Schenkerien prolongation and the emotional connotations of major-minor tonality Richard Parncutt University of Graz, Austria SysMus Graz ICME3, Jyväskylä Finland, 2013 Theses 1. A passage of MmT* is perceived as a prolonged background triad 2. This prolonged triad is the origin of “major-minor = happy-sad” *major-minor tonality Questions arising Does this background triad exist? Why major=happy and minor=sad? More questions… Why two triads? Why these two? Why this specific mapping? What about Leonard Meyer’s theory? Origins of… …emotion: evolutionary psychology? …MmT: psychohistory? Evolutionary functional origins of emotion Arousal High: energy available or necessary Low: energy unavailable or unnecessary Valence positive: clear situation safety, confidence negative: unclear situation fear or anger Psychohistoric origin of MmT Five psychohistorical steps 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Diatonic scales in ancient music Leading tones in medieval polyphony Importance of major and minor triads Implied pitches in these triads No consecutive semitones in scales Major-minor Tonality (MmT) Most music in MmT can be reduced to chord progressions. Most chords correspond to one of these diatonic triads. Pairs of harmonic complex tones with frequencies or pitches in common Octave and fifth relations Circle of fifths Harmonics in common perceptual similarity ethanhein.com Origin of pentatonic, diatonic, chromatic scales Leading tones in early music Guillaume de Machaut (1300-1377). Rondeau Ma fin est mon commencement F# tonicizes G - C# tonicizes D Origin of leading tones? Consonance / prevalence of individual tones in chant 70000 5 Data 60000 Model 4 50000 3 40000 30000 2 20000 1 10000 0 0 1 A 2 B 3 C 4 D 5 E 6 F 7 G 1 A 2 B 3 C 4 D 5 E F6 G7 Data: Counted in the Liber Usualis (DDMAL, Fujiniaga et al., McGill) Model: No. of harmonic pcs (P5, M3, m7, M2) at diatonic scale steps Comparison: df = 5, r = 0.90, p<.01 Finding: C (F) more prevalent than B (E) origin of leading tone? Cf. Parncutt, R. & Prem, D. (2008 ). The relative prevalence of Medieval modes and the origin of the leading tone (poster). International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC10), Sapporo, Japan, 25-29 August. Based on Bryden, & Hughes (1969). An index of Gregorian chant. Origin of major and minor triads The most consonant sets of 3 pitch classes • Include P5 high harmonicity • No m2 or M2 low roughness (Parncutt, 1988) Only two chords satisfy this constraint! Major and minor triads dominate MmT Our attention is drawn to their difference Missing fundamentals in triads (Parncutt, 1988; Terhardt, 1982) Chord CEG has missing fundamentals at A, F Chord CEbG has missing fundamentals at F, Ab Why? e.g. E and G are harmonics of A Mistuning of a ¼ - ½ tone? No problem in pitch perception Do these missing fundamentals exist? Empirical evidence E.g. C-major triad goes better with F than F# (Parncutt, 1993) BUT: can also be explained by musical experience Logical argument Brain tries to locate fundamentals in ambiguous sounds Perception of missing fundamentals is inevitable Origin of major and minor scales Compatible with major and minor triads (tonic triads) Krumhansl’s key profiles as pitch salience profiles of tonic triads Parncutt, 2011 ∆ Krumhansl’s key profiles ▀ calc. pitch salience in tonic triad evidence that tonic in MmT is a triad, not a tone Avoiding consecutive semitones If there are consecutive semitones in a melody, middle tone perceived as passing middle tone not a scale step no consecutive semitones in (jazz) scales Pressing (1981) Common exceptions: #4-5-b6, #7-8-b9 This can explain why C-major & –minor include D & not Db. Alternative explanations: Chord V is important. It must have P5 to be consonant. Use of standard diatonic scales Psychohistoric origin of major and minor scales 1. Diatonic scales in ancient music 2. Importance of major and minor triads CEG, CEbG 3. Leading tones CEGB, CEbGB 4. Implied pitches in these triads CEFGAB, CEbFGAbB 5. No consecutive semitones CDEFGAB, CDEbFGAbB Claim: The tonic in MmT is a triad not a single tone! Corollary: Any passage in MmT is perceived as a prolongation of its tonic triad Evidence: Qualitative • Success of Schenkerian approach Quantitative • Correlation between pitch salience in tonic triad and stability in scale • Transition probabilities in melodies Transition probabilities between scale steps in major-mode melodies (Huron, 2006, 2012) Why is the transition between scale steps 6 and 7 avoided? “Huron’s stereotype” 5 3 1 7 1 2 3 4 5 Music theory: Embellishment of tonic triad • Arpeggiation • Passing notes • Neighbor notes Psychological basis: Auditory scene analysis • Harmonicity • Pitch proximity 6 Pitch range of major-key melodies The lowest scale degree is often 1 or 7. The highest is often 6. A classical example Opening themes of 10 out of 18 Mozart piano sonatas conform to Huron‘s stereotype: KV 279, 280, 281, 283, 284, 310, 331, 332, 333, 545 Schenker’s Ursatz “Great music” (Bach, Beethoven, Brahms…) is a prolongation (elaboration, embellishment, Auskomponierung, Auswicklung) of the Ursatz. The Ursatz (background) prolongs the tonic triad. The Ursatz is elaborated middleground The middleground is elaborated foreground The whole piece prolongs the tonic triad Prolongation of prolongation? Not only “great” music Is all music in Mmt is a prolongation of the tonic triad? Not only music analysis Does chord prolongation have a psychological basis? Major-minor and valence some basic facts Positive valence Negative valence sadness, anger, fear, tension, solemnity, lament, tragedy, pathos, mourning, melancholy, frustration, depression, gloom… Major-minor effect can be overridden by tempo happiness, contentment, serenity, grace, tenderness, elation, joy, victory, majesty… E.g. fast, happy minor or slow, sad major Applies to tonalities not individual chords A major triad in a minor key can sound sad A minor triad in a major key can sound happy Leonard B. Meyer Emotion and meaning in music (1956) Minor tonality is more ambiguous Two versions of scale degrees 6 and 7 Extreme example: Carmen’s Habanera (“scandalous”: Susan McClary, 2005) Uncertainty negative emotion measured by information theory Sad speech and music Huron‘s approach. Some salient stuctural features: Mean pitch is lower than average 2. Smaller pitch intervals between successive syllables 3. Lower sound level 4. Slower 1. My hypothesis: Only 1 can explain origin of minor-sad Other are consequences Why is major happy? Most (births, weddings, feasts, preparation for war, entertainment, relaxation) not sad (funerals) Most music is happy music is major Major keys/chord more common than minor • Modulation to relative major; major dominant triad • Reason: more consonant? (higher harmonicity, less ambiguity) Minor keys the tonal Other Unhappy music the Other musical emotion Analogous asymmetries In everyday life and music, Positive valence is normal Negative valence is abnormal (emotional Other) In music, Consonance is normal Dissonance is abnormal (needs resolution) Consonance includes smoothness, harmonicity, diatonicity, familiarity; major is more consonant Why is minor sad? Two theories – competing or complementary? 1. Structural ambiguity (LB Meyer) Minor: root of tonic and tonic of scale are more ambiguous Is this dynamic (higher-arousal) negativity? afraid, angry, tense, frustrated… 2. Expected pitch in speech (Huron) Minor: 3rd and 6th scale degrees are lower than expected Is this static (lower-arousal) negativity? sad, solemn, gloomy, melancholy, depressed, lamenting, mourning, tragic, pathetic… In both cases, association between music and situation/speech role of duration & prolongation: keys not chords Theses 1. A passage of MmT is perceived as a prolonged background triad. 2. The prolonged triad is the ultimate origin of “major-minor = happy-sad”. Read all about it! Latest issue of Empirical Musicology Review Special thanks to David Huron and Matthew Davis for their study on mean interval size, which inspired this study