health insurance Child and teen deaths 5.9 million 27 per 100,000 -- 8.0 Teens who abuse alcohol or drugs -7.0 America’s Children, 2009-2010 [5] Family & Social Environment N (Millions) Percent In single-parent family 25.2 34.0 Head of household lacks high school diploma Living in high-poverty areas 11.1 15.0 8.2 11.0 America’s Children, 2009-2010 [6] Family & Social Environment Teen births Child maltreatment Under age 1 Ages 1-3 Ages 4-7 Ages 8-11 Ages 12-15 Ages 16-17 N (per 1,000) 39 21 12 11 9 8 6 America’s Children, 2009-2010 [7] Source: Annie E. Casey Foundation. (2012). 2012 Kids count data book. Retrieved from http://datacenter.kidscount.org/DataBook/2 012/OnlineBooks/KIDSCOUNT2012DataB ookFullReport.pdf America’s Children, 2009-2010 [8] Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. (2011). America’s children: Key national indicators of wellbeing, 2011. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved from http://www.childstats.gov/pdf/ac2011/ac_1 1.pdf M.H. and J.H vs. Monroe-Woodbury Central School District [1] M.H. and J.H., on their own behalf and on behalf of their daughter L.H., Plaintiffs-Appellees, -v.MONROE-WOODBURY CENTRAL SCHOOL DISTRICT, Defendant-Appellant. No. 07-1571-cv UNITED STATES COURT OF APPEALS FOR THE SECOND CIRCUIT 2008 U.S. App. LEXIS 21299 October 7, 2008, Decided M.H. and J.H vs. Monroe-Woodbury Central School District [2] ISSUE IN CASE Does L.H. need a residential program? Parents say “Yes” School says “No” M.H. and J.H vs. Monroe-Woodbury Central School District [3] APPELLATE COURT'S REASONING Administrative record reveals that the child was progressing, not regressing, in the day-program at the Summit School. Not only do her grades reflect that she was achieving academically, but reports from certified counselors demonstrate that she was making improvements in her social and emotional problems as well. M.H. and J.H vs. Monroe-Woodbury Central School District [4] APPELLATE COURT'S REASONING (Cont.) Moreover, none of several psychological reports suggested that, in order to advance academically, the child needed a residential program to deal with her emotional problems. M.H. and J.H vs. Monroe-Woodbury Central School District [5] APPELLATE COURT'S REASONING (Cont.) We find no substantial evidence to reverse the findings of the administrative hearing officers. We have considered all of the parties' arguments and examined the administrative record. For the foregoing reasons, the judgment of the District Court is REVERSED. Forest Grove School District v. T.A. Supreme Court Case (2009) [1] Court ruled that the parents of a student with a disability were entitled to private school tuition reimbursement T.A. had not been identified with a disability Or previously provided with special education services District multidisciplinary team had not identified T.A.’s disability Forest Grove School District v. T.A. Supreme Court Case (2009) [2] Only a limited psychoeducational evaluation was conducted Evaluation did not assess all areas of suspected disability Parents sought an independent comprehensive evaluation Comprehensive psychoeducational evaluation led to more targeted interventions in his private school placement Forest Grove School District v. T.A. Supreme Court Case (2009) [3] T.A. performed better in private school after comprehensive evaluation Case highlights the need for school-based teams to conduct comprehensive evaluations in all areas of suspected disability Forest Grove School District v. T.A. Supreme Court Case (2009) [4] SOURCE Dixon, S. G., Eusebio, E. C., Turton, W. J., Wright, P. W. D., & Hale, J. B. (2011). Forest Grove School District v. T.A. Supreme Court Case: Implications for school psychology practice. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(2), 103–113. doi: 10.1177/0734282910388598 Forest Grove School District v. T.A., 129 S. Ct. 2484 (2009). OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [1] To: State Directors of Special Education From: Melody Musgrove, director Subject: Response to Intervention (RTI) and possible delay of an evaluation under IDEA http://www.rti4success.org/images/stories/ RTI%20Memo_1-21-11r.pdf OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [2] IMPLEMENTING IDEA 2004 Identification of all children with disabilities residing in the State must occur in timely manner No procedures should result in delaying or denying identification Reports indicate local school districts may be using RTI strategies to delay or deny evaluation for students suspected of having a disability OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [3] IMPLEMENTING IDEA 2004 (Cont.) States must examine their procedures to ensure that school districts implementing RTI do not delay or deny timely evaluations and services to students suspected of having a disability If Local Educational Authority (LEA) denies request for an initial evaluation it must provide written notice to parents explaining why OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [4] IMPLEMENTING IDEA 2004 (Cont.) Parent can challenge this decision by requesting a due process hearing OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [5] MEMO DESCRIBES RTI A multi-tiered instructional framework Schoolwide approach that addresses the needs of all students Integrates assessment and intervention within a multi-level instructional and behavioral system OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [6] MEMO DESCRIBES RTI (Cont.) Aims to maximize student achievement and reduce problem behaviors Schools identify students at-risk for poor learning outcomes Monitor student progress Provide evidence-based interventions OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [7] MEMO DESCRIBES RTI (Cont.) Adjust the intensity and nature of those interventions depending on a student’s responsiveness Children who do not respond to RTI and are potentially eligible for special education and related services are referred for evaluation OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [8] MEMO DESCRIBES RTI (Cont.) Thus, those children who simply need intense short-term interventions are provided those interventions Information obtained through RTI may also be used as a component of evaluations for children suspected of having disabilities other than SLD, if appropriate OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [9] OTHER MEMO POINTS The regulations at 34 CFR §300.301 (b) allow parent to request an initial evaluation at any time to determine if a child is a child with a disability RTI cannot be used to delay or deny the provision of a full and individual evaluation OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [10] OTHER MEMO POINTS (Cont.) LEA must seek parental consent within a reasonable period of time after the referral for evaluation, if the LEA agrees that an initial evaluation is needed An LEA must conduct the initial evaluation within 60 days of receiving parental consent for the evaluation or, OSEP’s Memo Dated: January 21, 2011 [11] OTHER MEMO POINTS (Cont.) If the State establishes a timeframe within which the evaluation must be conducted, within that timeframe An LEA cannot reject a referral and delay provision of an initial evaluation on the basis that a child has not participated in an RTI framework Legal Implications of RTI [1] By Jose L. Martin, Attorney at Law, Austin, TX At what point should schools suspect that students who are struggling with the curriculum while receiving regular education interventions might actually have a specific learning disability? Legal Implications of RTI [2] How long should a student receive regular interventions before a school initiates an IDEA evaluation? Is the child-find obligation triggered if a child moves through tiers of interventions with some improvement, but nevertheless continues to show deficits in achievement? Legal Implications of RTI [3] How should schools handle parents’ requests for evaluations when interventions have only just been initiated and/or appear to show promise? How can schools avoid failure-to-identify IDEA hearing claims while attempting to make best use of regular education interventions prior to a referral? Legal Implications of RTI [4] SUGGESTED SOLUTIONS Meeting with parents to discuss intervention options, agreed timelines, and available courses of action Making clear to parents their right to request an IDEA evaluation and providing written notice of IDEA procedural safeguards Legal Implications of RTI [5] SUGGESTED SOLUTIONS (Cont.) Reaching a consensus on a course of action in a collaborative manner If the consensus decision is to pursue regular education interventions, sharing progress data frequently with parents Legal Implications of RTI [6] SUGGESTED SOLUTIONS (Cont.) Initiating follow-up communication regarding progress or lack thereof Convening follow-up meetings to review progress and renew consensus on current course of action Documenting the steps above Legal Implications of RTI [7] SOURCE: http://www.rtinetwork.org/learn/ld/legalimplications-of-response-to-interventionand-special-education-identification NEW FINDINGS ON CHILD WELL-BEING AND LIFE EXPECTANCY Child Abuse Spikes as U.S. Economy Flounders [1] Some families driven to the brink because of recession. Child-protection agencies (CPA) overwhelmed. Parents can’t pay for needed drugs. Referrals to CPA rose 30% in first two months of 2009. Child Abuse Spikes as U.S. Economy Flounders [2] SOURCE Szep, J. (2009). Child abuse spikes as U.S. economy flounders. Retrieved from: http://www.reuters.com/article/domesticNe ws/idUSTRE53F00Y20090416 Life Expectancy Tied To Education [1] Life expectancy is 82 for individuals with more than 12 years of education Life expectancy is 75 for individuals with 12 or fewer years of education. Life Expectancy Tied To Education [2] Possible Reasons Those with less education: Are likely to have more smoking-related diseases, such as lung cancer and emphysema—35% of Americans with an 9th to 11th grade education smoke, while only 7% with a graduate degree smoke Are likely to have lower incomes Life Expectancy Tied To Education [3] Possible Reasons (Cont.) Are likely to live in areas that have their own health threats, either through crime or poor housing conditions Are likely to have limited health insurance and limited access to health services Are more likely to agree with the statement: “It doesn't matter if I wear a seat belt, because if it’s my time to die, I'll die.” Life Expectancy Tied To Education [4] Summary and Recommendations The less affluent and less educated are also, invariably, less healthy. Disparities in health are a major challenge in the United States. Health is not a product of health care per se, but of one's life course and opportunities. Life Expectancy Tied To Education [5] Summary and Recommendations (Cont.) The less educated must learn the following: “It does matter. Life is uncertain, but that's no reason to surrender to fate.” Fighting poverty and improving education are keys to increasing life expectancy among less-advantaged Americans. Life Expectancy Tied To Education [6] SOURCE Meara, E. R., Richards, S., & Cutler, D. M. (2008). The gap gets bigger: Changes in mortality and life expectancy, by education, 1981–2000. Health Affairs, 27, 350–360. Multimethod Assessment [Cognitive, pp. 8-9] [1] Obtain information about the child’s medical, developmental, academic, familial, and social history Obtain information about the child’s current cognitive, academic, behavioral, social, and interpersonal functioning Multimethod Assessment [Cognitive, pp. 8-9] [2] Determine the child’s cognitive, academic, and social strengths and weaknesses Understand the nature, presence, and degree of any disabling conditions that the child might have Multimethod Assessment [Cognitive, pp. 8-9] [3] Cross-validate impressions provided by multiple informants Determine the conditions that inhibit or support the acquisition of appropriate skills Obtain baseline information prior to the implementation of an intervention program Multimethod Assessment [Cognitive, pp. 8-9] [4] Develop useful instructional programs Guide students in selecting educational and vocational programs Monitor cognitive, academic, or social changes in the child (and in the family, school, and community as needed) Measure the effectiveness of interventions Students Ages 6 to 21 Served Under IDEA in 2008-2009 [1] Disability N % 2,476,000 40.4 1,426,000 23.3 478,000 7.8 Emotional disturbance 420,000 6.9 Multiple disabilities 130,000 2.1 Hearing impairments 78,000 1.3 Specific learning disabilities Speech or language impairments Intellectual disability Students Ages 6 to 21 Served Under IDEA in 2008-2009 [2] Disability N % Orthopedic impairments 70,000 1.1 Other health impairments 659,000 10.7 Visual impairments 29,000 0.5 Autism 336,000 5.5 Traumatic brain injury 26,000 0.4 Students Ages 6 to 21 Served Under IDEA in 2008-2009 [3] SOURCE U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics (2011). Digest of Education Statistics, 2010 (NCES 2011-015). Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id =64 IDEA 2008-2009 Five Highest Disability Categories: Learning disabilities: 40.4% Speech/language impairments: 23.3% Other health impairments: 10.7% Intellectual disability: 7.8% Emotional disturbance: 6.9% IDEA 2004 Student Graduation Rates Graduate with regular high school diploma: 54.5% Dropped out: 31.1% Other: 14.4% (includes certificate of completion, reached maximum age, or died) IDEA 2004 Student Graduation Rates Other 14% Dropped out 31% Graduate with regular high school diploma 55% IDEA: Full and Appropriate Individual Initial Evaluation [RG p. 247-1] Multiple assessment tools and strategies Multiple types of information Multiple criteria Technically sound instruments IDEA: Full and Appropriate Individual Initial Evaluation [RG p. 247-2] Nondiscriminatory assessment procedures Trained and knowledgeable personnel Consideration of sensory, manual, and speaking skills Comprehensive coverage Ambiguous Components of IDEA Fails to clearly define what assessment procedures to be selected and administered so as not to be ethnically or culturally discriminatory Fails to specify any acceptable or unacceptable assessment procedures Comment on IDEA [RG p. 266-267-1] The ultimate measure of IDEA: The quality of the education received by each child with a disability How the education is used to benefit the child and society How society integrates individuals with disabilities into the mainstream of daily living Assessment of English Language Learners (ELL) [1] Assessment Considerations Assess both languages Uncritical use of translated tests is not recommended Assistance of a trained linguistic or cultural broker can be helpful Assessment of ELL [2] Assessment Considerations (Cont.) Children may not be fully culturally adapted Norm- or criterion-referencing do not apply for translated tests Examiner may not be fluently bilingual in both English and in the child’s language Trained interpreters and/or cultural brokers from the child’s background may be unavailable Assessment of ELL [3] Assessment Considerations (Cont.) Little documentation exists on the typical and atypical course of development of most foreign languages in the US Child’s native language abilities may be weaker than those of children in the home country because of language attrition Assessment of ELL [4] Assessment Considerations (Cont.) Diversity of minority languages spoken in America underlies most of the limiting factors Assessment of ELL [5] Test Accommodations Test translation Linguistic modification (a.k.a. simplified English) Providing bilingual or customized dictionaries Adding glossaries to the margins of test booklets to define specific words Extended testing time Assessment of ELL [6] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions Has the accommodation changed the construct measured? Are scores from accommodated tests comparable to scores from the standard version? Assessment of ELL [7] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions (Cont.) Do the scores from accommodated tests provide more accurate measurement of ELLs’ knowledge and skills relative to scores from the standard test? Does the accommodation provide an unfair advantage to ELLs? Do items function differentially across ELL and non-ELL groups? Assessment of ELL [8] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions (Cont.) Does linguistic simplification affect the construct validity of the test? Do original and translated items function differentially? Is the factor structure equivalent across standard and accommodated test administrations? Assessment of ELL [9] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions (Cont.) Is the factor structure consistent across ELL and non-ELL groups of students? Is the predictive utility of an assessment consistent across ELL and non-ELL groups? Do test accommodations lead to improved scores for ELLs relative to non-ELLs? Assessment of ELL [10] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions (Cont.) Do ELL and non-ELL students use the same processes in responding to test items? Do ELL and non-ELL students differ with respect to the time needed to answer test items? Assessment of ELL [11] Accommodations Raise Validity Questions (Cont.) Do teachers of ELL and non-ELL students have the same opinions of standardized tests? Have state-mandated assessments differentially affected instruction for ELLs and non-ELLs? Assessment of ELL Children [14] Sources Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Duncan, T. S. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002. Assessment of ELL Children [15] Sources Sireci, S. G., Han, K. T., & Wells, C. S. (2008). Methods for evaluating the validity of test scores for English language learners. Educational Assessment, 13(2– 3), 108–131. doi:10.1080/10627190802394255. Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-1) Studied 1,500 Mexican-Americans in Los Angeles and San Antonio from 1960 to 2000 Results Nearly every one spoke and read English by the second generation, though they remained bilingual Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-2) Later generations were more likely to become Protestants, vote Republican, and marry non-Latinos 3/4 of immigrants liked Mexican music best, while 1/2 of later generations preferred Black American music Mexican-American neighborhoods are more segregated in 2000 than in 1960 because of the influx of immigrants Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-3) Third generation Mexican-Americans’ income continues to lag behind EuroAmericans Loss of middle-class manufacturing jobs Prejudice fueled by immigration debate Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-4) Third generation Mexican-Americans’ income continues to lag behind EuroAmericans (Cont.) Subpar school systems Years of education rose substantially for children of immigrants, but high-school graduation rates actually decreased slightly by the fourth generation Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-5) Recommendations To reverse the slide, we need an education-focused “Marshall Plan” to boost school spending. Without it, too many Mexican-Americans may be running in place for generations to come Generations of Exclusion: Hispanic Americans (Telles & Ortiz, 2008-6) Sources Telles, E., & Ortiz, V. (2008). Generations of exclusion: Mexican Americans, assimilation, and race. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Adapted from Newsweek, March 24, 2008, p. 15. Pollution Lowers IQ [1] Research by Perera et al. (2009) reported that children of mothers’ exposed to high levels of environmental pollutants during pregnancy have a four-point drop in their IQs by age 5 The exposure was to substances called polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs), a by-product of the incomplete burning of gas, diesel, oil, and coal. Pollution Lowers IQ [2] An earlier report found that higher prenatal exposure to PAHs is associated with lower weight and smaller head size at birth and developmental delays at age 3. The developing fetal brain is particularly vulnerable to neurotoxic chemicals and exposure to pollution could cause direct genetic damage. Pollution Lowers IQ [3] We need to reduce these dangerous emissions by, for example, reducing diesel truck idling and requiring cleaner fuels. Pollution Lowers IQ [4] SOURCE Perera, F. P., Li, Z., Whyatt, R., Hoepner, L., Wang, S., Camann, D., & Rauh, V. (2009). Prenatal airborne polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon exposure and child IQ at age 5 years. Pediatrics, 124(2), e195-e202. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3506 Assessment Strategy [1] Note the presence of any symptoms Note the number, type, severity, and duration of any symptoms Note the situations in which any symptoms are displayed Assessment Strategy [2] Evaluate verbal abilities, nonverbal abilities, short- and long-term memory abilities, other cognitive abilities, personality, motor skills, perceptual skills, and adaptive behaviors skills Note presence of any co-occurring disorders Note if there are any educational and instructional needs listed in educational files Assessment Strategy [3] Inspect cumulative school records, including attendance history, reports of behavioral problems, school grades, standardized test scores, number of schools attended, and other relevant information Review relevant medical information Interview parents, teachers, and the child Assessment Strategy [4] Observe child’s behavior in the classroom and on the playground (and at home if possible) Administer rating scales to parents, teachers, and a child who can read Administer a battery of psychological tests Behavioral Observation [1] Electronic Coding Software !Observe http://www.psycsoft.com/products.shtml BASC-2 Portable Observation Program including Student Observation System (SOS) http://www.pearsonassessments.com/HAI WEB/Cultures/enus/Productdetail.htm?Pid=paa38206 Behavioral Observation [2] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools™ (BOSS™) http://www.pearsonassessments.com/NR/r donlyres/5439724A-BE33-487E-AECA09F3F8BC923E/0/BOSS_BW_SR.pdf BehaviorLENS http://itunes.apple.com/us/app/behaviorlen s/id459755410?mt=8 Behavioral Observation [3] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) Ecobehavioral Assessment Systems Software (EBASS) http://www.jgcp.ku.edu/products/EBASS/ eCOVE Observer http://www.ecove.net/home/eco/smartlist_ 228 Behavioral Observation [4] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) iBAA (Behavioral Assessment Application) http://itunes.apple.com/us/app/ibaa/id3837 05019?mt=8 INTERACT http://www.mangoldinternational.com/en/software/interact/wha t-is-interact.html Behavioral Observation [5] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) iSOFT (Mobility Suite Recording Clinical Observations on iPhone/iPod Touch) http://www.isofthealth.com/enAU/Newsroom/Multimedia.aspx?yt=%7BF DFF0D04-DF55-4B0D-AE5E6FAFEEC45378%7D MOOSES™ (Multi-Option Observation System for Experimental Studies) http://mooses.vueinnovations.com Behavioral Observation [6] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) NoBox (TimerData Observation/Evaluation Software) http://noboxinc.com/timer_data/index.html Praxis Behavioral Observation Software (PBOS) http://www.sbir.gov/sbirsearch/detail/2796 00 Behavioral Observation [7] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) Scribe 4 http://img.uoregon.edu/scribe/html/images/ Scribe_Manual.pdf SOTO (Student On-Task Observation) http://itunes.apple.com/us/app/sotostudent-on-taskobservation/id428809608?mt=8 Behavioral Observation [8] Electronic Coding Software (Cont.) The Observer® XT http://www.noldus.com/human-behaviorresearch/products/the-observer-xt Behavioral Observation [9] Pencil-and-paper vs. Electronic Recording Methods Advice from Bakeman & Quera (2012): Behavioral Observation [10] Pencil-and-paper vs. Electronic Recording Methods (Cont.) It is the usual trade-off: electronic recording methods provide richer data, more analytic options, less tedious coding, fewer clerical errors, more tasks automated—as well as greater expense, longer learning times, and more resources devoted to maintenance. Behavioral Observation [11] Pencil-and-paper vs. Electronic Recording Methods (Cont.) As always, the right recording system is the one that matches resources with needs, and when simpler, less precise data are sufficient to answer key research questions, simple and inexpensive may be best. (p. 215, with changes in notation) Behavioral Observation [12] Source Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (2012). Behavioral observation. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.) , APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol 1: Foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics (pp. 207– 225). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Ratings in Observation Anchoring the frequency descriptions to percentages of time. Example: 1 Almost never (0% to 15% of the time) 2 Rarely (16% to 35% of the time) 3 Occasionally (36% to 65% of the time) 4 Frequently (66% to 85% of the time) 5 Almost always (86% to 100% of the time) Observing Problem Behavior [1] Does the student’s behavior significantly differ from that of his/her classmates? Does the student’s behavior lessen the possibility of successful learning for the student and others? Have past efforts to address the student’s behavior using standard interventions been unsuccessful? Observing Problem Behavior [2] Does the student’s behavior represent a behavioral deficit or excess, rather than a cultural difference? Is the student’s behavior serious, persistent, chronic, or a threat to the safety of the student or others? If the behavior persists, is some disciplinary action likely to result? Observing Problem Behavior [3] Times when the problem behavior occurs Location of the problem behavior Conditions when the problem behavior occurs Individuals present when the problem behavior occurs Conditions that typically occur before the problem behavior Observing Problem Behavior [4] Conditions that typically occur after the problem behavior Setting events Peer behaviors associated with the problem behavior Observing Problem Behavior [5] Functional Interview with Teacher “In what settings and under what conditions do you observe the behavior?” “Are there any settings/situations in which the behavior does not occur?” “Who is present when the behavior occurs?” “What activities or interactions take place just prior to the behavior?” Observing Problem Behavior [6] Functional Interview with Teacher (Cont.) “What activities or interactions usually take place immediately following the behavior?” “Are there other behaviors that occur along with the problem behavior?” “Can you think of any reasons why Mandy might behave this way?” “What would be a more acceptable way for the student to achieve the same outcome?” Observing Problem Behavior [7] Functional Interview with Student “Is there anything that is happening outside of school lately that bothers you?” “Is there something new that is happening to you?” “Does it bother you?” “What was the lesson about that was being taught right before you made the comments that made your classroom laugh?” Observing Problem Behavior [8] Functional Interview with Student (Cont.) “What was your teacher doing?” “Do you remember what were you thinking right before you made the comments?” “How do you feel about getting homework in science?” “Can you tell me what Mr. Smith was expecting of you during science class?” Observing Problem Behavior [9] Functional Interview with Student (Cont.) “When you make the funny comments in class, what usually happens afterward?” “How does that make you feel?” Observing Problem Behavior [10] Source The information for Observing Problem Behavior came from the Center for Effective Collaboration and Practice, pp. 6, 7, Appendix D; most of the material has been quoted directly from these pages. Observing Problem Behavior [11] Source Centers for Collaboration and Practice. (1998). Addressing student problem behavior—Part II: Conducting a functional behavioral assessment. Retrieved from http://cecp.air.org/fba/problembehavior2/Fun ctional%20Analysis.PDF Self-Monitoring Assessment [1] Step 1: Check whether the child can read at a level needed to fill out the selfmonitoring form Step 2: Focus on a single target behavior Step 3: Select a positive target behavior rather than a negative one, if possible Step 4: Give the child a clear and simple definition of the target behavior Self-Monitoring Assessment [2] Step 5: Give the child clear and simple instructions on how to monitor and record the target behavior Step 6: Instruct the child to record the target behavior as soon as possible after its occurrence rather than waiting until the end of the day Step 7: Ask the child to define the target behavior Self-Monitoring Assessment [3] Step 8: Demonstrate the self-monitoring procedure by using the recording form or device, modeling the recording procedure, and discussing potential problems Step 9: Role-play the self-monitoring procedure Step 10: Ask the child if he or she has any questions about the self-monitoring procedure Self-Monitoring Assessment [4] Step 11: Conduct several trials to see whether the child understands and can carry out the self-monitoring procedure Step 12: Conduct accuracy checks randomly and inform the child that you will be doing so Step 13: Give the child reinforcements contingent on recording accurate data Self-Monitoring Assessment [5] Sources Korotitsch, W. J., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1999). An overview of self-monitoring research in assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 11(4), 415– 425. Self-Monitoring Assessment [6] Sources Mace, F. C., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1988). Self-monitoring. In J. C. Witt, S. N. Elliott, & F. M. Gresham. (Eds.), Handbook of behavior therapy in education (pp. 489– 522). New York, NY: Plenum. Computer Based Assessment and Future Directions [1] Some assessment tasks are best performed with computer-only assessment. For example, when you want information about response times in milliseconds or when you must administer a stimulus at a specific time, computer-only testing is optimal. Computer Based Assessment and Future Directions [2] Another promising area for use of computer-only test administration is the Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics (ANAM), which may provide measures of continuous performance, learning, memory, logical reasoning, spatial processing, mathematical processing, and visuospatial discrimination. Computer Based Assessment and Future Directions [3] Some practitioners use email to send and receive interview data, background information, and assessment information for computer-based processing. Computer-generated reports raise many legal, ethical, clinical, professional, and philosophical issues. Computer records, like all assessment records, must be kept confidential. Computer Based Assessment and Future Directions [4] Clinical professionals need to establish procedures for determining who has access to computer equipment, where and how information is stored, and whether reports will be sent by email to other parties. Guidelines are still evolving along with computer technology. Scatter and Validity of WISC-IV FSIQ [1] Mark H. Daniel’s Research Fiorello et al. (2007) and Hale et al. (2007) contend that FSIQ should not be interpreted when index scores are diverse Simulation methodology used to investigate index score scatter Results showed that FSIQ is equally valid at all levels of scatter FSIQ can be interpreted in populations with variable index-score profiles Scatter and Validity of WISC-IV FSIQ [2] SOURCES Fiorello, C. A., Hale, J. B., Holdnack, J. A., Kavanagh, J. A., Terrell, J., & Long, L. (2007). Interpreting intelligence test results for children with disabilities: Is global intelligence relevant? Applied Neuropsychology, 14(1), 2–12. Scatter and Validity of WISC-IV FSIQ [3] SOURCES Hale, J. B., Fiorello, C. A., Kavanagh, J. A., Holdnack, J. A., & Aloe, A. M. (2007). Is the demise of IQ interpretation justified? A response to special issue authors. Applied Neuropsychology, 14(1), 37–51. Scatter and Validity of WISC-IV FSIQ [4] SOURCES Daniel, M. H. (2007). ‘Scatter’ and the construct validity of FSIQ: Comment on Fiorello et al. (2007). Applied Neuropsychology, 14(4), 291–295. WISC–IV and Traumatic Brain Injury [1] Allen, Thaler, Donohue, & Mayfield (2010) N = 61 Ages 6 to 16 yrs., M = 12.1 yrs. FSIQ = 85 VC = 88 PR = 92 WM = 90 PS = 82 WISC–IV and Traumatic Brain Injury [2] SOURCE Allen, D. N., Thaler, N. S., Donohue, B., & Mayfield, J. (2010). WISC–IV profiles in children with traumatic brain injury: Similarities to and differences from the WISC–III. Psychological Assessment, Vol 22(1), 57–64. doi: 10.1037/a0016056 WISC–IV and Academic Achievement [1] Mayes & Calhoun (2007) N = 92, ADHD and LD Ages 6 to 16 yrs. FSIQ = 108 WISC–IV and Academic Achievement [2] WIAT-II and FSIQ (Mayes & Calhoun, 2007) Word Reading r = .63 Reading Comprehension r = .64 Numerical Operations r = .76 Written Expression r = .54 WISC–IV and Academic Achievement [3] WISC-IV Indexes and WIAT-II Subtests* (Mayes & Calhoun, 2007) VCI r = .43 to .64 PRI r = .38 to .62 WMI r = .53 to .64 PSI r = .33 to .52 *Word Reading, Reading Comprehension, Numerical Operations, and Written Expression WISC–IV and Academic Achievement [4] SOURCE Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2007). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children– Third and –Fourth Edition predictors of academic achievement in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, Vol 22(2), 234–249. doi: 10.1037/10453830.22.2.234 Executive Functions in Children [1] Executive Functions (EF) Allow Child to (Baron, 2004): Perceive stimuli from his or her environment Respond adaptively Flexibly change direction Anticipate future goals Executive Functions in Children [2] EF Allow Child to (Baron, 2004) (Cont.): Consider consequences Respond in an integrated or commonsense way Utilize all of the above to serve a common purposive goal Executive Functions in Children [3] SOURCE Baron, I. S. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the child. New York, NY: Oxford University Press Executive Functions in Children [4] Primary Cognitive Abilities Involved in EF Planning and goal setting Organizing Prioritizing Working memory Shifting or mental flexibility Inhibition Self-regulation Executive Functions in Children [5] Working Memory vs. Short-Term Memory Working memory— structures and processes used for temporarily storing and manipulating information Short-term memory—storage of information without involving the manipulation or organization of material held in memory Updating—addition or subtraction of information from working memory Executive Functions in Children [6] Intelligence and EF Relationship between intelligence and EF not clear Overall, intelligence and EF measures overlap in some areas but not in others Working memory appears to be related both intelligence and EF Executive Functions in Children [7] EF and Academic Tasks 1.Written essay: Planning and defining the first step Rephrasing and paraphrasing the written work (cognitive flexibility) Organization and prioritizing (crafting a complete thought) Executive Functions in Children [8] EF and Academic Tasks (Cont.) 2.Independent studying, homework, and long-term projects: Planning ahead (time management) Acquiring materials and information Setting long-term goals (completing tasks) Self-regulation (sequencing information) Executive Functions in Children [9] EF and Academic Tasks (Cont.) 2.Independent studying, homework, and long-term projects: Self-monitoring (remembering to submit completed assignments by a specific time) Cognitive flexibility (ability to modify how one goes about doing projects). Executive Functions in Children [10] EF and Academic Tasks (Cont.) 3.Test taking: Prioritizing and focusing on relevant themes Managing time Allocating sufficient time to answer all questions on the test Completing the test within the time limits. Executive Functions in Children [11] EF and Academic Tasks (Cont.) 4. Higher grades: Coordinate and integrate multiple skills independently Executive Functions in Children [12] How EF Are Compromised Attention difficulties Fatigue Anxiety Stress Depression Motivational deficits Executive Functions in Children [13] Children with Disabilities ADHD—deficits in attention and inhibitory control Learning disabilities—problems with selfregulation, problem solving, cognitive flexibility, and organization Autistic spectrum disorder—deficits in planning ability, flexibility, responding to social cues, and regulating social interactions Executive Functions in Children [14] EF and Frontal Lobes Associated but also dependent on the links with others areas of the brain Involve a network of brain structures and not exclusively localized in the frontal lobes Executive Functions in Children [15] Assessment of EF Tests cannot measure completely: Planning Problem-solving Organizational ability Self-regulation Scores on tests of EF may give too static a picture of the child’s functioning Executive Functions in Children [16] Assessment of EF (Cont.) Assessment Techniques Interviewing the child Interviewing the child’s parents Interviewing the child’s teachers Observing the child at school, in the test situation, at home, and other settings Administering appropriate tests of EF Executive Functions in Children [17] Assessment of EF (Cont.) Assessment Techniques Analyzing task demands and how the child meets them Analyzing child’s completed home work assignments Analyzing the child’s test performance Using testing-of-limits procedures Executive Functions in Children [18] Note of Caution 1. Complexity of EF Tasks (van der Sluis, de Jong, & van der Leij (2007): Executive function tasks tend to be complex and multi-cognitive in nature May be difficult to know which function(s) is(are) impaired or not impaired Executive Functions in Children [19] SOURCE van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.001 Executive Functions in Children [20] Note of Caution (Cont.): 2. Reliability of EF Tasks (Friedman & Miyake, 2004): Reliability may suffer when children gain experience with tasks Tasks most valid when novel Executive Functions in Children [21] SOURCE Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control processes: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. doi:10.1037/0096-3445-133.1.101 Executive Functions in Children [22] Note of Caution (Cont.): 3. Validity of BRIEF (Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2009): BRIEF scores may not correspond to performance on EF tasks measuring the same area Respondents’ knowledge about child may be imperfect Executive Functions in Children [23] SOURCE Toplak, M., Bucciarelli, S., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2009). Executive functions: Performance–based measures and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 15(1), 53–72. doi:10.1080/09297040802070929 Executive Functions in Children [24] Interviewing an Older Age Child Do you plan events in advance? Tell me more about that. Can you start and stop a conversation appropriately? Tell me more about that. Do you adjust your voice depending on the setting? Tell me more about that. Executive Functions in Children [25] Interviewing an Older Age Child (Cont.) Do you adjust the topic of conversation depending on the setting? Tell me more about that. What is your ability to initiate activities such as going out of the house? How do you arrange things so that you get to school on time? Executive Functions in Children [26] Interviewing an Older Age Child (Cont.) How well do you express your emotions? Tell me more about that. Do you think that you have a full range of emotions or just a few emotions? . . . Tell me more about that. What future plans do you have? Tell me about how your room is organized. Executive Functions in Children [27] Observation in the Classroom Has materials ready at the beginning of a lesson Begins and stops working appropriately Switches from one task to another task appropriately Recognizes that another children’s feelings and ideas are equally important Executive Functions in Children [28] Observation in the Classroom (Cont.) Is considerate of others Has difficulty with writing Organizes his or her desk and backpack appropriately Completes task assignments on time Plans, recalls, and uses decoding strategies Reads words fluently Executive Functions in Children [29] Observation in the Classroom (Cont.) Understands and uses information read in a sentence, passage, or longer article Make appropriate inferences when reading Uses appropriate strategies for reading comprehension Executive Functions in Children [30] Observation in the Classroom (Cont.) Responds appropriately when called upon by the teacher Shows the ability to listen and glean needed information for the lecture Executive Functions in Children [31] Improving Deficits in EF Help children with EF deficits learn (Meltzer & Krishnan, 2007): Strategies for planning, organizing, prioritizing, memorizing, shifting flexibly, and checking their work How, when, and why specific strategies can be used for different academic tasks Strategies for organizing their time, materials, ideas, deadlines, and completed work Executive Functions in Children [32] Improving Deficits in EF (Cont.) Help children with EF deficits learn (Meltzer & Krishnan, 2007): How to modify strategies to match their own learning styles Different ways of applying strategies to different academic tasks and settings Self-monitoring skills, including how to identify their most common mistakes Executive Functions in Children [33] Improving Deficits in EF (Cont.) Help children with EF deficits learn (Meltzer & Krishnan, 2007): Learning that “learning how to learn” is an important step in doing well in school Executive Functions in Children [34] SOURCE Meltzer, L., & Krishnan, K. (2007). Executive function difficulties and learning disabilities. In L. Meltzer (Eds.), Executive function in education: From theory to practice (pp. 77–105). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Executive Functions in Children [35] Enhancing Self-Regulation (Ylvisaker & Feeney, 2008): Encouraging parents to provide a stable, organized, and predicable a home environment Encouraging important adults to form reasonable emotional attachments with the children Encouraging parents to provide adequate opportunities for their children to exercise control over events in the children’s lives Executive Functions in Children [36] Enhancing Self-Regulation (Ylvisaker & Feeney, 2008): (Cont.) Encouraging parents to follow an “authoritative/responsive” style of parenting (neither too permissive or too authoritarian) Encouraging parents and teachers to reward self-regulation, autonomy, and personal responsibility Executive Functions in Children [37] Enhancing Self-Regulation (Ylvisaker & Feeney, 2008): (Cont.) Giving children opportunities to work with adults who actively use self-regulatory strategies for themselves and who foster self-regulatory skills in the children Giving children opportunities to develop effective language development Giving children opportunities at home, school, and neighborhood to develop a coherent sense of personal identity Executive Functions in Children [38] SOURCE Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (2008). Helping children without making them helpless: Facilitating development of executive selfregulation in children and adolescents. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson, (Eds.), Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective. (pp. 409– 438). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. Executive Functions in Children [39] Recommended Book Dawson, P., & Guare, R. (2010). Executive skills in children and adolescents: A practical guide to assessment and intervention (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Executive Functions in Children [40] Assessment Procedures Beery Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (BVMI; Beery & Beery, 2004): Planning and organization Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF; Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000): Inhibition, shifting, emotional control, initiation, working memory, planning and organization, and self-monitoring Executive Functions in Children [41] Assessment Procedures (Cont.) Bender-Gestalt (Bender, 1938): Planning and organization Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II; Conners, & MHS Staff, 2000): Inhibition Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D–KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001): Flexibility and fluency Executive Functions in Children [42] Assessment Procedures (Cont.) Interview: Planning and organization NEPSY–II (Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007): Shifting, attention, inhibition, planning and organization, self-monitoring, selfregulation, flexibility, novel-problem solving, working memory, and fluency Executive Functions in Children [43] Assessment Procedures (Cont.) WISC–IV (Block Design, Coding, Digit Span, and Letter Number-Sequencing; Wechsler, 2003): Planning and organization, Selective and sustained attention, maintaining a set, selfmonitoring, and working memory Executive Functions in Children [44] Assessment Procedures (Cont.) Woodcock-Johnson III Test of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III-COG-Executive Process Cluster and Broad Attention Cluster; Woodcock, Schrank, Mather, & McGrew, 2007): Inhibition, flexibility, planning, and attention Executive Functions in Children [45] Source Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2004). Beery VMI (5th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Executive Functions in Children [46] Sources Bender, L. (1938). A Visual Motor Gestalt Test and its clinical use. American Orthopsychiatric Association Research Monograph, No. 3. Conners, C. K., & MHS Staff. (2000). Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II). North Tonawanda, NY: MultiHealth Systems. Executive Functions in Children [47] Sources Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E. F., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. L. (2007). NEPSY II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment: Administration Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Executive Functions in Children [48] Sources Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. Woodcock, R. W., Shrank, F. A., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2007). WoodcockJohnson® III Normative Update (WJ III NU). Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside. RIAS [p. 688-1] STRUCTURE Verbal Intelligence Guess What Verbal Reasoning Nonverbal Intelligence Odd-Item Out What’s Missing RIAS [p. 688-2] STRUCTURE Composite Memory Verbal Memory Nonverbal Memory Composite Intelligence Index: Verbal Intelligence + Nonverbal Intelligence RIAS Validity [1] Results of construct validity and convergent validity studies: General factor is supported (CIX) Verbal index is supported (VIX) Nonverbal index is not supported (NIX) Memory subtest should be separated from IQ, has poor g loadings RIAS Validity [2] Results of construct validity and convergent validity studies: (Cont.) In one case, RIAS Nonverbal IQ was 10 points higher than WAIS–III Performance Scale IQ Recommendation is to use the CIX but it may yield higher scores than the Wechsler tests RIAS Validity [3] SOURCES Beaujean, A. A., Firmin, M. W., Michonski, J. D., Berry, T., & Johnson, C. (2010). A multitrait-multimethod examination of the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales in a college sample. Assessment, 17(3), 347–360. doi:10.1177/1073191109356865 RIAS Validity [3] SOURCES Beaujean, A. A., McGlaughlin, S. M., & Margulies, A. S. (2009). Factorial validity of the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales for referred students. Psychology in the Schools, 46(10), 932–950. doi:10.1002/pits.20435 RIAS Validity [3] SOURCES Dombrowski, S. C., Watkins, M. W., & Brogan, M. J. (2009). An exploratory investigation of the factor structure of the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales (RIAS). Journal Of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(6), 494–507. doi:10.1177/0734282909333179 RIAS Validity [4] SOURCES Krach, S. K., Loe, S. A., Jones, W., & Farrally, A. (2009). Convergent validity of the Reynolds intellectual assessment scales (RIAS) using the Woodcock— Johnson tests of cognitive ability, third edition (WJ-III) with university students. Journal Of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(5), 355–365. doi:10.1177/0734282909331749 RIAS Validity [4] SOURCES Nelson, J. M., Canivez, G. L., Lindstrom, W., & Hatt, C. V. (2007). Higher-order exploratory factor analysis of the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales with a referred sample. Journal of School Psychology, 45(4), 439–456. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.03.003. RIAS Validity [4] SOURCES Smith, B. L., McChristian, C. L., Smith, T. D., & Meaux, J. (2009). The relationship of the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(1), 30–40 RIAS Validity [5] SOURCES Umphress, T. B. (2008). A comparison of low IQ scores from the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46(3), 229–233. doi:10.1352/2008.46:229-233 WJ III COG [p. 698-1] NONUNIFORM RANGE OF STANDARD SCORES Example 1: A 15-year-old with 1 point on 14 tests GIA = 1 Auditory Working Memory = 27 Analysis-Synthesis = 40 WJ III COG [p. 698-2] NONUNIFORM RANGE OF STANDARD SCORES Example 2: A 15-year-old with the highest score on 14 tests GIA = 177 Concept Formation = 129 Incomplete Words = 200 WJ III COG [p. 700] ITEM GRADIENTS Research by Krasa (2007) Results Only 7 of 20 cognitive tests have adequate item gradients Remaining 13 tests are too steeply graded Conclusion The 13 tests provide less sensitive measures of ability WJ III COG [p. 700] SOURCE Krasa, N. (2007). Is the WoodcockJohnson III a test for all seasons? Ceiling and item gradient considerations in its use with older students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25(1), 3– 16. doi:10.1177/0734282906291768. WJ III COG [1] SCORING ERRORS N = 36 graduate students, 108 protocols Total of 500 errors 46% of the protocols had 0 or 1 error Most frequently occurring errors: Use of incorrect ceilings Failure to record errors Failure to encircle the correct row for the total number correct WJ III COG [2] SOURCE Ramos, E., Alfonso, V. C., & Schermerhorn, S. M. (2009). Graduate students' administration and scoring errors on the Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Psychology In The Schools, 46(7), 650–657. doi:10.1002/pits.20405