Uploaded by Kelly Adams

Fatigue & Reaction Time Research Paper

advertisement
Running Head: REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE​
Effects of Fatigue on Reaction Time
California Polytechnic State University
Kelly Adams, Jourdan Muro, Benjamin Stanely, Ivy Tseng
1
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
2
Reaction Time and Fatigue
Physical performance is essential to almost every sport, whether it be in football,
basketball, dance, rock climbing or pole vaulting. People generally want to perform well so that
they may excel in their sport, but many sports require long bouts of muscular endurance that can
lead to muscular fatigue. Fatigue is something that has been known to play a big role in limiting
performance; so when people tire out, how can they continue to perform at the same level they
were? Muscle fatigue is defined as the decline in the ability of a muscle to generate force. When
a muscle becomes tired, the ability of the muscle fiber to contract is reduced and the nerve’s
ability to generate a signal becomes limited. Much of this can happen as a result of vigorous
exercise. Prior to conducting our own tests, we examined several articles researching the effects
that fatigue may have on performance. An article on the effect of lower extremity muscular
fatigue on motor control performance aimed to determine whether lower extremity fatigue
affects the ability of an individual to balance on an unstable platform (Howard, Cawley & Losse,
1998). Another study examined the effects of fatigue on the kinematics of free-throw shooting
(Uygur, Göktepe, Ak, Karabörk & Feza Korkusuz, 2010). Other researchers studied the effects of
an intermittent exercise fatigue protocol on the biomechanics of soccer kick performance (Kellis,
Katis & Vrabas, 2006). Smilios studied the effects of varying levels of muscular fatigue on
vertical jump performance (Smilios, 1998). The results all indicated that there was a connection
between fatigue and performance. The purpose of our study was to determine if fatigue has an
effect on reaction time in the transverse flexors and the flexors of the digits. From our own
understanding and the research we did prior to conducting out tests, we hypothesized that
muscular fatigue would have a negative effect on reaction time.
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
3
Eighteen Cal Poly students in the KINE 319-02 class participated in the study, consisting
of nine male and nine female participants. Inclusion criteria required that participants were Cal
Poly kinesiology students between the ages of 19 and 23. The exclusion criteria required that
participants had no current or recent injuries and were not absent during either day the testing
was conducted. Testing began with attaining baseline reaction times for all participants in both
the pinch-grip reaction test and clap reaction test. For the pinch-grip ruler-drop test, participants
were instructed to hold their dominant hands out in front of them with their fingers four inches
apart. A foot long ruler was held in the middle of their “claw-shaped” hand, directly above the
index finger. The tester then dropped the ruler without warning and the subject had to react and
pinch their fingers to their thumb to catch the ruler, activating the flexors of the digits. Wherever
the fingers ended up on the ruler after catching was distance recorded. During the clap ruler-drop
test, participants were instructed to hold their arms out in front of them, perpendicular to their
body, with their hands four inches apart. The foot-long ruler was held directly over their index
finger. When the ruler was randomly dropped by the tester, the participants were instructed to
clap their hands together in order to catch the ruler, activating the transverse flexors. After
obtaining baseline data from all participants, each subject was randomly assigned to either
complete the dead-hang or half-pushup hold to failure. The dead-hang would fatigue the flexors
of the digits while the half-pushup hold would fatigue the transverse flexors. After completing
either the dead hang or half push-up fatigue protocol until perceived failure, the participants
immediately completed either the pinch-grip or clap ruler-drop test, respectively. ​
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
Table 1
Total # Participants
18 (9 male, 9 female)
Age (years)
19-23
Height (cm)
172.56 ± 10.435
Weight (kg)
70.3 ± 12.716
Our study included 18 participants including 9 males and 9 females between the ages of 19-23.
The average height of our participants was 172.56cm ± 10.435cm and the average weight was
70.3cm ± 12.716cm.
4
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
5
In order to analyze our collected data, we ran two dependent t-tests using JMP software. One
compared the pinch grip ruler test at baseline, to the same test post-dead-hang. The second
compared the clap ruler test at baseline and post-half-pushup hold. These t-tests allowed us to see
if there was a difference between the two conditions at baseline and following fatigue.
Table 2:
The Post-dead-hang reaction time was .56cm better than the baseline reaction time. However, the
p-value was .74 which was far greater than our alpha level which we set at 0.05. This indicates
that the difference between the two groups was non-significant.
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
The post-pushup hold reaction time was .25cm better than the baseline reaction time. However,
the p-value is .89. This indicates that the difference between groups is nonsignificant.
Based on our statistical analysis we found that both the half push-up and dead hang to fatigue
resulted in faster reaction times, however, these improvements were not statistically significant.
6
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
7
After analyzing the results, we came to the conclusion that our hypothesis was not
supported. Our hypothesis stated that both muscle groups would have a slower reaction time
after the fatigue protocol, but there was no statistically significant data to support our claim. The
data was very scattered: some participants performed better after the fatiguing and some
performed worse. We also hypothesized that the pectoralis major would have a faster reaction
time than the wrist flexors, but the data showed slower reaction times overall in the wrist flexors
pre/post fatigue than in the transverse flexors pre/post fatigue.
One of the main strengths of our study was that we tested two different functional muscle
groups of various sizes located at different distances from the central nervous system. Most of
the studies done on fatigue and reaction time have focussed around one specific muscle group
without comparing how reaction time might be affected differently during fatigue based on
muscle size, type and distance from the central nervous system. Another strength of our study
was that it was a between-subjects randomized cross-over study, so each participant performed
both tests pre and post fatigue protocols and the randomization of the groups lowered the
possibility of confounding variables between the two groups. The study also consisted only of
Kine 319 lab students at Cal Poly so there is high internal validity for Cal Poly Kinesiology
majors.
There were a few limitations to this study. The first was that, our subject group was
limited to Kine 319 lab students at Cal Poly. This meant there would be little external validity
when applying our results to the general population. Another limitation was testing and
instrumental error. The study was not blinded to the researcher recording the data and could have
had a bias towards our hypothesis that may have affected the results that were recorded. The
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
8
main weakness of this study, however, is the poor execution of the protocols we set in place. Our
hypothesis was rejected based on our data because we found muscular fatigue to have no
statistically significant effect on reaction time. After doing more research, we found that Sant'
Ana and colleagues conducted a study on Taekwondo kicks and the impact it has on reaction
time. They determined that muscular fatigue to negatively impacts reaction time (Sant’Ana et al,.
2017). The likeliest cause of our inconsistent data was due to the fact that most participants did
not perform their fatigue protocol to failure as instructed. Many of the participants said they had
gone to failure, but displayed signs of energy and lack of muscular fatigue. Other participants
were more honest about their perceived exertion, but still ended before muscle failure or even
fatigue because they were not having fun or were planning to exercise later and didn’t want to
pre-fatigue themselves. Despite the reasons, many of the participants were not receiving proper
muscular fatigue during the exercise and, rather than exhausting the muscle, these participants
might have done just enough to stimulate the muscle and quicken neural activation, leading to a
quicker reaction time on the post fatigue test.
Future research comparing the effects of fatigue on the reaction time of various muscle
groups, testing neural activation of skeletal muscle when performing a fatigue protocol would be
something worth conducting next. The researcher should also ask for a rate of perceived exertion
after the fatiguing exercise has been completed, so if the data does seem off one can look back at
the RPE and see if that was a possible cause. Providing the participants with some type of
incentive might also cause more energy exertion during the fatiguing period and lead to less
threat to validity.
REACTION TIME AND FATIGUE ​​
​
​
​
​
​
​
9
References
Howard, M. E., Cawley, P. W., & Losse, G. M. (1998). Effect of lower extremity muscular​
​
fatigue on motor control performance. Medicine and science in sports and exercise,​
​
30(12), 1703-1707.
Kellis, E., Katis, A., & Vrabas, I. S. (2006). Effects of an intermittent exercise fatigue protocol​
​
on biomechanics of soccer kick performance. Scandinavian journal of medicine &​
​
science in sports, 16(5), 334-344.
Sant’Ana, J., Franchini, E., da Silva, V., & Diefenthaeler, F. (2017). Effect of fatigue on reaction​
​
time, response time, performance time, and kick impact in taekwondo roundhouse kick.​
​
Sports biomechanics, 16(2), 201-209.
Smilios, I. (1998). Effects of varying levels of muscular fatigue on vertical jump performance.​
​
Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 12, 204-208.
Uygur, M., Goktepe, A., Ak, E., Karabörk, H., & Korkusuz, F. (2010). The effect of fatigue on​
​
the kinematics of free throw shooting in basketball. Journal of human kinetics, 24, 51-56.
Download