Juliet Goldbart Research Institute for Health & Social Change Manchester Metropolitan University 1. Brief consideration of Evidence-Based Practice. 2. Hierarchy of research evidence. 3. Where there isn’t sufficient evidence… 4. Options for practitioners to add their own evidence. Research evidence Service user values Professional expertise To provide the best possible education/ therapy/ intervention for a particular individual. To meet increasing demands for accountability. To avoid unethical use of ineffective education/ therapy/ intervention. To provide a common language for use among multidisciplinary teams. To promote transparency for service users and other stakeholders. Whose evidence? Publication bias (Dickersin, 1990; Chalmers et al., 1990; Fanelli, 2011; Torgerson, 2006). Non-reporting of negative evidence. What research gets funded? Remember: An absence of evidence does not mean negative evidence. Who are the experts? Us? Are the experts right? What factors inform and constrain our professional decision-making? Eclecticism versus protocol driven or centrally determined approaches? Which is Intensive Interaction (II)? Universal vs targeted vs individual(ised) approaches? Where does II fit? In the era of “consumer choice” is there a tension between choice and evidence? What role do learners/clients/family members want? How are comfortable are we with involving learners/clients/family members in decisionmaking? (How) can we ensure that learners/clients with significant cognitive impairments have the knowledge and skills for active choice? Level 1a 1b 2a 2b 3a 3b 4 5 Type of Evidence Systematic Review or Meta-Analysis of RCTs A single Randomised Controlled Trial (RCT) Systematic Review of Cohort Studies A single Cohort Study Systematic Review of Case Control studies or Quasi Experimental studies A single Case Control Study or Multiple Baseline SCED design Non experimental descriptive studies eg correlation studies, other single case experimental designs Expert opinion, textbooks, “first principles” research Government guidance and policy? Initial professional education? In-service courses? Textbooks? Professional magazines? Academic journals? Internet: Wikipedia to SpeechBITE? Current knowledge & experience. Learners’/clients’/other stakeholders’ apparent preferences. Context e.g. school vs. adult service. Aims: individual, group, service. Assessment data. Resources. Access to research literature. Availability of relevant evidence. Production of knowledge Is the evidence base in intellectual disability, speech path and special education sufficient? Research – Practice Gap Are teachers, therapists and other staff using the available evidence? If not, why not? (e.g. Burton & Chapman, 2007; Odom et al., 2005) Challenges of controlled research studies in education and social care settings, e.g. • • • • Controlling social variables, Frequent staff changes, Service emphasis on individualisation, Complex and multiple interventions. Low prevalence and heterogeneity of potential participants. Many interventions in learning disabilities are “of uncertain value and … have never been tested” (Parmenter, 2001, p.191). In relation to PMLD, “researchers have shown a limited interest in providing an empirical base for these interventions” (Vlaskamp & Nakken, 2008, p.334). “randomized trials are rarely applicable for students from a low incidence population” (Snell, 2003, p.143). (Burton & Chapman, 2007; McDonnell & O’Neill, 2003; Snell, 2003) Available research is not relevant to practice. Practitioners may have limited access to research findings. Research findings may contradict long held beliefs and practices. Family, legal and service pressures may affect decision-making. Take part in conferences & training! Collaborate with researchers to obtain research funding. Make the best use of what there is: o See tomorrow’s session Contribute to the evidence base yourselves: o Well described Case Studies; SCEDs. Establish a track record through small scale research e.g. Masters degree. Collaborative bids between schools, health service & universities. NHMRC: e.g. Translating Research Into Practice (TRIP) Fellowships Using NHMRC’s Research Help Centre and other supports. ARC’s National Competitive Grants Program, but less clear for education and social care than health. Resources may not be available to undertake large scale studies. Teachers, therapists & other staff have great data or the potential to collect such data. Single Case Experimental Designs (e.g. Kazdin, 2011; Romeiser-Logan et al., 2008) and Case Study designs allow practitioner research to contribute meaningfully to the evidence base. An empirical inquiry “that investigates a contemporary phenomenon within its real-life context; when the boundaries between phenomenon and context are not clearly evident; and in which multiple sources of evidence are used.” (Yin, 1984, p. 23) A “how” or “why” research question. Choose your “cases.” Describe the context. What data do you need? Often quantitative AND qualitative data. Use a table or database to organise the data. Collect data systematically but flexibly over time. Look for patterns and links but also contradictions and disagreements. Case study template to support experimental design in AAC and AT (Murray et al., 2013) Origins in Cochrane review (Pennington et al) showing insufficient reporting of study details in SLT for children with cerebral palsy. Pennington, Marshall & Goldbart (2007): a detailed, ICF-based reporting framework. Murray et al., using an expert reference group, refined the framework for use in practice. Template allows 24 pages of consistent detailed information on participants, communication partners, the intervention and outcomes to be uploaded to a database . Database can be interrogated by practitioners and researchers on submission of ethically approved request. www.communicationmatters.org.uk/aac-evidencebase Since May 2013, 35 case studies uploaded. Similar model could be used in other areas. What would be needed to set up an Intensive Interaction Case Study Framework/ Who would manage it? Would you contribute to it? Would you use the findings? One type of “Time Series” design Data are collected on one or more participants during a baseline phase then during a treatment/intervention phase. Not a robust design because few threats to validity are controlled for. BUT by aggregating detailed case study data we can build up meaningful evidence. Increasing Classroom Attending Percent Attending Behavior 110 90 70 Baseline Intervention phase 50 30 10 5 9 15 20 No. of 10-min. Observation Sessions Walker, H.M., & Buckley, N.K. The use of positive reinforcement in conditioning attending behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 245-250 Types of “Time Series” designs. Looking at change over time in response to treatment/intervention. Each participant serves as one trial and as their own control. Avoid the ethical problem of no-treatment controls. Practitioners can contribute to the evidence base (Cakiroglu, 2012; Horner et al., 2005) Usually require the establishment of a stable (or decreasing) baseline. Use of repeated measurements during baseline and treatment sessions or frequent “probes.” Analysis can be statistical, but more usually visual: variability, slope, overlapping data points. Use multiple iterations/replications to demonstrate effect. Various design options: Multiple Baseline designs - (across participants, behaviours or settings). Withdrawal, Reversal or ABAB designs – assume behaviour can be reversed - relevance? Alternating treatment designs - the rapid alternating between two or more conditions. No baseline needed. Evaluation of Intensive Interaction in a hospital setting. 6 participants: adults “who experienced severe difficulties in learning and in relating to others.” Multiple baseline across participants. Baseline observational data on initiation and maintenance of interaction plus PVCS. Findings: Increases in social and communicative abilities. Quality appraisal through Tate et al (2008) or more recently RoBiNT (Tate et al, 2013) – used by SpeechBITE. Multiple baseline designs more robust. Nine replications across studies: “well established” (Parker et al., 2008, p.545). Three replications across studies: “probably efficacious” (Parker et al., 2008, p.545). Evidence based practice is important for service delivery and for clients (inter alia). The evidence base can be expanded by researchers and practitioners. We need to be creative in working out ways to use the evidence we have, generate new evidence and influence the evidence that is sought. Thanks for your interest Any questions? Juliet Goldbart j.goldbart@mmu.ac.uk