DIVIDED ATTENTION DIVIDED ATTENTION Duncan (1993) asked participants to make judgments about single object. They could make two simultaneous judgments about this object –what it was, as well as where it was located- without any loss accuracy. However, they made many errors when asked to make two simultaneous judgments about two different objects, for example where both objects were located. In other words, our perceptual system can handle some divided-attention tasks, but we fail when the tasks become to demanding. DIVIDED ATTENTION Mark Reinitz and his colleagues (1994) asked their research participants to look at skrtches of faces, with dots across each face. People in the fullattention condition received no instruction about the dots, whereas people in the divided-attention condition were instructed to count the dots. Later, everyone was asked to judge whether each face in a series was old or new. The result was shown in the table below: Test condition Old Faces Conjunction Faces Full attention 0,81 0,48 Study condition Divided attention 0,48 0,42 When our attention is divided, we often fail to perceive stimuli accurately. DIVIDED ATTENTION The research on practice and divided attention confirms the wisdom of “practice makes perfect”. For example, in two classic studies, college students were trained to read stories silently at the same time that they copied down irrelevant words dictated by the experimenter (Hirst et al., 1980; Spelke et al., 1976). At first, the students had trouble combining the two tasks; however, after six weeks of training, they could read as quickly while taking dictation as when they were only reading. Still, even at this well-practiced stage, the students were not really attending to the dictated words. In fact, they were able to recall only 35 of the several thousand words they had written down. However, with more extensive training, they became so accomplished at this divided-attention task that they could even categorize the dictated word without any decline in their reading rate. As Hirst (1986) argues, practice apparently alters the limits of attentional capacity. Humans do not seem have a built-in, fixed limit to the number of task they can perform simultaneously (Allport, 1989).