20 07 Variable horizon control predicts speed-accuracy tradeoffs and velocity profiles in aimed movements Robin T. * Bye and Peter D. Neilson 1. Introduction 3. Intermittent response planning The principles underlying the speed-accuracy tradeoffs in rapid aimed movements have attracted major interest and controversy both experimentally and theoretically for over a century. While the logarithmic tradeoff, Fitts' law, holds for movements emphasizing spatial accuracy, the linear tradeoff occurs for movements emphasizing both spatial and temporal accuracy. Typically, the first kind of movements produces asymmetrical velocity profiles, whereas the latter kind produces symmetrical velocity profiles. We propose that the combination of intermittent response planning using variable horizon predictive control and the existence of signal-dependent noise in the nervous system predicts both kinds of speed-accuracy tradeoffs as well as both kinds of velocity profiles. The response planning (RP) system in AMT operates intermittently. During each planning interval (100 ms) an optimal minimum acceleration trajectory is generated while at the same time the previously planned trajectory is being executed as well as afferent signals being stored. This is achieved by using parallel array processing readily available in the parallel structures of the central nervous system. 2. Adaptive Model Theory Adaptive model theory (AMT) is a computational theory about information processing within the perceptual-motor loop involved in the control of purposive movement. It proposes the adaptive formation of task-dependent feedback/feedforward controllers able to generate stable, noninteractive control and render nonlinear interactions unobservable in sensory-motor relationships. These controllers exist in the central nervous system as neural adaptive filters based on cerebellar circuitry. 4. Noise in the nervous system AMT suggests that broadband noise is introduced at the level of motor commands and that its standard deviation increases with the size of the motor command. It is this stochastic noise that causes every movement to be a little different from the previous in a repetitive movement task. 5. Simulator A Simulink model has been developed that by simulating intermittent adaptive optimal control in the central nervous system reproduces the logarithmic and linear speed-accuracy tradeoff with corresponding asymmetrical and symmetrical velocity profiles by employing receding and fixed horizon control, respectively. 6. Speed-accuracy tradeoff 6a. Logarithmic tradeoff Spatially constrained movements (movements that must end within a target region, typically as fast as possible) yield a logarithmic tradeoff formalised as Fitts’ law: T = a + b log2(2D/W) = a + b Id The movement time T increases logarithmically with an increase in target distance D and target width W. The logterm is often substituted with the index of difficulty Id = log2(2D/W). When humans perform aimed movements, a speed-accuracy tradeoff occurs: Increasing movement speed normally decreases accuracy whereas increased accuracy requires the movement to slow down. A variety of speed-accuracy tradeoffs has been found. Apart from the two most important tradeoffs, namely the logarithmic and linear speed-accuracy tradeoffs, variants of these two as well as power laws, quadratic laws, and the deltalognormal law have been proposed. The velocity profiles for spatially constrained aimed movements are asymmetrically (leftskewed) bell-shaped. The profile becomes increasingly asymmetrical as movement is slowed. Velocity profiles 6b. Linear tradeoff Temporally constrained movements (movements that must reach target as well as have a pre-specified duration) result in a linear tradeoff: We = a + b (D/T) T ≈ b D/We (a small) The standard deviation of movement endpoint We, termed effective target width analogous to target width W in Fitts’ law, increases linearly with increased target distance D and decreased movement time T. The velocity profiles for temporally constrained aimed movements have a symmetrical bell shape. No matter the speed, timed movements produce symmetrical velocity profiles. Fitts’ law holds for tapping, throwing, and rotating Behavioural evidence for the linear tradeoff that Behavioural movements both on land, under water, and in occurs in temporally constrained movements evidence aircraft flights. Subjects include children, young and exists not only in single tapping tasks, but also elderly adults, mentally challenged in saccadic eye movements, wrist people, Parkinsonians, and rotations, and most other time7. Variable horizon control drugged people. matching tasks. The RP system can vary the prediction horizon for the response to be generated at each planning 7b. Fixed horizon control 7a. Receding horizon control interval. At the extremes lie the receding and At every planning interval the The prediction horizon is held fixed horizon control strategies, which yield a prediction horizon is reduced with constant throughout the movement logarithmic and linear speed-accuracy tradeoff, the distance moved during the leading to a logarithmic speedrespectively. Combinations of the two strategies interval until the target is reached, accuracy tradeoff and an can lead to other desired goals such as minimising leading to a linear tradeoff and a asymmetrical (left-skewed) velocity the infinity norm of the error or the integral of the symmetrical velocity profile that profile that match that of spatially error squared used in LQR optimal control. match that of temporally constrained aimed movements. constrained movements. Results# UNSW Logarithmic tradeoff # Simulation Asymmetric profile results for a 500 ms prediction horizon * Corresponding author. E-mail: robin.bye@student.unsw.edu.au Symmetric profile Linear tradeoff NEUROENGINEERING @ UNSW Neuroengineering Laboratory EE223, Systems & Control Research Group, School of Electrical Engineering & Telecommunications, University of New South Wales, UNSW Sydney NSW 2052 AUSTRALIA