ebook THE GUILFORD PRESS Biofeedback Also Available Biopsychosocial Assessment in Clinical Health Psychology Edited by Frank Andrasik, Jeffrey L. Goodie, and Alan L. Peterson Passive Muscle Relaxation: A Program for Client Use (CD-ROM) Mark S. Schwartz and Stephen N. Haynes Biofeedback A Practitioner’s Guide F o u r t h E d i t i o n Edited by Mark S. Schwartz Frank Andrasik THE GUILFORD PRESS New York London © 2016 The Guilford Press A Division of Guilford Publications, Inc. 370 Seventh Avenue, Suite 1200, New York, NY 10001 www.guilford.com Paperback edition 2017 All rights reserved No part of this book may be reproduced, translated, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the publisher. Printed in the United States of America This book is printed on acid-free paper. Last digit is print number: 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 The authors have checked with sources believed to be reliable in their efforts to provide information that is complete and generally in accord with the standards of practice that are accepted at the time of publication. However, in view of the possibility of human error or changes in behavioral, mental health, or medical sciences, neither the authors, nor the editors and publisher, nor any other party who has been involved in the preparation or publication of this work warrants that the information contained herein is in every respect accurate or complete, and they are not responsible for any errors or omissions or the results obtained from the use of such information. Readers are encouraged to confirm the information contained in this book with other sources. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Names: Schwartz, Mark S. (Mark Stephen), editor. | Andrasik, Frank, 1949– editor. Title: Biofeedback : a practitioner’s guide / edited by Mark S. Schwartz, Frank Andrasik. Description: Fourth edition. | New York : The Guilford Press, [2016] | Includes bibliographical references and index. Identifiers: LCCN 2015049500 | ISBN 9781462522545 (hardcover : alk. paper) | ISBN 9781462531943 (paperback : alk. paper) Subjects: LCSH: Biofeedback training. Classification: LCC RC489.B53 S39 2016 | DDC 615.8/514—dc23 LC record available at http://lccn.loc.gov/2015049500 In Memoriam David E. Krebs, DPT, PhD We include this dedication in profound honor and in memory of David E. Krebs because he was an integral part of the first three editions of this book; because of his illustrious career and accomplishments; and because we wanted him to know that we remain very appreciative of him and in awe of his many accomplishments. We prepared this dedication and shared it with him, via his family, in 2013, several months before he died, on February 7, 2014. He was 57. In late June 2009, David had a fall at his home that left him unable to continue his illustrious professional career. However, he is still being honored for his 30-plus years of work in the physical therapy field, and his students and colleagues have continued his important work and contributions. His loving family continued to remain close with him and were dedicated to his welfare and happiness. David’s curriculum vitae is long and very impressive. We put it, along with other information about him, at www.marksschwartzphd.com. His obituary is at http://hosting-24864. tributes.com/obituary/show/david-e-krebs-99398993. For those readers who did not know about David and his career, we include the following summary: David E. Krebs received his BS in Physical Therapy in 1977 and MA in Applied Physiology in 1979 from Columbia University, his PhD in Pathokinesiology and Physical Therapy in 1986 from New York University, and his DPT in 2002 from the MGH Institute of Health Professions. In 1983, he agreed to write a major chapter for the first edition of Biofeedback: A Practitioner’s Guide (published in 1987). Although this was very early in his career, it was apparent that his accomplishments would be legendary. We have been fortunate and grateful that he agreed to join the first edition and remained a contributor to this book throughout his career. v He became Professor of Physical Therapy and Clinical Investigation at the MGH Institute of Health Professions and Director of Massachusetts General Hospital’s Biomotion Laboratory. He also held academic appointments in Orthopaedics at Harvard Medical School and in Mechanical Engineering at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. David had more than 300 publications and was awarded more than $5 million as principal investigator on federal (National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research) and foundation research grants, primarily in the area of neural and biomechanical constraints of human locomotor control. He was the featured speaker at the 12th annual Eugene Michels Research Forum of the American Physical Therapy Association and received the Association’s 2003 Helen J. Hislop Award for Outstanding Contributions to Professional Literature, its 1998 Marian Williams Award for Research in Physical Therapy, and its 1994 Golden Pen Award for Scientific Writing. We are very proud to continue to have David’s name and chapter, for which he was the original author and major contributor, included in this book. Thank you, Dave, from the many professionals and patients who forever are very respectful of and very grateful to you. For the information about David E. Krebs’s career and other documents about his life, we are indebted to his family and to Timothy Fagerson, DPT, his former student and coauthor. They all helped in the review and editing of this dedication. vi About the Editors Mark S. Schwartz, PhD, is past chair of the Biofeedback Certification Institute of America (now Biofeedback Certification International Alliance; BCIA); serves on the Mayo Clinic Emeritus Staff; and has a private practice in Jacksonville, ­Florida. He recently served as Visiting Professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of North Florida. Past president of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB), Dr. Schwartz is board certified by the BCIA, a Diplomate in Clinical Sexology of the American Board of Sexology, and a Diplomate of the American Board of Assessment Psychology. His website is www.marksschwartzphd.com. Frank Andrasik, PhD, is Distinguished Professor and Chair of Psychology at the University of Memphis. He is Editor-in-Chief of Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, Associate Editor of Cephalalgia, and past Editor-in-Chief of Behavior Therapy. His extensive ­publications include, most recently, the coedited volume Biopsychosocial Assessment in Clinical Health Psychology. Dr. Andrasik is past president of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies and the AAPB. He is board certified as a Senior Fellow by the BCIA and is a Fellow of the Society of Clinical Psychology and Society for Health Psychology (Divisions 12 and 38 of the American Psychological Association), the Association for Psychological Science, the Society of Behavioral Medicine, the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, and the American Headache Society. vii Contributors Frank Andrasik, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Memphis, Memphis, Tennessee John G. Arena, PhD, Mental Health Service Line, Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, and Department of Psychiatry and Health Behavior, Medical College of Georgia, Augusta, Georgia Gerard A. Banez, PhD, Pediatric Pain Rehabilitation Program, Cleveland Clinic Children’s Hospital for Rehabilitation, Cleveland, Ohio Anat Barnea, DSc, private practice, Givat Chaim Ichud, Israel Dana Bassett, MA, Hornsby Psychology Clinic, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Niels Birbaumer, PhD, Institute for Medical Psychology and Behavioural Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany Keith I. Block, MD, Block Center for Integrative Cancer Treatment, Skokie, Illinois Eugenia Bodenhamer-Davis, PhD, Department of Rehabilitation, Social Work and Addictions, University of North Texas, Denton, Texas Jeffrey E. Bolek, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Cleveland Clinic, and Motor Control Restoration, LLC, Cleveland, Ohio Rex L. Cannon, PhD, Neural Potential, West Palm Beach, Florida Thomas F. Collura, PhD, BrainMaster Technologies, Inc., Bedford, Ohio Timothy Culbert, MD, FAAP, Integrative Medicine, PrairieCare Medical Group, Chaska, Minnesota Richard E. Davis, MS, Neurotherapy Associates of Texas, Denton, Texas Peter T. Dorsher, MSc, MD, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida Eugene Eisman, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Psychology, University of Milwaukee, Milwaukee, Wisconsin Timothy L. Fagerson, DPT, Spine–Orthopaedic–Sports Physical Therapy, Wellesley, Massachusetts ix x Contributors Herta Flor, PhD, Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany Udi Gal, MA, private practice, Manof, Israel Richard N. Gevirtz, PhD, Department of Clinical Psychology, California School of Professional Psychology, Alliant International University, San Diego, California Alan G. Glaros, PhD, School of Dentistry, University of Missouri–Kansas City, Kansas City, Missouri Charlotte Gyllenhaal, PhD, Block Center for Integrative Cancer Treatment, Skokie, Illinois Daniel Hamiel, PhD, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, IDC Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel; Cohen–Harris Resilience Center, Haifa, Israel; and Tel-Aviv Brull Community Mental Health Services, Tel-Aviv, Israel Timothy Harkness, MA, Chelsea Football Club, London, United Kingdom Joe Kamiya, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Medical Psychology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California Maria Katsamanis, PsyD, Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, New Jersey Tobias Kaufmann, PhD, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Boris Kotchoubey, PhD, Institute for Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany David E. Krebs, DPT, PhD (deceased), Institute of Health Professions and Biomotion Laboratory, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts Andrea Kübler, PhD, Institute for Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany Deloris M. Lakia, DNP, CNP, CDE, College of Nursing, University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio Leonard L. Lausten, DDS, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, Kansas Paul M. Lehrer, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, New Jersey Wolfgang Linden, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Joel F. Lubar, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee and Southeastern Neurofeedback Institute, Pompano Beach, Florida Angele V. McGrady PhD, LPCC, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio Susan Middaugh, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Anesthesiology and Perioperative Medicine, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina Vincent J. Monastra, PhD, FPI Attention Disorders Clinic, Endicott, New York Nicola Neumann, PhD, Institute of Diagnostic Radiology and Neuroradiology, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany C. J. Peek, PhD, Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, Minnesota Jeanetta C. Rains, PhD, Center for Sleep Evaluation, Elliot Hospital, Manchester, New Hampshire Contributors Andrea Reid-Chung, MA, ADD Centre, Biofeedback Institute of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada Arnon Rolnick, PhD, Rolnick’s Clinic, Ramat Gan, Israel Ronald L. Rosenthal, PhD, private practice, Miami, Florida Mark S. Schwartz, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, and private practice, Jacksonville, Florida Nancy M. Schwartz, MA, Department of Psychology, University of North Florida, Jacksonville, Florida Keith Sedlacek, MD, private practice, Stress Regulation Institute, New York, New York Fredric Shaffer, PhD, BCB, Center for Applied Psychophysiology, Truman State University, Kirksville, Missouri Richard A. Sherman, PhD, Department of Psychology, Saybrook University, Oakland, California Wesley E. Sime, PhD, MPH, Emeritus Staff, Department of Nutrition and Health Sciences, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, and First Step Wellness, Lincoln, Nebraska Jonathan C. Smith, PhD, Department of Psychology, Roosevelt University, Chicago, Illinois Estate M. “Tato” Sokhadze, PhD, Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine, Greenville, South Carolina Ute Strehl, PhD, Institute for Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany Sebastian Striefel, PhD, Emeritus Staff, Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, Utah Robert W. Thatcher, PhD, Applied Neuroscience Research Institute, Largo, Florida James W. G. Thompson, PhD, Evoke Neuroscience, New York, New York Lynda Thompson, PhD, ADD Centre, Biofeedback Institute of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada Michael Thompson, MD, ADD Centre, Biofeedback Institute of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada Kirtley E. Thornton, PhD, Neuroscience Center, Charlotte, North Carolina Jeannette Tries, PhD, OTR, Aurora West Allis Memorial Center, West Allis, Wisconsin, and Clinic for Neurophysiological Learning, Milwaukee, Wisconsin David L. Trudeau, MD, Foundation Neurofeedback and Neuromodulation Research (FNNR, formerly ISNR-RF), Murfreesboro, Tennessee Frederick Wamboldt, MD, Department of Medicine, National Jewish Health, Denver, Colorado Robert Whitehouse, EdD, private practice, Boulder, Colorado Vietta Sue Wilson, PhD, School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Marcie Zinn, PhD, Department of Community Research, DePaul University, Chicago, Illinois Mark Zinn, MM, Department of Community Research, DePaul University, Chicago, Illinois xi Preface The responses to the first three editions of Biofeedback have been very rewarding in terms of acceptance and reviews. The second edition essentially doubled the size, and despite our sincere efforts and the publisher’s urging, the third edition remained the same size. The publisher reemphasized the need for a substantial reduction in the fourth edition, and we again earnestly sought to comply. Because new research areas and interests in biofeedback have emerged in recent years, it was clear that more topics warranted inclusion. We therefore deemed it necessary to expand the chapters involving electroencephalographic (EEG) biofeedback/neurotherapy and other topics. Readers will find 11 new chapters among the total of 42. Several of the chapters from the third edition have been combined and condensed into single chapters. Seven chapters focus entirely on EEG biofeedback/neurofeedback, and several other chapters include substantial discussions of this modality. The remaining chapters have been revised, updated, and streamlined to varying degrees. One chapter was eliminated. Three instrumentation chapters are entirely new, focusing on surface electromyography (Chapter 4), quantitative EEG (Chapter 7), and consumer products (Chapter 9). Other totally new chapters focus on relaxation (Chapter 12), cognitive-behavioral therapy and ways to integrate it with biofeedback (Chapter 11), and workplace applications (Chapter 36). Five wholly new chapters are devoted to disorders receiving increased attention by biofeedback therapists and researchers: anxiety disorders (Chapter 26), asthma (Chapter 29), traumatic brain injury (Chapter 38), autism spectrum disorders (Chapter 39), and substance use disorders (Chapter 41). These new chapters provide unique content that is not found within any other single volume. We have retained chapters on the same conditions and disorders that were present in the prior editions, because these remain the focus of continued research and practice—recurrent headaches, temporomandibular disorders, Raynaud’s disease, essential hypertension, diabetes mellitus, tinnitus, fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, neuromuscular reeducation, and bowel/bladder and pelvic floor disorders—as well as chapters on applications for performing artists, sports, and pediatrics. All of these contributions have been significantly updated. As in prior editions, we include a chapter on frontier applications that focuses on several other disorders and conditions. The volume again begins with a historical perspective and now includes the new definition of biofeedback and a preliminary definition of applied psychophysiology. The chapter on entering the field and ensuring competence has been thoroughly updated. Biofeedback now includes a total of 65 authors, nearly half of whom (32) are new contributors. This compares with 39 authors in the third edition, 21 authors in the second edition, and 7 in xiii xiv Preface the first edition. To the extent that is possible, we and the other authors have striven to integrate academic/research content along with considerable applied content of a clinical and/or educational nature. Interested readers may find it useful to refer to the breadth of materials relevant to biofeedback, and the specific topics discussed in this book, that are accessible at www.marksschwartzphd.com. This website allows for posting of supplemental photos, graphics, text, protocols, patient/client/ subject educational material, and examination items for qualified educators, as well as for reader comments and questions, and updating of content by some authors where appropriate. This book continues to provide the broadest scope of topics in the field of biofeedback and applied psychophysiology, prepared by a diverse and highly acclaimed set of authors. On the occasion of the publication of this fourth edition, we thank all readers of the previous editions for their support, ideas, dedication, and feedback, and look forward to receiving continued feedback from readers of this new edition. Mark S. Schwartz Frank Andrasik Acknowledgments Mark S. Schwartz: In the prior three editions, I expressed my detailed acknowledgments to many people, and I still am appreciative of them for all the reasons expressed previously. (I am including acknowledgments from all three earlier editions at www.marksschwartzphd.com.) For this edition, I will add a few that were not mentioned in the prior editions. I am incredibly grateful that Frank Andrasik again agreed to coedit the text. He diligently coped with the myriad factors that resulted in delays from many sources. His incredible and varied skills, his patience, his perseverance, his devotion, and his humor all were clearly crucial in the completion of the book. Frank Andrasik: In the third edition (my first as coeditor), I expressed my heartfelt thanks to Mark Schwartz, for affording me the privilege and opportunity to assist him in revising what had already become a classic text in the field. Now, as then, I treasured every step along the way, especially those enabling me to get to know him more as a person and a friend. It is my continued honor to be able to work with and learn from Mark, whose deep humility will leave him to disagree with me when I acknowledge him as one of the clinical pioneers and most influential leaders in our field. He has contributed in innumerable ways to the growth of biofeedback and applied psychophysiology. For this and more, all of us are indebted. I also expressed my deep appreciation to the many mentors and colleagues who helped educate me, guide me, and shape my career, and to family and loved ones who helped sustain me over my entire career. (My prior acknowledgments also appear at www.marksschwartzphd.com, as I continue to appreciate all those mentioned, even more, if that is possible.) Mark S. Schwartz and Frank Andrasik: Many thanks to The Guilford Press staff for continued acceptance, support, patience, and flexibility. Special thanks to Jane Keislar, Senior Assistant Editor, and Jim Nageotte, Senior Editor, for their incredible patience, flexibility, and attention to detail during the lengthy process of compiling a book of this magnitude. Thanks also to Carolyn Graham, who managed many of the preproduction administrative matters for the book; to the wonderful copy editor, Jacquelyn Coggin, for her skills and tact; and to Senior Production Editor Anna Nelson, for her patience and flexibility in complet­ing the final editorial and production stages for the book. We also express our considerable appreciation to our authors, who remained devoted, patient, and persistent with the unexpected and unplanned delays along the way. Finally, we remain somewhat at a loss for words to adequately thank our wives, Nancy and Candy, for coping with our late nights and long weekends, while we sat at our keyboards when we should have been sitting with them. xv Contents Part I. Orientation to Biofeedback 1.The History and Definitions of Biofeedback and Applied Psychophysiology 3 Mark S. Schwartz, Thomas F. Collura, Joe Kamiya, and Nancy M. Schwartz 2.Entering the Field and Assuring Competence 24 Fredric Shaffer and Mark S. Schwartz Part II. Instrumentation 3. A Primer of Traditional Biofeedback Instrumentation 35 C. J. Peek 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography: Instrumentation and Applications 68 Jeffrey E. Bolek, Ronald L. Rosenthal, and Richard A. Sherman 5.Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment in Applied Psychophysiology 85 Richard N. Gevirtz, Mark S. Schwartz, and Paul M. Lehrer 6.Electroencephalographic Measures and Biofeedback: A Primer 98 Nicola Neumann, Ute Strehl, Niels Birbaumer, and Boris Kotchoubey 7. Quantitative Encephalography and Electroencephalographic Biofeedback/Neurofeedback Robert W. Thatcher xvii 113 xviii Contents 8. Introduction to Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines 128 John G. Arena and Mark S. Schwartz 9. Consumer‑ and Home‑Based Biofeedback 154 Mark S. Schwartz and Frank Andrasik PART III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS 10. Dietary Considerations 163 Keith I. Block, Charlotte Gyllenhaal, and Mark S. Schwartz 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive‑Behavioral Interventions: Reciprocal Contributions 176 Daniel Hamiel and Arnon Rolnick PART IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS 12. Relaxation Today: Self‑Stressing and Psychological Relaxation Theory 189 Jonathan C. Smith 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback 196 Richard N. Gevirtz, Paul M. Lehrer, and Mark S. Schwartz PART V. PRACTICE ISSUES 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention 217 Mark S. Schwartz 15. Adherence 233 Jeanetta C. Rains and Mark S. Schwartz 16. Problems Associated with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Guidelines for Management 249 Mark S. Schwartz, Nancy M. Schwartz, and Vincent J. Monastra 17. Ethical Practice Issues and Concerns 260 Sebastian Striefel 18. Myths, Insurance, and Other Professional Topics and Issues 272 Sebastian Striefel, Ronald L. Rosenthal, Robert Whitehouse, and Mark S. Schwartz 19. Evaluating Research in Clinical Biofeedback Frank Andrasik and Mark S. Schwartz 290 xix Contents Part VI. Clinical Applications: Traditional General Biofeedback Practice 20. Headache 305 Frank Andrasik and Mark S. Schwartz 21. Temporomandibular Muscle and Joint Disorders 356 Alan G. Glaros and Leonard L. Lausten 22. Raynaud’s Disease and Raynaud’s Phenomenon 369 Maria Katsamanis, Mark S. Schwartz, and Keith Sedlacek 23. Essential Hypertension 383 Wolfgang Linden and Angele V. McGrady 24. Diabetes Mellitus 400 Angele V. McGrady and Deloris M. Lakia 25. Tinnitus: Nothing Is as Loud as a Sound You Are Trying Not to Hear 422 Herta Flor and Mark S. Schwartz 26. Anxiety Disorders 439 Arnon Rolnick, Dana Bassett, Udi Gal, and Anat Barnea 27. Fibromyalgia Syndrome 453 Peter T. Dorsher and Mark S. Schwartz 28. Irritable Bowel Syndrome 466 Mark S. Schwartz 29. Asthma 480 Paul M. Lehrer and Frederick Wamboldt Part VII. Clinical Applications: Electroencephalographic, Neuromuscular, and Pelvic Floor Biofeedback Specialties 30. Attention‑Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 493 Vincent J. Monastra and Joel F. Lubar 31. Neuromuscular Reeducation and Gait Training 525 Timothy L. Fagerson and David E. Krebs 32. Bowel, Bladder, and Pelvic Floor Disorders Jeannette Tries and Eugene Eisman 545 xx Contents Part VIII. Optimization 33. Performing Artists 587 Marcie Zinn and Mark Zinn 34.Sports 607 Vietta Sue Wilson, Wesley E. Sime, and Timothy Harkness Part IX. Other Special Populations and Applications 35. Pediatric Applications 629 Timothy Culbert and Gerard A. Banez 36. Work‑Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 651 Susan Middaugh Part X. The Frontier and a Nod to the Future 37. Biofeedback of Slow Cortical Potentials in Epilepsy 671 Ute Strehl 38.Traumatic Brain Injury, Quantitative Electroencephalography, and Electroencephalographic Biofeedback 677 Kirtley E. Thornton 39. Biofeedback Interventions for Autism Spectrum Disorders 686 Lynda Thompson, Michael Thompson, James W. G. Thompson, and Andrea Reid‑Chung 40. Brain–Computer Communication: An Alternative Communication Channel for Paralyzed Patients 697 Tobias Kaufmann, Niels Birbaumer, and Andrea Kübler 41.Substance Use Disorders and Neurofeedback 707 Estate M. “Tato” Sokhadze, David L. Trudeau, Rex L. Cannon, Eugenia Bodenhamer‑Davis, and Richard E. Davis 42. More Frontiers and Further Forward 717 Mark S. Schwartz and Frank Andrasik Index 739 Part I Orientation to Biofeedback Chapter 1 The History and Definitions of Biofeedback and Applied Psychophysiology Mark S. Schwartz, Thomas F. Collura, Joe Kamiya, and Nancy M. Schwartz This chapter conveys the converging trends that influenced the development and journey of applied biofeedback, and the broader field of applied psychophysiology.1 This historical perspective is designed to help the reader understand the origins of the multifaceted and multimodality field of biofeedback, including a history of specialty modalities and applications (e.g., electroencephalographic [EEG] biofeedback, a.k.a. neurofeedback). It also seeks to help illuminate the broader concept of applied psychophysiology, and to give perspective to the name changes of the primary professional membership organization and its journal. Applied biofeedback began in the United States with the convergence of many disciplines in the late 1950s. The major antecedents and fields from which it developed include the following. (Italics on the first use of a term indicate that the term is included in the Glossary.) 7. Consciousness, altered states of conscious- ness, and electroencephalography (EEG biofeedback also known as neurofeedback) 8. Cybernetics 9. Cultural factors. 10. Professional developments2 11. Definitions The order of the items in this list reflects neither historical sequence nor relative importance. Other historical perspectives on biofeedback may be found in Basmajian (1989), Shaffer (2010), and Peper and Shaffer (2010). (See www.marksschwartzphd.com for other references and links to selected historical perspectives.) Instrumental Conditioning of ANS Responses Learning theory developed within experimental psychology. Reinforcement is necessary for operant conditioning or instrumental conditioning to occur. From this perspective, both overt behaviors and covert behaviors, such as thoughts, feelings, and physiological responses, are functions of the antecedents and consequences of such behaviors. This model describes the learning of responses instrumental to obtaining positive or avoiding negative consequences. 1. Instrumental conditioning of autonomic nervous system (ANS) responses 2. Psychophysiology 3. Behavior therapy and behavioral medicine 4. Stress research and stress management strate- gies 5. Biomedical engineering 6. Surface electromyography (EMG), diagnostic EMG, and control of single motor units 3 4 The prevailing scientific viewpoint for several decades has been that only the voluntary musculoskeletal system, mediated by the central nervous system (CNS), is responsive to operant conditioning. The older view held that the ANS functioned automatically beyond conscious awareness, and hence beyond voluntary control. Most scientists thought that the internal, homeostatic controls for functions such as circulation and digestion were innate and unaffected by self-regulatory learning. Most scientists assumed that ANS functioning or visceral learning was modifiable only via classical conditioning, if subject to learning at all. In this view, responses are automatic after conditioning occurs. In classical conditioning, thoughts can even become conditioned stimuli (CSs) and elicit physiological responses. The strong biases against instrumental conditioning of the ANS and the visceral responses it controls limited the amount of experimental work in this area until a few decades ago (Miller, 1978). Studies with humans and animals showed that instrumental training could produce increases and decreases in bodily responses (see early reviews by Harris & Brady, 1974; and Kimmel, 1979, and Taub, 2010). Research indicated that individuals could gain volitional control over several different ANS functions without learning that could be attributed to cognitive factors. Many scientists and professionals were very skeptical of these findings. There was much disagreement concerning whether the research really demonstrated cortical control over ANS activity. As research advanced, it became clear that to show operant learning effects in the ANS, researchers needed more sophisticated designs. They had to rule out skeletally mediated mechanical artifacts and visceral reflexes. The best organized and most articulate history of the very challenging research on instrumental conditioning of autonomic response systems, and in particular the brilliance of Neal Miller, was provided by Ed Taub (2010). Every student of the history of biofeedback, the history of psychology, and indeed research methods, should read this presentation by Taub. (With permission, the entire article is reproduced at www.marksschwartzphd. com for readers who are interested.) • Eliminating or ruling out somatic mediation of the autonomic responses was the problem to be resolved. • Studies of heart rate changes with chemically paralyzing and artificially respirated rats, thus I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK without somatic mediation, were reported (Miller & DiCara, 1967; DiCara & Miller 1968a, 1968b, 1968c, 1968d). • Instrumental condtioning of autonomic functioning was controversial among psychophysiological researchers. • Several attempts by other researchers and by Miller and his students to replicate these studies were unsuccessful. Most researchers who were familiar with this research attributed the DiCara and Miller (1967a) results to an anomaly despite other, similar and reliable, albeit smaller, results by other investigators (Trowill, 1967; Hothersall & Brener, 1969; Slaughter et al., 1970). • Miller and his students made extensive and meticulous efforts to reproduce the studies and although unsuccessful, the process provided an outstanding example of the Strong Inference (Dworkin & Miller, 1986; Taub, 2010) research model. They evaluated a large number of alternative hypotheses. • Adverse publicity about the unsuccessful replications created a strong negative association. • Taub (2010) pointed out the terrifying limitations of any attempted learning research with paralyzed rats or any vertebrate. • The second experimental question regarding biofeedback instrumental conditioning of any ANS response without somatic mediation was reported by Miller and Brucker (1979) with patients with quadriplegia, thus without sufficient somatic muscle activity mediating the strong increase in blood pressure enough to manage the low blood pressure (i.e., orthostatic) due to the patients typical reclining position. Miller and Brucker noted that the results were “strongly indicating that these patients can learn unusually large increases in blood pressure and that this visceral response can be performed independently of skeletal responses” (Taub, 2010, p. 113). • Taub’s thermal biofeedback studies (Slattery & Taub, 1976; Taub & School, 1978) extended the research regarding instrumental conditioning of ANS without somatic mediation. Temperature biofeedback from varied and specific locations on a hand resulted in “very clear anatomical differentiation of the temperature response” and “a large response around the feedback locus, and much less or none at other locations” (Taub, 2010, p. 113). • Taub (2010) reported his research with 11 participants attempting to alter skin temperature 5 1. History and Definitions up or down on one digit compared to another. He reported that for eight subjects, there was “significantly greater temperature response on the designated digit than at the other one” (Taub, 2010, p. 114). With other controls, they concluded that “the anatomical specificity results represented differential alterations in blood flow and were not due to an artifact . . . [and] in particular, not to somatic mediation involving muscle activity changes from any of the locations . . . recorded” (Taub, 2010, p. 114). The research with instrumental conditioning of visceral responses mediated by the ANS provided a major impetus to the development of clinical biofeedback. It appeared to resolve the controversy over whether such conditioning was a legitimate phenomenon. An assumption of clinical biofeedback is that it can help persons improve the accuracy of their perceptions of their visceral events. These perceptions allow them to gain greater selfregulation of these processes. This operant model of biofeedback has significant heuristic value. One can apply principles of instrumental conditioning to physiological selfregulation. Although it is helpful to view biofeedback primarily as instrumental conditioning of visceral responses, this model is limiting in that some professionals believe that human learning includes major cognitive dimensions, as well as environmental reinforcers, for example, thinking, expectation, visualization and imagery, foresight and planning, and problem-solving strategies. One can include cognitive factors within the operant conditioning model. However, professionals adhering to more stringent interpretations of the model consider cognitive factors inadmissible, because one cannot observe or objectively measure them. Nevertheless, studies of motor skill learning (Blumenthal, 1977) show that humans develop mental models (“motor programs”) of what a skilled movement should be like. Furthermore, research shows that one may acquire behavior without obvious practice or even reinforcement. This evidence comes from latent learning experiments (Harlow & Harlow, 1962), studies of discovery learning (Bruner, 1966), and studies of observational learning involving imitation of a model (Rosenthal & Zimmerman, 1978). Increased acceptance for the role of mental processes in learning led to cognitive-behavioral therapies and studies of cognitively mediated strategies in the changes occurring during biofeedback therapies. The emphasis on cognitive learning also supported the applications of cybernetics to biofeedback. Psychophysiology David Shapiro offered the first academic course in psychophysiology at Harvard University in 1965. The Handbook of Psychophysiology, a major publication, appeared 7 years later (Greenfield & Sternback, 1972). Psychophysiology involves the scientific study of the interrelationships of physiological and cognitive processes. Some consider it a special branch of physiology. Others also consider it an offspring of psychobiology, which in turn is the child of the marriage between the physical and social sciences (Hassett, 1978). Physiological psychologists often manipulate physiology and observe behavior. In contrast, psychophysiologists often facilitate, manage, guide, hinder, or obstruct human psychological variables and observe the physiological effects. As a form of “applied psychophysiology,”3 clinical biofeedback helps people alter their behaviors with feedback from their physiology. Some providers of clinical biofeedback used to refer to themselves as “clinical psychophysiologists.” Behavior Therapy and Behavioral Medicine The fields of behavior therapy and behavioral medicine are related outgrowths of both learning theory and psychophysiology. “Behavior therapy” developed in the 1950s as an alternative to insightoriented psychodynamic theories and therapies for mental disorders. The roots of behavior therapy include the notion that one learns maladaptive behaviors; therefore, in most cases, one can unlearn them. The model is largely educational rather than medical. It applies the principles of operant and respondent conditioning, as well as of cognitive learning, to change a wide range of behaviors. Many professionals view some biofeedback applications as a form of operant learning. Others view biofeedback more cognitively within an information-processing model. “Behavioral medicine” is another outgrowth of learning theory, psychophysiology, and behavior therapy. This specialty developed within behavior therapy and psychosomatic medicine. It appeared as a distinct entity in the late 1970s. Behavioral medicine focuses on applications of learning theo- 6 I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK ries to medical disorders and other health-related topics. It does not focus on psychopathology or mental disorders. G. E. Schwartz and Weiss (1978) reported a definition of behavioral medicine proposed at the Yale Conference held in 1977: Behavior medicine is the field concerned with the development of behavior science knowledge and techniques relevant to the understanding of physical health and illness and the application of this knowledge and these techniques to diagnosis, prevention, treatment, and rehabilitation. Psychosis, neurosis, and substance abuse are included only insofar as they contribute to physical disorders as an end point. (p. 379) Behavioral medicine also developed because traditional medical approaches were insufficient for managing and treating many chronic diseases, conditions, and health-damaging or maladaptive behaviors. This new specialty goes beyond the traditional germ theory of the etiology and progression of diseases. It recognizes the important roles of stress, lifestyle, habits, and environmental variables in the development, maintenance, and treatment of medical and dental diseases and conditions.4 Behavioral medicine places much emphasis on the patient’s role in prevention of and recovery from organic diseases and conditions. The same emphases are clear in applied or clinical biofeedback. In fact, some professionals consider clinical biofeedback to be a major specialty within the broader field of behavioral medicine (Birk, 1973; Olton & Noonberg, 1980). The contributions of behavior therapy and behavioral medicine to the development and applications of applied biofeedback and applied psychophysiology are clear. The interactions among professionals from all of these fields will continue to be enriching. Stress Research, Relaxation Therapies, and Other Stress Management Techniques An important area of behavioral medicine is research on the effects of stress on causing physical symptoms and altering the immune system. However, research on stress began long before the development of behavioral medicine or biofeedback; in fact, both fields have their roots partly in stress research. One has only to remember Hans Selye’s (1974) report of more than 130,000 entries on stress that showed the extent of this already immense body of research. Pioneering research was conducted by the physicians Claude Bernard and Walter B. Cannon, as well as by Selye. Pi Suñer (1955) observed that Bernard developed the concepts of physiological “homeostasis” as the major process by which the body maintains itself. As Langley (1965) noted, the concept became integral to the discipline of physiology. Physical and mental disease are thought to occur because some homeostatic feedback mechanism is malfunctioning. One of the major effects of such homeostatic imbalance is stress. In his book The Wisdom of the Body, Cannon (1932) indicated the natural causes and results of the innate stress response. He named this response fight or flight. Selye’s (1974, 1976, 1983) extensive research led to a triphasic conceptualization of the nature of the physiological stress response: the stages of alarm, resistance, and exhaustion. The brilliant and pioneering work of Cannon and Selye contributed significantly to the development of the field of psychosomatic medicine. Their work increased awareness of the role of stress in physical and mental diseases. This awareness nurtured applied biofeedback, and many of these applications focused on stress-related disorders. Furthermore, as noted by Miller (1978), the emphasis of biofeedback on measuring and producing changes in bodily processes contributes to other behavioral techniques for relieving stress effects. Many stress management systems evolved with the awareness of the effects of stress on health and disease. Included among these are many relaxation therapies, and some observers perceive biofeedback as a specific treatment modality within this group. In practice, the effects of relaxation have a major role in achieving the therapeutic effects with some forms of biofeedback. A very early form of physical relaxation is “hatha yoga,” adopted from the Far East and popularized in Western countries in the 1960s. In the United States in the 1930s, Edmund Jacobson (1938, 1978) developed “progressive relaxation” (PR), sometimes also called “progressive muscle relaxation,” which consists of a series of muscle activities designed to teach people ways to distinguish degrees of tension and relaxation, and to reduce specific and general muscle tension. It also helps reduce or stop many symptoms and some causes and adverse effects of stress. McGuigan and Lehrer (2007), as two of Jacobson’s students and ardent authorities, discussed the history and techniques from their unique perspec- 7 1. History and Definitions tive. Lichstein (1988) provided one of the most thorough detailed texts on relaxation strategies and research results. Other very useful resources are the books by Smith (1989, 1990, 2001, 2005; also see Chapter 12 in this volume). Modifications of progressive relaxation have been developed by Wolpe (1973), Bernstein and Borkovec (1973), Bernstein, Carlson, and Schmidt (2007), and Jacobson and McGuigan (1982). A related technique developed in England by Mitchell (1977, 1987) involves stretch–release procedures. In addition to the physiological relaxation procedures, there has been a proliferation of primarily mental techniques, most of which involve some form of meditation. Islamic Sufis, Hindu yogis, Christian contemplatives, and Hasidic Jews have practiced religious meditation for centuries. Meditation became popularized in the United States in the 1960s as a result of the development of Transcendental Meditation (TM), practiced and promoted by a teacher from India named Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (Forem, 1974). More Westernized variations of TM were subsequently developed as “clinically standardized meditation” (Carrington, 1977, 1978, 1998, 2007) and the “relaxation response” (Benson, 1975). Stroebel’s (1982) “quieting reflex” is a modification of a meditation technique combined with physiological relaxation. Another meditation approach is “open focus,” developed by Fehmi and Fritz (1980), which has recently experiencined a contemporary updating (Fehmi & Robbins, 2009). It is closer to Soto Zen meditation in its goal of seeking a content-free and quiet mind, by contrast with the focused concentration of yoga and TM. The emigration of Zen Buddhist teachers to the United States beginning in the 1940s was yet another factor contributing to the meditation movement. See Carrington (2007) and Kristeller (2007) for more history of modern forms of mantra meditation and for mindfulness meditation, respectively. There are still other approaches involving relaxation/meditation: Ira Progoff’s (1980) “process meditation,” José Silva’s (1977) “Silva mind control,” and C. Norman Shealy’s (1977) “biogenics.” Practitioners often combine relaxation/meditation techniques with biofeedback instrumentation to enhance the learning of psychophysiological selfregulation. Hypnosis is yet another approach developed to help persons to control pain and stress. Hypnosis developed slowly from the 1700s until the 20th century. Over the past few decades it has become more sophisticated and empirically grounded as a set of therapeutic techniques. Liebeault, Charcot, and Freud were among the first to apply the techniques to patients (Moss, 1965). Twentiethcentury researchers, such as Hull, Barber, Hilgard, Weitzenhoffer, and Erickson, conducted rigorous investigations into the parameters of hypnosis. Some, like Wickramasekera (1976, 1988), reported integrations of hypnosis and biofeedback. In Germany, early in the 20th century, J. H. Schultz developed a form of physiologically directed, self-generated therapy called “autogenic training” or “autogenic therapy.” Wolfgang Luthe (1969) brought it to North America and reported extensive research and therapeutic applications of this technique, variations of which are now also in common practice. Biomedical Engineering Without high-quality instrumentation for measuring physiological events accurately and reliably, there would be no biofeedback. As Tarlar-Benlolo (1978) reminds us, “prior to World War II, available equipment was not sufficiently sensitive for measuring most of the body’s internally generated electrical impulses” (p. 728). Progress occurred after the war. Biomedical engineers have developed technology that is both noninvasive and very sophisticated. Surface recordings used for biofeedback measurement provide feedback for many different physiological activities. Feedback can also be provided for angles of limbs and the force of muscles and limbs. Instruments continuously monitor, amplify, and transform electronic and electromechanical signals into audio and visual feedback— understandable information. Now multiple and simultaneous recordings of several channels of physiological information are available with instrumentation linked to computers. Computers allow great storage capabilities, rapid signal processing and statistical analyses, simultaneous recording and integration of multiple channels, and displays that only a few years ago were impossible. EMG, Diagnostic EMG, and Single‑Motor‑Unit Control The workhorse of the biofeedback field has long been surface electromyography (abbreviated here as EMG, though SEMG is also used). According to 8 Basmajian (1983), EMG instrumentation grew out of the studies of neuromuscular and spinal cord functions. He reminds us that “it began with the classic paper in 1929 by Adrian and Bronk, who showed that the electrical responses in individual muscles provided an accurate reflection of the actual functional activity of the muscles” (p. 2). Physicians have used EMG for diagnosing neuromuscular disorders for many decades. As early as 1934, reports indicated that voluntary, conscious control over the EMG potential of single motor units was possible (Smith, 1934). Marinacci and Horande (1960) added case reports of the potential value of displaying EMG signals to assist patients in neuromuscular reeducation. Basmajian (1963, 1978) also reported on the successful control of single motor units. Several investigators reported EMG feedback in the rehabilitation of patients after stroke (Andrews, 1964; Binder-MacLeod, 1983; Brudny, 1982; Basmajian, Kukulka, Narayan, & Takebe, 1975; Wolf & Binder-MacLeod, 1983). Such research was important in the development of applied biofeedback, especially for the field of neuromuscular rehabilitation. Thus, EMG biofeedback gained solid support among researchers and clinicians. Practitioners have also used EMG feedback for treating symptoms and disorders such as tension headaches and tension myalgias, temporomandibular disorders, pelvic floor disorders that include incontinence, and many other conditions (see Part VI, this volume). Consciousness, Altered States Of Consciousness, and EEG Feedback Humanistic psychology reestablished the human self as a legitimate source of inquiry, and scientists in transpersonal psychology and neurophysiology renewed the study of human consciousness. Theorists such as Tart (1969), Krippner (1972), Ornstein (1972), Pelletier and Garfield (1976), G. E. Schwartz and Beatty (1977), and Jacobson (1982) are among those who have made significant contributions to our understanding of human consciousness. Many studies of altered states of consciousness induced by drugs, hypnosis, or meditation have added to our knowledge of the relationships between brain functioning and human behavior. Such research helped stimulate the use of electro- I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK encephalography (EEG) in biofeedback, which also focuses on the functional relationships between brain and behavior. In the early 1960s, studies began appearing on the relationships between EEG alpha wave activity (8–12 hertz [Hz]) on the one hand, and emotional states and certain states of consciousness on the other. Alpha biofeedback, commonly reported as associated with a relaxed but alert state, received its most attention in the late 1960s. Clinical applications were mostly for general relaxation. Kamiya (1969) reported that one could voluntarily control alpha waves—previously believed impossible Support came from Brown (1977), Nowlis and Kamiya (1970), and Hart (1968). “Though these studies tended to lack systematic controls, they nonetheless caught the imagination of many serious scientists as well as the media” (Orne, 1979, p. 493). Some investigators and practitioners continued to advocate the value of alpha biofeedback through the early 1980s (e.g., see Gaarder & Montgomery, 1981, for a discussion), despite recognizing that “there was no clear-cut and concrete rationale to explain why it should help patients” (p. 155). In contrast, Basmajian (1983) noted that “alpha feedback . . . has virtually dried up as a scientifically defensible clinical tool. . . . It has . . . returned to the research laboratory from which it probably should not have emerged prematurely. Through the next generation of scientific investigation, it may return as a useful applied technique” (p. 3). Other investigators studied specialized learning processes and other EEG parameters, such as theta waves, evoked cortical responses, and EEG phase synchrony of multiple areas of the cortex (Beatty, Greenberg, Deibler, & O’Hanlon, 1974). Selected brain areas and EEG parameters (e.g., sensorimotor rhythm and slow-wave activity) became the focus of well-controlled studies. These emerged as effective therapeutic approaches for carefully selected patients with CNS disorders such as epilepsy (Lubar, 1982, 1983; Sterman, 1982; see Strehl, Chapter 37, this volume), as well as for some patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Lubar, 1991; see also Monastra & Lubar, Chapter 30, this volume). More recently, EEG feedback procedures purport to be successful in treating patients with a wide variety of other symptoms and disorders. The growth and scope of EEG biofeedback is partially reflected in the changes in this text now, with eight chapters compared to two in the third edition and one in the first two editions. 1. History and Definitions History and Development of EEG Biofeedback Technology EEG biofeedback, sometimes referred to as “neurofeedback,” began with the approach of enhancing a particular frequency band, generally alpha, as a means of achieving benefits associated with greater presence of that band in the EEG. When initial work began, some systems used conventional EEG systems, and augmented them with additional circuitry. Others were developed entirely “standalone,” with amplifiers, processing circuits, and output devices (lights, speakers, etc.) as an integral part of the design. Early research used such custom-engineered systems to produce important initial results (e.g., Nowlis & Kamiya, 1970). As the field began to mature, manufacturers began to introduce products capable of measuring and feeding back EEG signals as their primary purpose. A major limitation of early EEG feedback devices was that they filtered the desired band to indicate its presence but had no provision to ensure that out-of-band signals did not also contribute the feedback. For example, low-frequency signals due to theta waves, eye movement, motion artifact, or other non-alpha phenomena, if sufficiently large, could still produce enough output to trigger the reward. Similarly, high-frequency signals, including EMG and other artifacts, could also produce output within the desired training band, again imprecisely rewarding the trainee. As a result, early alpha trainers produced inconsistent results that contributed to a general lack of acceptance as useful professional tools. Early “recreational” alpha trainers, circa 1975, were primitive and not only trained alpha but also rewarded various artifacts such as muscle twitches and eye movements. (Interested readers can find photos of an early recreational alpha trainer, circa 1975, and an Autogenics 120 analog EEG trainer at www. marksschwartzphd.com). During this time, professional biofeedback trainers were also being developed and applied. These remained entirely analog, and provided a display meter, and generally simple tones. A great deal of research was conducted using these devices, so that by 1978, dozens of studies including EEG biofeedback had appeared in the literature (Butler, 1978). As EEG feedback equipment became more refined, and in particular when digital computers began to be used, it became possible to introduce “inhibit” bands, which were used to block feed- 9 back from occurring when these signals were present. The ability to withhold feedback when excessive slow or fast waves were present was a key step in the refinement of EEG biofeedback, and made it possible to produce useful and consistent clinical results. The use of these outer “guard” bands became very common and produced a generation of feedback trainers that accurately rewarded the desired EEG frequency components, without providing false feedback due to artifacts (Ayers, Sams, Sterman, & Lubar, 2000). Early PC-based EEG biofeedback systems were implemented on platforms such as the Apple and IBM PC, and used simple “text-based” operating systems. Supplementary graphics and sound were generally very simple, yet effective. With the introduction of Windows and the Apple Macintosh, software became increasingly sophisticated. It became possible for programmers to incorporate advanced signal processing, graphics, video, multimedia, interactive games, and other capabilities, further enriching the feedback and improving responses. Computers were used for delivering EEG biofeedback as early as the late 1970s. However, the processing speed was insufficient to keep up with the signal processing demands, which limited their utility It was not until the second and third generations of processors, when math coprocessors became available in the mid-1980s, that it was possible, for example, to perform a 256-point fast Fourier transform (FFT) in substantially less than a second. In the 1990s, computer speed became fast enough to provide real-time signal processing and adequate displays for useful training. During the evolution of these techniques, certain aspects became paramount and hotly debated. Among these was “response time,” which was construed to mean “the delay between the time something happens in the brain and the time it appears on the screen.” While apparently straightforward, this definition cannot really be applied as such. In the world of real-time signals, digital filters, frequency transforms, and such, signals do not simply “come and go.” Rather, they wax and wane, have varying amplitudes and time courses, and responses are analog, or graded, not simply on or off. As a result, it is necessary to consider filter response and other factors in evaluating system capabilities. In an early Windows-based EEG biofeedback system (Ayers) there was one enhance band (labeled “Facilitate”) and one inhibit band (see examples at www.marksschwartzphd.com). 10 The response curve shown demonstrates that in order to respond to the narrow bandwidth of the 15.0–18.0 Hz range, the filter requires several cycles of the input wave in order to respond. This response characteristic is identical to that seen with analog filters. In other words, while digital processing provides benefits in the form of programmability, flexibility, and exotic displays, it cannot violate the basic laws of physics, and the ability to respond to EEG waves is in principle the same in digital as it is in analog systems. Currently, most EEG biofeedback systems are PC-based. Thus, the hardware typically consists of an amplifier/digitizer (“encoder”) that transmits EEG data to the PC in a digital form. From then on, the system depends entirely on the PC software, which can consist of thousands of lines of software code, developed over many programmer years of effort. EEG biofeedback has thus followed the trend of many other industries that have become dominated by software issues, and follow an aggressive and rapid evolution spurred on by continuous competition and the continual entry of new developers. With the flexibility of computerized EEG biofeedback (Collura, 1995), seemingly rigid rules have been stretched and even broken. For example, with the introduction of multiple frequency bands for analysis and the ability to either reinforce or to inhibit any of them, a variety of creative protocols emerged. Based on principles of learning theory, different methods of adjusting thresholds exploit different aspects of the nervous system having to do with perceived rewards, motivation, level of difficulty, and so on. Where computerized EEG biofeedback systems have excelled is in the use of complex rules to compute and deliver feedback, and the control of engaging and meaningful displays such as animation, video, games, and various types of specially designed software. The use of computerized signal processing has also allowed the introduction of a plethora of alternative methods and approaches, embodying physiological and mathematical concepts including nonlinear systems, chaos, coherence and stability, synchrony, selfadaptive systems, and normative databases. Some EEG biofeedback systems appeal to concepts generally derived from “quantum physics,” “subtle energy,” and other seemingly esoteric areas. While these include systems that are well studied and published, there are others that appeal more to an article of faith than to peer-reviewed studies. I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK One element that has carried forward from initial systems, and continues to be in contention, is the issue of “monopolar” versus “bipolar” recording. Depending on whether the EEG is referenced to a neutral site or to another active site, the type of information available is profoundly different, and impacts the ability to train synchrony, connectivity, and other brain properties (Collura, 2009; Fehmi & Collura, 2007). Early Investigations Leading to Neurofeedback Our aims in this section are to (1) describe some of the early work that led to the field of research and therapeutic application of EEG biofeedback/ neurofeedback and (2) address the question of what internal behaviors or private experiences are involved in learning to produce changes in specific EEG measures with the aid of neurofeedback. My (Kamiya) research with the EEG was conducted to pursue questions concerning relationships between the EEG of persons and their consciousness. This interest in EEG research developed when I was working in the sleep laboratory of Nathaniel Kleitman and his student and research assistant William Dement, at the University of Chicago. It was from that laboratory that Aserinsky and Kleitman (1953) and Dement and Kleitman (1957) published the pioneering papers that indicated dreaming during sleep usually was accompanied by specific changes in the sleeping person’s EEG and eye movements as monitored by the electrooculogram (EOG). Their papers did much to put private experience on the scientific map. Kleitman generously offered me the use of his laboratory to conduct some studies of my own. Dement taught me the technology of EEG and EOG recording of sleeping subjects. I completed a study on other physiological concomitants of drowsiness and sleep (Kamiya, 1961). My student and colleague, Johann Stoyva, joined me in the laboratory. In addition, in response to confusion and disagreements in the field on the problem of how to interpret the occasional fact that reports of dreaming would occur despite the absence of their EEG and EOG indicators, and the absence of dream reports when the EEG and EOG indicate dreaming had occurred, we published an analysis of the logic of the relations between verbal reports and physiological indicators as convergent indicators of private events such as dreaming (Stoyva & Kamiya, 1968). This problem is worth mentioning here because it arises in connection with 1. History and Definitions the validity of the evidence of any sort of private experience, not just dreams. In the course of preparing a subject for all night recording with EEG and EOG electrodes, I always conducted a test of the EEG on the polygraph for a minute while the subject was still awake to make sure that all the electrode contacts with the scalp provided clean traces. It was during these tests that I noticed the irregularly timed appearance and disappearance of the EEG alpha rhythms. I wondered if they were related to any features of the consciousness of the person. How this interest led to the development of methods for studying that possibility, and eventually to the adoption of the method by others in the treatment of neurological disorders, is described in what follows. There are wide variations in the characteristics of the trains of alpha rhythms. The question that motivated me was whether there are subjective concomitants associated with the moments when alpha rhythms are present as opposed to when they are absent. Might there be a difference in the feel or mental activity between the two EEG states in the relatively short-term alternations between the two that occur several times a minute? Considering what is known about conditions affecting alpha would help provide hints toward an answer. (For more on alpha rhythms, see www. marksschwartzphd.com). Later, when I joined the Department of Psychiatry at the University of California at San Francisco (UCSF) in 1961 and moved my laboratory equipment there, I added an improvement over the on-or-off character of the feedback in earlier studies. A continuously graded tone volume from silent to loud now reflected the 1-second moving average amplitude of alpha, thus improving the information in the signal. The participant now could monitor his or her performance more accurately. We also changed the score presented every minute from total time of alpha above threshold to the average amplitude of alpha for the minute. With these improvements in the feedback, the performance of the trainees improved, and interest level was maintained. I believe the several reported failures by other investigators to replicate the results we had obtained showing increases in the average trainee of alpha relative to initial baseline scores were, in many cases, due to inadequate equipment. But a major part of the reported failures of replication to train increases in alpha amplitude was due simply to insufficient total duration of training, as discussed by Hardt 11 and Kamiya (1976) and Ancoli and Kamiya (1978). Several of the reported failures simply reflected the stopping of training after one or two sessions. In our laboratory we found that the first and second training sessions, with each session lasting about 45 minutes, resulted in average scores actually lower than the initial session baselines for each session. Plotting the average performance over six sessions of training, we saw a substantial drop in alpha relative to baseline scores in the first session, followed by a gradual increase in performance across sessions until the third session, when the trial scores and session baseline scores were about the same. It was not until the fourth session was reached that the trainees had increased their performance sufficiently to exceed their baseline for that session. At least one factor, probably the major one, to account for the puzzling drop in performance relative to the session initial baseline and slow recovery across three sessions is that the challenge to find a mental state, feeling, and so forth, requires a busy mind in search mode, but that reduces alpha activity. Many a trainee has commented on the fact that trying to solve how to increase the tone level served only to reduce it. It is also possible that the lack of progress is perceived as failure by the trainee, and the resulting ego-threat and anxiety cause more alpha reduction. Some of the trainees’ comments may best describe the situation: “The harder I try, the more the tone goes away”; “I gave up trying to increase the tone, and damned if it didn’t get louder”; “I seem to do best when I just wait and let it come on by itself and be happy when it does.” Overall, it seems that even though verbal descriptions of the two states tend to agree among trainees, and tend to support descriptions made by earlier investigators, I believe that the use of everyday language has its limits as a way of characterizing the subjective experiences associated with the two states, particularly the state of alpha dominance. In short, the results indicate that it is far easier to detect a difference between these two brain states than it is to be able to describe what comprises the difference. However, it is also possible that learning to discern that there is a difference between alpha and non-alpha is only a first stage of coming into awareness of what the factors behind the difference are. The best hint yet of an answer to the question posed by our results may come from another experiment that I had completed earlier. Because 12 it might be that the apparent rise in alpha in the feedback experiment with the tone was at least to some degree due to the trainee becoming accustomed to the laboratory over repeated session, I gave new trainees alternating blocks of alphaincrease trials with alpha-decrease trials. I used five 1-minute increase trials, providing a quantitative score of alpha output after each minute. Then I delivered five 1-minute trials to decrease their alpha tone, also with a score of alpha output after each minute. The subjects now learned to increase and decrease their scores quite efficiently (Kamiya, 1968). It seemed more helpful in sharpening their differentiation of the two in their verbal reports than the task given them earlier to only increase alpha amplitude. It is possible that the improvement reflects an increased opportunity to sense the difference between the two states by having them alternate within close temporal sequence the internal behaviors or mental states that are instrumental in producing the two states. Applications of the Method to Clinically Relevant Physiological Measures The field of EEG feedback or neurofeedback has not been particularly concerned with research issues such as the ones I (Kamiya) have been describing. Instead, rather quickly it became clear that the method of feedback training could yield some immediately practical results in the clinic as a method of treating neurological and psychological disorders that were known to be related to specific characteristics in the EEG. The fields of neurofeedback for the treatment of epilepsy and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are two of the best examples. Sterman led the way in neurofeedback for treating epilepsy when he and Wyrwicka (Wyrwicka & Sterman, 1968) reported that in cats, the Sensory Motor Rhythm, their name for a burst of synchronized EEG activity over the motor cortex in an awake animal, could be brought under control by operant conditioning. The same sensory motor rhythm, when brought under operant control by humans, was found to suppress epileptic seizures (Sterman & Friar, 1972). Lubar and Shouse (1976) reported that a hyperkinetic child could be treated successfully with sensory motor rhythm training, thus starting the use of the method for the treatment of ADHD in many different laboratories and clinics, including that of Michael and Lynda Thompson (1998). Thompson and Thompson (2003) have I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK since published a comprehensive book, The Neurofeedback Book, that has become a standard reference, as well as an aid for training therapists in the methods of EEG. The investigators who have worked and published their results in these fields for several decades now, led by Sterman, Lubar, the Thompsons, and several others have developed protocols that will eventually revamp the thinking of the medical fields toward neurofeedback as a treatment alternative to traditional pharmaceutical or surgical approaches. Cybernetics The term “biofeedback” is a shorthand term for external psychophysiological feedback, physiological feedback, and sometimes augmented proprioception. The basic idea is to provide individuals with increased information about what is going on inside their bodies, including their brains. The field that deals most directly with information processing and feedback is called cybernetics. A basic principle of cybernetics is that one cannot control a variable unless information is available to the controller. The information provided is termed “feedback” (Ashby, 1963; Mayr, 1970). Another principle of cybernetics is that feedback makes learning possible. Annett (1969) reviewed the evidence for this principle. In applied biofeedback, individuals receive direct and clear feedback about their physiology. This helps them learn to control such functions. For example, from a surface EMG instrument, persons receive information concerning their muscle activity. This helps them to reduce, increase, or otherwise regulate their muscle tension. From a cybernetic perspective, operant conditioning is one form of feedback. It is feedback provided in the form of positive or negative results of a particular behavior. The point is that another significant contribution to the development of applied biofeedback is an information-processing model derived from cybernetic theory and research. Proponents of this model in the field of biofeedback include Brown (1977), Anliker (1977), Mulholland (1977), and Gaarder and Montgomery (1981). Cultural Factors Several cultural factors have contributed to the development of applied biofeedback. The gradual 13 1. History and Definitions merging of the traditions and techniques of the East and West is one major factor. The rise in popularity of schools of meditation was an expression of a cultural change and provided a context in which applied biofeedback developed. Yogis and Zen masters reportedly alter their physiological states significantly through meditation. Related phenomena presumably occur in some forms of biofeedback experiences. Therefore, some have referred to biofeedback as the “yoga of the West” and “electronic Zen.” Within the United States, there are other cultural factors adding to a Zeitgeist encouraging biofeedback applications. These are the heightened costs of health care and the resulting need for more efficacious and cost-effective treatments. In addition, it is commonly recognized that pharmacotherapy, with its many benefits, is of limited value for certain patients. Some patients cannot take medications because of untoward side effects; many patients avoid compliance; others prefer not to consume medications; and some physicians deemphasize pharmacotherapy. Perhaps even more significant is the current popular public health emphasis on prevention. The movement toward wellness has continued to grow since the 1960s. Practitioners of holistic health also emphasize self-regulation and self-control. The result of these emphases is that more people are involving themselves in lifestyle changes to regulate their health. These changes include enhancing physical fitness, avoiding caffeine and nicotine, reducing or stopping alcohol use, and pursuing better weight control. More people are thus assuming increased responsibility for their physical, as well as their mental and spiritual, well-being. In addition, more people are accepting responsibility for their recovery from illness. Many believe that biofeedback therapies facilitate and fit well into these efforts at greater self-regulation, wellness, and growth. Professional Organizations Homer’s epic poem The Odyssey served as a metaphor for the past, present, and future of biofeedback and applied psychophysiology. From the title of this epic, an “odyssey” has come to mean any long series of wanderings, especially when filled with notable experiences, hardships, and the exploration of new terrain. Just as Homer’s Odysseus experienced setbacks but was ultimately successful in his journey to reach home, the journey of psychophysiological self-regulation with biofeedback has experienced and will continue to experience setbacks and successes. The Biofeedback Society of America (BSA) was entering its 20th year, thus completing one full generation of development, when similar words were first delivered (M. S. Schwartz, 1988). Twenty years constitute one generation, or the average period between the birth of parents and the birth of their offspring. Thirteen years then remained until the year 2001, the date of the famous book and movie 2001: A Space Odyssey. However, our field does not seek the universality of something as monolithic as Arthur C. Clarke’s and Stanley Kubrick’s odyssey. The Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback and Its Various Names How the Journey Began The Biofeedback Research Society (BRS) was formed in 1969, largely by a handful of research psychophysiologists. After 6 years, the BRS became the BSA, with both an experimental and an applied division. Age 6 is about the age at which children go through the transition from home to school; similarly, the scope of the organization and the field broadened into applied areas. This change in name reflected the growth and importance of applied aspects. How the Journey Continued Professional Developments Also adding to the development of applied biofeedback are the organizations of professionals engaged in research and clinical, educational, and performance enhancement applications. Issues considered here include the professional organizations themselves, the status of the literature in this field, the professional journal of the primary organization (and the journal’s name), and finally, the scope of the field. At age 19, as a result of the field’s expanding scope, the BSA went through its second transformation—into the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB; www.aapb. org). This is about the age at which many students graduate to institutions of higher learning. The organization returned to some of its roots in psychophysiology at the same interval. The consistency with the journey metaphor first struck M. S. Schwartz (1988) then, as Odysseus also took 20 years to return home. 14 As later reported by M. S. Schwartz (1999a), the name . . . change was a hotly debated topic. Many argued for a need to expand the implied scope of the organization. One factor was that most practitioners utilized a wider array of therapy methods than biofeedback. Presentations at the annual meetings of the BSA encompassed much more than biofeedback. Researchers at universities . . . maintained that the term biofeedback alone was not viewed as sufficiently credible by some individuals and that this hampered their abilities to publish their research in some quality journals and to obtain external research funding. The researchers further contended that the term “biofeedback” was insufficient for them to obtain the kind of recognition they needed in their academic departments. Thus, both applied practitioners and researchers were contending that a name change was needed. Psychophysiology was the birthplace of the field of biofeedback, and so it was time to return to these roots. The emphasis was placed on the term applied to distinguish it from [its] grandparent organization and field, the Society for Psychophysiological Research. Many members of the BSA . . . argued for dropping the term biofeedback but the supporters of the term successfully argued for the preservation of the term. . . . The term “applied psychophysiology” reflected the evolution of science and clinical practice. (p. 3) The AAPB continues to be a productive, intellectually stimulating, useful, scientifically sound, and vibrant organization. There are several Interest Groups, Sections, and Divisions, including Sections for Applied Respiratory Psychophysiology, Educational, International, Mind–Body, Optimal Functioning, and Performing Arts Psychophysiology. There also now is a section for the U.S. Stress Management Organization, which is part of the International Stress Management Association (ISMA) with another interesting history dating from 1973, with illuminary founders Edmund Jacobson, F. J. McGuigan, and Marigold Edwards. Prior names for the international organization included the International Stress and TensionControl Association and the International Stress Management Association (ISMA). The Neurofeedback Division and the sEMG/SESNA (Surface Electromyography Society of North America) Division reflect the two major modalitites and areas of biofeedback. Each of these has major tracks at the Annual Meeting of the AAPB. Disagreement occasionally still arises about the most appropriate name for both the membership I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK organization AAPB and its journal (see below). Some occasionally argue for dropping the term “biofeedback,” but those who advocate retaining the term “biofeedback” in the names of the organization and journal focus on the established place of this term in the minds of professionals and the lay public, as well as on its history, brevity, and ease of communication. Other Related Membership Organizations and Groups Sponsoring Meetings Another national membership organization, the American Association of Biofeedback Clinicians, started in 1975 but went out of existence in the late 1980s. This left the BSA, now the AAPB, as the major organization with a major emphasis on biofeedback. Biofeedback’s impact is growing and spreading beyond the borders of the United States, as evidenced by the rise of the Biofeedback Foundation of Europe (BFE) (www.bfe.org). This excellent, international organization has hosted an annual meeting, featuring indepth workshops and scientific sessions, since 1996. Since 1995, with the resurgence and expansion of EEG biofeedback, a specialty organization, the International Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR; www.isnr.org) has become a major organization in this area. The ISNR was formed in response to the need for a group that was undividedly focused on EEG biofeedback. There had been previously created, within the AAPB, an “EEG Division” that attempted to serve the needs of this community. However, the influence of those primarily interested in peripheral (or “traditional”) biofeedback was considered by some to be diluting these efforts, and it motivated certain individuals to create a new entity. Like the AAPB, its name and focus has evolved over time, but much more quickly. ISNR is an outgrowth of the Society for the Study of Neuronal Regulation, founded in 1993, whose name was shortened in 1998 to Society for Neuronal Regulation for simplicity, and then changed again in 2002, to the International Society for Neuronal Regulation. In 2006, it was renamed ISNR “to better reflect the fact that members of the society now came from all parts of the globe, not just North America and that research is a critical function of the society” (www.isnr.org). The ISNR also provides publications, research support, education, and an annual meeting. 15 1. History and Definitions Each of the aforementioned organizations has excellent websites with extensive and useful information. The Biofeedback Certification International Alliance The Biofeedback Certification International Alliance (BCIA), previously known as the Biofeedback Certification Institute of America, is a professional organization that has greatly influenced the continued development of the field. As its name indicates, the BCIA maintains a credible credentialing program. Before 1979, credentialing was in the hands of a few state biofeedback societies. These societies, well-meaning as they were, suffered from the understandable problems of small groups of professionals who typically had little or no training and experience with the complexities of credentialing. Thus, there was considerable variability in the credentialing across states. In most states, there was no credentialing at all or even the hope of any. Ed Taub, then president-elect of the BSA, had the foresight and wisdom to inspire the development of an independent, credible, nationwide credentialing program. The BSA sponsored and supported the official establishment of the BCIA (named by Bernard Engel, later the first chair of the BCIA board) in January 1981. Three months later, when Engel became President of the BSA, he graciously relinquished the chair of BCIA to M. S. Schwartz. The BCIA evolved with more stringent criteria for education, training, experience, and recertification. Professionals continue to seek and earn the BCIA credential as the only credible one of its kind. In recent years, another so-called credentialing organization arose in association with instrumentation its members refer to as “biofeedback,” but all credible professionals known to at least the first author consider this group or what its members call biofeedback to be inaccurate or to lack credibility and not worthy of mention in this chapter or book. Although the BCIA holds primacy in credentialing, educational opportunities exist in many undergraduate and graduate courses in biofeedback. Private training programs and workshops are offered by national, state, and regional professional organizations, as well as some biofeedback companies/distributors. There are also many companies manufacturing biofeedback instrumentation, and several “distributor” companies selling and servicing a variety of instruments from different manufacturers. The Journey of a Family or Separate Journeys? All professionals in this field share some joint responsibility and custody for the young adult we call “biofeedback and applied psychophysiology.” Some professionals proceed on their own individual journeys; they seek their own destinations, their own Ithacas, instead of common ones. However, the AAPB continues as the leading administrative, facilitative, educational, and coordinating member organization dedicated to integrating professional disciples and conceptual frameworks that involve varied scientific and applied areas of applied psychophysiology and biofeedback. It is the nuclear family for biofeedback. Status of the Literature in the Field The number of publications is one barometer of the history, growth, and possibly the future of a field. The first bibliography of the biofeedback literature (Butler & Stoyva, 1973) contained about 850 references. The next edition, 5 years later, listed about 2300 references (Butler, 1978). Thousands more have appeared since then (Hatch, 1993; Hatch & Riley, 1985; Hatch & Saito, 1990). There are dozens of papers published each year in non-English-speaking countries, including Russia (Shtark & Kall, 1998; Shtark & Schwartz, 2002; Sokhadze & Shtark, 1991), and many others are published in Europe, Israel, and elsewhere. Note that there are dozens of papers published each year in non-English-speaking countries. For example, the important Japanese literature was still in its early stages in 1979, but rapidly increased in the 1980s (Hatch & Saito, 1990; Shirakura, Saito, & Tsutsui, 1992). Their leading journal on biofeedback, Japanese Journal of Biofeedback Research, is nearing its 40th volume. There is also a rich history of research publications and clinical applications in Russia and other countries that were formerly part of the USSR (Shtark & Kall, 1998; Shtark & Schwartz, 2002; Sokhadze & Shtark, 1991). This foreign literature is not well known in the United States. The Primary Journal, Its Name, and Other Publications A measure of the maturity of a field is the existence and quality of its primary professional journal(s). The journal Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, published by Plenum Press, was started in 1976. The journal’s name was changed to Applied Psycho- 16 physiology and Biofeedback as of Volume 22, in 1997. The editors, board, and publisher noted that “the journal has long had a broader focus than the title implied, and this new name more accurately reflects its expanded scope” (Andrasik, 1997, p. 1). Frank Andrasik has been the Editor-in-Chief since 1995, having followed many notable prior editors—Johann Stoyva, the first editor, Al F. Ax, coeditors Edward B. Blanchard & Mary R. Cook from 1984 until 1992, and Robert R. Freedman until 1995. It is still the major publication in this field. However, AAPB also publishes another very useful and important publication, called simply Biofeedback. For the past several years, with Donald Moss, as Editor-in-Chief, this has become an excellent quarterly publication. Another noteworthy journal is the Journal of Neurotherapy, which focuses on EEG biofeedback/neurofeedback. Definitions of Biofeedback and Applied Psychophysiology Historical Review of Definitions The history of biofeedback has witnessed many definitions. Olson (1987, 1995) noted 10 definitions starting from 1971. In the second and third editions of this text, Schwartz and Schwartz (2003) elaborated and discussed various historical definitions; the models from which they were derived; and the issues, elements, and factors involved in prior definitions. For example, whether or not the specific feedback signals as such result in changes and at what level does the signal become biofeedback, per se, was a focus of much debate in the 1980s. See the invigorating exchange and debate between Furedy (1987) and Shellenberger and Green (1987), a valuable and appreciated attempt to moderate and create perspective by Rosenfeld (1987), and the review and discussion of this by Schwartz and Schwartz (2003). Some persons might still consider these topics interesting. However, we decided to deemphasize these topics in this edition partly in view of the 2008 official definition (AAPB, BCIA, and ISNR) (Schwartz, 2010) presented and discussed later in this chapter. This was done chiefly to reduce confusion and not detract from the official definition. Increased information and patient education are common elements in all models. We suggest a conceptualization that includes different levels and types of information received by patients during biofeedback sessions. This discussion acknowledges the contributions of G. E. Schwartz (1982, I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK 1983), who emphasized the contextual, organistic, multicategory, and multicausal approach to understanding biofeedback. Schwartz and Schwartz (2003) presented and discussed their multilevel patient education model involving seven levels or facets of information about biofeedback. Readers are referred to the third edition and to www.marksschwartzphd.com for a full discussion of this model. This model proposed that patient education is an active ingredient of biofeedback, regardless of the discipline within which it is used. This component is not explicitly included in the new and official definition but it is implicitly “in conjunction with changes in thinking” (Schwartz, 2010, p. 90). Toward the First Official Definition of “Biofeedback” By Olson’s (1995) definition, a competent therapist is an important part of biofeedback therapies. Moreover, computerized biofeedback is like having a high-tech electronic chalkboard for teaching and a built-in ability to measure progress. It is up to the therapist to use this technology to be the best possible teacher and communicator. In essence, biofeedback, used in the broad sense of signals, explanations, and patient education, provides missing or deficient information in the intervention context. This information is helpful for the patient/client, the therapist, or the interaction. One does not evaluate a school book when it is presented to students by itself. Some students have the following: sufficient motivation, sufficient capabilities, no significant interference, sufficient times and places to study, other resources to use as references, an experiential background conducive to independent learning, confidence in their ability, and a teacher for help if they reach an impasse. Therefore, some students do well with self-study and never need to go to class. Others need classroom instructions and review of the text. Some of these others need extensive text review—paragraph by paragraph, page by page, and chapter by chapter. Some learn the material sufficient for earning an average grade. Others seek or “need” a grade of A. Some never learn much, if any at all. None of this is news. However, the point here is that we do not attribute the problem to the book unless it is written poorly and/or not tailored well to the student. In Schwartz and Schwartz (2003) a comprehensive definition was offered that involved additions to Olson’s (1995) definition. 1. History and Definitions The 2008 Official Definition In mid-2007, the leadership of AAPB5 started the process and coordinated the creation of the Task Force6 on Nomenclature, a task force to develop an agreed-upon definition of “biofeedback” that would be endorsed by the three major organizations, the AAPB, the BCIA, and the ISNR. The task force’s diligent work on this challenging project over several months culminated in a definition that was then submitted to the Boards of the three organizations that had contributed task force members. The Boards voted their agreement in 2008, and the definition became the first, official, agreed-upon definition in the field. The story of this process may be found in M. S. Schwartz (2010). Biofeedback is a process that enables an individual to learn how to change physiological activity for the purposes of improving health and performance. Precise instruments measure physiological activity such as brainwaves, heart function, breathing, muscle activity, and skin temperature. These instruments rapidly and accurately “feed back” information to the user. The presentation of this information—often in conjunction with changes in thinking, emotions, and behavior—supports desired physiological changes. Over time, these changes can endure without continued use of an instrument. (Approved May 18, 2008, by the AAPB, the BCIA, and the ISNR). A Definition of “Applied Psychophysiology”— Sort Of Defining the term “applied psychophysiology” still remained a need, goal, and challenge as of 1998, several years after the name change for AAPB and its journal. As noted by M. S. Schwartz (1999a, p. 4), “One can only surmise that everyone apparently knew what applied psychophysiology meant. . . . What everyone apparently knew, no one had written. What everyone apparently knew, was unclear.” It was the broader term, a rubric term, that subsumes biofeedback. J. Peter Rosenfeld (1992), in his AAPB presidential address, was the first to address a definition of “applied psychophysiology.” He identified some of its elements “and touched on elements of a definition” (M. S. Schwartz, 1999a, p. 4). Sebastian Striefel (1998), a later president of the AAPB, again raised the question of a definition of “applied psychophysiology” in his 1998 presidential address. At the same meeting, “Paul Lehrer, chairperson of the AAPB Publication Committee, 17 convened an ad hoc committee to deal with a wide array of topics. . . . One of these topics was . . . the lack of a formal . . . definition of ‘applied psychophysiology’ ” (M. S. Schwartz, 1999a, p. 4). The committee assigned the task of establishing an operational definition for the term. Apparently, no one thought to establish a task force. The AAPB asked one person to develop a definition (M. S. Schwartz, 1999a, 1999b). A provisional definition was drafted and a paper documenting the rationale for each component was written. An array of notable and diverse professionals provided their critiques to the provisional definition in the initial paper by M. S. Schwartz (1999b). The author of the definition then prepared a response to the panel of independent critical reviewers (M. S. Schwartz, 1999b). The development of a definition that is acceptable to everyone is unlikely. Amendments and modifications were expected. The published discussions of the key elements, examples of topics included and excluded, rationales for these choices, critiques, and responses are best read in their original form. There is still no formal and agreed-upon definition of “applied psychophysiology”—only a tentative and certainly unofficial operational definition (M. S. Schwartz, 1999a, p. 5) presented here only for historical interest and, we hope, to motivate others to refine and shorten it. Applied psychophysiology reflects an evolving scientific discipline and specialty involving understanding and modifying the relationship between behavior and physiological functions by a variety of methods including noninvasive physiological measures. The term “applied psychophysiology” is a rubric encompassing evaluation, diagnosis, education, treatment, and performance enhancement. Applied psychophysiology includes a group of interventions and evaluation methods with the exclusive or primary intentions of understanding and effecting changes that help humans move toward and maintain healthier psychophysiological functioning. Applied psychophysiology involves helping people change physiological functioning and psychological functioning (measured, theoretical, and potential) and/or to achieve sensorimotor integration and motor learning within physical rehabilitation. The group of interventions use all forms of biofeedback, relaxation methods, breathing methods, cognitive-behavioral therapies, patient/client education, behavioral changes, hypnosis, meditative techniques, and imagery techniques (some commentators would add: when directed at changing physiological functioning). In some situations, dietary and other biochemical (nonmedication) changes and some 18 truth detection research and applications may be considered under the rubric of applied psychophysiology. Evaluation methods use all forms of physiological measurements. The physiological functioning includes but is not limited to accurately measured changes in skeletal muscles, all autonomic physiology, breathing measures, biochemistry, electroencephalographic activity, both normal and abnormal and imaging techniques. Autonomic measures include electrodermal, skin temperature, blood pressure, heart rate, gastrointestinal motility, and vasomotor. The interventions need to be part of or have implications for applications to humans. These could, but do not need to, involve the raw procedures and/or symptoms of medical and psychophysiological disorders. Glossary Alpha wave activity. EEG activity (8–12 hz) commonly, but not always, thought to be associated with an alert but relaxed state. Autonomic nervous system (ANS). The part of the ner- vous system that is connected to all organs and blood vessels, and transmits signals that control their functioning. It consists of two branches, the sympathetic and parasympathetic, which usually produce opposite responses. Once thought to be totally involuntary, it is now known to be under some significant voluntary control, although less so than the CNS. Central nervous system (CNS). The part of the nervous system including human thought, sense organs, and control of skeletal muscles. Once believed to be totally separate from the ANS, it is now known to interact with the ANS. Classical conditioning. Originating with Pavlov, the type of conditioning or learning that assumes that certain stimuli (unconditioned stimuli, or UCSs) evoke unconditioned or unlearned responses (UCRs) (e.g., acute pain evokes crying, withdrawal, and fear), and that other, previously neutral stimuli (conditioned stimuli, or CSs) associated with the pairing of these events develop the capacity to elicit the same or similar responses or conditioned responses (CRs). Curarized animals. Animals intentionally paralyzed by the drug curare to control for body movements during visceral conditioning, such as biofeedback of heart rate. Cybernetics. The science of internal body control sys- tems in humans, and of electrical and mechanical systems designed to replace the human systems. Electroencephalography (EEG). The measurement of electrical activity of the brain. I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK Electromechanical. A term describing devices that mea- sure mechanical aspects of the body (e.g., position of a joint or degree of pressure or weight placed on it), rather than a property of the body (e.g., its direct electrical activity or temperature). Examples of these mechanical aspects include degrees that a person’s knee bends after knee surgery, steadiness of the head of a child with cerebral palsy, and the weight pressure placed on a leg and foot by someone after a stroke. Instruments transform these mechanical forces into electrical signals. Electromyography (EMG). The use of special instru- ments to measure the electrical activity of skeletal muscles. In recent years, also called “surface electromyography” and sometimes abbreviated as SEMG. Extinction. The behavioral principle predicting that abruptly and totally stopping all positive reinforcements after specified behaviors will lead to the behavior’s no longer occurring. Fading. Gradually changing a stimulus that controls a person’s or animal’s performance to another stimulus. As a behavioral procedure, it does not always mean disappearance of a stimulus. Fight or flight. Walter Cannon’s well-known concept of the body’s psychophysiological arousal and preparation for fighting or fleeing actual or perceived threatening stimuli. Galvanic skin response (GSR). A form of electrodermal activity—increased resistance of the skin to conducting tiny electrical currents because of reduced sweat and dryness. Older term less ofen used now, but still accepted. Opposite of “skin conductance” (SC). Insight-oriented psychodynamic theories and therapies. A wide range of psychological theories and therapies, starting from the time of Sigmund Freud. A basic assumption is that patients need to gain insight into the psychological origins and forces motivating their current psychological problems and behaviors before they can achieve adequate relief of symptoms. Instrumental conditioning. Same as operant condition- ing (see below). The behavioral theories and therapies originated by B. F. Skinner. For example, reinforcers are said to be instrumentally linked to the recurrence of behaviors. Observational learning. Learning that takes place by means of the organism’s observing another organism doing the task to be learned. Operant conditioning. The same as instrumental condi- tioning (listed earlier), originating with B. F. Skinner. “Operant” means that a response is identified and understood in terms of its consequences rather than by a stimulus that evokes it. Stimuli and circumstances emit responses rather than evoke them, as in classical conditioning. 19 1. History and Definitions Proprioception. Perception mediated by sensory nerve terminals within tissues, mostly muscles, tendons, and the labyrinthal system for balance. They give us information concerning our movements and position. Examples include (1) the sense of knowing when we are slightly off balance; and (2) the ability to perceive (even with eyes closed) the difference between, and approximate weights of, objects weighing 5 ounces and 7 ounces held in each hand. Psychophysiology. The science of studying the causal and interactive processes of physiology, behavior, and subjective experience. Reinforcers. Events or stimuli that increase the prob- Zeitgeist. The spirit or general trend of thought of a time in history. Often used to refer to a time in history when new ways of thinking and technologies are more likely to be accepted by the culture in question. Acknowledgments We gratefully acknowledge and will always be thankful to R. Paul Olson for his earlier contributions to this chapter. His creativity and scholarly writing of the original version of this chapter continue to be the model we follow. ability of recurrence of behaviors they follow. Schedules of reinforcement. Usually, forms of intermit- tent reinforcement of an operant behavior. A common schedule in life, and most resistant to extinction, is a variable-ratio schedule—one in which the number of times a reinforcement follows a specific behavior varies randomly, so the person or animal never knows when the reinforcer will occur. This contrasts with variable-interval, fixed-interval, and fixed-ratio schedules. Sensorimotor rhythm. An EEG rhythm (12–14 Hz) recorded from the central scalp and involving both the sensory and motor parts of the brain, the sensorimotor cortex. Used in the EEG biofeedback of some persons with seizure disorders. Shaping. A behavioral principle from operant con- ditioning, referring to procedures designed to help learning of complex new behaviors by very small steps. Also known as “shaping by successive approximations.” Single motor units. Individual spinal nerves or neu- rons involved in movement. Biofeedback training of single spinal motor neurons was a major advance in the late 1950s and early 1960s. This training requires fine-wire EMG electrodes. Skeletally mediated mechanical artifacts. Artifacts in instrumentation-recorded signals that are caused by intentional body movements. Examples include moving a body part such as the head or neck during recordings of resting muscle activity, or clenching the teeth during EEG recordings. Slow-wave activity. EEG activity (3–8 Hz) included in the frequency range often called theta activity, also reported as 4–7 Hz. Vasomotor. Affecting the caliber (diameter) of a blood vessel. Visceral learning. Learning that takes place by body organs, especially those in the abdominal cavity, such as the stomach and bowels. Visceral reflexes. Reflexes in which the stimulus is a state of an internal organ. Notes 1. Although the term “applied psychophysiology” is now usually given first in this pairing, the order is reversed here to relect the emphasis on biofeedback. 2. The 25th anniversary meeting of the primary professional membership organization, the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB), was held in 1994. The commemorative AAPB Silver Anniversary Yearbook published for that meeting contains articles about the history and development of the biofeedback field and the organization. Reading it is enriching and informative. It is available from the AAPB, 10100 West 44th Avenue, Suite 304, Wheat Ridge, CO 80033; phone: 303422-2615; fax: 303-422-889; website: www.aapb.org. 3. Note that this sentence appeared in the first edition of this book in early 1987. It does not seem to be a coincidence that the Biofeedback Society of America (BSA) went through the process of changing its name to include board meetings or other public or private meetings concerning the name change. The term was written into an early draft of this chapter several years before 1987. 4. “Health psychology” is a more recent field with similar roots and ties to behavioral medicine. The focus is more on prevention and health enhancement. 5. The AAPB Executive Board and, specifically and most notably, Aubrey Ewing, then President-Elect and Alan Glaros, the President, along with Executive Director Francine Butler, were the prime movers on the project. They coordinated with the leadership of two other major organization in this field, the BCIA and the ISNR. 6. See Schwartz (2010) for a list of the members. References Ancoli, S., & Kamiya, J. (1978). Methodological issues in alpha biofeedback training. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 3(2), 159–183. Andrasik, F. (1997). Editorial. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 22, 1. Andrews, J. M. (1964). Neuromuscular re-education of the 20 hemiplegic with aid of electromyograph. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 45, 530–532. Anliker, J. (1977). Biofeedback from the perspective of cybernetics and systems science. In J. Beatty & H. Legewie (Eds.), Biofeedback and behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Annett, J. (1969). Feedback and human behavior. Baltimore, MD: Penguin Books. Aserinsky, N., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility and concomitant phenomena during sleep. Science, 118, 274–284. Ashby, W. R. (1963). An introduction to cybernetics. New York: Wiley. Ayers, M. E., Sams, M. W., Sterman, M. B., & Lubar, J. (2000). When to Inhibit EEG activity instead of reinforcing and inhibiting simultaneously. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4(1), 83–90. Basmajian, J. V. (1963). Conscious control of individual motor units. Science, 141, 440–441. Basmajian, J. V. (1978). Muscles alive: Their functions revealed by electromyography (4th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Basmajian, J. V. (Ed.). (1983). Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Basmajian, J. V. (Ed.). (1989). Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Basmajian, J. V., Kukulka, C. G., Narayan, M. G., & Takebe, K. (1975). Biofeedback treatment of foot drop after stroke compared with standard rehabilitation technique: Effects on voluntary control and strength. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 56, 231–236. Beatty, J., Greenberg, A., Deibler, W. P., & O’Hanlon, J. F. (1974). Operant control of occipital theta rhythm affects performance in a radar monitoring task. Science, 183(4), 871–873. Benson, H. (1975). The relaxation response. New York: Morrow. Bernstein, D. A., & Borkovec, T. D. (1973). Progressive relaxation training. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Bernstein, D. A., Carlson, C. R., & Schmnidt, J. E. (2007). Progressive relaxation: Abbreviated methods. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3nd ed., pp. 88–122). New York: Guilford Press. Binder-MacLeod, S. A. (1983). Biofeedback in stroke rehabilitation. In J. V. Basmajian (Ed.), Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (2nd ed., pp. 73–89). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Birk, L. (Ed.). (1973). Biofeedback: Behavioral medicine. New York: Grune & Stratton. Blumenthal, A. L. (1977). The process of cognition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Brown, B. (1977). Stress and the art of biofeedback. New York: Harper & Row. Brudny, J. (1982). Biofeedback in chronic neurological I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK cases: Therapeutic electromyography. In L. White & B. Tursky (Eds.), Clinical biofeedback: Efficacy and mechanisms (pp. 249–288). New York: Guilford Press. Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University. Butler, F. (1978). Biofeedback: A survey of the literature. New York: Plenum Press. Butler, F., & Stoyva, J. (1973). Biofeedback and self-control: A bibliography. Wheat Ridge, CO: Biofeedback Society of America. Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body. New York: Norton. Carrington, P. (1977). Freedom in meditation. Garden City, NY: Doubleday/Anchor. Carrington, P. (1978). Clinically Standardized Meditation (CSM) professional pack. Kendall Park, NJ: Pace Educational Systems. Carrington, P. (1998). The book of meditation. Kendall Park, NJ: Pace Educational Systems. Carrington, P. (2007). Modern forms of Mantra Meditation. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3nd ed., pp. 139–168). New York: Guilford Press. Collura, T. F. (1995). History and evolution of computerized electroencephalography. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 12(3), 214–229. Collura, T. F. (2009). Towards a coherent view of brain connectivity. Journal of Neurotherapy, 12(2–3), 99–110. Dement, W., & Kleitman, N. (1957). The relation of eye movements during sleep to dream activity: An objective method for the study of dreaming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 339–346. Dewan, E. D. (1967). Occipital alpha rhythm, eye position and lens accommodation. Nature, 214, 299–304. DiCara, L., & Miller, N. E. (1968a). Instrumental learning of systolic blood pressure responses by curarized rats: Dissociation of cardiac and vascular changes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 30, 489–494. DiCara, L., & Miller, N. E. (1968b). Instrumental learning of peripheral vasomotor responses by rat. Communications in Behavioral Biology, 1, 209–212. DiCara, L., & Miller, N. E. (1968c). Instrumental learning of vasomotor responses by rats: Learning to respond differentially in the two ears. Science, 159, 1485–1486. DiCara, L., & Miller, N. E. (1968d). Long term retention of instrumentally learned heart-rate changes in the curarized rat. Communications in Behavioral Biology, A2, 19–23. Dworkin, B. R., & Miller, N. E. (1986). Failure to replicate visceral learning in the acute curarized rat preparation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 299–314. Fehmi, L., & Collura, T. F. (2007). The effects of electrode placement upon EEG biofeedback training: The monopolar/bipolar controversy. Journal of Neurotherapy, 11(2), 45–63. Fehmi, L. G., & Fritz, G. (1980, Spring). Open focus: The attentional foundation of health and well being. Somatics, pp. 24–30. 1. History and Definitions Fehmi, L., & Robbins, J. (2009). The open-focus brain: Harnessing the power of attention to heal mind and body. Boston: Trumpeter/Shambhala. Forem, J. (1974). Transcendental meditation. New York: Dutton. Furedy, J. J. (1987). Specific versus placebo effects in biofeedback training: A critical lay perspective. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 12, 169–184. Gaarder, K. R., & Montgomery, P. S. (1981). Classical biofeedback: A procedural manual for behavioral medicine (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Greenfield, N. S., & Sternback, R. A. (1972). Handbook of psychophysiology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Hardt, J., & Kamiya, J. (1976). Some comments on Plotkin’s self-regulation of the electroencephalographic alpha. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105(1), 100–108. Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social deprivation in monkeys. Scientific American, 207, 136–146. Harris, A. H., & Brady, J. V. (1974). Animal learning: Visceral and autonomic conditioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 25, 107–133. Hart, J. T. (1968). Autocontrol of EEG alpha [Abstract]. Psychophysiology, 4, 506. Hassett, J. (1978). A primer of psychophysiology. San Francisco: Freeman. Hatch, J. P. (1993, March). Declining rates of publication within the field of biofeedback continue: 1988–1991. In Proceedings of the 24th annual meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, Los Angeles. Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Hatch, J. P., & Riley, P. (1985). Growth and development of biofeedback: A bibliographic analysis. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 10(4), 289–299. Hatch, J. P., & Saito, I. (1990). Growth and development of biofeedback: A bibliographic update. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 15(1), 37–46. Hothersall, D., & Brener, H. (1969). Operant conditioning of changes in heart rates in curarized rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 68, 338–342. Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Jacobson, E. (1978). You must relax. New York: McGrawHill. Jacobson, E. (1982). The human mind: A physiological clarification. Springfield, IL: Thomas. Jacobson, E., & McGuigan, F. J. (1982). Principles and practice of progressive relaxation: A teaching primer [Cassette]. New York: BMA Audio Cassettes. Kamiya, J. (1961). Behavioral, subjective, and physiological aspects of drowsiness and sleep. In D. W. Fiske & S. R. Maddi (Eds.), Functions of varied experience (pp. 145– 174). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Kamiya, J. (1968). Conscious control of brain waves. Psychology Today, 1, 56–63. Kamiya, J. (1969). Operant control of the EEG alpha rhythm and some of its reported effects on conscious- 21 ness. In C. T. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness (pp. 489–501). New York: Wiley. Kimmel, H. O. (1979). Instrumental conditioning of autonomically mediated responses in human beings. American Psychologist, 29, 325–335. Krippner, S. (1972). Altered states of consciousness. In J. White (Ed.), The highest state of consciousness (pp. 1–5). Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Kristeller, J. (2007). Mindfulness meditation. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd. ed., pp. 393–427). New York: Guilford Press. Langley, L. L. (1965). Homeostasis. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Lichstein, K. L. (1988). Clinical relaxation strategies. New York: Wiley. Lubar, J. F. (1982). EEG operant conditioning in severe epileptics: Controlled multidimensional studies. In L. White & B. Tursky (Eds.), Clinical biofeedback: Efficacy and mechanisms (pp. 288–310). New York: Guilford Press. Lubar, J. F. (1983). Electroencephalographic biofeedback and neurological applications. In J. V. Basmajian (Ed.), Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (2nd ed., pp. 67–90). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Lubar, J. F. (1991). Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedback treatment for attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorders. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 16, 201–225. Lubar, J. F., & Shouse, M. N. (1976). EEG and behavioral changes in a hyperkinetic child concurrent with training of the sensory motor rhythm (SMR): A preliminary report. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 3, 293–306. Luthe, W. (Ed.). (1969). Autogenic therapy (Vols. 1–6). New York: Grune & Stratton. Marinacci, A. A., & Horande, M. (1960). Electromyogram in neuromuscular re-education. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society, 25, 57–71. Mayr, O. (1970). The origins of feedback control. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. McGuigan, F. J., & Lehrer, P. M. (2007). Progressive relaxation: Origins, principles, and clinical applications. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed., pp. 57–87). New York: Guilford Press. Miller, N. E. (1978). Biofeedback and visceral learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 373–404. Miller, N. E., & Brucker, B. S. (1979). A learned visceral response apparently independent of skeletal ones in patients paralyzed by spinal lesions. In N. Birbaumer & H. D. Kimmel (Eds.), Biofeedback and self-regulation (pp. 287–304). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Miller, N. E., & DiCara, L. (1967). Instrumental learning of heart rate changes in curarized rats: Shaping and specificity to discriminative stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 12–19. Mitchell, L. (1977). Simple relaxation: The physiological method for easing tension. New York: Atheneum. 22 Mitchell, L. (1987). Simple relaxation: The Mitchell method for easing tension (rev. ed.). London: Murray. Moss, C. S. (1965). Hypnosis in perspective. New York: Macmillan. Mulholland, T. (1977). Biofeedback as scientific method. In G. E. Schwartz & J. Beatty (Eds.), Biofeedback: Theory and research (pp. 9–28). New York: Academic Press. Nowlis, D. P., & Kamiya, J. (1970). The control of electroencephalographic alpha rhythms through auditory feedback and the associated mental activity. Psychophysiology, 6, 476–484. Olson, R. P. (1987). Definitions of biofeedback. In M. S. Schwartz (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press. Olson, R. P. (1995). Definitions of biofeedback and applied psychophysiology. In M. S. Schwartz & Associates (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Olton, D. S., & Noonberg, A. R. (1980). Biofeedback: Clinical applications in behavioral medicine. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Orne, M. T. (1979). The efficacy of biofeedback therapy. Annual Review of Medicine, 30, 489–503. Ornstein, R. E. (1972). The psychology of consciousness. San Francisco: Freeman. Pelletier, K. R., & Garfield, C. (1976). Consciousness: East and west. New York: Harper & Row (Harper Colophon Books). Peper, E., & Shaffer, F. (2010). Biofeedback history: An alternative view. Biofeedback, 38(4), 142–147. Pi Suñer, A. (1955). Classics of biology. New York: Philosophical Library. Progoff, I. (1980). The practice of process meditation. New York: Dialogue House Library. Rosenfeld, J. P. (1987). Can clinical biofeedback be scientifically validated?: A follow-up on the Green–Shellenberger–Furedy–Roberts debates. Biofeedback and SelfRegulation, 121(3), 217–222. Rosenfeld, J. P. (1992). New directions in applied psychophysiology. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 17, 77–87. Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B . J. (1978). Social learning and cognition. New York: Academic Press. Schwartz, G. E. (1982). Testing the psychosocial model: The ultimate challenge facing biobehavioral medicine. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50(6), 1040–1053. Schwartz, G. E. (1983). Social psychophysiology and behavioral medicine: A systems perspective. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Perry (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook. New York: Guilford Press. Schwartz, G. E., & Beatty, J. (Eds.). (1977). Biofeedback: Theory and research. New York: Academic Press. Schwartz, G. E., & Weiss, S. M. (1978). What is behavioral medicine? Psychosomatic Medicine, 39(6), 377–381. Schwartz, M. S. (1988). The biofeedback odyssey: Nearing one score and counting (Presidential address). Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 13(1), 1–7. Schwartz, M. S. (1999a). What is applied psychophysiol- I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK ogy?: Toward a definition. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 24, 3–10. Schwartz, M. S. (1999b). Responses to comments and closer to a definition of applied psychophysiology? Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 24, 43–54. Schwartz, M. S. (2010). A new improved universally accepted official definition of biofeedback: Where did it come from? Why? Who did it? Who is it for? What’s next? Biofeedback, 24, 88–90. Schwartz, N. M., & Schwartz, M. S. (2003). Definitions of biofeedback and applied psychophysiology. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 27–39). New York: Guilford Press. Selye, H. (1974). Stress without distress. Philadelphia: Lippincott. Selye, H. (1976). The stress of life (rev. ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Selye, H. (Ed.). (1983). Selye’s guide to stress research (Vol. II). New York: Scientific and Academic Editions. Shaffer, F. (2010). Biofeedback tutor (CD). Available online at www.biosourcesoftware.com (also at www.aapb. org). Shealy, C. N. (1977). Ninety days to self-health. New York: Dial Press. Shellenberger, R., & Green, J. (1987). Specific effects and biofeedback versus biofeedback-assisted self-regulation training. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 12(3), 185– 209. Shirakura, K., Saito, I., & Tsutsui, S. (Eds.). (1992). Current biofeedback research in Japan. Tokyo: Sginkoh Igaku Shuppan. Shtark, M. B., & Kall, R. (1998). Biofeedback-3: Theory and practice. Novosibirsk, Russia: CERIS. Shtark, M. B., & Schwartz, M. S. (2002). Biofeedback-4: Theory and practice. Novosibirsk, Russia: CERIS. Silva, J. (1977). Silva mind control method. New York: Simon & Schuster. Slattery, P., & Taub, E. (1976). Specificity of temperature self-regulation to feedback loci. Biofeedback and SelfRegulation, 1, 316. Slaughter, J., Hahn, W. W., & Rinaldi, P. (1970). Instrumental conditioning of heart rate in the curarized rat with varied amounts of pretraining. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 72, 356–359. Smith, J. C. (1989). Relaxation dynamics. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Smith, J. C. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral relaxation training. New York: Springer. Smith, J. C. (2001). Advances in ABC relaxation: Applications and inventories. New York: Springer. Smith, J. C. (2005). Relaxation, meditation, and mindfulness: A mental health practitioner’s guide to new and traditional approaches. New York: Springer. Smith, O. C. (1934). Action potentials from single motor units in voluntary contraction. American Journal of Physiology, 108, 629–638. Sokhadze, E. M., & Shtark, M. B. (1991). Scientific and 1. History and Definitions clinical biofeedback in the USSR. Biofeedback and SelfRegulation, 16(3), 253–260. Sterman, M. B. (1982). EEG biofeedback in the treatment of epilepsy: An overview circa 1980. In L. W. White & B. Tursky (Eds.), Clinical biofeedback: Efficacy and mechanisms (pp. 311–334). New York: Guilford Press. Sterman, M. B., & Friar, L. (1972). Suppression of seizures in epileptic following sensorimotor EEG feedback training. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 33, 89–95. Stoyva, J., & Kamiya, J. (1968). Electrophysiological studies of dreaming as the prototype of a new strategy in the study of consciousness. Psychological Review, 75, 192–205. Striefel, S. (1998). Creating the future for applied psychophysiology and biofeedback: From fantasy to reality. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 23, 93–106. Stroebel, C. (1982). The quieting reflex. New York: Putnam. Tarlar-Benlolo, L. (1978). The role of relaxation in biofeedback training: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 727–755. Tart, C. T. (Ed.). (1969). Altered states of consciousness: A book of readings. New York: Wiley. Taub, E. (2010). What psychology as a science owes Neal Miller: The example of his biofeedback research. Biofeedback, 38(3), 108–117. (This article originated as a presentation to the Claude Bernard Society, at the annual meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, in San Diego, CA, March, 26, 2010.) Taub, E., Miller, N. E., Morris, R. L., & Robinson, W. (1993). Anatomical specificity in thermal biofeedback 23 responding. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, New Orleans, LA. Taub, E., & School, P. J. (1978). Some methodological considerations in thermal biofeedback. Behavioral Research Methods and Instrumentation, 10, 617–622. Thompson, L., & Thompson, M. (1998). Neurofeedback combined with training in metacognitive strategies: Effectiveness in student with ADD. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 23(4), 243–263. Thompson, M., & Thompson, L. (2003). The neurofeedback book: An introduction to basic concepts in applied psychophysiology. Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology. Trowill, H. A. (1967). Instrumental conditioning of heart rate in the curarized rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 7–11. Wickramasekera, I. E. (Ed.). (1976). Biofeedback, behavior therapy and hypnosis: Potentiating the verbal control of behavior for clinicians. Chicago: Nelson Hall. Wickramasekera, I. E. (1988). Clinical behavioral medicine: Some concepts and procedures. New York: Plenum. Wolf, S. L., & Binder-MacLeod, S. A. (1983). Electromyographic biofeedback in the physical therapy clinic. In J. V. Basmajian (Ed.), Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (2nd ed., pp. 62–72). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Wolpe, J. (1973). The practice of behavior therapy (2nd ed.). New York: Pergamon Press. Wyrwicka, W., & Sterman, M. B. (1968). Instrumental conditioning of sensory motor cortex EEG spindles in the waking cat. Physiology and Behavior, 3, 703–707. Chapter 2 Entering the Field and Assuring Competence Fredric Shaffer and Mark S. Schwartz Biofeedback, neurofeedback, and applied psychophysiology constitute a multidisciplinary and heterogeneous field of many professional disciplines and types of applications. Educational and training opportunities in the field range from courses at universities, webinars, and individual workshops to comprehensive biofeedback training programs. The Biofeedback Certification International Alliance (BCIA) provides accreditation for programs that teach courses based on BCIA’s Blueprints of Knowledge in biofeedback (BF), neurofeedback (NF), heart rate variability biofeedback (HRVB), and pelvic muscle dysfunction biofeedback (PMDB). Since BCIA only recognizes regionally accredited academic institutions, the courses offered by universities are already accredited through those regional boards; however, BCIA does monitor their course content to assure it appropriately covers Blueprint content. For many professionals, the sources of education are the annual meetings, webinars, and workshops of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB), the Biofeedback Federation of Europe (BFE), and the International Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR); BCIA clinical update and mentoring webinars; and workshops sponsored by state and regional societies. Additional sources include professional organizations, societies dedicated to specific disorders, commercial training programs that offer multiday programs, and self-paced distance educa- tion; articles published in Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB), Biofeedback (AAPB), and NeuroRegulation (ISNR); and mentoring from experts in the field. General Suggestions for Entering the Field and Maintaining Competence The development and maintenance of clinical competence require active participation in a variety of educational and training experiences. Responsible professionals seek continuing education and training. Mentors and others involved with the education and training of professionals in their setting have a responsibility to budget time and financial resources for this type of continuing education. To follow are some general suggestions for ways to obtain and maintain competence. We urge our readers to consider them seriously and to try as many as are feasible. Training and Continuing Education 1. Enroll in carefully selected workshops, commercial programs, and academic courses. BCIA’s website (www.bcia.org) lists accredited programs and regionally accredited universities that teach its Blueprints of Knowledge. AAPB, BFE, ISNR, state and regional biofeedback societies, and vendors list didactic and continuing education pro- 24 25 2. Entering the Field and Assuring Competence grams on their websites. For feedback about the quality of the presentations, ask sponsors and presenters for the names of those who have attended in the past, and talk to them. Inquire about the equipment used and impartiality toward other manufacturers. Many of these courses are accredited by professional organizations like the American Psychological Association (APA). 2. Read recommended books, journal articles, multimedia tutorials, manuals, AAPB, BFE, and ISNR publications, and patient education booklets. ISNR offers an extensive online neurofeedback bibliography (www.isnr.net). Five indispensable publications are BCIA’s (2015) Professional Standards and Ethical Principles; Khazan’s (2013) The Clinical Handbook of Biofeedback: A StepBy-Step Guide for Training and Practice with Mindfulness; Peper, Tylova, Gibney, Harvey, and Combatalade’s (2008) Biofeedback Mastery: An Experiential Teaching and Self-Training Manual; Tan, Shaffer, Lyle, and Teo’s (2016) Evidence-Based Practice in Biofeedback and Neurofeedback (3rd ed.); and Thompson and Thompson’s (2015) The Neurofeedback Book: An Introduction to Basic Concepts in Applied Psychophysiology (2nd ed.). A superb BFE publication is Peper and Gibney’s (2000) Healthy Computing with Muscle Biofeedback. Study the references listed in BCIA’s Core Reading Lists and Advanced Reading Lists. Furthermore, review recorded presentations from webinars and national meetings. 3. Study the BCIA Blueprints of Knowledge and prepare for and attain BCIA certification. BCIA offers certification for clinicians and technicians in biofeedback and neurofeedback, pelvic muscle dysfunction biofeedback exclusively for clinicians, and a certificate of completion in heart rate variability biofeedback for all applicants who have completed appropriate didactic training. 4. Regularly read the principal journals in this field (Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, and NeuroRegulation), the core magazine Biofeedback, and other journals that publish pertinent articles. Subscribe to abstracting services such as the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) at the U.S. National Library of Medicine (NLM). Professional Networking 1. Learn from experienced professionals with a track record of success. Review their patient education documents and didactic handouts. When feasible, consult with them to discuss treatment of selected patients and observe their clinical approaches. While face-to-face opportunities may be limited in some geographic areas, many BCIA Board-certified clinicians provide distance mentoring within their region and internationally. This is especially important outside of North America. BCIA certificants who provide mentoring services are listed on the BCIA website. Also, in a number of professional listserves in diverse biofeedback and neurofeedback specializations, one can communicate with many experts in the field. 2. Attend the annual meetings of AAPB, BFE, ISNR, state/regional biofeedback societies, and/or clinical organizations such as the American Academy of Pain Management (www.aapainmanage. org) and the EEG & Clinical Neuroscience Society (www.ecnsweb.com). These meetings are the best chance to attend a wide variety of symposia, panels, and workshops. They also present an excellent chance to talk with professionals in this field. These meetings are high caliber and attended by many clinicians and researchers who are interesting, competent, academically sound, and encouraging. Many of the presentations are accredited by organizations like the American Medical Association (AMA) and American Psychological Association (APA). AAPB’s website: www.aapb.org. BCIA’s website: www.bcia.org. BFE’s website: www.bfe.org. ISNR’s website: www.isnr.net. 3. Become involved in a state or regional biofeedback society. AAPB lists these societies at www.aapb.org. 4. Invite highly credible and experienced professionals who are good therapists, educators, and/ or researchers to your professional setting. Institutions or other groups of professionals can cooperate to absorb the costs. Practice Considerations 1. Beginners should usually limit the number of biofeedback modalities they initially offer. For example, clinicians might start with heart rate variability (HRV), respiration (RESP), and skin temperature (TEMP) biofeedback for the treatment of stress. These modalities are easier to teach to clients, are widely used in stress management 26 protocols, and provide a relatively inexpensive entry into biofeedback practice. New professionals should be encouraged to “test drive” the field without having to “offer their first born.” After they have mastered these modalities, they can add neurofeedback and surface electromyographic (SEMG) biofeedback, which are more expensive and present a steeper learning curve. Beginners should not try to master all the major modalities because this approach often compromises a professional’s learning and unduly complicates assessment and therapy sessions. 2. If you plan to incorporate surface electromyographic (SEMG) biofeedback into your practice, learn surface muscle anatomy in order to place sensors accurately. Study muscle kinesiology to help in interpreting SEMG measurements. For neurofeedback, learn the International 10-20 System and the Modified Combinatorial System for precise electrode placement. Review brain anatomy and physiology, and the main electroencephalographic (EEG) generators, to understand the significance of EEG values. 3. Perform tracking tests before recording data to ensure that the biofeedback display mirrors the client’s behavior. For example, masseter SEMG amplitude should increase when a client clenches his or her jaw. Know the typical range of values for each biofeedback modality to identify false values quickly. Use your own body as a benchmark. For example, if a person’s resting heart rate is typically 65 beats per minute, a clinician should be suspicious of readings over 85 beats per minute. Whenever possible, examine raw waveforms to ensure signal fidelity. Learn to identify and then remove or minimize artifacts in physiological recordings. Most importantly, acquire a healthy skepticism about the accuracy of measurements. 4. Understand how to mitigate the risk of infection through client and provider hand-washing, cleaning exposed surfaces and reusable sensors, and using disposable sensors. 5. Initially, limit the number of disorders for which biofeedback and/or neurofeedback services are offered. A clinician needs time to read the clinical literature and develop expertise in assessment and treatment. Select more prevalent disorders that are of interest, that are likely to generate referrals and be reimbursed by insurance and supported by efficacy research, and that are appropriate for one’s professional training and license. Always work within your scope of practice as dictated by your state-issued health care license and I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK prior experience and expertise. Consult Tan et al.’s (2016) Evidence-Based Practice in Biofeedback and Neurofeedback (3rd ed.) for an overview of recent outcome studies. If there are several competing practices in the area, a clinician should offer services within his or her scope of practice and competence that are unique. 6. Once sufficient clinical expertise has been developed, be prepared and willing to accept patients with difficult problems, to invest more time with these patients, and to adjust therapeutic goals accordingly. Even some moderate improvement can be very satisfying to such patients and to the referral source. Referral sources will probably appreciate a practitioner’s willingness to accept such patients. 7. Review sample assessment and therapy protocols from credible and experienced professionals. Standardized assessment and therapy protocols may be useful for some applications, but tailoring assessment and treatment to individual patients can also be successful and cost-effective. Practitioners can always adapt the protocols of other therapists to fit their own needs, preferences, and situations. Collaboration and professional networking promote clinical competence. Technical Considerations 1. Be familiar with a few equipment manufacturers before purchasing instruments, and discuss instrumentation with professionals experienced with different manufacturers and models. The AAPB, BFE, and ISNR annual meetings, state and regional biofeedback meetings, and some professional society meetings can provide such exposure to new equipment. Explain your needs and ask for a demonstration before purchasing equipment. A number of independent distributors sell instruments from several manufacturers. Shop around and get good advice about what will best meet the anticipated needs of the clinical setting and be most cost effective. Manufacturers’ online forums can provide valuable information about product performance and manufacturer support from professionals who own the products under consideration. 2. Since most professionals require training and regular technical support to make full use of their systems, select vendors who excel in customer support. This is one of the most overlooked issues when purchasing equipment. Check with a prospective vendor’s customers about the speed, 27 2. Entering the Field and Assuring Competence supportiveness, and effectiveness of their technical support. Since even hardware from respected manufacturers may malfunction during normal use, choose vendors and manufacturers who will quickly repair or replace the equipment. This will reduce “down time,” thus minimizing clients’ or research participants’ cancellations. 3. EMG instrumentation that allows simulta- neous recording from at least two sites is critical for evaluation and therapy. Likewise, neurofeedback providers increasingly use the quantitative EEG (QEEG), which analyzes and topographically displays the EEG power spectrum of signals recorded from multiple electrodes, to guide treatment planning and enrich ongoing assessment. 4. Develop a good working relationship with the vendor and/or manufacturer to quickly resolve problems and take advantage of the system’s features. Mentoring 1. BCIA has moved from a supervision model to a mentoring model. While supervisors provide comprehensive oversight of their supervisees’ actions and assume legal liability for patient care, mentors function as educators and are not liable for their mentees’ actions. An unlicensed provider may work with both a supervisor and a mentor; the supervisor takes responsibility for client care and the mentor provides biofeedback guidance. 2. Mentored providers or technicians should attain the basic training and knowledge base that qualify them for BCIA certification. 3. Mentors and their mentees should maintain close and frequent communications about patients and services. It is important that mentoring be provided by professionals with sufficient biofeedback and/or neurofeedback experience and expertise to match a mentee’s clinical background and needs. 4. Competent use of biofeedback and neurofeedback in a clinical setting begins with a fundamental understanding of the symptoms and conditions to be treated. Recognizing the limits of one’s expertise and collaborating with other health care professionals involved in a case can help ensure treatment success. Interprofessional communication should be clear and reliable, and include interpretations of psychophysiological and clinical data that are accurate, scientifically valid, and pertinent to the present problem. 5. Mentors should maintain their own proficiency by regularly treating patients with biofeedback, neurofeedback, and/or other applied psychophysiological therapies. There is no substitute for this type of direct experience. Education and Training Programs Selecting courses, workshops, and training programs is often difficult. One source of information about training programs is the BCIA website (www.bcia.org). BCIA provides separate listings with contact information for colleges/universities offering biofeedback coursework, didactic education/accredited trainers, and distance education programs. Remember that BCIA only accepts accredited training to satisfy its certification requirements. When you evaluate educational and training programs, the following steps are essential: 1. Assess the presenter’s experience. This includes the number of years using biofeedback, neurofeedback, and other applied psychophysiological therapies; the percentage of time devoted to providing these services; and the number of patients treated. Evaluate the presenter’s qualifications and experience to teach about the specific topic, as well as the number of workshops, courses, and other presentations that he or she has provided. Finally, consider the presenter’s reputation as a clinical practitioner and potential conflicts of interest, which should be listed in a disclosure statement. 2. Select a program sponsored or accredited by a credible organization, such as the American Psychological Association (APA) or the Biofeedback Certification International Alliance (BCIA). 3. Consider comments by previous attendees of the specific education and training program. 4. Check the time available for the topics listed in the program. A minimum of 1 hour is often necessary to cover even very specific topics. Half-day and full-day workshops are often required to explore topics thoroughly. It is desirable for presenters to know the needs and preferences of enrollees a few weeks ahead of time. 5. Ask about the meaning of the term “handson experience.” Will the presenter observe enrollees preparing a subject, attaching 28 electrodes and thermistors, adjusting the instruments, and providing a few minutes of physiological monitoring and biofeedback? Someone who wants to observe and briefly become familiar with an instrument does not need much hands-on experience. However, if someone wants to learn more about using the instruments in assessment and therapy, then more time with the instruments is preferable and necessary. 6. Confirm that the presenter has clearly identified specific goals that are relevant, and that the level is appropriate for one’s experience. 7. Verify which instrumentation will be available for demonstration or use. For vendorsponsored courses, identify which brands of equipment the vendor is authorized to sell. 8. Ask about time for audience questions and discussion. 9. Consider the cost–benefit ratio of sponsoring or attending a continuing education program. Experienced and talented professionals deserve and have the right to expect reasonable compensation for their educational services. Promotional materials, space, administrative factors, transportation for the presenter, and daily expenses all add to the cost. It is also necessary to consider preparation time, even if the instructor has presented the same or similar content before. 10. Check whether the instructor has earned BCIA Board certification. While this is not necessary, it is one piece of useful information about the presenter’s broader knowledge, skills, and involvement in the field. 11. Some manufacturers offer training on their equipment. Check with the manufacturer of the equipment and then ascertain the qualifications of the presenters in the applications of interest. University-based educational opportunities are available at various regionally accredited universities and colleges, at both the undergraduate and graduate levels. Examples of such institutions and their opportunities include the following: • The graduate program in Clinical Psychology at Brigham Young University offers speciality training in clinical health psychology and biofeedback as part of its APA-approved clinical psychology program. Students receive both didactic and hands-on training as part of their doctoral studies. I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK • California School of Professional Psychology at Alliant University in San Diego, California, offers training in clinical psychophysiology and biofeedback as a part of its APA-approved Clinical Psychology PhD program Health Track. Students get both didactic and hands-on training as a part of their doctoral studies. • East Carolina University in Greenville, North Carolina, was the first to offer a university-based distance education course in biofeedback and now offers courses both on campus and through a global classroom. • San Francisco State University’s Biofeedback and Self-Regulation Laboratory and academic program offers undergraduates a minor and a certificate in holistic health. The program includes a semester course on biofeedback, a 4-hour-perweek class, which consists of a 2-hour lecture and 2-hour laboratory practicum; a semester course on the basic theory and technique of autogenic training, and a self-generating therapeutic approach for clinical and nonclinical applications; and an independent study for biofeedback research and mentoring. These classes are also available to nonmatriculating students through the San Francisco State University College of Extended Learning. • Saybrook University offers Basic and Intermediate training and education in biofeedback and an Advanced Biofeedback/Neurofeedback Practicum course taught by BCIA-certified faculty as part of its master’s and doctoral programs in the College of Integrative Medicine and Health Sciences. These courses combine residential and distance learning components. The same college also offers a 10-credit Certificate in Biofeedback and Neurofeedback. Saybrook’s Clinical Psychology degree program offers a PhD in Psychology, with a Psychophysiology Specialization, that combines two required “in person” training sessions each year with distance education. • Sonoma State University in Sonoma, California, has offered a unique upper-division biofeedback professional training sequence in the Psychology Department, open to all university students since 1981. This sequence teaches the BCIA Blueprint and comprises three classes: Biofeedback and Somatics, Biofeedback Practicum, and Biofeedback Internship. • Southwest College of Naturopathic Medicine in Tempe, Arizona, has offered formal elective courses in biofeedback and neurofeedback since 29 2. Entering the Field and Assuring Competence 2005. Both courses cover the BCIA Blueprint and are offered once per year as an elective. • Truman State University in Kirksville, Missouri, offers an undergraduate, 3-hour Applied Psychophysiology course and a 1-hour Research Practicum in its Center for Applied Psychophysiology that may be a part of the bachelor’s degree in psychology. Truman’s undergraduate research team has explored questions in applied psychophysiology since 1977 and annually presents its studies at national and international meetings. • Widener University’s Institute of Graduate Psychology, in Chester, Pennsylvania, teaches biofeedback to about 10 graduate students and 8 online nonmatriculated students each semester, and offers clinical supervision and BCIA mentoring. BCIA anticipates the development of new programs to keep up with the need to educate clinicians. Check BCIA’s website (www.bcia.org) for a current listing of university programs. While many universities may include an introduction to biofeedback as part of a more comprehensive course, it is important to look for coursework that is specific to biofeedback or neurofeedback and based on BCIA’s Blueprints of Knowledge. Certification of Biofeedback Professionals Rationale The primary reason for certification is to set uniform educational and training standards to use biofeedback and neurofeedback competently in clinical, optimal performance, and/or research practice. Biofeedback and neurofeedback are modalities that do not belong to a single profession. BCIA was created in 1981 to set minimal standards for service providers with varied training experience in diverse professions. At that time, as now, no profession had identified specific training and experience criteria for biofeedback and neurofeedback providers. BCIA is the oldest and most widely recognized international board for the certification of biofeedback and neurofeedback professionals. BCIA’s mission is indispensable: “BCIA certifies individuals who meet education and training standards in biofeedback and progressively recertifies those who advance their knowledge through continuing education” (www. bcia.org). Attaining BCIA certification has many advantages for providers of biofeedback and neurofeedback services (e.g., researchers, practitioners, technicians, and presenters of biofeedback educational and training programs). Certification is valuable for both mentors and mentees, although there are certainly competent practitioners who are not BCIA Board certified. Certification is not a guarantee of competence, and it was never intended to guarantee a full range of competencies. However, certification provides a useful index of fundamental knowledge and basic instrumentation proficiency. There are ten compelling reasons why most practitioners using biofeedback and neurofeedback should seriously consider attaining and maintaining BCIA certification: 1. Certification reflects involvement in this field and increases professional credibility. 2. It attests that the certified individual has met specified criteria to use biofeedback and/or neurofeedback competently. 3. It increases “market value” and mobility for many. 4. It gives employers a credible index of training. Many employers require or give preference to applicants who have earned a BCIA certification. 5. Some reimbursement systems view the BCIA credential as an important criterion for reimbursement. 6. BCIA advocates for its certificants and the field when interest groups attempt to restrict who may offer biofeedback and neurofeedback services; and explains the field and BCIA’s (2015) Professional Standards and Ethical Principles to state health care regulators and insurers. 7. BCIA provides free “Find a Practitioner” and job board listings. 8. BCIA awards scholarships for neurofeedback (Eugenia B. Davis and Celeste DeBease Scholarships) and biofeedback (Francine Butler), which waive all certification application fees, to promote the entry of young professionals into the field. 9. A credible certification program is a cornerstone and important sign of the maturation of the field. It improves the image of biofeedback to health care professionals, referral sources, and others outside the field. We should not undervalue its importance. 10. Preparing for certification and maintaining it through continuing education (CE) and 30 I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK recertification every 3 or 4 years involve considerable studying and learning—a benefit for applicants, certificants, and clients/patients. BCIA identifies open access articles that certificants may read and take brief tests over for inexpensive CE credit. Also, BCIA’s clinical update and mentoring webinars provide additional CE opportunities. The certification process acts to deter the least competent practitioners and is an incentive for increasing competence. Epstein and Hundert (2002) defined competence as “the habitual and judicious use of communication, knowledge, technical skills, clinical reasoning, emotions, values, and reflection in daily practice for the benefit of the individual and community being served” (p. 226). It is an objective and acceptable criterion for would-be practitioners to assess their entrylevel competence. Since biofeedback and neurofeedback are unregulated fields, certification helps both the public and professionals select qualified providers. BCIA Certification Programs BCIA now offers biofeedback, neurofeedback, and pelvic muscle dysfunction biofeedback (PMDB) certification for clinicians and technicians. For biofeedback, the requirements for the clinical track include an earned degree in a health carerelated field, anatomy and physiology courses, didactic training in a core curriculum, mentored self-regulation training, clinical biofeedback experience (which includes SEMG, electrodermal, and thermal modalities, and may also encompass an introduction to EEG, heart rate variability [HRV], and respiration), and biofeedback case conferences. The applicant must also successfully complete a psychometrically validated examination on the materials covered in the Blueprints of Knowledge, which provides a detailed outline of information needed to enter the biofeedback field and prepare for the examinations. BCIA added certification in neurofeedback in 1997 and in pelvic muscle dysfunction biofeedback in 2004. Each of these certification programs involves a corresponding health care-related degree, training, and experience requirements, a detailed set of Blueprint Knowledge Statements, a core reading list, and agreement to adhere to BCIA’s Professional Standards and Ethical Principles. All certificants require a license for independent practice when treating a medical and/or psycho- logical disorder. They should read applicable state Practice Acts and understand how they regulate their advertising and provision of services. The BCIA website (www.bcia.org) provides a complete description of the certification process, certification requirements, and helpful resources. Briefly, the Blueprint of Knowledge areas for biofeedback certification (2015 revision) include the following: I. Orientation to Biofeedback II. Stress, Coping, and Illness III. Psychophysiological Recording IV. Research Methodology V. Surface EMG (SEMG) Applications VI. Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) Applica- tions VII. Respiratory Applications VIII. Intervention Strategies IX. Professional Conduct The Blueprint of Knowledge areas for neurofeedback certification (2014 revision) include the following: I. Orientation to Neurofeedback II. Basic Neurophysiology and Neuroanatomy III. Instrumentation and Electronics IV. Research Evidence Base for Neurofeedback V. Psychopharmacological Considerations VI. Patient/Client Assessment VII. Developing Treatment Protocols VIII. Treatment Implementation IX. Current Trends in Neurofeedback X. Professional Conduct The Blueprint of Knowledge areas for PMDB certification (2011 revision) include the following: I. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback II. Pelvic Floor Anatomy, Surface EMG Assess- ment of Pelvic Floor Musculature, and Clinical Practice Procedures III. Clinical Disorders: Bladder Dysfunction IV. Clinical Disorders: Bowel Dysfunction V. Clinical Disorders: Chronic Pelvic Pain Syndromes The Blueprint of Knowledge areas for the heart rate variability certificate of completion (2015) include the following: I. Cardiac Anatomy and Physiology II. Respiratory Anatomy and Physiology 31 2. Entering the Field and Assuring Competence III. Autonomic Nervous System Anatomy and Physiology IV. Heart rate Variability V. HRV Instrumentation VI. HRV Measurements VII. HRV Biofeedback Strategies VIII. HRV Biofeedback Applications Improved BCIA Examinations BCIA task forces carefully evaluate and revise the three certification exams on a regular basis to keep them current and relevant. During the last examination revisions, the reading lists were simplified, making them more compact, accessible, inexpensive, and closely tied to actual examination items. The reading lists consist of current textbooks and articles recommended by experienced educators. Several of these articles may be downloaded for free. Readers can find a current reading list and access articles from the BCIA website (www.bcia. org). During regular examination evaluation and revision, experts review each examination to ensure that their questions adequately represent key Blueprint concepts, are sourced to a suggested reading list, and conform to the highest psychometric standards. They replace outdated examination questions with new ones contributed by leaders in the field and validated by certificants. While BCIA has maintained its high standards, applicant performance on BCIA examinations has improved over the last several years. More candidates than ever are passing with higher scores. More than 85% of candidates pass on their first attempt. Certification by Prior Experience As the field has grown and matured, the BCIA Board discovered that many experts in the biofeedback and neurofeedback fields, who did not become certified earlier in their careers, had already surpassed BCIA entry-level certification requirements. BCIA now offers Certification by Prior Experience (CPE) in biofeedback, neurofeedback, and PMDB. BCIA designed CPE in biofeedback and neurofeedback for licensed professionals who have completed a minimum of 100 hours of continuing education and more than 3,000 patient/client hours using biofeedback, formal study of anatomy/physiology, and formal mentoring. The PMDB CPE program requires a minimum of 72 hours of continuing education and 3,000 hours of patient care using SEMG for elimination disorders. CPE makes BCIA certification more accessible to experienced professionals who have exceeded the entry-level requirements for traditional certification and are leaders in the field. While the majority of these role models did not financially need CPE, they attained it for “the good of the field.” BCIA Certification 2.0 Web-based technology has made BCIA certification and recertification increasingly accessible and affordable: 1. Professionals can now enroll in distance learn- ing courses to obtain didactic and continuing education and to satisfy the anatomy and physiology requirement. 2. Distance mentoring allows professionals in rural communities and foreign countries to receive clinical training by BCIA-approved mentors. Cutting-edge technology allows both parties to see each other via webcams, to view each other’s desktops, and allow a mentor to demonstrate software features on the mentee’s computer. 3. Web-based examination administration allows applicants to take proctored examinations at their local library or university at their convenience. BCIA Certification Goes Global BCIA has accomplished certificants in 32 countries that include Australia, Austria, Canada, Japan, Mexico, the Netherlands, Norway, South Africa, and Spain. BCIA has two international affiliates, BCIA-Australia and BCIA-Mexico, who assist BCIA in setting education and training standards, reviewing applications, and providing guidance for finding qualified mentors and legally using biofeedback and neurofeedback within the laws that regulate health care. There are many reasons for the growth of international BCIA certification. First, several of the best instructors have offered their didactic programs abroad and have subsequently provided distance mentoring for their students. Second, BCIA’s University Initiative has facilitated the creation of the first BCIA-accredited neurofeedback curriculum offered entirely in French by Vincent Paquette and Johanne Levesque at the Institut of Neurofeedback du Quebec. Third, 32 I. ORIENTATION TO BIOFEEDBACK BCIA has worked hard to eliminate obstacles to international certification through distance learning and distance mentoring, and by providing online access to continuing education and testing. Finally, AAPB, BCIA, BFE, and ISNR have enthusiastically reached out to international biofeedback professionals and provided extensive opportunities for collaboration. currently members of AAPB, BFE, and/or ISNR should join or rejoin. Those who are not certified by BCIA should consider this credential for themselves. Those currently certified should continue to stay certified. Those who were certified in the past, but are not currently certified, should return. Our field ensures its integrity when practitioners are “more than qualified, BCIA Board certified!” BCIA Recertification Acknowledgments BCIA has made recertification easier and more affordable than it has ever been. Certificants can now read online articles from Biofeedback, Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, NeuroRegulation, and other open access journals and then complete brief online examinations to earn continued education credit at minimal cost. They can receive credit for scholarly activities such as professional presentations, teaching, and writing. They can also attend educational webinars offered by organizations such as the AAPB, BCIA, and ISNR in the comfort of their home or office. Summary In this chapter we have provided ideas and suggestions for persons entering the biofeedback field. For those already in the field, these ideas and suggestions may help them maintain and enhance their competence. Biofeedback is a broad, heterogeneous, and complex field. Practitioners need infusions of new knowledge, ideas, and skills. Deciding when and where these infusions are to take place and determining who is to provide them are not always easy. In this chapter, we have offered some guidance. AAPB, BCIA, BFE, and ISNR have become international resources and centers for continued maturation of the field. Those who are not We wish to thank Celeste DeBease, Aubrey Ewing, Zachary Meehan, Brian Milstead, Donald Moss, Randy Neblett, and Erik Peper for their invaluable contributions to this chapter. We especially want to recognize the exceptional contributions of BCIA Executive Director Judy Crawford to this chapter and for her decades of dedication to the field we love. References BCIA Board. (2015). Professional standards and ethical principles of biofeedback. Wheat Ridge, CO: BCIA. Epstein, R. M., & Hundert, E. M. (2002). Defining and assessing professional competence. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 226–235. Khazan, I. Z. (2013). The clinical handbook of biofeedback: A step-by-step guide for training and practice with mindfulness. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Peper, E., & Gibney, K. H. (2000). Healthy computing with muscle biofeedback: A practical manual for preventing repetitive motion injury. Woerden, The Netherlands: BFE. Peper, E., Tylova, H., Gibney, K. H., Harvey, R., & Combatalade, D. (2008). Biofeedback mastery: An experiential teaching and self-training manual. Wheat Ridge, CO: AAPB. Tan, G., Shaffer, F., Lyle, R. R., & Teo, I. (2016). Evidencebased practice in biofeedback and neurofeedback (3rd ed.). Wheat Ridge, CO: AAPB. Thompson, M., & Thompson, L. (2015). The neurofeedback book: An introduction to basic concepts in applied psychophysiology (2nd ed.). Wheat Ridge, CO: AAPB. P a r t II Instrumentation Chapter 3 A Primer of Traditional Biofeedback Instrumentation C. J. Peek Monitoring Psychophysiological Arousal: The Central Focus of Biofeedback My aim in this chapter is to put into ordinary language the basic technical matters of practical importance in biofeedback. Technical concepts are introduced through analogy or heuristic description, such that they can become a usable part of the reader’s biofeedback language. This chapter contains many judgments on the practical importance of things encountered in biofeedback, and to that extent represents my own views on the subject, especially in matters where no definitive conceptual, empirical, or practical view holds sway in the field. This chapter is focused on basic electronic and measurement concepts for EMG, temperature, and electrodermal biofeedback. Electroencephalographic biofeedback is covered in Chapter 6 of this volume. This chapter focuses on the “front end,” at which electrodes, basic electronics, and feedback modes interact with clinicians and clients. Other chapters address the “back end,” where computers and myriad forms of feedback and data recording are devised for clinical biofeedback or research. Although computerized biofeedback is common and sophisticated, many biofeedback users still work with freestanding biofeedback instruments or simple devices, such as those used in this chapter as vehicles for illustrating basic electronic and measurement principles. Moreover, users with sophisticated computer systems still need to under- A major application for biofeedback is to provide tools for detecting and managing psychophysiological arousal. As health care fields matured, by the early 1970s it became clear that frequent, excessive, and sustained psychophysiological tension and overarousal cause or exacerbate many health problems. Interest in detecting and managing these states intensified. By the same time, improved biomedical electronics had made it practical to monitor previously “invisible” physiological processes associated with overarousal. The natural combination of these developments in health and technology found expression in the new field of biofeedback, in which the languages and concepts of psychology, physiology, and electronics freely intermingle. The terms “stress,” “anticipation,” “autonomic arousal,” and “muscle fibers” are found in the same sentences as “electromyography” (EMG), “microvolts,” “bandwidths,” and “filters.” Such hybrid sentences usually contain at least some mystery to those (i.e., most of us) who are not fluent in all these languages. Probably the greatest mystery among biofeedback devotees and beginners is the language of electronics. Of the three languages spoken in biofeedback, this is the least similar to ordinary language. 35 36 stand the fundamental principles and methods for detecting and measuring physiological processes, even at times when much may be automated and invisible behind the computer screen. This chapter is therefore created as a primer. Its focus is practical rather than comprehensive. It is simplified rather than highly technical. It stays with basic principles and methods that are foundational to highly technological approaches rather than delving into today’s advanced technology. It is heuristically presented, with emphasis on principle, as well as fact. It also addresses basic instrument functions that may be more hidden to users in this era of computers but remain important to understand even now. Correlates of Arousal: Three Physiological Processes of Interest in Biofeedback Three physiological processes commonly associated with overarousal are skeletal muscle tension, peripheral vasoconstriction, and electrodermal activity. These three, especially the first two, are the most common biofeedback modalities. This is no surprise, as these processes have been recognized all along as intimately involved in anger, fear, excitement, and arousal. This association can be seen by recalling common expressions or idioms found in everyday language. For example, when a person is said to be “braced” for an onslaught, one gets a picture of muscles “at the ready.” The person is tense and may have fists “clenched” and jaw “set”; in a word, the person is “uptight.” If this tension were unrealistic or simply habitual, commonplace advice would be to loosen up,” “relax,” or “let go.” The expression “my blood ran cold” evokes the connection between fear and cold extremities, as does the image “cold hands, warm heart.” In both is the recognition that having cold hands is a sign of emotional responsivity—in other words, the common knowledge that peripheral vasoconstriction is a sign of arousal. In referring to electrodermal activity, a person might illustrate fear with the image of “a cold sweat” or of “sweating bullets.” A picture of calm and ease is drawn by the term “no sweat.” As these idioms illustrate, people already know that muscle tension, peripheral vasoconstriction, and electrodermal activity are related to arousal. The systematic study and modification of these processes are in the domain of biofeedback. Biofeedback devices exist to aid in the study and especially in the modification of these processes. II. INSTRUMENTATION Biofeedback Equipment Terminology A piece of biofeedback hardware may be referred to as “instrument,” “machine,” “device,” “equipment,” “apparatus,” “unit,” and even “gadget” or “gizmo.” Most of these terms are used interchangeably and with little or no uniformity or consistency; often the choice is based simply on preference or whim. This is not offered as a criticism, for people often have many terms for things that are interesting to them. It may simply be a case of the ancient Chinese proverb, “A child who is loved has many names.” In any case, it is worthwhile to outline the connotations for the more popular terms for biofeedback hardware. “Instrument” is the most formal of the terms, denoting a measuring device for determining the value of a quantity under observation. Many items of biofeedback hardware do not qualify as “instruments” under this definition, since actual measurements are not being made; only changes or relative magnitudes are being monitored. For example, “mood rings” and other simple biofeedback “gadgets” or “gizmos” are not considered instruments. The terms “apparatus,” “equipment,” and “device” leave unspecified whether measurement is made, and hence are safe general terms, although “device” implies the performance of a highly specific function. The term “unit” is even more neutral, claiming nothing more than reference to an entity. The term “machine” denotes a mechanism that transmits forces, action, or energy in a predetermined manner. Those familiar with electronics see electronic equipment abstractly transmitting forces, motion, and energy within their circuits, and hence often use the term “machine” in describing biofeedback equipment. In this chapter, most of these terms are used, and (as in common practice) they are used more or less interchangeably. Nothing beyond the ordinary meanings and connotations is intended. What Biofeedback Instruments Are Supposed to Do A biofeedback instrument has three tasks: 1. To monitor (in some way) a physiological pro- cess of interest. 2. To measure (objectify) what is monitored. 3. To present what is monitored or measured as meaningful information. 37 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation The following sections outline how access is gained to three important psychophysiological processes in biofeedback. EMG: An Electrical Correlate of Muscle Contraction A biofeedback device cannot measure muscle contraction in a simple, direct way. When a muscle contracts, it tries to pull its two anchor points together; this is what is meant by “muscle contraction.” It is a kinetic phenomenon involving force and sometimes movement. Practically speaking, this is not easily monitored. One cannot insert a strain gauge between one end of a muscle and its anchor point to measure grams of pull. (Force and movement gauges, called “goniometers,” are used as muscle contraction monitors in physical medicine applications, but these are not sensitive to the levels or locations of muscle contraction involved in relaxation and low-arousal applications of biofeedback.) Because muscle contraction itself is inaccessible, some aspect or correlate of it will have to do. Biofeedback exploits the electrical aspect of muscle contraction. Muscle contraction results from the more or less synchronous contraction of the many muscle fibers that constitute a muscle. Muscle fibers are actuated by electrical signals carried by cells called “motor units,” and muscle contraction corresponds to the aggregate electrical activity in these muscle fibers. This electrical activity can be sensed with fine wire or needle electrodes that actually penetrate the skin above the muscle. More commonly, it is sensed with surface electrodes that contact the skin above the muscle, where there exist weakened electrical signals from muscle fibers beneath the skin. This is the preferred biofeedback method for monitoring muscle contraction, because it is practical and corresponds well to actual muscle contraction. Note that this electrical method, surface EMG, does not directly monitor muscle contraction, but monitors an electrical aspect of muscle contraction that bears a more or less regular relationship to muscle contraction. The important point is this: Surface EMG (hereafter referred to simply as EMG) is the preferred method for monitoring muscle contraction, but it does not directly measure muscle contraction. An EMG device does not read out in units of force or movement, such as grams or millimeters. Instead, it measures an electrical correlate of muscle contraction and reads out in electrical units (microvolts—millionths of a volt). This is because it is making an electrical, not a kinetic, measurement. This explains the initial puzzlement that often comes over the biofeedback novice upon learning that muscle contraction is measured in volts—electrical units that, at face value, seem to have little to do with muscle contraction. Peripheral Temperature: A Correlate of Peripheral Vasoconstriction A biofeedback device cannot directly measure the changing diameter of peripheral blood vessels or the smooth muscle activity that brings about these changes. Therefore, some correlate of vascular dilation will have to do. Dilated vessels pass more warm blood than constricted vessels do. Therefore, surrounding tissue tends to warm and cool as vascular diameter increases and decreases, providing a good correlate of vascular diameter. This effect is most pronounced in the extremities (especially the fingers and toes), where changes in vascular diameter are pronounced, and where the relatively small amount of surrounding tissue warms and cools rapidly in response to changes in the blood supply. Here again the physiological process of interest (peripheral vasoconstriction) is inaccessible, but an accessible correlate (peripheral temperature) is a practical indicator. Biofeedback devices typically read out in degrees Fahrenheit as the indirect indicator of peripheral vasoconstriction. This shows that only indirect access to peripheral vasoconstriction is possible in biofeedback. Finger Phototransmission: Another Correlate of Peripheral Vasoconstriction A second indirect way to gain access to peripheral vasoconstriction takes advantage of the fact that a finger or toe having less blood in its vessels allows more light to pass through than an extremity with more blood. That is, pale skin passes light more readily than infused skin. A small light is shone through the flesh of a finger and is reflected off the bone back to a light sensor. The variation in light intensity at the sensor, and the resulting electrical signal, indicate variation in blood volume. This device is commonly called a “photoplethysmograph” and is sometimes used in biofeedback. It monitors pulse, and (with appropriate circuitry to average out the pulses) can give an indication of relative blood volume, another correlate of vasoconstriction. Such devices read out only in relative units. That is, they read changes 38 but are not anchored to some outside standard reference point. Photoplethysmography is not employed nearly as often as peripheral temperature to indicate peripheral vasoconstriction. Further description of photoplethysmography is beyond the scope of this chapter. For more information, see Jennings, Tahmoush, and Redmond (1980). For more on the psychophysiology of peripheral blood flow, see Cacioppo, Tassinary, and Berntson (2000). Skin Conductance Activity: A Correlate of Sweat Gland Activity Sweat gland activity is another physiological process that is not directly accessible. One cannot tell whether a sweat gland is “on,” how much sweat is being secreted, or how many such glands are active. However, sweat contains electrically conductive salts that make sweaty skin more conductive to electricity than dry skin. Hence skin conductance activity (SCA) corresponds well to sweat gland activity. SCA, along with other electrical phenomena of the skin, is known as electrodermal activity (EDA); historically, it has also been known as “galvanic skin response” (GSR). A skin conductance device applies a very small electrical pressure (voltage) to the skin, typically on the volar surface of the fingers or the palmar surface of the hand (where there are many sweat glands), and measures the amount of electrical current that the skin will allow to pass. The magnitude of this current is an indication of skin sweatiness and is read out in units of electrical conductance called “microsiemens,” formerly referred to as “micromhos.” Here again, an electrical unit (conductance) serves as the indirect measure of a physiological phenomenon (sweat gland activity). This explains what might initially seem odd: that EDA is measured in electrical units that at face value have nothing to do with sweat gland activity. Objectification and Measurement As described earlier, direct monitoring of muscle contraction, peripheral vasoconstriction, and sweat gland activity is not feasible. Therefore, biofeedback devices gain access indirectly through monitoring more accessible correlates of these physiological processes. This means that a biofeedback reading should be taken as a convenient indication of a physiological process but should II. INSTRUMENTATION be understood as separate from the physiological process itself. Practitioners must distinguish the physiological process beneath the skin from the instrumentation schemes outside the skin used to gain access to it. This distinction is important for understanding measurement, objectification, artifact, and the interpretation of biofeedback data. To compare a person’s biofeedback readings from one occasion to another, or to compare readings between different individuals, an objective scale permitting such measurement is advantageous. “Measurement” takes place when the device is calibrated to and reads out in standardized quantitative units that show how the monitored process is varying. An example is a thermometer calibrated to the Fahrenheit temperature scale. On the other hand, what I call “indication of relative magnitude” takes place when an observable signal such as a meter reading is made to correspond to a particular process (e.g., skin temperature or muscle contraction) but the correspondence is not displayed in standardized quantitative units. An example would be a homemade thermometer that reads out on an arbitrary “warm– cool” scale of 1–10. The advantage of measurement is that different observers can make direct quantitative comparisons. Without measurement, observers can only compare relative magnitude or change. Measurement tends to increase replicability of procedures and comparability of results. Measurement in this sense is often not possible in biofeedback, due to the lack of clearly defined and/or widely accepted standardized scales for measurement. For example, EMG devices typically have meters or scaled outputs that give readings in microvolts. These numbers appears to give objectivity to the readings and to permit actual measurement. In fact, however, there is no widely accepted and standardized scale for EMG microvolts. Consequently, different equipment gives different readings for the very same degree of muscle contraction. Therefore, EMG readings can be compared only when the same (or very similarly designed) equipment is used for all the readings, including methods of digital processing (Bolek, 2013). Explanations for this will become clear later, when I describe the design of the EMG device. The important point to remember now is that EMG readings are better thought of as indications of relative magnitude than as measurements. The same is true for skin conductance readings. Skin temperature readings, however, are measurements, as long as the temper- 39 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation ature device is properly calibrated to a temperature scale such as degrees Fahrenheit. Operation of the EMG Instrument The EMG instrument picks up weak electrical signals generated during muscle action. Each muscle consists of many muscle fibers, with “motor neurons” electrically connected to higher levels of the nervous system. Muscle contraction occurs when these motor neurons carry electrical activating signals to the muscle fibers. A small part of this electrical energy leaves the muscle and migrates through surrounding tissue. Some of this energy becomes available for monitoring at the surface of the skin. The tasks of an EMG machine are as follows: 1. To receive the very small amount of electrical energy from the skin. 2. To separate EMG energy from other extrane- ous energy on the skin, and to greatly magnify the EMG energy. 3. To convert this amplified EMG energy into information or feedback that is meaningful to the user. Receiving EMG Energy from the Skin: Electrodes Surface electrodes and the wires attached to them complete the electrical pathways from the skin to the EMG machine. Electrodes come in many forms. Snap-on one-use disposable electrodes are common today, while older equipment often used reusable electrodes (usually small metallic discs mounted on plastic or rubber) attached with tape or double-stick adhesive washers. Other electrodes come on strips or on a headband for simultaneous application of the three electrodes generally required for EMG biofeedback. Some electrodes are permanently attached to electrode cables, whereas others are made to snap onto the cable, permitting changes of electrodes without changing the cable. Many electrodes are made of simple materials, such as nickel-plated brass or stainless steel; others are made of rare materials, such as gold or silver chloride over silver (silver/silver chloride). The precious metal electrodes have historically been the materials of choice for physiological monitoring, because the materials do not interact significantly with skin or other substances with which they are in contact. However, the simpler and cheaper electrodes have been found to be quite satisfactory for biofeedback EMG applications and are now in widespread use. Modern equipment can well tolerate imperfect electrodes and skin preparation, and thus has greatly reduced the need for precious metal electrodes for EMG biofeedback. Electrode Cream or Gel Some EMG electrodes are made for use with an electrode gel or cream. This conductive substance flows into the irregularities of the skin and the electrode, establishing a stable and highly conductive connection between them (see Figure 3.1). Modern equipment is often very resistant to artifact from differences in electrode contact and employs flat electrodes that do not require electrode gel. Electrode gel was more common in the past when cupped, reusable electrodes were in more common use. Skin Preparation A standard part of electrode application is to remove skin oil, makeup, or dead skin cells, because these impede the travel of bioelectric signals from the skin to the electrode. Most manufacturers suggest using alcohol wipes for this purpose. In the past, some manufacturers suggested using an abrasive skin cleaner, although modern equipment and technology has made the use of the alcohol swab sufficient and far more common. The risk in underpreparing the skin is that EMG readings will be erroneously high if the skin remains very oily or covered with makeup. The risk of overpreparing the skin, particularly with the abrasive compounds, is that the skin (and client) will become irritated. There is nothing to be gained by actually scrubbing skin unless there is significant dirt, oil, or makeup present. As said earlier, some equipment operates with simple metal electrodes requiring little or no electrode gel or elaborate skin FIGURE 3.1. EMG electrode and gel. 40 II. INSTRUMENTATION cleaning. This is not to say, however, that one can just forget about skin preparation. Separating EMG Energy from Extraneous (“Noise”) Energy “Noise” is the general term for unwanted or extraneous signals. In EMG machines, there are two kinds of noise: electrical interference and internally generated noise. Electrical Interference and the Differential Amplifier The environment is continuously saturated with electrical energy transmitted through space from power lines, motors, lights, and electrical equipment. Human bodies and EMG electrodes pick up this energy. The EMG apparatus receives these unwanted electrical noise signals in addition to the desired bioelectric signals from the muscles. The EMG unit must therefore find a way to reject the noise so that only EMG signals remain. Interference is rejected in an ingenious way, using an electrical subtraction process in a “differential amplifier.” The electrodes establish three independent pathways from an area of the skin to the EMG instrument. One pathway, called the “reference,” is used by the instrument as a point of reference from which the minute electrical pressure (voltage) exerted from the other two “active” electrodes is gauged. (Remember that any electrical pressure or voltage measurement is defined as a pressure difference between one point and another point. There is no such thing as a voltage measurement without respect to some second point of reference.) This results in two “sources” feeding the instrument, each using the reference electrode as the point of reference (see Figure 3.2). Note that the reference electrode can be placed nearly anywhere on the body, but it is shown in Figure 3.2 between the two active electrodes for the sake of illustration, and because it is a common arrangement. The differential amplifier requires these two sources in order to separate the EMG energy from the extraneous energy. To see why, we must remember that this extraneous energy is the hum or noise transmitted through space from power lines, motors, and appliances that is picked up by the body acting as an antenna. Most of this extraneous electrical noise energy rises and falls rhythmically at 60 cycles per second. At any given moment, this energy is in exactly the same place in its rhythm (“in phase”) at any point on the body and at any point that an electrode can be placed. Hence it is possible for the differential amplifier to continuously subtract the voltage at source 1 from that at source 2. This cancels the noise voltage. Only slightly simplified, this is illustrated graphically in Figure 3.3; it is assumed that the muscle is at rest and giving off no EMG signals. The following steps explain Figure 3.4: 1. Electrical interference is received by the body acting as an antenna. 2. The interference is in the same place in its rhythm for both active electrodes. 3. Therefore, the active inputs (from source 1 and source 2) of the differential amplifier “see” exactly the same interference signal at any given moment (interference is in the “common mode”). 4. Because the output of the differential amplifier is proportional to the difference between the signals at its two active inputs (from sources 1 and 2). 5. And the interference signals are always identical (restatement of point 3). 6. Therefore, the output of the differential amplifier is zero for electrical interference. FIGURE 3.2. Active and reference EMG electrodes. 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation 41 FIGURE 3.3. Differential amplifier eliminating the electrical interference picked up by the body acting as an antenna. But What about EMG Signals? Suppose that motor neurons now signal the resting muscle to contract. Each electrode receives signals most strongly from the area of muscle directly beneath it. Because electrodes are spaced along the muscle, they each receive a different pattern of EMG signals. Here is an analogy: If two microphones were placed in a room full of speaking people, each one would pick up a different pattern of sounds, even if the overall loudness of sound in each microphone were the same. Similarly, at any given moment, the electrodes feed “differential” EMG signals superimposed on the previously discussed identical “common-mode” signals. As the differential amplifier continuously subtracts the signal at source 2 from that at source 1 (thus amplifying only differences between them), the common-mode noise signals will be canceled, while the differential EMG signals will always leave a remainder to be amplified and ultimately displayed on a meter. The operation of the differential EMG amplifier with the desired EMG signals is shown graphically in Figure 3.4 and is summarized below. 1. Different EMG signals arrive at the two e­lectrodes as the muscle beneath them contracts. 2. Therefore, sources 1 and 2 feed “differential” EMG signals to the inputs of the differential amplifier. 3. At the same time, identical (“common-mode”) interference signals are superimposed on the differential EMG signals. 4. Thus, the inputs (from sources 1 and 2) “see” composite signals that have an identical component (common-mode noise) and a differing component (differential EMG signals). 5. Because the output of the differential amplifier is proportional to the difference between the signals at its two inputs (from sources 1 and 2). 6. A portion of the signals are identical (common-mode) and a portion are different (differential-mode) (restatement of point 4). 7. So the output of the differential amplifier is zero for common-mode interference and high for differential-mode EMG signals. FIGURE 3.4. Differential amplifier “canceling” the common-mode interference while amplifying differential EMG signals. 42 II. INSTRUMENTATION The Chemist’s Balance Analogy The operation of the differential amplifier can be illustrated by another analogy. Imagine a sensitive chemist’s balance scale, with its two pans, center fulcrum, and a set of weights. With no weights in the pans, the scale balances. With equal weights in the pans, it also balances. Even if we stretch our imaginations to envision the weights constantly changing (but always remaining equal in both pans), the scale will remain balanced. However, a fly landing on one pan during this process will upset the balance, and the pointer will move off center. Moreover, if two flies of equal weight hop up and down, one on each pan, each with its own idiosyncratic rhythm, the pointer will move from side to side. The deflection indicates, at any given moment, the difference in weight on the two pans. Only differences in total weights can lead to a pointer deflection. By now, the reader will recognize the differential amplifier as an electronic version of the chemist’s balance. Figure 3.5 and Table 3.1 spell out the correspondence between the two. The preceding discussion of the differential amplifier makes it easier to see why deteriorated electrodes or high-resistance contact with the skin can lead to erroneously high EMG readings. For example, if one of the active electrodes makes poor contact with the skin, it will feed a reduced signal to the differential amplifier. Since the other electrode is feeding a full-sized signal to the differential amplifier, the common-mode noise signals applied to the two inputs are of different size. Therefore, when the subtraction process takes place, there is a noise remainder that artificially elevates the reading. Figure 3.6 illustrates this. The ratio of differential signal amplification to common-mode signal amplification for a particular differential amplifier is the “common-mode rejection ratio.” “Input impedance” is the differential amplifier specification that indicates its level TABLE 3.1. Correspondence between Chemist’s Balance and Differential Amplifier Chemist’s balance Differential amplifier 1. Pans 1. Inputs 2. Pointer 2. Output 3. Fulcrum 3. Reference 4. Equal wights in the pans 4. Common-mode → balance (pointer remains straight) 5. Different weights in the pans → imbalance (pointer deflects) 6. Two equal weights in the pans and Two unequal weights in the pans and → imbalance (pointer reflects the difference between the unequal weights only, as equal weights cancel out) signals → zero output 5. Differential signals → nonzero output 6. Common-mode signals and differential signals only, as equal signals cancel out) of protection from inaccuracy due to unequal electrode contact. Both are quite high for reputable instruments. Further discussion of these specifications is beyond the scope of this chapter. Internal Noise: Filters and Bandwidth The task of removing extraneous signals is still not complete. Electrical “filters” further reduce interference from power lines and limit the noise inevitably generated within the circuits of the EMG amplifier itself. These filters are comparable to tone or “equalization” controls on a stereo amplifier, except that they are usually set in one position at the factory. Their purpose is to make the EMG amplifier sensitive to some frequencies (or pitches) of incoming signals and insensitive to others. FIGURE 3.5. Graphic representation of data in Table 3.1. 43 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation FIGURE 3.6. Unequal common-mode noise inputs leading to a noise remainder. Speech or music consists of a wide range of frequencies or pitches, all combined to give us the familiar sounds. Tone controls alter these by increasing or decreasing bass and treble, depending on the listener’s preference. For example, turning down the treble may improve the sound of a particularly hissy tape or scratchy old phonograph record by reducing some of the high-frequency scratch and hiss sounds. Turning down the bass may improve the sound of an amplifier that has a boomy bass or hums. In both cases, a modification of the amplifier’s “frequency sensitivity” or “bandwidth” or “bandshape” is being made. There are reasons to do something similar with an EMG device. For example, much of the electrical interference or noise from power lines is concentrated at a narrow pitch of 60 cycles or vibrations per second (hertz [Hz]). Anyone with a stereo or electric guitar with a bad input cord knows this humming or buzzing sound. To further reduce this noise signal, a special filter can make the EMG amplifier much less sensitive to this pitch. Sometimes, especially in older equipment, the entire bass response of the amplifier is “rolled off” to further reduce any 60 Hz electrical interference remaining after the differential amplifier, even though significant EMG energy appears in the “bass” portion. There is also good reason to limit the EMG amplifier’s “treble” frequency sensitivity. All amplifiers unavoidably generate high-pitched noise within their own circuits that sounds like hiss. The EMG amplifier’s treble response is typically “rolled off” (e.g., above 1000 Hz) to diminish internal noise contributions to EMG readings. The EMG instrument’s range of sensitivity between the bass frequency limit and the treble limit is called the “bandwidth.” Like speech and music sounds, EMG signals are comprised of a range of frequencies or pitch. They tend to vary from about 10 to 1000 Hz. The graph in Figure 3.7 shows two idealized bandwidths superimposed on a hypothetical EMG frequency distribution. This shows that even with treble and bass limits, an EMG amplifier is sensitive to significant amounts of EMG energy. In both cases, the amplifier’s bandwidth (range of sensitivity) includes a significant area of EMG energy. However, the wide bandwidth shown in Figure 3.7 includes more EMG energy (and noise) than the narrower bandwidth. This means that (other things being equal) the instrument set with a wider bandwidth will give higher readings than the one with the narrower bandwidth. A stereo can also illustrate this. Turning the bass and treble controls all the way down narrows the bandwidth and produces not only a different tone but also less volume of both sound and noise. Other things being equal, the wider the bandwidth, the higher the readings for both EMG and noise. The proportion of the reading that is noise (the “signal-to-noise ratio”) may be the same in both cases, but the levels of both EMG and noise will be higher with a wide-bandwidth EMG amplifier. EMG biofeedback devices are made with different bandwidths, due to differing design philosophies. The most important message here is that different bandwidths lead to different readings. This has to be taken into account when one is comparing readings and noise specifications between different models of EMG equipment. For example, an instrument with a lower noise or sensitivity specification may not really be any more sensitive or noise resistant than another; it may just have a narrower bandwidth. Readers who wish to know more about EMG frequency distribution, filters, and bandwidth are referred to Mathieu and Sul- 44 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.7. Two hypothetical bandwidths. livan (1990) and to Bolek (2013) for how digital sampling and processing affects readings. Converting EMG Energy to Information At this point in our story, EMG energy has been picked up from the skin and separated from extraneous noise energy. The resulting signal is proportional to the electrical activity of the motor neurons in the muscle being monitored, and is often referred to as “raw EMG.” Raw EMG Raw EMG resembles auditory static; it is a rushing sound that rises and falls in loudness in proportion to muscle contraction. This “raw” or “raw filtered” EMG is one form of audio feedback. Commercial EMG units usually do not provide raw EMG audio output. Instead, they generate an audio tone or series of beeps, or a signal that is turned into some form of feedback via a computer. The pitch or repetition rate is made proportional to the amplitude or “loudness” of the raw EMG, and therefore to the muscle contraction. Raw EMG amplitude can also be displayed on a meter or other visual display. Smoothing and Integration Smoothing and integration are two ways of quantifying EMG energy over time. “Smoothing” refers to continuously averaging out the peaks and valleys of a changing electrical signal. “Integration” refers to measuring the area under a curve over a time period. Both require processing the raw EMG signal, as described in the next section. Alternating Current and Pulsating Direct Current Raw EMG is an alternating current (AC) signal. Alternating current pushes alternately back and forth or “vibrates” like a reed in the wind or a swinging clock pendulum, as represented in Figure 3.8. The curve represents the change in electrical pressure over time, first in one direction and then in the opposite direction. The “+” represents pressure in one direction, and the “–” represents pressure in the other direction. The centerline represents the point of zero voltage, analogous to the position of the reed at rest or the clock pendulum in its straight-down position. The height of a wave represents its peak amplitude or peak voltage. Figure 3.8 shows an electrical signal “vibrating” at a specific frequency (number of oscillations per second, or hertz). Not only is the electrical signal oscillating, but the amplitude or magnitude of the oscillations first builds to a high point and then diminishes. It is the measurement of this overall increase and then decrease that is significant for EMG biofeedback. The first step in accomplishing this is to “flip” the negative peaks up above the zero line with the positive peaks, a process called “rectification.” Without rectification, the sum of the negative peaks and positive peaks would always equal zero (they would cancel each other). Without rectification, it would be hard to recognize overall trends in magnitude unless one was viewing the oscillations on an oscilloscope or computer screen or listening to the raw EMG over a speaker. Figure 3.9 shows the rectified EMG wave. The negative peaks have been electronically “flipped” up with the positive peaks, 45 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation FIGURE 3.8. Gradually increasing, then decreasing alternating voltage. so that all the peaks are positive. This means that the electrical signal now pushes in just one direction; hence the term “direct current” (DC). In this case, the signal is pulsating DC. Smoothing the EMG Signal for Moment‑to‑Moment Quantification Electronic smoothing can be performed on the rectified EMG signal leading to an “average” EMG. The outputs of smoothing circuits are then used to drive an analog or digital display, as well as audio feedback circuitry (see Figure 3.10 and discussion below). Electronic smoothing is essential for digital meters, because unlike old-style mechanical meters with needles, they have no mechanical inertia to smooth out the pulses. Electronic smoothing or filtering circuits also afford a wide choice of time constant or response time. The designer has wide choice of how fast the digital or analog display or other feedback modality will respond to momentary changes in EMG level, referred to as “tracking time.” The most common form of smoothing or filtering found in small, self-contained EMG equipment employs a fixed time constant (or tracking time) suitable for general purpose use. Some EMG instruments and computer-based systems have selectable tracking times, which require the user to decide how much smoothing of the curve is desired. Long tracking time leads to a smoother output that is less responsive to momentary ups and downs in the EMG level. Unsmoothed output may seem too jumpy for relaxation training, and overly smoothed output may cover or delay information. There is no generally agreed-upon optimum tracking time. The choice is based on application, technique, and subjective preference. It does not appear that any one tracking time is particularly advantageous for relaxation training. This view is apparently shared by the manufacturers, who build their instruments with various fixed or adjustable tracking times. Integration for Cumulative EMG or Average EMG over a Fixed Time Period A second quantification scheme involves letting the area under the EMG curve (in microvolt minutes) accumulate over a period of time, such that the reading starts at zero and continually builds until the time period ends, as shown in Figure 3.11. The accumulated area under the curve at the end of the trial indicates the accumulated number of microvolt minutes of EMG received over that time. Dividing the accumulated microvolt minutes of integrated EMG by the accumulated time in minutes yields the average level of EMG (in microvolts) during that time. Then the timer and integrator are reset to zero, and a new time period or trial begins. Integration establishes relaxation trials of many seconds (e.g., 30, 60, 120, or more seconds). Comparisons can then be made over multiple trials—something that is more difficult FIGURE 3.9. Rectified alternating voltage. 46 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.10. Rectified filtered or smoothed EMG. FIGURE 3.11. Integration for cumulative EMG or average EMG over a fixed time period. to do when only moment-to-moment EMG levels are used. This method was used more commonly in the past. Audio Feedback Audio feedback encodes the EMG level in auditory form and is very important in biofeedback, because it transmits information without the need for visual attention. A common way to do this is to use the smoothed EMG signal to vary the pitch of an electronic tone generator. The higher the EMG level, the higher the pitch. Current equipment often goes far beyond use of simple tones— allowing the therapist or researcher to import all kinds of sounds that can be customized as audio feedback. The range of possibilities for audio feedback is virtually limitless, and many forms have appeared. There is no one optimum form of audio feedback. Preferences develop on the basis of purely subjective criteria, as well as application require- ments and user preferences for modalities such as imported sound tracks. Visual Feedback: Meters, Lights, and Computer Displays Over the years, meters, lights, and computer-based displays have been used for visual EMG feedback. In early years, analog meters with needles were often used, calibrated in microvolts, just using a relative scale. Digital meters also came into use. Analog and digital meters each have advantages and disadvantages for particular applications, as described in earlier editions of this volume. Later, computers and other electronic displays became common—and are capable of an almost limitless range of ways to provide visual feedback. Objective Units of Measurement Several factors besides degree of muscle contraction affect the number of microvolts an EMG 47 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation device displays. A brief review of the earlier section on objectification and measurement may be helpful. The microvolt is the unit of EMG measurement; this is an electrical term used as a measure of muscle contraction. The microvolt is not literally a measure of muscle contraction, but a measure of an electrical correlate of muscle contraction. Therefore, microvolt readings involve the characteristics of the electrical apparatus (the EMG unit) that monitors and processes the EMG signals. Because of differences in design philosophy, EMG devices differ from one another, and so do the readings obtained for any given degree of muscle tension at a given site on a given person. Consequently, microvolt readings are only objectively comparable from one model to another if the instruments are known to have the same bandwidth and quantification method. For the technically inclined, EMG instruments are AC voltmeters that make objective AC voltage measurements. However, the internal characteristics of bandwidth or bandshape and quantification method affect these measurements. Accuracy, if specified, is only at a given frequency within the bandwidth. Because EMG voltages sensed by surface electrodes are composed of an ever-changing blend of frequencies (see Figure 3.7), the bandwidth or bandshape of any particular unit will affect the readings. (This has been discussed in the section on internal noise, filters, and bandwidth.) Quantification method also affects EMG instrument readings—the numbers on a display. For those technically inclined: Early EMG instruments were calibrated in “average,” “root mean square (RMS),” or “peak to peak” microvolts, though there is little practical difference between these quantification methods or in the action of a meter or other display, just different scales. Calibration is done using conveniently available constant-amplitude AC signals called “sine waves” rather than nonstandardized EMG signals (see Figure 3.8). • The term “peak-to-peak microvolts” refers to the voltage difference between the positive peaks and the negative peaks of the unrectified AC sine wave. Quantification by the “averaging” method usually involves rectification and smoothing, then moment-to-moment display, or integration and division by time, both described earlier. • The “average” voltage of a sine wave after rectification as displayed on a meter is equal to just less than one-third of the “peak-to-peak” value. To convert from peak-to-peak values to average values, divide by 3.14. • Quantification by the RMS method involves electronically making a mathematical computation on the filtered EMG signal to arrive at an RMS voltage. RMS quantification reflects the electrical power as contrasted with voltage, carried by the signal. RMS values for EMG are usually within 20% of average values. In any case, the user of EMG equipment should become familiar with the range of readings obtained under various conditions, and should be cautious about comparing microvolt readings between units that are not known to have similar characteristics. The lesson here is that even though EMG instruments are AC voltmeters, EMG readings are not made on standardized scales and are not standardized measurements of muscle contraction. Variability exists between EMG instruments, and there is no standardized scaled correspondence between EMG microvolts and muscle contraction. Thresholds A threshold control allows the user to set a particular EMG level as a criterion for some form of feedback (e.g., to turn on audio feedback only when EMG exceeds the threshold). Visual feedback, such as lights or a computer display, may indicate when EMG exceeds or drops below the chosen threshold level. Thresholds are adjusted over time as training goals change. Other Feedback Modes The smoothed EMG level can be used to operate virtually any feedback method, including lights, sound, appliances, computers, or tactile feedback devices. All forms of feedback are ways of encoding EMG level as meaningful information or consequences. Choice of feedback mode depends on the requirements of the application and the people using the feedback. Although complex or novel feedback may be interesting, the best feedback modes for a given application are the ones that get the information across with a minimum of distraction and ambiguity. Simple, well-designed feedback often fits this criterion. Practitioners often settle on a limited number of practical feedback modes. 48 Safety EMG equipment makes direct electrical connection to a person via surface electrodes, thereby establishing a path for bioelectric signals between the person and the instrument. Although this path is intended for bioelectric signals, electricity from other sources can also take this path under some conditions. The presence of other currents in the signal path is a risk. Consequently, great care is taken in the design and manufacture of top-grade biomedical instruments to minimize the possibility of exposing patients to extraneous electrical currents. Despite this, no equipment, no matter how well made and installed, is 100% immune from electrical hazards for all time. The chance of risky electrical faults developing is small, especially in battery-operated equipment, but the manner in which an equipment user sets up and maintains the equipment is at least as important to patient safety as the soundness of the equipment design. It is therefore the responsibility of the professional using these instruments to be aware of potential electrical hazards and to take standard safety precautions in installing, using, and maintaining the equipment. If there is any question about the safety of a particular installation, the professional must consult the manufacturer of the equipment or a qualified biomedical engineer or technician. This is particularly important when there are multiple instruments or any connections to power-line-operated equipment or accessories. A good rule of thumb is to be skeptical of the safety of all setups involving power-line-operated auxiliary equipment, such as audio amplifiers, computers, or other devices (especially if not part of a commercially purchased biofeedback setup), until the safety of the installation is positively established. This is because the potential consequences of leakage current from the AC power line can be extreme. For example, it takes only 0.009 amperes (9 milliamperes) or less to cause a person to be unable to release his or her grasp on an object through which the leakage current flows. Respiration may be affected at approximately 18 milliamperes, and heart fibrillation (and death) may occur at about 50 milliamperes. This is hundreds of times less than the current required to blow a standard household fuse or circuit breaker, so they provide no protection. There are several precautions to take, some of which require the consultation of a biomedical engineer or technician: II. INSTRUMENTATION 1. Each power-line-operated piece of auxiliary equipment should be periodically evaluated technically and certified by a biomedical technician for electrical safety. Power-line operated EMG equipment should also be periodically tested for leakage currents. Consult the manufacturer or your biomedical technician. 2. Keep all patients or subjects out of arm’s reach of all metal building parts, such as radiators and plumbing. 3. Ground all equipment properly. Use a “ground fault interrupter,” a device that senses a diversion of electricity from the normal pathway established by the two legs of the standard power circuit. This device shuts down power to the equipment if more than about 5 milliamperes of current is “lost” through non-normal pathways, such as leakage current to ground through a person. Troubleshooting with a “Dummy Subject” High-grade EMG circuitry is quite reliable, but electrodes, cables, and batteries may need frequent service in heavily used installations. Diagnosing failure of these parts is usually simple and requires few tools. Faulty electrodes or electrode contact usually leads to spuriously high readings. Follow the electrode maintenance and application instructions supplied with the instrument. If unexpected or suspiciously high readings are observed, determine whether the problem is in the electrodes, the electrode contact, the cable, or the EMG unit. Use a “dummy subject,” which is nothing more than two resistors that can be snapped to the electrode cable in place of the normal electrodes. This simulates a subject with zero EMG (see Figure 3.12). The dummy subject supplies about the same “input resistance” as actual electrodes on skin, but generates no EMG signals. With the dummy subject in place, the readings should therefore be close to the residual noise level of the instrument as given in its specifications. For a fair test, hold the electrode cable between the fingers at least a foot away from the dummy subject as it dangles toward the floor. This distance prevents excessive noise from being coupled from your body to the dummy subject. EMG readings with the dummy subject typically vary as you twist the cable between the fingers, much as television reception on “rabbit ears” varies as one rotates the antenna. If a dummy subject test done in the patient area results in a reading near the instrument’s residual 49 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation FIGURE 3.12. Dummy subject. noise specification, then it is safe to conclude that electrical noise in the area is not overpowering. This means that suspiciously high readings with the real subject are not the result of failure of the EMG unit or electrode cable. In this case, the fault is most likely with the electrodes or electrode contact. If the reading goes off scale and stays there while the dummy subject is rotated, there is likely to be a break in the electrode cable. Verify this by substituting another cable. If the repeat test still leads to off-the-scale or very high readings, then it is likely that the fault is with the EMG unit itself, or that the work area is saturated with electrical noise. High dummy subject readings that do not go off the scale may be attributable to excessive noise from nearby electrical equipment. Check this by moving the machine to other locations and repeating the dummy subject test. If the dummy subject test indicates that the instrument and cable are working properly, but abnormally high readings with the real person being tested remain, consider removing the electrodes, cleaning the person’s skin again, and reapplying the electrodes. Construct a dummy subject if you do not have one already. Experiment with the dummy subjects when you know that your instrument and cables are working properly. You will then be in a better position to judge test results with dummy subjects when actual failures occur. Battery Failure Abnormally high or low readings may result from battery failure. Most instruments have a built-in battery check or battery-checking instructions in the user’s manual. Use it whenever there is doubt about the accuracy of the readings. Aging batteries may pass the check and work fine early in a session, deteriorate during the session, then “self-rejuvenate” after a few idle hours. The usable time after these self-rejuvenations gets shorter and shorter, until the batteries are unable to power the equipment at all. Summary A summary block diagram of a hypothetical EMG instrument with several outputs is presented in Figure 3.13. Operation of the Temperature Biofeedback Instrument Temperature biofeedback instruments measure changing skin temperature, which is significant because it is linked, through vasoconstriction, to sympathetic arousal. Vasoconstriction affects perfusion of blood and therefore skin temperature, particularly in the extremities (especially the fingers and toes). Typically, sympathetic arousal leads to increased vasoconstriction, which leads to a reduction in blood volume and hence to a cooling effect at the skin. Although this neurovascular phenomenon involves the constriction and dilation of vessels, the single term “vasoconstriction” is used here to denote all changes in vascular diameter. For example, “reduced vasoconstriction” is used to express the idea of vasodilation. 50 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.13. Block diagram of hypothetical EMG instrument with several outputs. The tasks of a temperature biofeedback instrument are as follows: 1. To let the skin heat a temperature-sensitive probe. 2. To make the probe serve as a temperature- sensitive electrical “valve” that modulates an electric sensing current applied to the probe. 3. To display temperature-dependent variations in probe current as temperature in degrees, and to provide other temperature feedback or information meaningful to the user. Letting the Skin Heat a Probe A typical temperature probe is made of one or more small pieces of heat-sensitive electrical material (called “thermistors”), encased in electrically insulating material with wires protruding for connection to the temperature unit. A temperature probe is not an electrode. It is specifically designed to make only thermal contact, not electrical contact with the skin, where it is usually taped or strapped. The probe accepts heat from the skin and remains at nearly the same temperature as the skin immediately beneath it. As the skin warms and cools, the probe warms and cools accordingly—but with a slight delay, as probe temperature takes a little time to “catch up” with the skin temperature. The probe is attached to either side of a finger. No single site is standard, nor has any particular site been shown to be superior. However, consistency from session to session is important, because temperature or speed of response may vary from site to site. The dorsal surface (back side) of the fingers is a common site. This permits the person to rest the hand on the chair or lap without artificially warming the probe between the finger and the chair or body. Furthermore, the dorsal surface has fewer sweat glands, so the chance of evaporative cooling is less. It is no doubt possible to make a case for the use of other sites as well, but consistency will probably remain more important than the specific choice of finger site. Making the Probe Serve as a Temperature‑Sensitive Electrical Valve The heat-sensitive probe acts like a “valve” for electricity applied to it from the instrument, analogous to a water valve that gradually opens and closes to regulate water flow. But in this case, probe temperature operates the “valve” and regulates the flow of electricity. As the probe heats, its electrical resistance decreases, and more electric current flows. As the probe cools, its resistance increases (the “valve” closes a little), and less electric current flows. In this way, probe (and skin) temperature is encoded in the electrical flow through the probe. 51 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation Displaying Temperature and Other Feedback TABLE 3.2. Ohm’s Law: Voltage, Resistance, and Current The temperature instrument measures the current flow through the probe and displays this quantity (properly scaled) as degrees or as other feedback. Electrical law Internal Workings of Temperature Feedback Devices Temperature feedback instruments can perform the required operations in more than one related way. Use of temperature biofeedback equipment does not require detailed knowledge of internal workings. However, it is important to understand the basic scheme shared by all temperature feedback devices. Hydraulic analogy Units Volt: Unit of electrical pressure Pounds per square inch: Unit of water pressure Ampere: Unit of electric current flow Gallons per minute: Unit of water flow Ohm: Unit of resistance to electric current flow Unspecified unit of resistance to water flow Circuit description Pressure (in volts) pushes the current (in amperes) Quantification Ohm’s Law Temperature feedback devices operate on one or another form of Ohm’s law. Georg Ohm was the Bavarian scientist who, in 1827, specified the quantitative relationships among three basic elements of an electrical circuit: voltage, resistance, and current. In 1891, the Electrical Congress in Paris agreed that electrical pressure would be measured in volts, after Volta, an Italian; electrical flow volume would be measured in amperes, after Ampère, a Frenchman; and resistance in ohms, after Ohm, a German. Because there is a convenient hydraulic analogy to Ohm’s law, the law and the analogy are presented together in Table 3.2. Ohm’s Law and a Temperature Feedback Device According to Ohm’s law, the amount of current flowing in a circuit powered by a constant voltage depends entirely upon the resistance in the circuit. The resistance of the probe varies with its temperature. Therefore, when the probe is the only resistance element in a constant-voltage circuit, the current flow in the circuit is proportional to the temperature of the probe. The quantitative relationship between temperature and probe resistance is a property of the probe and varies greatly from one model to another. For this reason, probe models are usually not interchangeable. A suitable current-sensing circuit with meter displays a reading in degrees. Figure 3.14 shows a hypothetical temperature feedback device. Pressure (in pounds per square inch) pushes the water flow (in gallons per minute) through the resistance of the pipes Current = pressure/resistance; that is, Amperes = volts/ohms (Ohm’s Law) Algebraic formulas Volts = amperes × ohms Ohms = volts/amperes Conventional abbreviations Voltage: V or E Current: I Resistance: R Parameters of Temperature Feedback Devices: Ways They Differ from One Another Temperature feedback devices come in a wide range of performance and cost. The following three parameters—response time, absolute accuracy, and resolution—provide a basis for judging or comparing the performance of temperature feedback devices. An exploration of various device factors that may influence feedback and training results in computer biofeedback systems appears in Otis, Rasey, Vrochopoulos, Wincze, and Andrasik (1995). Response Time “Response time” indicates how rapidly the unit responds to a change in skin temperature. It is mostly a property of the probe; if a probe responds quickly, feedback delay is minimal, and small 52 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.14. A hypothetical temperature feedback device. temperature changes are readily apparent. However, quick response time is usually gained at the expense of increased cost and fragility. A very fastresponding probe is very small and light, encased in a material that gains or loses heat very rapidly in step with skin temperature. Larger, bulkier probes are cheaper and more durable, but they take more time to heat and cool as skin temperature changes. A very fast-responding probe is not thought necessary in most applications. To understand why, recall that skin temperature is important because it provides indirect access to peripheral vasoconstriction. There is already a considerable time delay between a change in vascular diameter and the resultant change in skin temperature. Probe response time adds a second delay to the overall delay between the vascular event and the resulting temperature event. One could argue that because of these delays, it is important to minimize probe response time, so that further delay is kept to a minimum. A counterargument is that skin temperature is a relatively slow-changing phenomenon to which rapid response time does not add value for relaxation applications. Neither view holds obvious sway. Successful thermal biofeedback appears to have been conducted with temperature devices of widely differing response times. Absolute Accuracy “Absolute accuracy” refers to how closely the displayed temperature corresponds to the actual probe temperature. Virtually any temperature machine will follow temperature changes (delayed by its particular response time), but there is variation between instruments in the accuracy of the temperature readings. Although a given unit may respond very sensitively to changes in temperature, it is unlikely that readings will exactly equal the true temperature of the probe; it may read up to a few degrees higher or lower than the true temperature. Furthermore, two identical units monitoring the same site will probably not give exactly the same readings. This variability in absolute accuracy is to be expected, and the error range for a given unit is usually included in its specifications. Absolute accuracy of ±1°F is considered sufficient. Absolute accuracy is a tradeoff against cost, because ensuring a high degree of absolute accuracy tends to be very expensive. And practical advantages of highly accurate temperature equipment for clinical biofeedback are not evident. Successful biofeedback takes place with widely differing degrees of absolute accuracy—and includes devices that are not calibrated to the Fahrenheit or Celsius standard at all, giving only relative indications of warming and cooling. The question of accuracy arises for temperature feedback equipment because there exist standardized temperature scales (Fahrenheit and Celsius). Remember that although temperature is measured on a standardized scale, vasoconstriction is not. An absolutely accurate temperature reading does not imply an absolutely accurate gauge of vasoconstriction, much less sympathetic arousal. Resolution “Resolution” refers to the smallest temperature change that the instrument can discern and display. Resolution affects length of feedback delay. For example, a digital unit that resolves 53 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation to 1°F will feed back that a temperature change has taken place when a 1° change has occurred. Since temperature change occurs over time, the feedback will be delayed by however long it takes for the temperature to change 1°F. A resolution of 0.1°F will provide much more rapid feedback, because it takes far less time for the temperature to move 0.1° than 1°. Instruments could be built to resolve 0.01°F, which would reduce feedback delay even further. However, extremely high resolution also increases the risk of mistaking artifact for vasoconstriction-caused temperature change. For example, the effects of movement, a light breeze, and room cooling are much more likely to affect the readings from an instrument with exceedingly fine resolution than from one with coarser resolution. Furthermore, a high-resolution temperature instrument must be manufactured with much more exacting tolerances and increased expense. Otherwise, it may create discernible change in the readings through “drift” in its own circuits. An instrument with exceedingly high resolution is more likely to display distracting information or artifacts superimposed on true vasoconstrictive effects. A resolution of 0.1°F is historically a typical resolution value for temperature instruments and appears to be a suitable general purpose value. Digital and analog feedback have different resolving power. For example, a digital meter with three digits (10’s, 1’s, and 10ths) can resolve to 0.1°F. However, an audio tone (e.g., the sensitive pulsed-tone feedback described in the EMG section) indicates even finer differences that occur during the interval between changes of the 10ths digit on the meter. heat from the skin. Cool air may also directly cool the probe. Artifacts Room Temperature and the Temperature Feedback Instrument Because peripheral temperature is an indirect index of peripheral vasoconstriction, there are several sources of misleading readings. In looking for sources of artifact, the question to ask is this: “What conditions lead to temperature changes that are not linked to vasoconstrictive changes?” Cool Room Temperature Air temperature in the room where measurements are being made may affect the readings. For a given degree of vasoconstriction, skin temperature may be cooler in a cool room than in a warm room, simply because the cool air absorbs more Breeze Moving air exaggerates the cooling effect mentioned earlier in two ways. First, breeze removes heat from the skin more rapidly than does still air. Second, breeze evaporates sweat more rapidly than does still air. Warm Room Temperature Room temperature sets an approximate lower limit for hand temperature. That is, a hand cannot cool very much below the temperature of the air around it. This is because cooling takes place through the dissipation of heat from the hand to the air. As soon as the hand cools down to the temperature of the air, there is no longer anyplace for heat to go. The hand remains at about that temperature regardless of further vasoconstriction, unless the skin cools a little further as sweat evaporates. Warm air effect is usually not a problem, because room temperature is usually below 72°F (close to the low end of the skin temperature range for most persons). However, in the event of a high room temperature, higher skin temperature will be observed than in a cooler room, even with an identical degree of vasoconstriction. For example, using thermal biofeedback in a 90°F room will lead to warmer hands for everyone, regardless of the degree of vasoconstriction. In this case, even the hand temperature of a cadaver, which has no warm blood at all, would be 90°F! Even if the temperature of the probe is held constant, temperature readings may change as the temperature unit itself is heated and cooled. The performance of electronic circuitry is vulnerable to change or “drift” as surrounding air temperature changes. This is a well-known phenomenon that designers take into account. Such “temperature compensation” is very important for temperature instruments, because they are required to resolve exceedingly small changes in electric current from the probe. If temperature compensation is inadequate, then readings vary with room temperature, as well as with skin temperature. This source of 54 artifact is not practically significant unless room temperature is known to vary over a wide range. Probe Contact and “Blanketing” Changes in probe contact caused by movement also affect temperature readings. If the probe begins to lift from the skin when pulled by its leads, lower readings are likely. The opposite occurs when the probe is covered by a hand, clothing, or materials used to secure the probe to the skin, all of which have the effect of “blanketing” the probe. It is important to allow normal heat dissipation into the surrounding air rather than trapping the probe or hand under such materials. Chill If the person to be monitored comes in chilled from the outside, cold hands are likely. Cold hands should be allowed to restabilize indoors before training begins. Otherwise, the natural warming of the hands after being exposed to cold may be mistaken for a training effect. It can take considerable time for skin temperature to stabilize after coming in chilled from outdoors. Testing for Absolute Accuracy Test the accuracy of temperature instruments by immersing the probe in a glass of water along with a laboratory thermometer of known accuracy, then stirring the water. Compare the readings after they have stabilized. This test is useful when the accuracy of the instrument or probe is questioned, or when the actual interchangeability of “identical” probes is assessed. If done carefully, this method can be used to test for temperature drift in the temperature instrument itself. With probe temperature stabilized in a thermos of water, heat and cool the instrument while noting any change in its reading. Other Feedback Different models of stand-alone or computerized temperature biofeedback instruments employ different variations on the basic audio and visual feedback described in this chapter, as well as different levels of response time, absolute accuracy, and resolution. A question remains about which combinations of these parameters and feedback modes are most effective for training various skills. II. INSTRUMENTATION There is some evidence that significant differences may exist (Otis et al., 1995), although a systematic research base on these many variations does not exist. Audio Feedback Digital meters are often used for visual feedback, because they resolve small differences over a very wide range. Audio tones cannot provide the same resolution over such a wide range. If a usable range of audio pitches is simply distributed over the working range of skin temperature, then persons with very low or high skin temperature will have to listen to feedback in the extremes of the audio range. This will be uncomfortable to listen to for long. Moreover, small changes in temperature will lead to only slight changes in the pitch of the tone. A good solution to this problem is to let the user move the entire pitch range of audio tones up and down the temperature range, so that high-resolution audio feedback in a comfortable pitch range can be obtained, regardless of the actual skin temperature. Moving the audio range is accomplished by turning a control that affects the pitch of the audio feedback but not the meter readings. In this way, the user adjusts the audio feedback for a comfortable pitch range around any temperature. Some temperature machines have a control that allows the user to select whether the pitch rises or falls with temperature. This encourages the user to fit the audio feedback to his or her warming images. For example, some users feel that the image of increasing blood flow through the fingertips calls for an increasing audio pitch. Others find decreasing pitch more natural as relaxation occurs. Derivative Feedback Derivative or “rate” feedback is sometimes found on temperature instruments. “Derivative” is a mathematical term referring to rate of change. In a temperature machine, this usually takes the form of a light or tone that turns on when skin temperature is changing at a certain rate. For example, a red light turns on when the person’s hand temperature is climbing at 1°F or more per minute. Another light or tone might come on if the person’s hand temperature is falling at that rate. This establishes a target hand-warming rate and permits a summary quantification, such as the percentage of time above the target warming rate. 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation Safety Because no electrodes are used, temperature biofeedback equipment may not pose the same electrical safety challenges as EMG equipment. The probe is deliberately electrically insulated from the subject, so the chances of a risky electrical fault’s developing may be lower than with EMG equipment. Nevertheless, temperature equipment should not be considered exempt from the safety precautions discussed earlier for EMG equipment. If, for example, a probe fails (internally or through a break in its insulation) so that it is no longer insulated from the skin, it becomes in effect an electrode. This increases the potential for electric shock or leakage currents, particularly since the temperature device is probably not specifically designed to operate safely with a direct electrical connection to a person. Therefore, to be as safe as possible, follow the safety guidelines for EMG equipment. Moreover, safety guidelines are best thought of as applying to entire biofeedback installations, not just the individual units in isolation. Electrodermal Biofeedback Early History of Electrodermal Research The early history of electrodermal research is an interesting story recounted by Neumann and Blanton (1970). They begin the story with Galvani’s discovery of the electrical processes in nerve and muscle action, which quickly stimulated research into the medical applications of electricity. By 1840, it was widely believed that electrical processes provided a basis for explaining disease and generating diagnoses and therapies. The authors note that this was strongly consistent with the physicalistic thinking of the day, in reaction to the vitalistic thinking of earlier times. By 1870, thensophisticated instrumentation and procedures had been developed as part of electrophysiological research methodology. (A fascinating collection of such literature and instrumentation exists at The Bakken Museum, Minneapolis, MN.) As the field developed, investigators noted that skin resistance varied over the body. Because investigations focused on the physical effects of electrical currents and static fields, the early workers noted that variations in skin resistance introduced variations in current flow through the body; hence they viewed variations in skin resistance as a source of artifact, and they built instruments 55 that controlled for this artifact. Most researchers continued to regard variations in skin resistance as artifact encountered while applying electric current or static fields for diagnostic or therapeutic purposes. But in 1879, Romain Vigouroux measured skin resistance as an experimental variable in cases of hysterical anesthesias. This, according to Neumann and Blanton (1970), is generally regarded as the first observation of psychological factors in electrodermal phenomena. In 1888, Vigouroux’s colleague, Charles Fere, studied the effect of physical stimulation on skin resistance, noting increases in current flow following stimulation. This, the reviewers say, was the first study of what by 1915 was called galvanic skin response (GSR), and was probably the first statement of an arousal theory. It is noteworthy that by Fere’s time, the French physicist D’Arsonval had developed silver chloride nonpolarizable electrodes for physiological research, as well as a sophisticated “galvanometer” (needle-type meter), a forerunner of modern meter movements that still bear D’Arsonval’s name. The German investigator Hermann linked GSR with sweat gland activity in 1881, thus establishing a physiological basis for the phenomenon. In 1889, the Russian investigator Ivan Tarchanoff, while investigating skin potentials, showed that not only physical stimuli but also mental activity (e.g., mental arithmetic and the recollection of upsetting events) led to skin potential changes. Moreover, he linked this phenomenon to the distribution of sweat glands and proposed that it was related to the action of “secretory nerves.” Neumann and Blanton (1970) report that Tarchanoff’s and Fere’s papers were followed by “several years of oblivion.” GSR was rediscovered in 1904. At that time, a Swiss engineer, E. K. Mueller, noticed that skin resistance changes with psychological events. He showed this to the Swiss neurologist Veraguth, and both believed this to be a newly discovered phenomenon. Mueller went on to assume the role of a psychological expert and to address the technical problems of measurement, reliability of electrode design, and experimentation with the use of AC. By 1905, Veraguth had finished some preliminary experiments when he embarrassedly discovered the earlier work of Tarchanoff and others. Veraguth and Carl Jung were friends, and somehow (each claimed to have suggested it to the other), GSR was used in Jung’s word association experiments. Jung then provided most of the 56 impetus for further studies in this area. By 1907 he considered GSR, known to Veraguth and Jung as “psychogalvanic reflex,” a means of objectifying heretofore invisible “emotional tones.” Jung embarked on extensive studies and exported this idea to friends in the United States. Neumann and Blanton (1970) report that a “flood” of papers in America appeared over the next two decades and established this field as a major research area. Since then, GSR has been recognized as a way to gain objective access to psychophysiological arousal. This physiological variable has appeared in countless psychological experiments, in clinical practice, in “lie detector” equipment, and even in toys and parlor games. Biofeedback has used it for access to autonomic arousal. GSR is recognized as distinctively sensitive to transitory emotional states and mental events, while often remaining more or less independent of other biofeedback measures such as muscle tension and skin temperature. It is a complex variable, responsive to a wide range of overt and covert activities and external and internal stimulation. Its responsivity to psychological content in actual or laboratory human situations apparently prompted Barbara Brown (1974) to dub GSR “skin talk.” This is an apt metaphor that does justice to its psychological responsivity, while legitimizing its often complex and seemingly unpredictable variations and individual differences. Like any actual language, “skin talk” must be studied and experienced to be understood. EMG and temperature biofeedback are, in comparison, more easily understood by virtue of their less articulated response to mental events. That is, EMG and temperature biofeedback tend not to reflect mental events as quickly or with as much resolution as GSR. Electrodermal phenomena are often regarded as more complex and less well conceptualized than other biofeedback measures because of rapid responsiveness, individual variability, methodological challenges in measurement, and the multiplicity of technical approaches. My purpose in this section is to conceptualize the skin conductance phenomenon, and to describe and critique some of the approaches to skin conductance measurement and instrumentation. As revealed in the history provided earlier, two forms of EDA have been studied. The most common is the exosomatically recorded activity of Fere, Veraguth, and Jung, in which an external electric current is passed through the skin. Activity is indicated by the skin’s electrical resistance (or II. INSTRUMENTATION its reciprocal, conductance). The second method, that of Tarchanoff, is endosomatically recorded activity (skin potentials), which involves monitoring voltage differences between electrodes at two points on the surface of the skin. The endosomatic method is not covered in this chapter, because it is much less common in biofeedback than exosomatically recorded skin conductance. For more on the endosomatic method, see Venables and Christie (1980). For more on EDA, see Dawson, Schell, and Filion (2000) and Boucsein (2012). Dawson et al.’s work is a chapter in the third edition of Cacioppo et al.’s (2000) Handbook of Psychophysiology. This handbook is also recommended for basic information relevant to biofeedback. Terms GSR is historically a universally recognized term for EDA, perhaps because the term has for a long time referred to a variety of exosomatic and endosomatic phenomena, and to both levels and responses. Although the term GSR will probably continue in widespread use, other terminology has been suggested that is more descriptive of specific electrodermal phenomena. Adopted from Venables and Christie (1980), the following nomenclature is used in this chapter. Electrodermal activity (EDA), electrodermal response (EDR), and electrodermal level (EDL) are general terms for either exosomatic or endosomatic phenomena. EDL refers to baseline levels; EDR refers to responses away from baselines; and EDA, the most general term, refers to levels and/ or responses. Skin conductance activity (SCA), skin conductance response (SCR), and skin conductance level (SCL) specify the exosomatic method and the conductance (in contrast to resistance) scale. Again, SCL refers to baseline levels; SCR refers to changes from baselines; and SCA refers to either or both. Parallel terms for skin resistance and skin potentials are sometimes used: skin resistance activity (SRA), skin resistance response (SRR), and skin resistance level (SRL); skin potential activity (SPA), skin potential response (SPR), and skin potential level (SPL). Table 3.3 clarifies the meaning of all these terms and their interrelationships. Although the table contains a dozen terms, this chapter is concerned only with SCA—that is, SCL and SCR. These are clearly the prevalent forms of electrodermal biofeedback. 57 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation TABLE 3.3. Organization of Electrodermal Terms Activity Response Level Exosomatic Endosomatic or exosomatic Conductance Resistance Endosomatic EDA EDR EDL SCA SCR SCL SRA SRR SRL SPA SPR SPL Electrical Model of the Skin The skin is electrically complex, and no one claims to have perfect knowledge of the physiology of EDA. But the following electrical model of the skin brings out the essential features of practical importance in biofeedback. The skin on the palm or volar surface of the hand contains up to 2000 sweat glands per square centimeter. Each sweat gland, when activated, can be considered a separate electrical pathway from the surface of the skin, which normally has high resistance, to deeper and more conductive layers of the skin. This is shown in Figure 3.15, based on Venables and Christie (1980). Each resistor represents the conductive pathway of a sweat gland. For illustrative purposes, a sweat gland is considered “on” or “off.” When it is “on,” it forms a low-resistance path from the skin surface to deeper layers. When it is “off,” it makes a very high-resistance pathway. In Figure 3.15, some glands are shown “on” and others are shown “off.” The inner layers of skin are highly conductive, but the outer layer is highly resistive. This means that the resistors are electrically tied together at the deeper layers within the skin but are electrically isolated from each other at the surface. This presents an opportunity to monitor sweat gland activity electrically. If two electrodes are placed over skin laden with sweat glands, and a voltage is applied to the electrodes, a circuit is formed, and an electric current will flow. The size of the current will depend (according to Ohm’s law) on the resistance of the skin, which in turn depends on the number of sweat glands turned “on.” See Figure 3.16 for an illustration. As more and more sweat glands turn “on,” more and more conductive pathways switch into the circuit, and (since some current flows through each pathway) more and more total current flows. In this case, Ohm’s law determines current flow, just as it does in temperature instruments. The difference is that the skin (instead of a temperature probe) acts as a variable resistor that regulates current flow through the circuit. The meter measures current flow in the circuit, and the reading is proportional to sweat gland activity. (Review this circuit in the section on temperature biofeedback instruments by substituting “skin resistance” for “probe resistance” in the explanation of Ohm’s law.) Scales and Measurement: Resistance and Conductance At this point, I distinguish resistance from conductance and explain why conductance is the preferred measurement unit. “Resistance” and “conductance” are defined as reciprocals of each other, FIGURE 3.15. Electrical model of the skin. Based on Venables and Christie (1980). 58 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.16. Basic skin conductance current loop. and they represent the same basic electrical property of materials. As discussed earlier, the ohm is the unit of resistance. The unit of conductance is the “mho” (“ohm” spelled backward); it is defined as the reciprocal of resistance (i.e., 1 divided by resistance). Therefore, resistance is also the reciprocal of conductance (1 divided by conductance). These are two scales for measuring the same phenomenon (see Table 3.4). A newer term for micromhos (one millionth of a mho) is “microsiemens” after the German inven- TABLE 3.4. Correspondence between Conductance and Resistance Conductance Resistance Units Mho Micromho (millionth) Ohm Megohm (million) Conversion formulas Conductance = 1/resistance Mho = 1/ohm Micromho = 1/megohm Resistance = 1/conductance Ohm = 1/mho Megohm = 1/micromho Sample correspondences 1 micromho ~ 1 megohm 10 micromhos ~ 0.1 megohm 100 micromhos ~ 0.01 megohm Range of skin conductance values Approx. 0.5 micromho to 50 micromhos Approx. 0.02 megohm to 2 megohms tor and industrialist Ernst Werner von Siemens. This term appears in textbooks and is synonymous with “micromhos.” I continue to use micromhos as a synonym for microsiemens in this chapter, because the older term may be more familiar to many readers. Although resistance and conductance scales measure the same property, there is a good reason to use the conductance measurement scale. Recall that as sweat glands turn “on,” they add conductance pathways within the skin. This means that conductance increases in a linear relationship to the number of activated sweat glands. Resistance, on the other hand, decreases in a nonlinear fashion as more and more sweat glands are activated. This is shown graphically in Figure 3.17. The linear relationship between sweat gland activity and skin conductance is statistically preferable for scaling and quantification. This is why skin conductance is now the standard unit. There are times (e.g., when one is using Ohm’s law or testing electrodes) when it is more convenient to think in terms of resistance rather than conductance. Once the relationship between these two scales is understood, shifting from one scale to the other presents no problem. Speaking of scales and measurement, note that skin conductance is not a direct measure of sweat gland activity (i.e., how many are turned “on”). Rather, it is an indirect measure that, except for artifact, correlates highly with sweat gland activity. That is, conductance is an electrical concept, not a physiological concept; it is not a direct measure of how many sweat glands are in operation. Because skin conductance results only when an electrical voltage is imposed from outside, the 59 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation FIGURE 3.17. Comparison of skin conductance (left) and resistance (right) scales. measurement apparatus is inextricably tied into the skin conductance phenomena and contributes heavily to the observations. For the technically inclined, skin resistance or skin conductance biofeedback instruments are designed to be ohmmeasuring or mho-measuring meters. As such, they objectively measure whatever electrical equivalent network is presented to their inputs. They are characterized in part by the means of applying electrical excitation to the skin— either a steady-state voltage (DC) or an alternating voltage (AC)—and by their readout in either ohms or micromhos (or “microsiemens,” the newer term for micromhos). If a calibrated readout is provided, calibration is usually done by presenting a known value or values of simple electrical resistors and by verifying that the unit displays those values to within the specified accuracy of the instrument. The problem is that skin presents a far more complex electrical network than simple calibration resistors. Sweat glands are not uniformly distributed in skin tissue, so sensing sites and electrode surface areas affect readings. If DC current loops are used, electrode material may be very important, because voltage may accumulate at the skin–electrode interfaces, which then act like tiny batteries and influence the readings. This is called “electrode polarization” and is discussed later. The use of silver/silver chloride electrodes will minimize but not eliminate this artifact. If AC current loops are used, polarization effects are minimized, but “reactive” components of the electrical equivalent network of the skin will cause an apparent increase in skin conductance. (These and other artifacts are discussed in a later section.) Finally, the electrical resistance of skin tissue may vary with the magnitude of the current in the current loop. In summary, biofeedback users should not assume that each other’s or published quantified SCA readings are actually comparable. Specifications of the conditions outlined earlier (plus the technical knowledge required to interpret the effects of these conditions) are necessary in order to compare SCA readings from different contexts. Parameters of SCA The hypothetical 20-second SCA record in Figure 3.18 yields three primary and two secondary parameters. Similar descriptions of measurement and typical waveforms appear in Stern, Ray, and Quigley (2001). Primary Parameters SCL or Tonic Level SCL expressed in micromhos represents a baseline or resting level. Although this level may change, in a resting, quiescent person it is likely to hover around a value identified as the tonic level. SCL or tonic level is thought to be an index of baseline level of sweat gland activity, an inferred indication of a relative level of sympathetic arousal. For example, conductance values above 5–10 micromhos are thought to be relatively high, whereas those below 1 micromho are thought to be low. Remember that these estimates depend on a number of other variables and should be taken only as a rule of thumb based on the use of 3/8-inch dry electrodes on the volar surface of fingertips. SCR or Phasic Changes Phasic changes are noticeable episodes of increased conductance caused by sympathetic arousal gen- 60 erated by a stimulus. For example, in the case of the stimulus introduced after 5 seconds, there is a 1- or 2-second delay, then an increase in conductance that peaks, levels out, and falls back to the baseline or tonic level. This is a phasic change, and its magnitude (height) is expressed as the number of micromhos reached above baseline. The size of phasic changes is thought to be an indication of the degree of arousal caused by stimuli (e.g., a startle or orientation to novel internal or external stimulus). SCR Half‑Recovery Time “SCR half-recovery time” is defined as the time elapsed from the peak of the phasic change to one-half of the way back down to baseline. SCR half-recovery time is thought to be an index of a person’s ability to calm down after a transitory excitation. It has been hypothesized that persons with chronic overarousal may have difficulty returning to relaxed baselines after even minor stimulation. Secondary Parameters “SCR latency” is defined as the time from stimulus onset until the beginning of an SCR. “SCR rise time” is defined as the time elapsed from the beginning of an SCR to its peak. These parameters have carried little significance in biofeedback; therefore, they are not discussed in detail here. Normative Values for the Parameters The hypothetical SCA record in Figure 3.18 shows specific values for the parameters. These values are II. INSTRUMENTATION actual mean values taken from normative samples of SCA records for tropical nonpatients, summarized in Venables and Christie (1980). However, these are not necessarily representative of values obtainable in ordinary biofeedback practice. Since large individual differences in SCL and SCR are common, readings far different from those cited in Figure 3.18 should come as no surprise. Furthermore, potential sources of normative variation include differences between patient and nonpatient groups, the effects of medications on SCL and SCR, differing procedures for establishing baselines and especially SCRs, and the great differences in instruments and electrodes likely to be used. For a discussion of the effects of such variables as temperature, humidity, time of day, or season, see Venables and Christie (1973, 1980). My advice to the reader is this: To increase your confidence in norms, find or build normative samples specific to the instruments you are using and to the populations with which you are working. At this time, there is no solid substitute for systematically accumulated experience with your own patient group, purposes, and equipment. This is not meant to be discouraging to the clinician or disparaging to the field; it is only a reflection of the present state of the art. Scales and Measurement: The “Percentage Increase” Scale for SCR Amplitude Displaying SCR amplitude as an increase in the number of micromhos is not the only alternative. SCR amplitude can also be expressed as a percentage change from the tonic level. For example, an SCR consisting of a 1-micromho change from 3 to 4 FIGURE 3.18. Parameters of skin conductance. SCA values shown are taken from Venables and Christie (1980). 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation micromhos is expressed as a 33% change. This has the effect of relativizing the SCR to the baseline from which it occurs. With this method, a change from 6 to 8 micromhos is also a 33% change, and so is a change from 1.5 to 2.0 micromhos. The rationale for this scale is the assumption that a given increase in autonomic arousal leads to a given percentage increase in conductance over the baseline level, and that this holds for all baseline levels. The following hypothetical examples and the electrical model of the skin illustrate this. Imagine that 200 sweat glands are turned on, giving an SCL of 2 micromhos. Now a stimulus comes along that turns on an additional 100 sweat glands, thus leading to a 1-micromho or a 50% increase. Now imagine another case in which there are 600 sweat glands turned on for an SCL of 6 micromhos. According to the percentage model, a stimulus with the same arousing properties as in the first case will lead again to a 50% increase in conductance by turning on an additional 300 sweat glands, for a 3-micromho increase in conductance. The assumption here is that changes in arousal are better gauged as percentage increases in conductance over existing baselines than as absolute increases in conductance with no regard to initial baselines. This is analogous in the economic domain to expressing a year’s growth in the gross domestic product as a percentage increase over the previous year’s level, rather than as an increase in the number of dollars. Loudness perception provides a second analogy: Achieving a given increase in perceived loudness takes a larger absolute increase in loudness above a noisy background level than above a quiet background level. If an SCR is some sort of “orienting response,” it is plausible that to be psychophysiologically “noticeable,” a stimulus must lead to a significant increase in conductance relative to existing baseline arousal—parallel to what occurs in loudness perception. Pitch perception supplies a third analogy. The difference in pitch between the note C and the note A above it sounds the same in any octave. (It is the musical interval of a sixth.) The difference between middle C (256 Hz) and the A above it (440 Hz) is 184 Hz, a 72% increase in frequency. The difference between the next C (512 Hz) and the next A (880 Hz) is 368 Hz, but it is also a 72% increase in frequency. In this case, the percentage increase in frequency, rather than the number of vibrations per second, leads to the perception of equal increases in pitch. 61 The absolute-micromho increase scale for SCR amplitude rests on an assumption opposite to that of the percentage increase scale, namely, that a micromho increase in conductance indicates a given increment in arousal, no matter where it is observed on the continuum of possible initial baselines—a fixed increment of arousal, regardless of initial baseline. This assumption is also plausible. There are, to my knowledge, no published data or definitive conceptual arguments to support or disconfirm either of the assumptions presented earlier. Each of these scales has plausibility and appeal, and it is apparently yet to be discovered whether either has distinct practical advantages or greater psychophysiological appropriateness. However, I prefer the assumptions supporting the use of the percentage increase scale for SCR amplitude. This is because the method of relating the magnitude of changes to initial baselines is appropriate and useful in perceptual contexts that to me are analogous to SCR. In addition, my informal observations suggest that persons with low SCL baselines often show fewer micromhos of SCR than persons with average SCL baselines. For me, intrinsic plausibility and these informal observations tip the balance toward the percentage increase scale for SCR amplitude. However, at very high SCLs, the percentage increase scale probably loses appropriateness, because most of the available sweat glands are already turned on to make the high SCLs. Convenient scaling follows from the percentage increase scale assumption. If the skin conductance continuum is plotted along a line, a logarithmic scale conveniently contains all possible SCL values while retaining a useful degree of resolution for SCRs all along the line. This scale is illustrated in Figure 3.19. It has the advantage of providing adequate resolution at the low end while avoiding excessive resolution at the high end. Recall that the percentage increase scale supposes that the difference between 1 and 2 micromhos is more significant than the difference between 10 and 11 micromhos, and is equivalent to the difference between 10 and 20 micromhos. On the logarithmic scale, equal distances along the line represent equal percentage changes. That is, the distance from 1 to 2 is the same as that from 10 to 20; both are 100% changes. This means that an SCR amplitude of any given percentage is represented by the same distance along the line, regardless of initial baseline. 62 II. INSTRUMENTATION Skin Conductance Record Interpretation The three primary parameters discussed earlier help professionals describe actual skin conductance records and extract data from them. But because records usually contain compounded changes in both responses and levels, interpretation is often required to specify values for the parameters. Below are paradigmatic descriptions of complex skin conductance records and interpretive hypotheses. Upward Tonic Level Shift The sample record in Figure 3.20 reveals a phasic change away from the beginning tonic level and incomplete return to that level. Think of this as an SCR that did not recover and led to a new and higher tonic level from which subsequent phasic changes depart. A hypothesis is that whatever arousal led to the phasic change did not completely “wear off,” thereby leaving the person with a new and elevated tonic level. Increase in conductance may be slow like “drift,” rather than rapid like a typical SCR. Downward Tonic Level Shift The arousal leading to the new or elevated tonic level discussed above may in time “wear off” or be “relaxed away,” leading to a downward trend in skin conductance. As shown in Figure 3.21, this record has downward slope to it, although SCRs may be superimposed. In this way, a new lower tonic level may eventually be reached. Stairstepping With multiple excitatory stimuli, especially for persons who show high-magnitude phasic changes and slow recovery time, a phenomenon called “stairstepping” may occur. As shown in Figure 3.22, this results when an excitatory stimulus occurs before the phasic changes from previous stimuli have had time to return to the prior tonic level. The SCA may then “stairstep” higher and higher. This stairstepping process could theoretically be implicated in development of overarousal. Figure 3.23 illustrates how individuals who show lower magnitude phasic changes and more rapid return to baseline are less susceptible to stairstepping from repeated stimulation. Nonresponsive Pattern A “nonresponsive pattern” is an unusually flat conductance level (see Figure 3.24), which does not respond to typically arousing stimuli even when there is a reason to believe that arousal or emotion is or should be present. A hypothesis for this pattern, when extreme, is inappropriate detachment, overcontrol, or helplessness rather than relaxation (Toomim & Toomim, 1975). FIGURE 3.19. Logarithmic scale for SCA values. FIGURE 3.20. Upward tonic level shift. 63 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation FIGURE 3.21. Downward tonic level shift. FIGURE 3.22. Stairstepping. FIGURE 3.23. Rapid return to baseline, reducing stairstepping. Optimal Skin Conductance Patterns Skin conductance is linked to arousal, but optimal SCA patterns are not necessarily the lowest or flattest patterns. This is because persistent minimal arousal, overcontrol, inattention, or flattened affect is not usually considered healthy or adaptive. There is a time for minimizing arousal during deep relaxation, in which a steady, low level of skin conductance may be desired, but uniformly invariant or flat levels are not necessarily desirable. For example, encountering a novel stimulus calls for recognizing and treating it appropriately. Habitual blunting of the arousal associated with orientation or action is not thought to be healthy or adaptive. However, after a person orients to the novel stimulus and takes appropriate action, arousal should drop to baseline levels, avoiding unnecessary arousal or wasted energy. It is possible for a person to react too vigorously to novel stimuli, so that the reaction is out of proportion. In this case, the person is treating stimuli as more alarming, dangerous, or exciting than warranted, and is paying a price in energy and physical tension. SCA is not something to be minimized but something to be optimized, and this requires judgment about what is appropriate for a given person in a given circumstance. At this time, no one 64 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 3.24. Nonresponsive pattern. claims to know optimum tonic levels and SCRs, or to be able to show that there is any such thing as specifiable optimums. What is clear is that it is possible to have overreaction and underreaction, and that this holds for both the tonic levels and phasic changes. Quick return to baseline after an SCR may be consistently desirable except when it is part of an underresponsive pattern. Because of large individual differences in SCA patterns and the lack of normative data under various standard paradigms of stimulation and measurement, it is difficult to specify clear and widely accepted procedures for relaxation training with SCA. Useful SCA biofeedback requires experience and judgment on the part of the clinician. The best way to acquire the “feel” of how SCA works under various conditions is to observe it within and between individuals, especially oneself. Those who work regularly with SCA are often quick to point out its ambiguities and uncertainties, but, undiscouraged, are also eager to discuss its unique responsiveness to transitory emotional states and thoughts. Its apparent complexity and ambiguity may conceal a wealth of valuable psychological as well as physiological information to those who have the patience to learn and further describe its patterns. Operation of the Skin Conductance Instrument Most Basic Constant DC Voltage Scheme Figure 3.25 shows the most basic SCA monitoring scheme. A constant voltage is impressed across the two electrodes. The variable resistance of the skin leads to a variable current through the circuit. A current amplifier monitors this current, and, through proper scaling, drives a display that reads out in micromhos or microsiemens. In this most basic form, it is similar to temperature instruments, as shown in Figure 3.14. However, to be practical, it must be refined. SCL baselines are spread over a wide range, yet it is important to distinguish small SCRs (e.g., a 5% change from any SCL) from all possible baselines. As an illustration, if the entire range of possible SCA values were made to fit on a meter face, SCL values would show, but an SCR would barely deflect the needle. This is the familiar issue of “resolution,” discussed earlier in connection with temperature biofeedback instruments. A digital FIGURE 3.25. Most basic SCA monitoring scheme. 65 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation meter overcomes resolution problems simply by having enough digits (e.g., 10ths or even 100ths). However, a digital meter is not suitable for observing SCRs, because changing digits during an SCR are hard to read. In contrast, the swing of a meter needle or light bar up and then back down or computer display is much more meaningful for SCRs. Such challenges are addressed in the designs of EDA instruments. A description of how this was typically addressed in early electrodermal biofeedback equipment appears in earlier editions of this volume. Simple SCR Devices Simple SCR devices use a manual, noncalibrated baseline adjustment and feed back SCR with an audio tone or noncalibrated meter scale. These devices quantify neither SCL nor SCR and are more susceptible to artifact than full-sized instruments. Even so, they are very convenient and provide very interesting and useful information to a person about patterns of SCR. For example, the rise and fall of an audio tone communicates a great deal about the person’s responsivity in actual situations, even when quantified SCL or SCR is absent. These devices have distinct advantages when it comes to ambulatory use in real life. Pocket-size miniaturization, dry finger electrodes, and an earplug for private feedback permit a person to wear the unit conveniently while walking, talking, driving, phoning, writing, thinking, reading, or carrying out other real activities. This provides insight into patterns of responsivity in active situations that are not obtainable in the clinic setting. It is a very good way for a person (including the therapist!) to discover his or her own patterns of responsivity. In any application, the therapist involved must provide adequate instruction in the use and limitations of the device and in the interpretation of results. Artifact The following points about artifact are important for all SCA devices. Therefore, electrode size must be standardized in order to assure comparability of quantified SCL readings. Movement Because electrode size affects SCA, anything that alters the effective contact area of an electrode also alters SCA. Finger or hand movement causes variations in contact pressure. The electrode may lift slightly and diminish the contact area, or press harder against the skin and increase the contact area. These effects are more pronounced for dry electrodes than for precious metal electrodes with electrode gel. The practitioner should encourage the monitored person to minimize hand movements and arrange the electrodes and cables for a reasonably stable position. When hand movement cannot be avoided (e.g., monitoring while the person is doing something with both hands), corresponding sites on the toes could be used. This would require exploring new norms for SCL and SCR on those sites. Fortunately, movement artifact is usually easy to spot, because the resultant patterns are often abrupt and uncharacteristic of true SCA patterns, and because movement can often be observed. Skin Condition Skin condition can affect conductance readings. For example, if a person has a skin abrasion or a fresh cut through the high-resistance skin surface, a high-conductance path may be established from the electrode to deeper layers of the skin and lead to an increased SCL. If a person has developed a callus, the high-resistance surface layer increases in thickness and dryness, leading to a much lower SCL and diminished SCR amplitude. Venables and Christie (1980) note that SCL falls markedly after a washing with soap and water, as residual salt is removed. Because salt builds up over time since the last wash, they recommend that persons begin sessions with freshly washed hands. It is not clear how important this is to clinical biofeedback, but it is clear that this standardizing procedure is not universally followed. Electrode Size Different-size electrodes lead to different readings. A larger electrode covers more skin and therefore places more sweat glands in the current loop. This leads to a higher SCL than does a smaller electrode that places fewer sweat glands in the loop. Room Temperature There is some evidence (Venables & Christie, 1980) that SCA is affected when individuals feel cold, and that warmer-than-usual office conditions appear to produce what they call more “normal” 66 II. INSTRUMENTATION responsivity. It is also plausible that the temperature-regulating function of sweating in an overly warm room leads to increases in SCL that are not psychophysiologically significant. A similar prolongation may occur in very humid climates even when dry electrodes are used. Artifactual prolongation of SCR recovery could lead to results mistakenly interpreted as “stairstepping.” Electrode Polarization Potentials and Electrode Design Use of AC to Control Electrode Polarization Artifact The exosomatic method involves the passage of current through the skin via surface electrodes. Polarization potentials develop at the skin–electrode interface as DC passes, and the polarization effect builds up over time. The size of polarization potential is variable and unknown. EDA units have historically varied widely in their susceptibility to the effects of electrode polarization. But in general, this is probably not a major problem, especially with DC instruments that apply very small electrical currents to the skin. Nevertheless, biofeedback clinicians who are interested in EDA and the devices that have been employed to assess it over the years should probably be aware of the issues concerning electrode polarization and methods that have been used to minimize it. A somewhat technical discussion of this follows. Dry electrodes are often used for EDA. They are made from various materials, including lead, zinc, chrome, stainless steel, gold, or silver-coated fuzz, and are often secured by Velcro straps that conveniently adjust to different finger sizes. They are simpler, cheaper, and more convenient than silver/silver chloride electrodes, especially in clinical practice. However, when used with DC EDA equipment, the simple dry electrodes suffer from polarization potentials to various degrees. When polarized, the skin–electrode interface is like a tiny battery charged by the passing current. Polarization voltage is thereby added to (or subtracted from) the constant voltage applied by the instrument. Because the polarization potential (voltage) is variable, the voltage in the current loop is no longer constant. Therefore, what appear to be changes in SCL may be due in part to variable electrode polarization potentials. Drift in skin conductance level due to the buildup of polarization potential causes artifact, but this effect may not be all that significant, for practical purposes. Nevertheless, silver/silver chloride electrodes have sometimes been used, because they develop minimal polarization potentials and therefore add minimal polarization artifact. But they are more expensive and less convenient than dry electrodes, and the gel used with silver/silver chloride electrodes may prolong the recovery phase of SCRs. Instrument designs have been evolved to circumvent the effects of polarization potentials. Use of an AC rather than a DC current loop is described in previous editions of this volume but is less relevant to biofeedback practitioners now than in previous times. Safety Electrical safety precautions for SCA devices are the same as those for EMG devices. Both are electrically connected to the person via electrodes; therefore, the same stringent standards for design, manufacture, installation, and maintenance should be followed for SCA and EMG devices, and for the entire installation of which any of these instruments are a part. The passage of DC from an electrode to the skin over a prolonged time may lead to the formation of chemical by-products on the skin if the voltage drop across the skin exceeds about 3 DC volts, such as might be encountered in “toy” or very early EDA gizmos (Leeming, Ray, & Howland, 1970). This effect is normally negligible, but if the current passed is high enough and is passed long enough, then skin irritation could develop. This effect is unlikely to occur in modern skin conductance instruments, but very old units, those that were made as novelties or toys, or those that have developed leakage currents, may be more likely to create this effect. As a rule of thumb, a device that passes current of 10 microamperes or less per square centimeter of electrode area in its current loop, and applies under 3 DC volts to the skin, will not lead to the accumulation of irritating chemicals on the skin. Acknowledgments My thanks to the late Wallace A. Peek, the late Roland E. Mohr, and John B. Picchiottino, who have acted so generously as my engineering mentors. I give special thanks to John B. Picchiottino, whose suggestions for this chapter in its first edition marked a long and muchappreciated history of helpfulness with biofeedback projects. Special thanks must also go to Mark S. Schwartz, without whose enthusiasm the first edition’s chapter and 3. A Primer of Biofeedback Instrumentation subsequent editions would doubtless have remained on my list of things to do someday. Thanks must also go to the late Peter G. Ossorio, founder of Descriptive Psychology, for his mentorship on how to clarify language and definitions in emerging fields and how to use analogies in explaining technical subject matter. References Bolek, F. (2013). Digital sampling, bits, and psychophysiological data: A primer, with cautions. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(4), 303–308. Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal activity (2nd ed.). New York: Springer Science and Business Media. Brown, B. (1974). New mind, new body: New directions for the mind. New York: Harper & Row. Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.). (2000). Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, L. (2000). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. Jennings, J. R., Tahmoush, A. J., & Redmond, D. D. (1980). Non-invasive measurement of peripheral vascular activity. In I. Martin & P. H. Venables (Eds.), Techniques in psychophysiology (pp. 70–131). New York: Wiley. Leeming, M. N., Ray, C., & Howland, W. S. (1970). Low- 67 voltage, direct-current burns. Journal of the American Medical Association, 214(9), 1681–1684. Mathieu, P. A., & Sullivan, S. J. (1990). Frequency characteristics of signals and instrumentation: Implication for EMG biofeedback studies. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 15(4), 335–352. Neumann, E., & Blanton, R. (1970). The early history of electrodermal research. Psychophysiology, 8(4), 463– 474. Otis, J., Rasey, H., Vrochopoulos, S., Wincze, J., & Andrasik, F. (1995). Temperature acquisition as a function of the computer-based biofeedback system utilized: An exploratory analysis. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20(2), 185–190. Stern, R. M., Ray, W. J., & Quigley, K. S. (2001). Psychophysiological recording (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Toomim, M., & Toomim, H. (1975, February). Psychological dynamic correlates of the paradoxically invariant GSR. Paper presented at the fifth annual convention of the Biofeedback Research Society, Monterey, CA. Venables, P. H., & Christie, M. J. (1973). Mechanisms, instrumentation, recording techniques, and quantification of responses. In W. F. Prokasy & D. C. Raskin (Eds.), Electrodermal activity in psychological research (pp. 111–116). New York: Academic Press. Venables, P. H., & Christie, M. J. (1980). Electrodermal activity. In I. Martin & P. H. Venables (Eds.), Techniques in psychophysiology (pp. 3–67). New York: Wiley. Chapter 4 Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography Instrumentation and Applications Jeffrey E. Bolek, Ronald L. Rosenthal, and Richard A. Sherman Surface electromyography (SEMG), as a science, has an uncanny ability to lure the unsuspecting into believing that the modality is much simpler than it really is. Unfortunately this leads to three outcomes: First, those new to the field suddenly find that what appeared to be a healthy application of SEMG with their patient in reality was not; second, often on the heels of the point just listed is the clinician’s understandable abandonment of the field of SEMG for other, more efficient methods to use with patients; and third, for those who stick with it, there is the tendency to rely on canned “protocols” or “menu driven” applications. These applications often have catchy titles such as “pelvic floor dysfunction protocol.” Unfortunately they are typically a one-size-fits-all arrangement, and the clinician places great trust that the creator of the program had enough clinical background to consider the myriad permutations involved in using SEMG with the patient group. This chapter is designed to shed some light on all of these points. In the first section, Jeffrey E. Bolek reviews some supposedly “basic” SEMG principles that are often misunderstood because they are typically given cryptic definitions, if they are defined at all. He also suggests a novel way of viewing SEMG data from multiple sites, a form of “quantified EMG,” but for surface, not needle electromyography. In the second section, Ronald L. Rosenthal then dives into the intricacies of neuromuscular retraining by providing a road map for hands-on applications in stroke rehabilitation, with special emphasis on muscle synergies, pattern training, and timing. Finally, in the third section, Richard A. Sherman discusses techniques for optimizing the results of rehabilitation biofeedback. Interpretation of the SEMG Signal When one studies the rich and convoluted interaction between the many factors that influence the information content of the EMG signal, it is reasonable to ask if there is any hope of using the EMG signal in a constructive fashion. . . . The answer is a confident “yes” for some . . . and a guarded “maybe” for other applications. —De Luca (1997, p. 141) In 1793, Volta proved that dissimilar metals in contact with an electrolyte (like those present in body tissue) could generate an electric current. In 1849, DuBois-Reymond was the first to report the detection of voluntarily elicited electrical signals from human muscles (Basmajian & DeLuca, 1985). The terms “electrical” and “current” are included in these discoveries. All too often their meaning in electromyography is obscured by theoretical definitions at best, or mere mention in a glossary at worst. This is unfortunate, because it tends to cover an important part of electromyography with a shroud of complexity that few have the engineering background to understand. The key to under68 69 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography standing electromyography is a working knowledge of the fundamentals of electricity, because it is the foundation upon which electromyography is built. My (J. E. B.) goal in this section is to present the origin, travels, and end processing of the SEMG signal in a technically comprehensible language, followed by further elucidation on the SEMG signal as an indicator of functional performance. There are two kinds of current, direct (DC) and alternating (AC). In DC, the electrons travel down a wire in one direction, and the direction remains constant over time. In AC, the current changes polarity constantly from positive to negative at the very high rate of 60 hertz (Hz) or 60 cycles per second. AC current produces a sine wave as the current quickly changes from positive to negative (Figure 4.1). Likening this to water flowing in a pipe, the “current” is the rate of flow of the water, and the “voltage” is the “push,” the force that makes it move. Current is the number of electrons flowing past a given point per second and is measured in amperes. Every circuit (whether wire or motor neuron) has some resistance, which is measured in ohms. “Watts” is the measure of power (like a 100 w light bulb). Voltage (or the “push”) can be measured by multiplying the amperage times the resistance. Amperage (or flow of electrons) can be measured by dividing volts by resistance and watts (power) can be found by multiplying voltage times current. SEMG is the measurement in microvolts of an action potential propagating down a motor neuron. A “motor neuron” is a nerve cell that has a motor function. The amplitude dimension of SEMG is measured in microvolts (mV); the frequency (because it is a sine wave) in hertz (Hz), or cycles per second. If SEMG were a string on an instrument, the amount of space the string travels when plucked is “voltage”; the speed that it travels while moving in that space is “frequency.” If one considers a vocalist singing a song, SEMG amplitude is to volume as change in pitch is to SEMG frequency. The vocalist can sing loudly or softly (amplitude) while singing in a given pitch (frequency). Similarly, a muscle may fire using more or less slow (20–90 Hz) or fast (90–500 Hz) twitch fibers. Three events may account for the increase in an SEMG reading: Additional motor units may “kick in,” the motor units currently in play may fire more quickly, or both. The frequency component allows one to see another dimension of the SEMG signal, that is, what is behind the increase– decrease in amplitude. A group of muscle fibers is supplied by one motor neuron, with some neurons supplying only a few muscle fibers (e.g., laryngeal) and other serving many (e.g., gastrocnemius) (Yolanda et al., 2007). This nerve cell plus its long axon running down the motor nerve, along with the terminal branches, constitute the “motor unit.” The muscle fibers it innervates can range Voltage Period (T) AMPLITUDE Time Wavelength FIGURE 4.1. A pictorial representation of a sine wave. 70 II. INSTRUMENTATION from 3 to 2000 (Buchthal & Schmalbruch, 1980). SEMG measures the electrical activity in microvolts (millionths of a volt or mV, not to be confused with millivolts, mV, thousandths of a volt) of all activated motor units or a whole muscle located in the vicinity of the skin via surface electrodes. The duration of electrical potentials of motor units ranges from a few to 14 milliseconds (ms); amplitude can range from 0.01 to 5 microvolts (Soderberg, 1992). An action potential(s) is given off by the motor unit(s) activated during a muscle contraction. The usual resting electrical charge that is negative on the axon interior and positive on the exterior is reversed as the wave of action potential passes at speeds of one-fourth of a .22 caliber rifle shot or 320 feet/second (Cottman & McGaugh, 1980). These action potentials last about 1 ms and quickly repolarize, causing movement along the axon. An impulse descending the motor neuron causes all the innervated muscle fibers to contract. A muscle consists of hundreds of muscle fibers, depending on size, grouped together in motor units in which each motor unit is innervated by a single motor nerve. When a signal is conducted via the nerve fiber servicing a motor unit, all the muscle fibers of that unit contract simultaneously. Selective activation of these different motor units is used as a means to control muscle contraction, with the electrical activity of active motor units being detected by the electrodes. All this motor unit activity is called motor unit action potential (MUAP). Raw SEMG produces a sine wave (Figure 4.2) as the signal flows from + to – with fluctuating peaks and troughs. Notice how irregular the signal is in Figure 4.2 compared to that in Figure 4.1. This is because that in Figure 4.1 is one frequency, 60 Hz, and the raw SEMG is made up of many frequencies. One cannot just take the average of the values, because that would represent a snapshot at a point in time, not across time. DC produces no wave; it is a constant value and simple to measure. Myoelectric signals (e.g., SEMG) are more challenging to interpret because they are bipolar and produce a sine wave. The only way to measure the value of AC (and SEMG) is by some calculation of the sine wave, the root mean square (RMS), which is the foundation on which SEMG is transformed from raw data to an intelligible display but is rarely explained in detail. It is a method of taking a signal that flows above and below the zero gradient line on the monitor (i.e., is bipolar) and making it monopolar (never dips below zero). One has a constantly moving signal over time, so the positive and negative values do not occur at the same time. Because of the fluctuating nature of the sine wave, one has to devise a method that enables a direct comparison with a nonoscillating energy source (e.g., comparing AC to DC). We want to know how big a nonoscillating energy source (e.g., DC) needs to be to deliver the same energy as the sine wave (as in an AC source, like the current in SEMG) in a set amount of time. The RMS produces a value that is an approximation of the energy about halfway through the peak and valley. The SEMG waveform resembles AC in that it swings from negative to positive, back and forth. The RMS voltage is a measure of the magnitude of a set of numbers; it gives one a sense for the typical size of the numbers, and displays this information in an “amplitude envelope.” For example, 1000 data points may be sampled consecutively from the raw data. RMS is performed by squaring the data (which removes the negative signs), summing the squares, determining the mean (central tendency), and taking the square root (which reverses the effect of squaring). If one were to measure the “average” voltage of the 120-volt AC coming into the house, it would be zero. Why, then, does one get a shock from 120-volt AC? The average voltage is zero, but the average power is not zero, it is 120 FIGURE 4.2. Raw SEMG sine wave. 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography volts RMS, and this is what the power meter is displaying. In the same way, one cannot just take the “average” raw SEMG reading. Why is the myoelectric signal measured in microvolts? Earlier I stated that power is measured in watts and voltage is merely the “push” that moves electrons past a given point in time, measured in amperage. Would not power (in watts) or current (in amps) be a better indicator? All electrical energy has two main characteristics: voltage and current. Recall that in SEMG, three electrodes are used (see Peek, Chapter 3, this volume). The two active electrodes pick up the voltage difference and send it to the encoder. Recall that to find current (in amps) we need to know the resistance in the body, which is very difficult to do, so SEMG uses the voltage difference as the next best choice, although it is not a very “clean” signal. This is why efforts are being made to develop new means to analyze the SEMG signal (Abdallah & Zahran, 2009). An important (and confusing) dimension of the SEMG signal is that SEMG can be displayed as two domains: The amplitude and frequency domains and frequency can be displayed as a power spectral density (PSD) or median frequency in time (MFT). The amplitude domain just discussed is a time-based measure of signal amplitude (or effort) with time on the x-axis and amount of effort (in mV) on the y-axis. This is the familiar real-time display observed as a moving line on the monitor. The PSD frequency domain is displayed as frequency (think “pitch” in hertz) on the x-axis and power (think “loudness”) on the y-axis (this is PSD). Since the PSD does not include the time dimension, one must specify the time interval (called an “epoch,” usually .5 to .2 seconds) used in the analysis (called an FFT, or fast Fourier transformation analysis). The MFT displays time on the x-axis and Hz on the y-axis but not power. Neither display is “better”; it depends on the kind of data desired. Therefore, the SEMG signal can be mined for different types of information depending on the needs of the clinician. For example, as fatigue accumulates, the frequency tends to shift downward as a result of a reduced conduction velocity of the action potential, while the amplitude remains the same or increases. The sequence of events to the microvolts displayed on the monitor is as follows: A group of muscle fibers is innervated by the axon of the motor unit, creating action potentials that flow through (and can be attenuated by) skin and adipose tissue before reaching the electrode. From 71 there the signal is sampled, filtered, quantified/rectified (typically by RMS) before finally producing a digital signal in millivolts. “Sampling” is a means of managing the digital data obtained from the raw SEMG. Unlike analog signals (e.g., varying light intensity), the digital signal processing (DSP) age born in the 1960s ushered in the ability to produce bits of data that can be stored and manipulated in a computer. Unlike our base 10 number system (from 10 fingers), computers use multiples of eight (8 bits = 1 byte), so samples are typically eight items or multiples thereof (e.g., 16, 32, 64 . . . 2048). Phonograph records are an example of analog data, in that a continuous stream of music is recorded. Compact discs (or iPods) play music that has been sampled off the continuous music stream at 44,100 times per second, producing “digital data.” Research has shown that the sampling rate must be at least twice the maximum frequency to be expected (for human hearing, 20,000 Hz). Five hundred hertz is often considered the limit of the usable SEMG range, which results in a sampling rate of 1000 Hz, which is the upper limit on many SEMG units. The range of frequencies capable of being processed by the typical SEMG unit is 20 to 1000 Hz, with the majority of EMG activity being below 200 Hz and very little found above 500 Hz. About 80% of the signal resides between 30 and 80 Hz (Cram, 1991). Twenty hertz and below is where movement artifact resides (De Luca, 1997). If a high-pass filter is set for 20 Hz (allowing only designated high frequencies to pass), movement artifact will be eliminated, but so will some of the spectrum of the internal oblique, which lives in the 8- to 150-Hz range. Error is inevitable by design in digital sampling, because the instantaneous snapshot of the analog data has to be rounded off to the nearest available digital value to make the sample. There is a limited number of binary digits (or bits) that can be used to sample a signal. The “bits” indicate the number of decimal points any given sample can possess; the greater the number of bits, the more detail the sample contains. The “sampling rate” defines how many samples are taken over X amount of time. Filters are used to eliminate energy radiating from the atmosphere (e.g., power lines) or energy from the body itself (e.g., heart rate) from contaminating the signal. But this energy (in the 50- to 60-Hz range) also contains desirable parts of the EMG signal (Winter, 1990). Furthermore, every frequency has a fundamental frequency and a “harmonic,” which is multiples of that frequency (for 60, 120, 180, 240 Hz, etc.), so the filter will not 72 II. INSTRUMENTATION filter these (Kasman, 1995). In addition, tissues separating the muscle fibers from the electrodes act as a low-pass (allowing low and blocking some high hertz) filter, depending on tissue thickness (De Luca, 1979), while the differential electrode acts as a high-pass filter allowing high hertz to pass and blocking low hertz (Barry, 1991). From this discussion, we see that the value displayed on the monitor (e.g., 35.6 mV) represents an approximation of the recruitment level of the targeted muscle. The signal has been filtered by the electrode, skin, and adipose tissue; sampled to enable analog-to-digital conversion; and filtered to cut out undesirable frequencies (along with desired ones), with parameters that exist within a bandwidth and are transposed from a raw to RMS signal! After all the filtering, sampling, bandwidth limitation, and approximation to create the RMS, the 35.6-mV value on the screen no longer looks so exact. The greater the number of decimal points in a number, the greater the tendency for humans to place unearned confidence in it. RMS is at best a rough but useful indicator of what is going on at the level of the muscle fibers. The frequency domains have problems, too, in that at low contraction forces, the signal-to-noise ratio is very low (i.e., more noise in the signal) (Baratta, Solomonow, Zhou, & Zhu, 1998). Given the convoluted nature of the SEMG signal, Bolek (2006, 2012) created a means whereby the relationship between the observed SEMG activity and function could be quantified, termed quantitative surface electromyography (QSEMG). In QSEMG, the practitioner must begin with an understanding of how the muscles involved in a movement act synergistically, because training on a single muscle site is not sufficient to make meaningful changes in function. Training goals are set for up to 10 muscles (five bilaterally), and the combination of recruitment/relaxation is rewarded, for example, by turning on–off a video. The key point is that the patient does not seek to control a single group of agonist–antagonist muscles but to produce a patterned response that produces a reward. This is similar to the z-score used in neurofeedback in which multiple variables are monitored at more than one brain site. The motor control score (MCS) is a measure of performance over time based on meeting or exceeding the recruitment goal for the targeted muscle and as such is based on an approximation, the RMS. For example, for each 0.5-second sample of RMS, that value is compared to a set threshold with a two-tailed decision tree: s ≥ x or s ≤ x, where s is the signal and x is the threshold setting. Multiple sites are used, so longer decision trees (formulated in Excel) are common. The MCS is the total time demonstrating proper muscle recruitment divided by the total time at work. Success over X amount of time can be plotted as in Figure 4.3. In motor reeducation, a premium is based on the use of SEMG in facilitating functional gain. Most clinicians target at most two muscles, but attending to the myotatic unit (or constellation of muscles) involved in a targeted skill leads to faster and more enduring learning (Sella, 2000). A “myotatic unit” is a functional unit or group of agonist and antagonist muscles (four in the case 18 Seconds thresholds met 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 1 384 767 1150 1533 1916 2299 2682 3065 3448 3831 4214 4597 4980 5363 5746 6129 6512 6895 7278 7661 8044 8427 8810 9193 9576 X.H. 3/6/09 Time of sessions in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.3. A functional display of patient progress. 73 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography below) that function together as a unit or have close functional relationships (Travell & Simons, 1993). For example, targeting the bilateral sternocleidomastoid (SCM) to be above threshold and bilateral cervical paraspinals to be below creates a program that facilitates mastering head control post–motor vehicle accident (MVA), in that it is sensitive to establishing a neutral head position in all four planes (flexion, extension, right and left lateral shifts; Bolek, 2006). Cram, Kasman, and Holtz (1998) noted the cooperative function between the SCMs and C4 paraspinals during head rotation, with the SCMs active during rotation and the C4 paraspinals providing a stabilizing function. Consider Figure 4.4, which displays the RMS of wrist extensor and wrist flexor over the course of a 50-minute treatment session with threshold-contingent signaling based on wrist extensor values being ≥ 16.6 and ≤ 51.8 and flexor values in the range of ≥ 15.8 and ≤ 49.2 (RMS). The threshold directions (recruit–relax) are set by the clinician and are visual assessments of the performance of the patient. In other words, this is incremental muscle training to use the wrist extensor while allowing for wrist stabilization with the flexor bundle. Figure 4.3 presents a composite picture of the same data called a motor control score (Bolek, 2012). The 9,778 samples obtained from both sites over the course of the session were placed into a decision tree in Excel, where “go” = 0.125 second if, and only if, the extensor and flexor values listed earlier are met. In other words, for each sample of 0.125 second, if both criteria are met, a “credit” of 0.125 second is listed; if criteria are not met, it is a “no go” and the credit is zero. The numbers on the x-axis are time of the session beginning at the left; on the y-axis, the total consecutive seconds is added where both values are met. For example, early in the session, there is a period of 15 consecutive seconds of both flexor and extensor meeting the criteria for reward, with a slow but definite trailing off as the session progressed. Figure 4.3 is a visual display of the functional progress made by the patient. It is a coupling of the data from both SEMG channels over time, designed to reflect the effect of the SEMG values (the independent variable) on the functional progress (the dependent variable) of the patient, and adds information to the treatment program that would not be easily ascertained by only viewing the SEMG RMS. Here we see nearly 16 consecutive seconds with the extensors between 16.6 and 51.8 and flexors between 15.8 and 49.2. It is a typical graph of motor learning with a few minutes to “find” the correct motor plan, followed by the best performance, then by a trailing off as fatigue sets in. Consider a program in which the reward is contingent on five sites, with some of the target muscles set to be recruited and others to be relatively relaxed. This patient, with a diagnosis of cerebral palsy, tended to stand on her right leg with the left leg externally rotated and slightly flexed. The RMS of specific sites and the planned direction of recruitment are shown in Figures 4.5–4.10. We see a slight but nonsignificant increase in the left gluteus maximus, a variable left adductor, a small decrease in the left hamstring, and no change on the left medial quadriceps but more variability than on the left lateral quad, which, again shows little change on SEMG RMS. Overall the SEMG FIGURE 4.4. RMS of wrist extensor (lighter) and wrist flexor (darker). 74 II. INSTRUMENTATION 70 60 µV 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 206 411 616 821 1026 1231 1436 1641 1846 2051 2256 2461 2666 2871 3076 3281 3486 3691 3896 4101 4306 4511 4716 4921 5126 Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.5. RMS of left gluteus maximus. 80 70 60 µV 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 206 411 616 821 1026 1231 1436 1641 1846 2051 2256 2461 2666 2871 3076 3281 3486 3691 3896 4101 4306 4511 4716 4921 5126 Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.6. RMS of left abductor. 80 70 60 µV 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 206 411 616 821 1026 1231 1436 1641 1846 2051 2256 2461 2666 2871 3076 3281 3486 3691 3896 4101 4306 4511 4716 4921 5126 Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.7. RMS of left hamstring. 75 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography 60 50 µV 40 30 20 10 0 1 206 411 616 821 1026 1231 1436 1641 1846 2051 2256 2461 2666 2871 3076 3281 3486 3691 3896 4101 4306 4511 4716 4921 5126 Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.8. RMS of left medial quadriceps. 21.5 21 20.5 µV 20 19.5 19 18.5 18 17.5 17 16.5 1 209 417 625 833 1041 1249 1457 1665 1873 2081 2289 2497 2705 2913 3121 3329 3537 3745 3953 4161 4369 4577 4785 4993 5201 Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.9. RMS of left lateral quadriceps. Seconds thresholds met 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 206 411 616 821 1026 1231 1436 1641 1846 2051 2256 2461 2666 2871 3076 3281 3486 3691 3896 4101 4306 4511 4716 4921 5126 COMPOSITE MCS Time of session in 1/8 seconds FIGURE 4.10. Composite motor control score. 76 RMS results show either a slight change in the desired direction or no change. The MCS reveals what would have been seen by an observer present in the session. There is a gradual rise in the consecutive seconds the patient met the threshold criteria, beginning with negligible time at the start of the session to nearly 80 seconds toward the end. The MCS is statistically richer in information, in that it pools all the data obtained from the SEMG and operationally displays it on a graph. It can be considered a form of QEMG that is limited in application to needle EMG in that it mines the SEMG data to perform a quantitative analysis. Every treatment session involves setting the bar for expected performance (i.e., during a typical session the clinician will be giving some kind of feedback as to the performance of the patient, such as “good job” or “don’t give up, keep trying!”). By looking only at the SEMG RMS values, particularly when many sites are involved, it is often difficult for the patient to read what is on the monitor and for the clinician to determine how to interpret the SEMG readings and construct a treatment plan. QSEMG can be a useful tool in giving immediate feedback and quantifying this judgment call. Neuromuscular Retraining: General Considerations Biofeedback systems have evolved considerably over the past several years. Computerized systems priced at under $10,000 typically record at least four channels of EMG, and systems that can record eight or more channels are available. Because these systems can sample over 2000 times/second, they are capable of recording the raw signal at bandwidths from 20 to 1000 Hz. The software can provide power spectra for multiple channels in real time with median frequencies. The software for these systems can program virtual channels and provide feedback based on thresholds for multiple channels. This gives the clinician a powerful tool to help clients learn to change motor recruitment patterns. Rather than being constrained to train for increases or decreases in one muscle at a time, feedback can be determined by changes in the recruitment patterns of multiple muscles. This is important, because even simple movements often involve an organized pattern of recruitment of many muscles. Impairments in movement or posture are rarely attributable to improper activation of one muscle; rather, there is II. INSTRUMENTATION a disruption in the timing and level of activity of all of the muscles involved in the activity. An early example of this approach was in the work of Will Taylor (1990) on the scapular rhythm. Taylor realized that normal movement of the arm in abduction is dependent on rotation of the scapula as the arm is raised. This is brought about by the simultaneous activation of the upper and lower trapezius muscles. As the upper and lower trapezius muscles fire, the scapula rotates laterally and the glenoid fossa moves in an upward direction. Without this action, the humerus would bump into the acromion and the range of motion would be restricted. For many patients with shoulder problems, the lower trapezius become inhibited and full range of motion in shoulder abduction becomes painful and difficult. Biofeedback training to increase lower trapezius recruitment in abduction is often very beneficial for these patients. An effective way to accomplish this training is to construct a virtual “proportional channel,” defined as the amplitude of the lower trapezius divided by the sum of the amplitudes of the upper and lower trapezius. This channel ranges from 0 to 1 and is .5 when the two signals are equal. The setup involves recording from the upper and lower trapezius and the medial deltoids, and instructions could include trying to keep the shoulder down as the arm is raised. Initially, the threshold for the proportional channel can be set quite low, in the range of .1 to .2, and auditory feedback is provided when the medial deltoid is above its threshold, the lower trapezius is above its threshold, and the proportional channel is above its threshold. If the balance of the upper and lower trapezius shifts too much toward the upper trapezius, the proportional channel will fall below threshold and turn off the auditory reward. As training progresses, all of the thresholds can be raised until the amplitudes approach full normal and the balance of the upper and lower trapezius is nearly equal, with the proportional channel consistently at or above .45. Muscle Pattern Training Scapular training can also be conducted for problems in shoulder flexion. In this case, the target muscle is the serratus anterior, below the axilla, or armpit. The serratus anterior rotates the scapula in an anterior direction, permitting full range of shoulder flexion that would be impossible without movement of the scapula. Biofeedback train- 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography 77 ing should emphasize the joint recruitment of the anterior deltoids and the serratus anterior as the arm is moved forward and up. Another example of muscle pattern training involves the knee. The quadriceps, which extends the knee and is also important in supporting the body weight while walking, has a lateral component (the vastus lateral, or VL) and a medial component (the vastus medialis oblique, or VMO). When the two muscles generate equivalent forces, the diagonal vectors cancel out and the patella remains in a central position in the trochlea. In some cases, the patella can be pulled out of the trochlea, which is called “patellar subluxation.” Invariably, the dislocation is in the lateral direction and a crucial aspect of conservative management of patellar subluxation is strengthening of the VMO. Biofeedback has often been used as an adjunctive modality, and the protocol should include simultaneous recording of the VMO and VL so that VL recruitment does not simultaneously increase while the VMO is uptrained. Proportional feedback is an excellent option, because it provides feedback that is sensitive to the balance of the two muscles. The training should include seated knee extension, an open-chain task, as well as work in standing and weight bearing, a closedchain task. A final example of muscle pattern training with biofeedback is in hand rehabilitation. SEMG training with hand function can be difficult and demanding. In contrast with the lower extremity, the forearm and hand contain a large number of muscles, many of them small and overlapping. Functional use of the hand also requires the coor- dination of muscles that control the wrist and fingers. Biofeedback training to improve hand functioning that uses only one or two channels runs the risk of training inappropriate muscle activity due to the possibility of volume conduction and overflow from adjacent muscles. Fortunately, with judicious placements, it is possible to get reasonably well-isolated signals from the major extensors and flexors of the wrist and fingers (Figures 4.11 and 4.12). Distal placements on the dorsal and ventral aspects of the forearm, 2–4 cm from the wrist crease, can still detect substantial activity of the finger extensors and flexors. These distal placements are relatively insensitive to activation of the wrist muscles. More proximal placements near the elbow respond to the wrist extensors and flexors. These proximal placements respond to finger extension and to finger flexion as well, but if the distal sites are low, then one can be relatively confident that the signals are from the wrist muscles. Finger extension and flexion both depend upon the simultaneous recruitment of the wrist extensors and flexors to stabilize the wrist joint, permitting the fingers to open or close. In the power grip, the wrist muscles must maintain the wrist in a position of moderate extension. Without this stabilization, the finger flexors would flex the wrist and the fingers would extend by a mechanical action called “tenodesis.” Similarly, in finger extension, the wrist extensors and flexors must maintain the wrist in a neutral or slightly flexed position. Biofeedback training for the power grip should emphasize the simultaneous recruitment of the finger flexors along with the wrist extensors and flexors. There is considerable variability in the balance of the wrist extensors to the wrist FIGURE 4.11. Ventral placements to record surface EMG activity from the wrist flexors and finger flexors. FIGURE 4.12. Dorsal placements to record surface EMG activity from the wrist extensors and finger extensors. 78 flexors, but in most cases it is beneficial to increase the level of extensor recruitment. In addition to the extrinsic muscles in the forearm, it is also possible to conduct biofeedback training with some of the intrinsic muscles of the hand (Lertprapamongkol & Suksathien, 2007). There are three sets of muscles in the palm of the hand, the dorsal interossei, the palmar interossei, and the lumbricales. While SEMG electrodes cannot distinguish among these three groups, use of SEMG can be enhanced with the addition of sites on the dorsal surface of the hand, between the metacarpal bones. This placement is responsive to activation of the lumbricales, and these muscles are critical for full extension of the fingers. An effective protocol is to combine two channels of intrinsic activity (one from between the middle and ring fingers and a second one between the index finger and thumb) with the extrinsic finger flexors and extensors. Training for finger extension would reward the simultaneous recruitment of both intrinsic sites along with the extrinsic extensors. Care should be taken to observe the patient carefully because the intrinsic sites will respond to activation of the palmar interossei muscles should the patient go into the power grip. Biofeedback for Stroke Rehabilitation Biofeedback has a long history in the treatment of motor dysfunction after stroke (Basmajian, Kukulka, Narayan, & Takebe, 1975). Initial reports revealed improvement in ankle dorsiflexion with biofeedback training and in a number of studies in the 1980s, and benefits in lower extremity functioning (and to a lesser degree in upper extremity functioning) with biofeedback were demonstrated in the 1990s. However, in recent years, there have been almost no studies of EMG biofeedback with stroke patients, and it appears that many clinics specializing in neurorehabilitation do not routinely offer biofeedback to stroke patients. It is difficult to account for this trend, and many factors are most likely involved. One important factor is a lack of training in SEMG biofeedback for most physical and occupational therapists. As a result, therapists may not appreciate the potential benefits of adding SEMG training to their therapeutic protocols. In order to implement a program using SEMG retraining to enhance the motor recovery of a stroke patient, it is necessary to understand some basic principles of motor control, as well as the deficits brought about by a stroke. Strokes typi- II. INSTRUMENTATION cally cause “hemiplegia,” a unilateral paralysis of the side of the body contralateral to the location of the stroke. The deficits may be relatively mild or they can be so profound as to lead to a complete inability to use the affected limbs in any functional activities. A simplistic notion of hemiplegia would be to think of it as generalized weakness, analogous to lowering the volume on a radio, but without distorting the basic patterns of movement. Regrettably, this is far from the case. Deficits in motor control after a stroke include spasticity, hypertonicity, ataxia, and several other conditions reflecting damage to central motor control systems. The complex deficits found in motor function subsequent to a stroke reflect the hierarchical nature of motor control. Movement is organized at multiple levels within the central nervous system. There are primitive, reflexive patterns of wholelimb movement that are organized at the spinal level. In the midbrain, we see the emergence of righting reactions involved in equilibrium and balance. Finally, in the cortex, we find the development of more sophisticated control systems that are able to organize movements into complex functional activities. The higher motor systems exert a strong inhibitory influence on the lower systems. Strokes can disrupt this inhibitory activity, leading to ineffective motor recruitment patterns and the emergence of more primitive wholelimb synergies. From a biofeedback perspective, several factors need to be addressed. These include hypertonicity, hyperactive stretch reflexes leading to spasticity, alterations in temporal patterns of motor recruitment, and the presence of wholelimb synergies. Hypertonicity and Spasticity Muscle tone is regulated at multiple levels to balance the needs of stability and mobility. Increased muscle tone leads to more stability, but at the cost of less fluidity of movement. Conversely, when muscle tone is too low, mobility is enhanced, but there may be significant postural instability. In intact individuals, muscle tone is regulated, so that there is enough activity to stabilize the trunk and limbs but not so much as to interfere with fluid movements. In stroke patients, the disruption of descending motor pathways may lead to hypertonicity and spasticity. It is more common to see it in flexors, although increased tone from extensors can also occur. Common sites affected include the biceps, wrist flexors, and pectoralis major. Muscles 79 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography involved in neck rotation and flexion may also be a factor. Biofeedback training for hypertonicity and spasticity is straightforward and consists of inhibitory training of the involved muscles. However, it is not clear that most stroke patients with excess motor tone or spasticity will respond favorably to such training. Initial efforts to downtrain the hyperactive stretch reflex of the biceps using biofeedback were largely unsuccessful, despite the use of thousands of feedback trials in some cases (Wolf, personal communication, 2014). However, Wolf and Segal (1990) reported modest success in downtraining the spastic stretch reflex of the biceps in elbow extension. Thus, the findings are not consistent and there may be some combinations of patients and protocols that are effective in reducing spasticity. A common observation when working with stroke patients is that motor recruitment will increase dramatically when resistance is provided. Isometric training can be effective in increasing motor recruitment of the triceps or wrist extensors, and the absence of movement eliminates the antagonistic corecruitment caused by disinhibition of the stretch reflexes. However, if the training does not include a progression to concentric movements without resistance, then it will be unlikely to lead to any functional improvement. The question of whether biofeedback training can be effective in reducing tone and spasticity of stroke patients may become moot because of the growing use of botox injections in stroke rehabilitation. Botox can be used in a graded manner to reduce or totally block the recruitment of spastic flexors. The injections are effective for up to 6 months and must be readministered or the dysfunctional tone will recur. Biofeedback training can be combined with botox injections, and it can be advantageous to train for increased extensor recruitment while the flexors are “chemically quiet.” It will be interesting to see whether research supports the efficacy of a combined approach. Synergies Synergies are a normal part of the process of recovery from a stroke. “Synergistic movements” are stereotyped patterns of movement across multiple joints that are activated as a unit. These patterns are commonly observed as the acute stroke patient moves from a flaccid state into a more spastic state. There are both flexor synergies and extensor synergies, and they have been well described in a number of sources (e.g., Brunnstrom, 1970). Flexor synergy is usually dominant in the upper extremity, while extensor synergy is stronger in the lower extremity. Effective biofeedback programs for stoke rehabilitation must take into account the presence of synergies. For instance, a common problem during ambulation after a stroke is poor heel strike, and many stroke patients need an ankle–foot orthosis (AFO) to compensate for poor control of ankle dorsiflexion and eversion. If dorsiflexion training is conducted while seated, the stroke patient may show fairly high levels of anterior tibialis recruitment if allowed to flex and externally rotate the hip. In order to be functional, dorsiflexion and eversion must be sustained throughout the swing phase, until the foot hits the ground. An effective biofeedback program must address this need. Dorsiflexion training should progress from a seated position, which allows the flexor synergy, to standing and stepping with a proper heel strike. An effective intermediate protocol gradually trains a combination of anterior tibialis recruitment with activation of the quadriceps to extend the knee. This combination is out of synergy and is quite difficult for most stroke patients. Mastery of this task marks a major progression in the recovery from a stroke and is a good prognostic indicator for continued improvement in the gait pattern. Timing Changes In Motor Recruitment In addition to the previously mentioned changes in motor recruitment patterns, stroke also plays havoc with the exquisite timing of muscular activation and relaxation in most skilled movements. Intact individuals are able immediately to recruit muscles as needed, and motor activity falls back to baseline levels rapidly when the movement no longer requires the activation of a given muscle. Patterns of EMG activity in rapid alternating movements are often too fast for basic biofeedback displays, and it is not uncommon to ask a patient to slow down when recording baseline patterns during alternation between wrist extension and wrist flexion if a nice display is desired. In contrast with the rapid activation and inhibition of muscles in normal movement, stroke patients show a delay in muscle recruitment during movement and a lag in the return to baseline levels after the completion of a given movement. This is particularly troublesome for rhythmic activities, such as walking, which require alternating patterns of motor recruitment as the legs move from the stance phase to the swing phase and back. Stroke 80 patients often strain to accomplish difficult motor tasks, increasing tone and making it difficult to inhibit motor activity that is no longer required. Biofeedback training that focuses solely on static patterns of motor recruitment is less likely to be as effective as programs that emphasize the ability to activate and inhibit appropriate motor activity quickly. Auditory feedback during ambulatory training can be programmed to help patients learn to terminate activity more rapidly as needed. Sensory Changes and Their Role in Motor Dysfunction In intact individuals, the central nervous system constructs a spatial map of the body derived primarily from the stretch receptors in the muscles. This map enables an individual to be aware of how the various body parts relate to each other without looking at them. This sensory ability was given the label “proprioception” by noted physiologist Charles Sherrington in 1906. Sherrington distinguished proprioception from the sensory inputs such as vision and audition, which were defined as exteroceptive inputs, as well as from visceral sensations, which were defined as interoceptive inputs. Proprioception is basically a positional sense and it is closely related to the sensations of movement (kinesthesia) and balance. Proprioceptive and kinesthetic abilities allow us to eat our breakfast safely while reading the newspaper. They also permit visually impaired individuals to move about freely. Proprioception is processed in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobes. Strokes can have a major impact on proprioception, and many stroke patients report little or no intrinsic awareness of their affected limbs when questioned. Proprioceptive deficits may be related to the problem of neglect, in which stroke patients may have limited awareness of objects in the field of vision contralateral to their lesion. They may also neglect body parts on the affected side, washing only half of their body and putting shoes or socks on only one foot. The presence of significant proprioceptive deficits will have a major impact on the results of a biofeedback training program with a stroke patient. Patients need to feel the movements trained with EMG feedback. Without an intrinsic awareness of what they have accomplished with feedback, patients are very unlikely to be able to repeat the movement on their own. In my own (R. L. R.) work with stroke patients, I have seen II. INSTRUMENTATION a young woman successfully open her hand while doing finger extension training, only to look down and gasp at what she had accomplished after seeing that her fingers were indeed straight. The experimental findings of Edward Taub (1977) with deafferented monkeys are relevant to this discussion. Taub reported that monkeys would fail to use an arm after surgical transaction of the sensory roots at the level of the spinal cord. However, if the nonoperated arm was immobilized with restraints, the monkeys would soon start to use the deafferented limb. There are a number of differences between a stroke victim and a monkey with a surgically deafferented limb, but Taub’s findings support the notion that primates are capable of functional use of a limb in the absence of proprioceptive feedback. Perhaps the best recommendation that can be made is to try to facilitate the recovery of proprioception. The affected limbs can be moved passively, and the patient can alternate between watching the movement and trying to sense the movement with the eyes closed. Patients can be shown how to range and stretch their affected limbs by themselves, and the simultaneous feedback from the intact hand and the impaired limb may serve to improve the sense of movement and position. Techniques for Optimizing the Results of Rehabilitation Biofeedback Effective incorporation of biofeedback into the rehabilitation process requires an in-depth knowledge of biofeedback techniques and devices, as well as numerous associated techniques. Therapists need to understand the underlying pathology causing the problem, how the muscles and other physiological systems relate to the problem, limitations of the recording technology, and how to set the psychophysiological recorder so that it will provide the required information. Very often therapists need to compare signals from two sets of “paired” muscles performing the same function, such as when recording from (1) the left and right paraspinals of the low back during a low back evaluation, (2) the left and right vastus medialis and lateralis (which control kneecap stability) during a subluxation of the patella evaluation, (3) the left and right upper trapezius (in the shoulders) during a headache evaluation, and (4) the major muscles of the residual limb during a phantom limb pain evaluation. It is crucial 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography to remember that a difference of even a few centimeters in location of the sensors over the muscle (including angle with respect to the direction the muscle runs and distance between the active sensors) or a small difference in impedance between the sets of sensors can result in huge differences in the microvolt levels from the two sets of muscles. Ignoring these factors also leads to very low test– retest reliability when relating changes in muscle tension to changes in pain over a period of weeks, so one needs to be sure the sensors are placed in virtually exactly the same spot and have similar impedance at each recording. Even with the best sensor placement and skin preparation, one is not likely to obtain similar readings from most sets of laterally paired muscles, because the muscles are not likely to be the same size. For example, the mass of paraspinal muscles can normally differ up to 20%, so the signals may differ up to 20%. For this reason, teaching a patient to match the levels of tension in order to decrease pain due to sustained muscle tension can be ineffective unless the differences are greater than about 20%. The patterns of SEMG activity recorded from different muscles (e.g., abdomen and bicep) appear different as the muscles go through a cycle of tensing and relaxing. When paired muscles (e.g., the left and right paraspinals of the low back) are recorded, their patterns of activity should be virtually the same regardless of minor differences in muscle tension. Thus, it is far more important to train the patterns to be the same rather than attempt to train the muscles’ tension levels to differ less than 20%. Patterns of muscle activity between two paired muscles frequently appear very different, because the muscles are not actually performing the same activities at the same time and to the same extent. Nearby muscles whose tension is recorded along with the muscles of interest due to cross talk, even when sensors are placed close together along the length of the muscle of interest, may be doing different accessory activities during the recording. Thus, it is important to control the motion of interest to the fullest extent possible and to observe very carefully how the patient is moving to guide the motion toward being as bilaterally symmetrical as practical. For example, when comparing muscle patterns during an evaluation of subluxation of the patella, the motion of both legs has to be carefully controlled, as does change in the angle of the feet during the motion, so that the patterns observed on the monitor will be reflective of symmetrical motions. This is usually accomplished by guiding 81 the motion with the assistance of a moving foot rest on the chair in which the patient sits. When working with a set of muscles for the first time, it is frequently not obvious which muscles are contributing to the abnormal situation, such as incorrectly sustained muscle tension that occurs with jaw musculoskeletal pain or incorrect patterns of motion that occur with subluxation of the patella. Just what a normal pattern of activity should look like is also not easy to determine when the practitioner is not familiar with the area being evaluated. Typical kinesiology books (e.g., Lippert, 2000) are profusely illustrated and go into great detail about which muscles contribute to which motions, as well as the order in which the muscles tense during different phases of the motion. For example, when evaluating pain and incontinence in the pelvic floor, it is crucial to record from the lower abdomen, because incorrect tension in these large muscles can easily overwhelm whatever the pelvic floor muscles are doing to maintain continence. Authors such as Kasman, Cram, and Wolf (1998) have published books showing many of the typical normal and abnormal patterns of muscle tension that are likely to be seen during an evaluation. Sometimes SEMG recording technology simply does not adequately record the signals from muscles that may well be contributing to a pain problem without mixing the signal with those from larger, overlaying, uninvolved muscles running in different directions and having different functions from the muscle of interest (Wolf, 1980). Setting the psychophysiological recorder correctly for recording and feeding back muscle activity is crucial. An area frequently overlooked is setting the bandwidth (the frequencies recorded by the amplifier) wide enough to encompass all of the muscle’s relevant activity. If the bandwidth is set too narrow (e.g., 100–200 Hz), the muscle’s power spectrum (the amount of power at each frequency) may change so much as the muscle’s tension changes from relaxed to very tense that much of the power may be outside the recording window during some parts of the tensing continuum. This leads to the false conclusion that the muscle is far less tense (by as much as 30%) than it actually is at that level of tension, because the power is never detected (Sherman, 2004). Many patients are incorrectly taught that their muscles are relaxed when they feel that they are tense. Numerous studies have shown that patients with chronic pain are less able to align how tense they think the muscles in painful areas are with actual levels of tension than either (1) people 82 who are pain free or (2) patients with pain who have muscles that are pain free (Flor, Schugens, & Birbaumer, 1992). This inability to calibrate actual levels of tension with sensations correctly is especially important among patients with musclerelated jaw area pain and low back pain, because these patients tend to permit their muscles to remain tense for so long that pain can result. Because a key part of muscle rehabilitation is to help patients correctly align sensations of tension with actual levels of muscle tension, it is vital not to confuse the situation with recordings that do not reflect actual levels of tension. The biofeedback display has to be set correctly to facilitate learning. For example, if the device is set to “autogain” (in which the software controls the gain for each channel recorded), with each channel of muscle tension responding independently, differences in tension are obscured when the machine keeps switching the gain for each channel to keep the signal about the same size on the display. Thus, channels may have very different levels of amplification, each of which changes independently during a single motion. Two muscles that appear to have the same amount of tension because the signals are the same size on the monitor may have very different levels of tension. Relative patterns of tension among several muscles contributing to a motion are obscured if the gain for each muscle’s display changes differently throughout the course of the motion. The therapist will probably notice the rapid changes in gain because the numbers on the vertical line indicating range of microvolts keep changing, but the patient is not likely to be able to track these rapid changes and incorporate them into learning to compare the signals. The sweep speed for each channel not only needs to be the same, so that the patterns of activity appear similar, but it also has to be appropriate to the situation being recorded. For example, if the patient is being asked to bend and rise as part of a low back evaluation or bend and straighten the legs as part of a subluxation evaluation, the sweep speed needs to be set so that several cycles of activity can be seen clearly on the screen. This permits patient and therapist to observe differences in patterns immediately as the patient is coached to correct the motion and tension patterns. Great care needs to be taken to adjust the amount of signal averaging or “smoothing,” so that important data are not lost. Essentially, the longer the integration time constant, the less momentary changes in the signal are reflected in the display. II. INSTRUMENTATION When a signal is averaged too much (too long a time constant), brief spasms can be obscured or take so long to show on the monitor that the spasm is long gone before it is observed. This prevents the patient from pairing the sensations accompanying the spasm with the signal displayed on the monitor. For example, when treating cramping phantom limb pain, the patient must observe the spasms causing the pain just as the spasms occur. Otherwise, the patient rarely learns to prevent the spasms, which means the treatment is far less effective or fails (Sherman, 2004). Too much smoothing or averaging of a signal essentially produces a delay in the feedback. When doing muscle retraining, any delay in feedback increases the number of times the task needs to be repeated before it is learned, therefore decreasing the effectiveness of the training. A proper assessment is crucial to knowing what information to feed back to the patient. For example, only about 7% of amputees with phantom pain are helped when treatments are not aligned with the underlying muscle and blood flow problems, while about 80% are helped when treatments match the underlying problem (Sherman, 2004). Also, a crucial part of any rehabilitation assessment is teasing out the role of stress in maintaining and intensifying muscle tension. For example, without conducting a psychophysiological stress profile, it may be impossible to relate the contribution of stress to changes in muscle tension to changes in low back pain. It is common for patients’ generalized musculoskeletal stress responses to result in muscles all over their bodies being chronically tense and fatigued. Learning to recognize specific patterns of stress responses related to onset of pain may be impossible if a patient’s overall muscle tension obscures specific reactions. A course of general relaxation training may be required before specific training can be effective. Heart rate variability feedback may be especially relevant when chronic sympathetic arousal underlies pain-related problems, such as irritable bowel syndrome and non-cardiac-based chest pain. An important part of the evaluation is knowing what to look for, beyond the psychophysiological assessment. For example, the contributions of posture to tension headache and stress to muscle tension-related jaw area pain should not be ignored (Middaugh, Kee, & Nicholson, 1994). Practitioners need to know which common psychological and physical tests might be useful, along with psychophysiological assessments, in designing an optimal 4. Advanced Topics in Surface Electromyography course of treatment. For example, incorporation of cognitive restructuring may be of value in helping patients with irritable bowel syndrome who reveal features of conversion disorder (Blanchard, Greene, Scharff, & Schwatz-McMorris, 1993). Trigger points need to be evaluated, because many muscle-related pain problems appear to be intensified by trigger points (Dommerholdt, Bron, & Fransson, 2006). The practitioner performing the evaluation need not be an expert in treating trigger points. Rather, the professional needs to be proficient enough to recognize them, so that an appropriate referral can be made. For example, piriformis syndrome, in which spasms in the hip’s piriformis muscle put pressure on the sciatic nerve, is a common cause of low back pain. Unless the piriformis is checked for problems, including trigger points, during a low back pain evaluation, the underlying cause will be missed. Biofeedback training in rehabilitation is frequently performed in a manner very different from that used with other applications. Especially for muscular rehabilitation, considerable use is made of traditional operant conditioning techniques, such as shaping and multitrial learning (Brucker, Maiatico, & Pauza, 1992; Brucker, & Bulaeva, 1996). Development of unconscious control through repetition of specific small tasks taken in steps is emphasized. Biofeedback devices are typically used, with their thresholds set so that it is easy to reach an immediate goal. When that level is reached, the threshold is reset so that the next goal can be strived for and met. Thus, muscle tension is gradually shaped to the correct pattern of activity. The authors believe that operant conditioning is more effective than guided practice and similar techniques for muscular reeducation. Stated more directly, practitioners using biofeedback in rehabilitation need to understand the principles of operant conditioning in order to be optimally effective in working with their patients. Further information about using psychophysiological techniques for assessing and treating patients whose primary symptom is chronic pain may be found in Sherman (2004). References Abdallah, M., & Zahran, B. (2009). Analysis: Signal-step analysis of surface myoelectric signal. European Journal of Scientific Research, 26, 298–304. Baratta, R. V., Solomonow, M., Zhou, B. H., & Zhu, M. (1998). Methods to reduce variability of EMG power 83 spectrum estimates. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 8, 279–285. Barry, D. T. (1991). AAEM minimonography #36: Basic concepts of electricity and electronics in clinical electromyography. Muscle and Nerve, 14, 937–946. Basmajian, J., & De Luca, C. (1985). Muscles alive. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Basmajian, J. V., Kukulka, C. G., Narayan, M. G., & Takebe, K. (1975). Biofeedback treatment of foot-drop after stroke compared with standard rehabilitation technique: Effects on voluntary control and strength. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 56(6), 231–236. Blanchard, E. B., Greene, B., Scharff, L., & SchwarzMcMorris, S. P. (1993). Relaxation training as a treatment for irritable bowel syndrome. Biofeedback and SelfRegulation, 18, 125–32. Bolek, J. (2006). Use of multiple-site performance-contingent sEMG reward programming in pediatric rehabilitation: A retrospective review. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 31, 263–272. Bolek, J. E. (2012). QSEMG: Quantitative Surface Electromyography: Applications in neuromotor rehabilitation. Biofeedback: A Clinical Journal, 40(2), 47–56. Brucker, B. S., & Bulaeva, N. (1996). Biofeedback effect on electromyography responses in patients with spinal cord injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 77, 133–137. Brucker, B. S., Maiatico, M. A., & Pauza, C. (1992). Long term recovery in spinal cord injury with the use of biofeedback techniques. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73(10), 960. Brunnstrom, S. (1970). Movement therapy in hemiplegia. New York: Harper & Row. Buchthal, F., & Schmalbruch, H. (1980). Motor unit of mammalian muscle. Physiological Reviews, 60, 90–142. Cottman, C. W., & McGaugh, J. L. (1980). Behavioral neuroscience. New York: Academic Press. Cram, J. (1991). Clinical EMG for surface recordings: Vol. 2. Nevada City, CA: Clinical Resources. Cram, J., Kasman, G., & Holtz, J. (1998). Introduction to surface electromyography. Alexandria, VA: Aspen. DeLuca, C. J. (1997). The use of surface electromyography in biomechanics. Journal of Applied Biomechanics, 13, 135–163. DeLuca, C. J. (1979). Physiology and mathematics of myoelectric signals. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 26, 313–325. Dommerholdt, J., Bron, C., & Fransson, J. (2006). Myofascial trigger points: An evidence-informed review. Journal of Manual and Manipulative Therapy, 14(4), 203–221. Flor, H., Schugens, M., & Birbaumer, N. (1992). Discrimination of muscle tension in chronic pain patients and healthy controls. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 17, 165–177. Kasman, G. (1995). Surface EMG and biofeedback in physical and behavioral medicine: Applications in chronic pain management. Seattle, WA: Virginia Mason Medical Center. 84 Kasman, G., Cram, J., & Wolf, S. (1998). Clinical applications in surface electromyography: Chronic musculoskeletal pain. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen. Lertprapamongkol, W., & Suksathien, R. (2007). Electromyographic activities during isometric contraction of interphalangeal joint extensors of the finger. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 90(8), 1657–1664. Lippert, L. (2000). Clinical kinesiology for physical therapy assistants (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Davis. Middaugh, S., Kee, W., & Nicholson, J. (1994). Muscle overuse and posture as factors in the development and maintenance of chronic musculoskeletal pain. In R. Grzesiak & D. Cicconie (Eds.), Psychological vulnerability to chronic pain. New York: Springer. Sella, G. E. (2000). Guidelines for neuromuscular re-education with SEMG/ biofeedback. Martins Ferry, OH: GENMED. Sherman, R. (2004). Pain assessment and intervention from a psychophysiological perspective. Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology. Sherrington, C. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Soderberg, G. L. (1992). Selected topics in surface electromyography for use in the occupational setting: Expert perspectives. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and II. INSTRUMENTATION Human Services, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. Taub, E. (1977). Movement in nonhuman primates deprived of somatosensory feedback. Exercise and Sports Science Reviews, 4, 335–374. Taylor, W. (1990). Dynamic EMG biofeedback in assessment and treatment using a neuromuscular re-education model. In J. Cram (Ed.), Clinical EMG for surface recordings: Vol. 2. Nevada City, CA: Clinical Resources. Travell, J., & Simons, D. (1993). Myofascial pain and dysfunction: The trigger point manual. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Winter, D. A. (1990). Biomechanics and motor control of human movement. New York: Wiley Interscience. Wolf, S. (1980). Video comparing cadaver views of muscle location with SEMG sensor placement. Recorded at Emory University in Atlanta about 1980. Retrieved from www.aapb.org. Wolf, S., & Segal, R. (1990). Conditioning of the spinal stretch reflex: Implications for rehabilitation. Physical Therapy, 70(10), 652–656. Yolanda, D., Ackah, H., Mandel, S., Manon-Espaillat, R., Abaza, M., & Sataloff, R. (2007). Laryngeal electromyography. Otolaryngological Clinics of North America, 40, 1003–1023. Chapter 5 Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment in Applied Psychophysiology Richard N. Gevirtz, Mark S. Schwartz, and Paul M. Lehrer In this chapter, we provide an overview of cardiorespiratory physiology and the most common measurement techniques used with some of their metrics. that separates the chest cavity from the abdominal cavity. It forms a flexible, moving floor for the lungs. When the diaphragm is at rest, its shape is a double dome, and it extends upward into the chest under the lungs. To start inhalation, the diaphragm contracts, flattens downward, and descends. This allows the lungs to fill. It displaces the abdominal contents, expanding the belly. The natural return of the diaphragm to its resting state occurs with exhalation. Other muscles involved in breathing include the intercostal muscles (which act on the rib cage) and the scalene muscles (which raise the chest by lifting the first and second ribs). In some cases, the muscles of the abdominal area contract to push the abdominal contents upward and push upward on the diaphragm. In the heart–lung system, special large molecules of a substance called hemoglobin carry the fresh oxygen to every part of the body. Oxygen crosses the respiratory tubules called pulmonary alveoli. CO2 plays an important role in how the hemoglobin releases the oxygen. As blood pH changes, based on breathing changes, the hemoglobin molecule releases its oxygen cargo. If too little CO2 is present, the oxygen is overbound to the hemoglobin and not available to fuel body organ tissue. To understand this process more clearly, consider the analogy of a milk truck trying to deliver Anatomy and Physiology The Respiratory System The respiratory system is among the most complex organ systems in the body. Descriptions of the anatomy and especially the physiology of this system are provided in medical textbooks (e.g., Guyton & Hall, 1995) and in abbreviated forms in Fried and Grimaldi (1993) and Naifeh (1994). A practitioner of applied psychophysiology needs to be able to provide a basic explanation for a client; we therefore offer a rationale for respiratory function that may be useful for such purposes. Oxygen is taken in through the trachea or windpipe and pumped through a system of increasingly smaller tubes, which have the characteristic of letting some gases through to the blood and in turn taking waste gases (mostly carbon dioxide, or CO2) back to be exhaled. This process, called “gas exchange,” takes place in he lungs. The lungs themselves have no intrinsic muscles for breathing; instead, the diaphragm is the major muscle for breathing. It is a sheet-like muscle stretching from the backbone to the front of the rib cage 85 86 individual bottles of milk to local stores. The oxygen is represented by the milk; the hemoglobin is the milk truck, and the store is the body tissue needing fuel to function properly. The dairy (the heart–lung system) loads the truck up with an excess of milk bottles, and the truck sets off on its appointed rounds. Once the truck arrives at the store, the cargo door must be opened wide enough to make an adequate delivery. Since CO2 controls the release of the oxygen from the hemoglobin, it would be seen as regulating the width of the door, so that in the scheduled time enough milk can be dropped off at the store. Not enough CO2 means an inadequate delivery and shortages. In physiology, this oxygen dissociation function is known as Bohr’s law. It says that the oxygen can be “overbound” to the hemoglobin, creating hypoxia, or lack of oxygen, which can produce symptoms such as lightheadedness, heart pounding, cold hands, nervous emotional states, or even mental “fog.” Hyperventilation (HV) means that the lungs are releasing too much CO2, because breath rate and/ or tidal volume (the amount of air that is breathed out) exceed the level needed for the current conditions. This is often referred to as “overbreathing,” hyperpnea, or hypocapnia. The shift in blood flow to the outer shell of the brain, where most complex thought is processed, can be dramatic. (See www. improve-mental-health.com/hyperventilation.html.) HV can be obvious or subtle. The image of a nervous person breathing rapidly in his or her upper chest and needing a paper bag is well known. Few people realize that sighing or breath holding and gasping can produce some of the same effects. When someone overbreathes frequently, as might happen in a very stressful period, the person can create a condition in which his or her body adjusts to the low level of CO2 and maintains it with continuing sighs, yawns, and the like. When this individual slows down his or her breathing, the respiratory center in the brain may try to reestablish the previous CO2 levels by using the sighs, yawns, or shallow breaths to blow off excess CO2. Although this is usually not extremely dangerous, it does create a chronic condition with many potential symptoms, such as dizziness, shortness of breath, palpitations, and so forth. This chronic overbreathing is sometimes called hyperventilation syndrome (HVS). A medical device called a “capnometer” is sometimes used to measure the amount of CO2 in the expired (exhaled) breath. Normal levels are 40 millimeters of mercury (mm Hg) or 40 torr. A level under 30 mm Hg or 30 torr is usually considered low. II. INSTRUMENTATION It should now be clear that breathing patterns are a powerful force within the body, and that learning to alter these patterns can be a powerful intervention for change. Assessing HV Methods for assessing HV include observations, interviews, self-report questionnaires, HV provocation, blood assays, a transcutaneous instrument that roughly estimates CO2, and a noninvasive instrument that measures the percentage of exhaled CO2. The last instrument is an infrared gas analyzer called a “capnometer,” which produces a “capnograph.” Physiological monitoring can be performed during rest and during office stress challenges, while the patient is supine, seated, or standing. During HV provocation, one compares the similarity of the symptoms during the provocation with the presenting complaints. Voluntary HV during provocation results in considerable individual variability of symptoms and patterns (Clark & Hemsley, 1982; Fried, Fox, & Carlton, 1990; Fried & Grimaldi, 1993), for adverse effects, dangers, and contraindications for HV provocation). Relying on patients to report all the symptoms is insufficient. They often do not recall or report many of the symptoms unless these are provoked in the office. Observing unprovoked breathing patterns in the office is also insufficient. The Nijmegen Questionnaire (van Dixhoorn & Duivenvoorden, 1985) is a self-report measure with reasonable psychometric properties. It consists of a short number of symptoms that have been found to be associated with HVS. Criteria for Diagnosing HV Criteria for diagnosing HV vary, but typically involve measures of CO2. Low CO2, less than an average of 38 torr (less than 5% partial pressure of expired CO2: petCO2, or the percentage of end-tidal CO2) at sea level is usually considered a normal level. This criterion is independent of symptoms. However, symptoms can emerge with higher and lower levels of petCO2 (Fried, 1987; Fried & Grimaldi, 1993). Practitioners typically focus on symptoms rather than CO2 level. One view (Fensterheim, 1994) is that clinicians want to avoid missing a diagnose HV when it is present. Fensterheim also agrees with Bass and Gardner (1985) and Gardner (1994), who believe that no symptom or clinical definition of HVS is widely 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment accepted. Thus, clinicians can be less precise than researchers, who wish to avoid including a person without HV in a group with HV, and hence must use a strict physiological criterion. Some practitioners and researchers use a criterion of a respiration rate at rest equal to or more than a specified number of breaths per minute (b/ min), such as 16. There is no universally accepted normal breathing rate. A resting breathing rate of 8–14 b/min, reported by Holloway (1994), is a commonly accepted criterion. However, there are reports of 16–17 b/min (nearly 3 b/min greater; Tobin, Mador, Guenther, Lodato, & Sackner, 1988) for younger and older persons monitored without their awareness. Other reports suggest that men and people in their early 20s show slightly lower rates than women or older people, as reported in Fried and Grimaldi (1993). Fried and Grimaldi suggest a goal of “no more than 9 to 12 b/min” (p. 246) for a person at rest. He assumes normal tidal volume and partial pressure of CO2 (pCO2). People with organic diseases that affect respiration typically show faster respiration rates, from about 18–28 b/min (Fried, 1987; Fried & Grimaldi, 1993). Therefore, using a breathing rate above 16 is a crude and insufficient criterion for HV, and one cannot rely on this. Ley (1993), Fried and Grimaldi (1993), Bonn, Readhead, and Timmons (1984), Folgering and Colla (1978), Howell (1990), and Timmons and Ley (1994) provide good discussions of this topic. For example, Ley (1993) states that even sound operational definitions are often insufficient unless they include information about the person’s medical conditions (pulmonary or heart disease, etc.) at the time. HV Provocation Test The “HV provocation test” (HVPT) involves directed, intentional, and very rapid and usually deep breathing. Instructions often include something like filling the lungs with each inhalation and exhaling as completely as possible. The purpose is to reproduce the patient’s symptoms and complaints as a diagnostic aid. Limited empirical examination of this test exists and thus it has it critics (Lindsay, Saqi, & Bass, 1991), especially related to the reliability and validity of the test, but this technique remains in common clinical practice. Methods reported usually involve a specified breathing rate of at least 20 b/min, and sometimes as many as 60 b/min. Patients do this for a speci- 87 fied time of at least 60 seconds and usually for 2–3 minutes. There are guidelines, but no standard protocol exists (Timmons & Ley, 1994). Howell (1990) suggests the 20 Deep Breaths Test. Folgering and Colla (1978) use “one minute of deep breathing”(p. 509). Bonn, Readhead, and Timmons (1984) have added breathing with the upper chest and 60 b/min. Some researchers and many practitioners rely only on the rate-and-time method. This can be very unpleasant for some patients. Therefore, practitioners sometimes stop when many of the symptoms appear. Some use a pacing device such as a fast metronome or audiotape (Salkovskis & Clark, 1990). Others also specify a percentage (e.g., 50%) drop in pCO2 as an alternative criterion (Craske & Barlow, 1990; Gardner, 1994; Nixon & Freeman, 1988: Salkovskis & Clark, 1990). One can specify a specific level of alveolar pCO2. Criteria vary—for example, less than 19 mm Hg (19 torr; Nixon & Freeman, 1988) to below 38 torr (Fried & Grimaldi, 1993). Gardner (1994) uses the criterion of “below about . . . 30 mm Hg at rest or during or after exercise, or remains low 5 minutes after voluntary over-breathing” (p. 1093). Opinions of normal pCO2 range from 38 to 40 mm Hg. There are individual differences in the level below which symptoms appear. This also always depends on other factors, such as altitude where collected. Measures of petCO2 are usually sea-level measures. The measurement drops as altitude increases because of decreased air pressure at higher altitudes; petCO2 is a percentage of CO2 relative to air pressure in the surrounding environment. petCO2 is less at higher altitudes and in high-elevation cities such as Denver. Some consider a patient’s awareness of the similarity of naturally occurring symptoms to HVPT-induced symptoms as “the most important element in the diagnosis of HVS” (Garssen, De Ruiter, & van Dyck,, 1992, pp. 149–150; see also Lewis & Howell, 1986). However, studies of this criterion are rare. In the few studies available, “the response to the HVPT does not predict the occurrence of HV during panic attacks” (Garssen et al., 1992, p. 150). This review summarized studies that concluded there was no difference between the recognized symptoms typical for HV during the HVPT and to those occurring during a stressful time-pressured task without decreased petCO2. One should read Ley’s (1993) comments about these studies and the Garssen et al. (1992) review. Ley is more favorable about the HVPT. However, he points out that the absence of an HVPT effect 88 does not mean that a patient does not have a HVrelated problem. Garssen et al. (1992) note that although some patients recognize symptoms during an HVPT, CO2 does not always drop during ambulatory monitoring of panic. This conclusion is drawn from the study by Hibbert and Pilsbury (1989), who used a transcutaneous estimate of the partial pressure of CO2 (ptcCO2). Furthermore, this study concluded that some patients do not recognize the provoked symptoms. These include patients who show large drops in ptcCO2 during panic. One conclusion is that panic does not always result in HV. The transcutaneous method is slow in showing changes that usually occur for each breath. Thus some practitioners and researchers who have tried it have not found it useful, especially for office assessments. One view is that HV is a necessary factor for most of the somatic symptoms associated with HVS, but is not sufficient for panic symptoms. For example, cognitive factors are also necessary, according to Garssen et al. (1992), and thus cognitive therapy is necessary for treatment. Ley (1992) agrees with this for a subset of patients. Others question the role of hyperventilation as a consistent risk factor (Hornsveld & Garssen, 1996, 1997). HVPT does not always evoke the symptoms in question for a specific person, which leads to other concerns. For example, chest pain is not easy to reproduce. Other views are critical about the role of HV for panic and the value of HVPT as the only or best criterion. For example, stressful mental tasks with only small decreases in alveolar CO2 can also evoke HVS symptoms (Garssen et al., 1992). However, the decreases in petCO2 were 1.2 mm Hg. Furthermore, a mental task of thinking about various stressful topics led to HV and decreased pCO2 among more subjects (33/54, or 61%) than did the HVPT (7/54, or 13%; Nixon & Freeman, 1988). Some practitioners (Fried & Grimaldi, 1993) are far more cautious about HVPT. Fried considers it hazardous and recommends against this procedure. However, he provides no guidelines or exceptions about when it may be acceptable. He is cautious partly because he is a nonphysician. He is very concerned about inducing changes in blood acid–base balance, coronary and cerebral vasoconstriction, and ischemic hypoxia. This is worrisome to him, as it should be to everyone, because a patient could have an undiscovered and undiagnosed organic disease that places him or her at risk. For example, the patient with diagnosed II. INSTRUMENTATION “functional chest symptoms” might turn out to have an organic cardiac disease for which induced biochemical and cardiovascular changes increase the risks of cardiac dysfunction. However, he adds that “in fairness to my colleagues . . . a number of them use the procedure and have reported no consequent ill effects in their clients” (p. 42). Compernolle, Hoogduin, and Joele (1979) caution against using voluntary HV with patients with chronic anemia or vascular diseases. They refer to loss of consciousness and fatal accidents when HV is followed by breath holding during underwater swimming and diving competition (Hill, 1973; Craig, 1976). Neurological impairment and fatal accidents have also occurred following HV in children with sickle-cell anemia (Allen, Imbus, Powars, & Haywood, 1976). However, aside from the examples cited, Compernolle et al. (1979) noted that “nothing in the literature to substantiate the fear that provoking hyperventilation may be dangerous. There are no reports of accidents resulting from two to five minutes of hyperventilation followed by breathing into a bag” (p. 616). Prudent practitioners are extra cautious with people at risk for syncope. Causes for syncope include certain cardiovascular, metabolic, or neurological disorders, such as seizures. Remember that hypocapnia-induced vasoconstriction from HV reduces cerebral blood flow (Berkow & Fletcher, 1992). Thus, caution is necessary for many older adult patients and those with compromised cerebral blood flow. The clinician should also be aware that many organic medical conditions can cause HV (Gardner, 1994). Practitioners must be very careful not to provoke unintended and potentially risky symptoms. For example, even psychologically distressed patients who hyperventilate as a result of habit and anxiety can also have diabetes. Induced HV may affect or at least interact with blood glucose (Guthrie, Moeller, & Guthrie, 1983; C. Lum, 1994; L. C. Lum, 1975). If blood glucose is very low because of poorly controlled diabetes, induced HV can add to the acidosis and intensify symptoms. Nonmedical practitioners and researchers who believe they need to use the HVPT or intend to do so for other clinical or research reasons must get medical clearance. This is crucial, particularly in patients with a history of respiratory, cardiovascular, or some neurological diseases. For example, a history of or evidence for coronary artery disease or unexplained chest pain clearly indicates caution. 89 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment A related caution stems from the potential for some patients to self-initiate breath holding or the Valsalva maneuver” to abort symptoms, including those from HVS and panic. Quiet breath holding combined with irregular breathing is a pattern associated with just as many problems as the obvious HV pattern (Holloway, 1994). There is one report (Sartory & Olajide, 1988) of the use of the Valsalva maneuver as a potential treatment for panic symptoms. Physicians suggest using the Valsalva to abort paroxysmal atrial tachycardia for selected patients. Such information may encourage patients to try these without proper consultation. For example, Rapee (1985) reported a case in which a woman began aborting her panic symptoms by holding her breath without instructions to do so. In most people, these attempts are not dangerous, but one must be cautious and complete in patient education instructions. In some weight lifters, “hyperventilation before lifting causes hypocapnia, cerebral vasoconstriction, and peripheral vasodilation” (Berkow & Fletcher, l992, p. 432). The lifting involves the Valsalva maneuver, which affects blood return to the heart, reducing cardiac output and altering CO2 levels. Potentially, systemic vasodilation and decreased blood pressure may occur, increasing the risk of syncope for some people engaged in similar activities. Although, for most people, the risks do not exist, there are enough reasons to be very cautious and obtain approval from a qualified physician. Prudent practitioners, especially nonmedical ones, may decide to avoid the procedure unless it is absolutely necessary to make a diagnosis or convince a patient of the diagnosis. Instrumentation‑Based Breathing Feedback There are several instrumentation-based breathing feedback systems available to enhance relaxed breathing. All are relatively simple and in use by practitioners. There is no research showing any differential outcomes among them. Practitioners use those systems that are available and with which they feel most comfortable. Nasal Airflow Temperatures A thermistor (an electrical resistor that measures temperature) taped below a nostril detects the changes in temperature of air inhaled (which is cooler) and exhaled air (warmer). With a sensitively set computer-based visual display, therapist and patient can clearly see the rapid changes in temperature. One sees hills and valleys in the curve on the screen, as the temperature falls during inhalation and rises during exhalation. One goal is to make the hills and valleys about the same size and duration. The patient watches the curve and, with this feedback, regulates the size and timing of breaths to create a regular rhythm of hills and valleys in the curve. The clinician should set the display screen width to a time reflecting a few inhalations and exhalations. The temperature should be centered in the middle of the screen, and the range of temperature (sensitivity) should be set to create hills and valleys that are easy to see. The temperatures should not extend beyond the limits of the screen. The range depends partly on the patient. Therapists who are just starting to use this technique are advised to try it on themselves first, starting with a display in which the range is at or less than about 5°F, and tailor it for the individual. This is a simple technique requiring only one temperature feedback unit and a simple computerbased software display. However, it does not give any other information, such as that about muscles used and CO2. Therefore, practitioners often use it in conjunction with other feedback instruments. Finally, in order to avoid transmission of infections from the nose, therapists must be very careful to disinfect the thermistor between sessions for all patients. Strain Gauges Many practitioners wrap stretchable devices around the patient’s abdomen, chest, or both. These allow monitoring and feedback about abnormal breathing patterns, such as irregularity, breath holding, and apneas. Therapists also use this to help teach new breathing patterns, as it is better than observation alone (Timmons & Ley, 1994). The device has sensors that convey the degree of expansion. These are connected to a computer-based feedback system that permits viewing the signals on a computer monitor. The purpose of this type of system is to allow one to see the expansions in each body area, as well as the difference between the abdominal area and the chest area during each breath. The specific numbers are unimportant and may be different for each person. The numbers depend on the tightness of the bands and the sen- 90 sitivity setting. The feedback signal provides hills and valleys that depend on the size of the breath and body area used. This is similar to the nasal airflow temperature measure; however, the advantage over the latter measure is that it gives information about abdominal versus chest breathing. Like the nasal airflow temperature measure, this method provides no information about other muscles or CO2 and has certain technical limitations, such as movement artifact. Recalibration after movements and position changes may be necessary (Timmons & Ley, 1994). The reader is directed to Fried and Grimaldi (1993, p. 40) and Timmons and Ley (1994, pp. 281–282) for more discussion and references for using strain gauges. Many of the newer biofeedback systems now have sophisticated breathing pattern guides that have yet to be studied systematically. These allow the practitioner to set the pattern to vary inspiration–expiration ratio, respiration rate, and pauses. EMG from Accessory Breathing Muscles Practitioners also use EMG feedback from accessory breathing muscles. The sternomastoids, upper back muscles (including the rhomboids and levator scapulae), and upper chest muscles (including the pectoralis and/or scalene muscles) are among the preferred choices. The selection of muscles depends on therapist preference and practical considerations. The purpose is to show whether there are EMG increases from these muscles during each inhalation, to indicate the degree of any increases, and to give feedback to help the patient reduce these increases. The Capnometer and Oximeter Method The capnometer and oximeter methods allow measurement and feedback of ETCO2 and good estimates of arterial blood oxygen saturation (paO2). One inserts into either nostril a narrow roughly 0.25-inch plastic tube, which is taped to the skin near the upper lip (Fried, 1987; Fried & Grimaldi, 1993; R. Fried, personal communication, March 13, 1994). Others have used a standard nasal cannula. The tube’s outer width is about 4 millimeters. This method allows continuous sampling of endtidal breath conducted to an infrared gas analyzer. The signal then goes to a computer that feeds back the wave of rising and falling petCO2 on a video monitor. It provides a hard copy output of the pattern and gives statistics for specified periods. The II. INSTRUMENTATION therapist can place a goal wave for size and rate of the breaths. One needs a system that recognizes and displays analyzed blood gas data. These plastic tubes are readily available and disposable after use. Spirometry For therapists without access to a capnometer, Lum (1991, cited in Timmons & Ley, 1994) recommends “spirometry.” This provides a measure of the amount of air moved. Nonphysicians and nonphysiologists should consider the smaller, less expensive, and relatively accurate Wright spirometer (Timmons & Ley, 1994). This allows an estimate of overbreathing by providing an estimate of minute volume. Normal resting minute volume is about 6 liters (Naifeh, 1994). An example of a criterion for overbreathing is 30 liters per minute (Lum, 1991, cited in Timmons & Ley, 1994). Arterial Blood Oxyhemoglobin Saturation: Oximetry Fried and Grimaldi (1993) use and recommend a measure of arterial blood oxyhemoglobin saturation, in which an oximeter attached to the patient’s index finger. One also could use an ear lobe, a practice commonly employed by sleep laboratories for overnight oximetry for assessing decreased oxygen saturations in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. The output from the oximeter connects to a physiological monitoring system The oximeter shows percentage of saturated hemoglobin, and the biofeedback screen displays the saturation of arterial blood oxygen over each breath cycle. This display shows variations of oxygen. It gives an index of the oxygen delivery to tissues during monitoring of petCO2. For example, normal pCO2 and elevated oxygen perfusion in the blood (SaO2) reflect oxygen perfusion expected with deep diaphragmatic breathing. Some experts wonder about the accuracy of this index. Contrast this to the reduced oxygen in the tissues associated with hypocapnia. Skin Temperatures: Hand and Head Apex Skin temperature is an indirect measure of breathing, according to Fried and Grimaldi (1993). He attaches a temperature sensor to the fifth digit of the nondominant hand while monitoring skin temperature. Other practitioners may use different digits. By itself, skin temperature is not a good 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment index; however, it is a common index of relaxation and is often monitored during breathing therapy procedures. Plethysmography: Pulse Rate and Sinus Rhythm Another indirect measure is obtained by pulse information (Stern, Ray, & Quigley, 2001). A biofeedback interface allows for measurement of variation of beat-to-beat pulse. This index of vagal tone and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) during the breath cycles provides an indirect index of cardiopulmonary status. Cardiopulmonary techniques such as RSA biofeedback are now becoming popular methods for breathing therapy. The Cardiovascular System: Heart Rate Variability The cardiac system, like most biological systems, demonstrates constant variation when in a healthy (or homeostatically balanced) state. It has long been postulated that heart rates that show a highly complex but organized pattern of variability (characterized as “chaotic”) are healthier than very steady rates or simpler patterns of variability. In recent years we have more understanding of the nature of variation in the human heart rate (or more technically in interbeat interval [IBI]). Several papers (Giardino, Lehrer, & Feldman, 2000; Norris, Gollan, Berntson, & Cacioppo, 2010) present a detailed discussion of the meaning of oscillations in biological and psychological systems. Several technical reviews of variability in heart rate also have been published (Berntson et al., 1997; Nunan, Sandercock, & Brodie, 2010; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). Measures of heart rate variability are derived from cardiac interbeat interval, measured most accurately from the R-spike in the electrocardiogram (ECG) but also estimated from interbeat intervals measured from pulses in the finger or the ear (Heilman, Handelman, Lewis, & Porges, 2008). Measures of heart rate variability include “time domain” measures such as standard deviation of normal heartbeats (SDNN), root mean square of sucessive differences (RMSSD), and percentage of adjacent heartbeats differing by more than 50 milliseconds (PNN50). Other “frequency domain” measures are derived from spectral analysis of interbeat intervals. 91 The various rhythms are often reported in hertz (Hz; cycles per second). The oscillators of interest are between 0.003 and 0.4 Hz. High-frequency (HF) rhythms are between 0.15 and 0.4 Hz. With a cardiotachometer, one can see the HF rhythms, but other slower oscillators are more difficult to see. To observe them, a spectral analysis is often used. A low-frequency (LF) rhythm occurs within the range of .08–.14 Hz, usually at around six per minute (0.1 Hz, with a period of 10 seconds). On a cardiotachometer, this would be difficult to see, but on the spectral it is clearly visible. This oscillator correlates with measures of a reflex that plays an important role in the regulation of blood pressure (BP). Small pressure sensors in the major arteries (“baroreceptors”) send information back to the sinus node of the heart to maintain homeostasis in the BP system. When BP rises, the baroreceptors stimulate the vagus brake to slow down the heart so as to reduce pressure. Similarly, with BP decreases, the baroreceptors send a signal to the sympathetic cardioaccelerator to speed up the heart and increase BP. A delay in the baroreflex system of approximately 5 seconds causes the 10-second (0.1 Hz) waves in heart rate. Baroreflex gain is currently of interest to cardiologists because it may be an early detector of cardiac disease. It is quantified as microseconds per millimeter of Hg (the change in IBI [in milliseconds] that co-occurs with changes in BP [in mm Hg]. A third oscillator, even more difficult to see in the cardiotachometer record, is the very-low-frequency (VLF) rhythm, defined as oscillations from .003 to .08 Hz. It is thought to be driven by a slow rhythm mediated by the sympathetic nervous system, possibly related to thermoregulation or gastrointestinal regulation. Vaschillo, Lehrer, Rishe, and Konstantinov (2002) published evidence suggesting that this wave reflects baroreflex effects on smooth muscle tone in the blood vessels (vascular tone). Oscillations in vascular tone and BP tend to show particularly large frequency peaks in the VLF range, centering at approximately 0.05 Hz (i.e., three times/minute, or having a period of 20 seconds). This oscillation thus suggests a delay in the vascular tone limb of the baroreflex system of about 10 seconds, perhaps due to plasticity of the blood vessels. This rhythm also is found in heart rate. Heart rate variability (HRV) has been used to measure both autonomic balance and regulatory capacity of the individual (Berntson, Norman, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2008). Decreased HRV has 92 been associated with increased cardiac mortality and morbidity (Buccelletti et al., 2009; Thayer, Yamamoto, & Brosschot, 2010) and mortality in kidney disease (Ranpuria, Hall, Chan, & Unruh, 2008), diabetes (Manzella & Paolisso, 2005), muscular dystrophy (Politano, Palladino, Nigro, Scutifero, & Cozza, 2008), schizophrenia (Koponen et al., 2008), and following surgery (Laitio, Jalonen, Kuusela, & Scheinin, 2007). It is also suppressed in stress, anger suppression, anxiety, and depressed affect (Horsten et al., 1999; Marques, Silverman, & Sternberg, 2010; Shinba et al., 2008), as well as in reaction to infection (Fairchild, Srinivasan, Moorman, Gaykema, & Goehler, 2011) and inflammation (Alvarez et al., 2007; Huston & Tracey, 2011; Jan et al., 2009; Thayer, 2009). Cardiac variability is suppressed in clinical depression, and compounded suppression of HRV in the frequent comorbidity of cardiac disease and depression is associated with compounded morbidity and mortality (Carney & Freedland, 2009). Suppressed HRV is, indeed, related to all cause mortality (Lauer, 2009). HRV also can be used to assess the balance between the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system (ANS; Berntson et al., 1997; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). Higher frequencies of HRV (0.15–0.4 Hz) are associated with breathing and are known as “respiratory sinus arrhythmia”; these are under parasympathetic control. Modulation of blood pressure through changes in heart rate appears to be influenced by both sympathetic and parasympathetic systems and is reflected in LF heart rate variability (0.05–0.15 Hz; Kuznetsova & Son'kin, 2008; Palma-Rigo et al., 2010). Vascular tone control, mediated primarily by alpha sympathetic activity, is reflected in HRV in the VLF range (0.005–0.05 Hz; Hayoz et al., 1995; Stauss, Rarick, Deklotz, & Sheriff, 2009). HRV appears to be indicative of autonomic control of the lung (Yasuma & Hayano, 2004), the gut (Cain, Jarrett, Burr, Hertig, & Heitkemper, 2007; Chen, Lin, Orr, Yang, & Kuo, 2004; Elsenbruch & Orr, 2001; Huang, Yang, Lai, & Kuo, 2010; Kaneko, Sakakibara, Mitsuma, & Morise, 1995; Suzuki et al., 2009), and perhaps facial muscles (Lindh, Wiklund, Sandman, & Hakansson, 1997; Stifter, Fox, & Porges, 1989), as well as the heart. The sympathetic and parasympathetic systems interact in a complex synergistic relationship that is sometimes reciprocal, sometimes additive, and II. INSTRUMENTATION sometimes subtractive. Particular rhythms characteristic of each of them operate through these pathways. Although the fight–flight reflex described in earlier literature on psychological stress is usually associated with sympathetic activation and parasympathetic suppression, it has long been known that sympathetic activity increases reactivity in the parasympathetic system (Gellhorn, 1957). This parasympathetic activity often rebounds to high levels after a high level of psychological stress decreases such as those found in nocturnal parasympathetic gastric (Nada et al., 2001) and asthma symptoms (Ballard, 1999; Gelder, Hubble, & Hetzel, 1988), where parasympathetic arousal is associated with increased symptomatology. Elevated parasympathetic arousal also occurs during states of relaxation (Lehrer et al., 1997) and can be associated with feelings of calmness and wellbeing. Another aspect of the relationship between the two branches of the ANS of particular interest to biofeedback practitioners is called accentuated antagonism. Vagal tone predominates over sympathetic tone at rest. Under normal physiological conditions, abrupt parasympathetic stimulation inhibits tonic sympathetic activation and its effects at rest during exercise (Olshansky, Sabbah, Hauptman, & Colucci, 2008). Frequency Domain Measures By using an ongoing spectral display that is sensitive to minute-by-minute changes in the ANS, we can obtain a rich picture of potential pathways for “mind–body” interaction. Together with other physiological measures, such as skin conductance, temperature, or muscle activity, the HRV measures can help us build a mediational model for disorders such as noncardiac chest pain, and so forth. For example, the spectral display provides an online assessment of sympathetic–parasympathetic balance (the ratio between HF and LF or VLF activity; Berntson et al., 1997) and a rough index of baroreflex gain (Bernardi et al., 1994). Porges (2007) has postulated that the vagus nerve in humans evolved into two pathways: One, more primitive and older in evolutionary terms, originates in the dorsal motor nucleus (DMNX), while the other, more recently evolved in higher mammals, originates in the nucleus ambiguous (NA). The DMNX system is best characterized as a primitive cardiac braking system, the best known example of which is the “diving reflex.” This system may be excited when the organism experiences cold water in the face or chest and greatly 93 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment reduces cardiac speed and output. The NA system is of greater interest within applied psychophysiology because it seems to be involved in heart rate pacing in nonthreatening social situations. This is observed as a rhythmic braking and speeding of the heart rate (or IBI) in respiratory sinus arrhythmia. With inhalation, the vagal braking is removed; thus, heart rate speeds up. Upon exhalation, the vagal brake is reapplied and slows the rate down again. Porges and his colleagues further have reported some evidence that vagus nerve activity, which mediates HF HRV, also reflects social relatedness, both in humans and across species (Grippo, Lamb, Carter, & Porges, 2007). Instrumentation Issues Measurement of HRV starts with measurement of heart rate or IBI. The most reliable means of obtaining an IBI is through the use of an ECG with sampling rates of at least 512 Hz. The interval between R-waves (the electrical representation of ventricular contraction) in milliseconds (ms) is estimated and recorded. This data are then processed as described earlier for use in either measurement or feedback. Often the beat-by-beat record is displayed with a strain gauge respiration trace. It is important to remember that the respiration trace is in arbitrary units. Many commercially available systems derive IBI from a pulse photoplethysmograph (PPG). While this method would not be adequate for research, it seems to work well in clinical biofeedback. One caution: Make sure the pulse amplitude trace is clear and distinct, or all the analyses will be corrupted. ECG electrodes can often be used from the wrists or ankle to wrist, while the PPG sensors are used on the fingers or ear lobe. Most manufacturers include documentation on electrode placement and skin preparation. Glossary Accentuated antagonism. A term that refers to the fact that “ . . . Vagal ‘tone’ predominates over sympathetic tone at rest. Under normal physiological conditions, abrupt parasympathetic stimulation will inhibit tonic sympathetic activation and its effects at rest and during exercise. This response is known as ‘accentuated antagonism’ ”(Olshansky et al., 2008, p. 863). Acidodis. A condition stemming from a buildup of acid or depletion of the alkaline reserve (bicarbonate content) in blood and body tissues. There is an increased concentration of hydrogen ions (i.e., decreased pH). “Hypercapnic acidosis,” also called “respiratory acidosis,” results from excessive retention of C02.“Compensated respiratory acidosis” occurs when the kidneys compensate and raise the low pH toward normal. There also are other causes. Compare with alkalosis (see below). Alkalosis. A condition stemming from a buildup of base or alkali or from a loss of acid without comparable loss of base in body fluids. There is a decreased hydrogen ion concentration (i.e., increased pH). “Respiratory alkalosis” results from excess loss of C02 from the body. “Compensated respiratory alkalosis” occurs when the blood pH returns toward normal by acid retention or kidney mechanisms that excrete base (bicarbonate). There are also other causes. Compare with acidosis (see above). Base. In chemistry, the nonacid part of a salt. It pro- duces hydroxide ions in liquids such as blood. Bicarbonate. A type of salt (HCO3-). “Blood bicarbon- ate” is an index of the alkaline reserve level. Buffer and bicarbonate buffering system. In biochemis- try, a buffer is any chemical system preventing change in concentration of another chemical substance such as hydrogen ion concentration (pH). The kidneys release bicarbonate as part of the bicarbonate buffering system of the body. -capnia. A suffix referring to C02. “Hypocapnia” is low or below-normal C02. “Hypercapnia” is high or above-normal C02. Dyspnea. Labored or difficult breathing. “Functional dyspnea” is dyspnea not related to exercise and without an organic cause. “Sighing intermittent dyspnea” is very deep sighing respirations without a significant change in rate, without wheezing. It has functional or emotional causes rather than organic causes. Hyperpnea. Breathing large volumes of air in each breath. Compare with tachypnea (see below). Hyperventilation (HV). Hyperventilation may be defined as more tidal volume (or total air flow) than is needed for metabolic demands. Alveolar carbon dioxide falls below normal. Hyperventilation syndrome (HVS). A condition where prolonged hyperventilation leads to systemic compensation and the loss of the alkaline buffering system. There is some controversy surrounding this concept. Hypocapnia. See “-Capnia.” Hypoventilation. A condition occurring when there is too little air entering the pulmonary alveoli. Hypoxia. A deficiency of oxygen in tissues. This can occur despite sufficient blood in the tissues. It can occur if not enough oxygen enters the blood, as with decreased barometric pressures at high altitudes. It can also result from decreased oxyhemoglobin in the 94 blood, which is partly a function of the pH of the blood and is affected by fluctuations of C02 and other gases. There are other causes. Isocapnic Overventilation Test (IOT). A technique that artificially prevents lowered percentage of end-tidal C02 (PetC02; see below) during hyperventilation by using a mixture of air enriched with C02. Paresthesia. An abnormal sensation, such as burning or prickling. PetCO2. Percentage end-tidal C02. Ph. The concentration level and ratio of alkalinity to acidity. A pH of 7.35 to 7.45 is neutral for blood. A pH above 7.45 means more alkalinity, and one below 7.35 means more acidity. The symbol pH refers to the hydrogen ion concentration or activity of a solution, such as blood. Photoplethysmograph (PPG). An optically obtained ple- thysmogram, a volumetric measurement of an organ. A PPG is often obtained by using a pulse oximeter which illuminates the skin and measures changes in light absorption. Pulmonary alveoli (or vesicles). Tiny sacs at the ends of the bronchial tree through which gas is exchanged with the pulmonary capillaries. Respiratory sinus arrythmia (RSA). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) is a naturally occurring variation in heart rate that occurs during a breathing cycle. Heart rate increases during inspiration and decreases during expiration. Heart rate is normally controlled by centers in the medulla oblongata. One of these centers, the nucleus ambiguus, increases parasympathetic nervous system input to the heart via the vagus nerve. Tachypnea. Rapid cycles of inhaling and exhaling. Compare with hyperpnea (see above). Torr. A unit of pressure equal to 1 millimeter of mercury (1 mm Hg). It is used in the measurement of ETC02. Valsalva Maneuver. The valsalva maneuver is a forced exhalation with the glottis closed. It substantially increases intra-thoracic pressure and disrupts venous blood returning to the heart. Volumes Minute volume (MV). (1) “Quantity of gas (air) expelled from the lungs per minute” (Dorland's Illustrated Medical Dictionary, 1988, p. 1847); (2) “volume of air expelled from the lungs per minute” (Dox, Melloni, & Eisner, 1979, p. 521); (3) “sum of tidal volumes breathed per minute” (Ley, 1988, p. 253); (4) “volume of air inspired per minute” (Kaufman & Schneiderman, 1986, p. 112). Tidal volume (Vy). Amount of gas inspired and expired (i.e., ventilation) during one respiratory cycle of a normal breath. II. INSTRUMENTATION References Allen, J. P., Imbus, C. E., Powars, D. R., & Haywood, L. J. (1976). Neurological impairment induced by hyperventilation in children with sickle cell anemia. Pediatrics, 58(1), 124–126. Alvarez, S. M., Katsamanis Karavidas, M., Coyle, S. M., Lu, S.-E., Macor, M., Oikawa, L. O., et al. (2007). Lowdose steroid alters in vivo endotoxin-induced systemic inflammation but does not influence autonomic dysfunction. Journal of Endotoxin Research, 13(6), 358–368. Ballard, R. D. (1999). Sleep, respiratory physiology, and nocturnal asthma. Chronobiology International, 16(5), 565–580. Bass, C., & Gardner, W. N. (1985). Respiratory and psychiatric abnormalities in chronic symptomatic hyperventilation. British Medical Journal, 290(6479), 1387–1390. Berkow, R., & Fletcher, A. (Eds.). (1992). The Merck manual of diagnosis and therapy. Rahway, NJ: Merck. Bernardi, L., Leuzzi, S., Radaelli, A., Passino, C., Johnston, J. A., & Sleight, P. (1994). Low-frequency spontaneous fluctuations of R-R interval and blood pressure in conscious humans: A baroreceptor or central phenomenon? Clinical Science (London), 87(6), 649–654. Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Jr., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 623–648. Berntson, G. G., Norman, G. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Cardiac autonomic balance versus cardiac regulatory capacity. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 643–652. Bonn, J. A., Readhead, C. P., & Timmons, B. H. (1984). Enhanced adaptive behavioural response in agoraphobic patients pretreated with breathing retraining. Lancet, 2(8404), 665–669. Buccelletti, E., Gilardi, E., Scaini, E., Galiuto, L., Persiani, R., Biondi, A., et al. (2009). Heart rate variability and myocardial infarction: Systematic literature review and metanalysis. European Review for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences, 13(4), 299–307. Cain, K. C., Jarrett, M. E., Burr, R. L., Hertig, V. L., & Heitkemper, M. M. (2007). Heart rate variability is related to pain severity and predominant bowel pattern in women with irritable bowel syndrome. Neurogastroenterology and Motility, 19(2), 110–118. Carney, R. M., & Freedland, K. E. (2009). Depression and heart rate variability in patients with coronary heart disease. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 76(Suppl. 2), S13–S17. Chen, C. L., Lin, H. H., Orr, W. C., Yang, C. C. H., & Kuo, T. B. J. (2004). Transfer function analysis of heart rate variability in response to water intake: Correlation with gastric myoelectrical activity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 96(6), 2226–2230. Clark, D. M., & Hemsley, D. R. (1982). The effects of hyperventilation; individual variability and its relation to personality. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 13(1), 41–47. 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment Compernolle, T., Hoogduin, K., & Joele, L. (1979). Diagnosis and treatment of the hyperventilation syndrome. Psychosomatics, 20(9), 612–625. Craig, A. B., Jr. (1976). Summary of 58 cases of loss of consciousness during underwater swimming and diving. Medicine and Science in Sports, 8(3), 171–175. Craske, M. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). Nocturnal panic: response to hyperventilation and carbon dioxide challenges. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(3), 302–307. Dorland's illustrated medical dictionary (27th ed.). (1988). Philadelphia: Saunders. Dox, I., Melloni, B., & Eisner, G. M. (1979). Melloni's illustrated medical dictionary. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Elsenbruch, S., & Orr, W. C. (2001). Diarrhea- and constipation-predominant IBS patients differ in postprandial autonomic and cortisol responses. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 96(2), 460–466. Fairchild, K. D., Srinivasan, V., Moorman, J. R., Gaykema, R. P. A., & Goehler, L. E. (2011). Pathogen-induced heart rate changes associated with cholinergic nervous system activation. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 300(2), R330–R339. Fensterheim, H. (1994). Hyperventilation and psychopathology: A clinical perspective. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and psycgological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Folgering, H., & Colla, P. (1978). Some anomalies in the control of paCO2 in patients with a hyperventilation syndrome. Bulliten Europeen de physiopathologie respitatoire, 14, 503–512. Fried, R. (1987). The hyperventilation syndrome: Research and clinical treatment. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Fried, R., Fox, M., & Carlton, R. (1990). Effect of diaphragmatic respiration with end-tidal CO2 biofeedback on respiration, EEG, and seizure frequency in idiopathic epilepsy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 602, 67–96. Fried, R., & Grimaldi, J. (1993). The psychology and physiology of breathing. New York: Springer. Gardner, W. N. (1994). Diagnosis and organic causes of symptomatic hyperventilation. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Garssen, B., De Ruiter, C., & van Dyck, R. (1992). Breathing retraining: A rationale placebo. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 141–153. Gelder, C. M., Hubble, D., & Hetzel, M. R. (1988). Parasympathetic nervous system in nocturnal asthma. British Medical Journal, 297(6645), 424. Gellhorn, E. (1957). Autonomic imbalance and the hypothalamus: Implications for physiology, medicine, psychology, and neuropsychiatry. London: Oxford University Press. Giardino, N. D., Lehrer, P. M., & Feldman, J. M. (2000). The role of oscillations in self-regulation: Their contribution to homeostasis. In D. T. Kenny, J. G. Carlson, F. T. Guigan, & J. L. Sheppard (Eds.), Stress and health: 95 Research and clinical applications (pp. 27–51). Amsterdam: Harwood Academic. Grippo, A. J., Lamb, D. G., Carter, C. S., & Porges, S. W. (2007). Social isolation disrupts autonomic regulation of the heart and influences negative affective behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 62(10), 1162–1170. Guthrie, D., Moeller, T., & Guthrie, R. (1983). Biofeedback and its application to the stabilization and control of diabetes mielitus American Journal of Clinical Biofeedback, 6, 82–87. Guyton, A., & Hall, J. (1995). Textbook of medical physiology (9th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders. Hayoz, D., Bernardi, L., Noll, G., Weber, R., Porret, C. A., Passino, C., et al. (1995). Flow-diameter phase shift. A potential indicator of conduit artery function. Hypertension, 26(1), 20–25. Heilman, K. J., Handelman, M., Lewis, G., & Porges, S. W. (2008). Accuracy of the StressEraserReg. in the detection of cardiac rhythms. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 33(2), 83–89. Hibbert, G., & Pilsbury, D. (1989). Hyperventilation: Is it a cause of panic attacks? British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 805–809. Hill, P. M. (1973). Hyperventilation, breath holding and alveolar oxygen tensions at the breaking point. Respiration Physiology, 19(2), 201–209. Holloway, E. A. (1994). The role of the physiotherapist in the treatment of hyperventilation. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Hornsveld, H., & Garssen, B. (1996). The low specificity of the Hyperventilation Provocation Test. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(5), 435–449. Hornsveld, H., & Garssen, B. (1997). Hyperventilation syndrome: An elegant but scientifically untenable concept. Netherlands Journal of Medicine, 50(1), 13–20. Horsten, M., Ericson, M., Perski, A., Wamala, S. P., Schenck-Gustaffson, K., & Orth-Gomer, K. (1999). Psychosocial factors and heart rate variability in health women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(1), 49–57. Howell, J. B. (1990). Behavioural breathlessness. Thorax, 45(4), 287–292. Huang, Y.-M., Yang, C. C. H., Lai, C. J., & Kuo, T. B. J. (2010). Involvement of sympathetic function in the sleep-related change of gastric myoelectrical activity in rats. Journal of Sleep Research, 19(1, Pt. 2), 192–200. Huston, J. M., & Tracey, K. J. (2011). The pulse of inflammation: Heart rate variability, the cholinergic antiinflammatory pathway and implications for therapy. Journal of Internal Medicine, 269(1), 45–53. Jan, B. U., Coyle, S. M., Oikawa, L. O., Lu, S.-E., Calvano, S. E., Lehrer, P. M., et al. (2009). Influence of acute epinephrine infusion on endotoxin-induced parameters of heart rate variability: A randomized controlled trial. Annals of Surgery, 249(5), 750–756. Kaneko, H., Sakakibara, M., Mitsuma, T., & Morise, K. (1995). Possibility of postprandial electrogastrography for evaluating vagal/nonvagal cholinergic activity in humans, through simultaneous analysis of postpran- 96 dial heart rate variability and serum immunoreactive hormone levels. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 90(4), 603–609. Kaufman, M. P., & Schneiderman, N. (1986). Physiological bases of respiratory psychophysiology. In M. G. H. Coles, E. Donchin, & S. W. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications. New York: Guilford Press. Koponen, H., Alaraisanen, A., Saari, K., Pelkonen, O., Huikuri, H., Raatikainen, M. J. P., et al. (2008). Schizophrenia and sudden cardiac death: A review. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 62(5), 342–345. Kuznetsova, O., & Son’kin, V. (2008). Autonomic control of the respiratory-hemodynamic system in 8- to 11-yearold children with different baroreflex sensitivities. Human Physiology, 34(5), 633–642. Laitio, T., Jalonen, J., Kuusela, T., & Scheinin, H. (2007). The role of heart rate variability in risk stratification for adverse postoperative cardiac events. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 105(6), 1548–1560. Lauer, M. S. (2009). Autonomic function and prognosis. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 76(Suppl. 2), S18– S22. Lehrer, P. M., Hochron, S. M., Mayne, T., Isenberg, S., Lasoski, A. M., Carlson, V., et al. (1997). Relationship between changes in EMG and respiratory sinus arrhythmia in a study of relaxation therapy for asthma. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 22(3), 183–191. Lewis, R. A., & Howell, J. B. L. (1986). Definition of the hyperventilation syndrome. Bulletin of European Clinical Respiratory Physiology, 22, 201–205. Ley, R. (1988). Panic attacks during relaxation-induced anxiety: A hyperventilation interpretation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 19(4), 253–259. Ley, R. (1992). The many faces of Pan: Psychological and physiological differences among three types of panic attacks. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(4), 347–357. Ley, R. (1993). Breathing retraining in the treatment of hyperventilatory complaints and panic disorder: A reply to Garssen, de Ruiter, and van Dyck. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(5), 393–408. Lindh, V., Wiklund, U., Sandman, P. O., & Hakansson, S. (1997). Assessment of acute pain in preterm infants by evaluation of facial expression and frequency domain analysis of heart rate variability. Early Human Development, 48(1-2), 131–142. Lindsay, S., Saqi, S., & Bass, C. (1991). The test–retest reliability of the hyperventilation provocation test. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 35(2–3), 155–162. Lum, C. (1994). Hyperventilation syndromes: Physiological considerations in clinical managment. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and physiological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Lum, L. C. (1975). Hyperventilation: The tip and the iceberg. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 19(5–6), 375– 383. II. INSTRUMENTATION Manzella, D., & Paolisso, G. (2005). Cardiac autonomic activity and Type II diabetes mellitus. Clinical Science, 108(2), 93–99. Marques, A. H., Silverman, M. N., & Sternberg, E. M. (2010). Evaluation of stress systems by applying noninvasive methodologies: Measurements of neuroimmune biomarkers in the sweat, heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. Neuroimmunomodulation, 17(3), 205–208. Nada, T., Nomura, M., Iga, A., Kawaguchi, R., Ochi, Y., Saito, K., et al. (2001). Autonomic nervous function in patients with peptic ulcer studied by spectral analysis of heart rate variability. Journal of Medicine, 32(5-6), 333–347. Naifeh, K. H. (1994). Basic anatomy and physiology of the respiratory system and the autonomic nervous system. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Ed.), Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Nixon, P. G., & Freeman, L. J. (1988). The ‘think test': A further technique to elicit hyperventilation. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 81(5), 277–279. Norris, C. J., Gollan, J., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). The current status of research on the structure of evaluative space. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 422–436. Nunan, D., Sandercock, G. R. H., & Brodie, D. A. (2010). A quantitative systematic review of normal values for short-term heart rate variability in healthy adults. Pacing and Clinical Electrophysiology, 33(11), 1407–1417. Olshansky, B., Sabbah, H. N., Hauptman, P. J., & Colucci, W. S. (2008). Parasympathetic nervous system and heart failure: Pathophysiology and potential implications for therapy. Circulation, 118(8), 863–871. Palma-Rigo, K., Baudrie, V., Laude, D., Petrel, C., Clauser, E., & Elghozi, J.-L. (2010). Cardiovascular rhythms and cardiac baroreflex sensitivity in AT1A receptor gainof-function mutant mice. Chronobiology International, 27(1), 128–137. Politano, L., Palladino, A., Nigro, G., Scutifero, M., & Cozza, V. (2008). Usefulness of heart rate variability as a predictor of sudden cardiac death in muscular dystrophies. Acta Myologica, 27, 114–122. Porges, S. W. (2007). A phylogenetic journey through the vague and ambiguous Xth cranial nerve: A commentary on contemporary heart rate variability research. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 301–307. Ranpuria, R., Hall, M., Chan, C. T., & Unruh, M. (2008). Heart rate variability (HRV) in kidney failure: Measurement and consequences of reduced HRV. Nephrology Dialysis Transplantation, 23(2), 444–449. Rapee, R. M. (1985). A case of panic disorder treated with breathing retraining. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 16(1), 63–65. Salkovskis, P. M., & Clark, D. M. (1990). Affective responses to hyperventilation: A test of the cognitive model of panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(1), 51–61. Sartory, G., & Olajide, D. (1988). Vagal innervation tech- 5. Cardiorespiratory Measurement and Assessment niques in the treatment of panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26(5), 431–434. Shinba, T., Kariya, N., Matsui, Y., Ozawa, N., Matsuda, Y., & Yamamoto, K.-I. (2008). Decrease in heart rate variability response to task is related to anxiety and depressiveness in normal subjects. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62(5), 603–609. Stauss, H. M., Rarick, K. R., Deklotz, R. J., & Sheriff, D. D. (2009). Frequency response characteristics of whole body autoregulation of blood flow in rats. American Journal of Physiology: Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 296(5), H1607–H1616. Stern, R. M., Ray, W. J., & Quigley, K. S. (2001). Psychophysiological recording (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Stifter, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Porges, S. W. (1989). Facial expressivity and vagal tone in 5- and 10-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 12(2), 127–137. Suzuki, H., Watanabe, S., Hamaguchi, T., Mine, H., Terui, T., Kanazawa, M., et al. (2009). Brain activation associated with changes in heart rate, heart rate variability, and plasma catecholamines during rectal distention. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(6), 619–626. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology & the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiol- 97 ogy. (1996). Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. European Heart Journal, 17(3), 354–381. Thayer, J. F. (2009). Vagal tone and the inflammatory reflex. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 76(Suppl. 2), S23–S26. Thayer, J. F., Yamamoto, S. S., & Brosschot, J. F. (2010). The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. International Journal of Cardiology, 141(2), 122–131. Timmons, B. H., & Ley, R. (Eds.). (1994). Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. van Dixhoorn, J., & Duivenvoorden, H. J. (1985). Efficacy of Nijmegen Questionnaire in recognition of the hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29(2), 199–206. Vaschillo, E., Lehrer, P., Rishe, N., & Konstantinov, M. (2002). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a method for assessing baroreflex function: A preliminary study of resonance in the cardiovascular system. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 27(1), 1–27. Yasuma, F., & Hayano, J.-I. (2004). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Why does the heartbeat synchronize with respiratory rhythm? Chest, 125(2), 683–690. Chapter 6 Electroencephalographic Measures and Biofeedback A Primer Nicola Neumann, Ute Strehl, Niels Birbaumer, and Boris Kotchoubey Understanding electroencephalographic (EEG) instrumentation requires a basic knowledge about the recorded parameter. This chapter begins with a description of the main EEG frequency bands, the event-related potentials (ERPs), and their behavioral significance. EEG instrumentation proper is then elucidated, with a particular emphasis on biofeedback practitioners’ interests and questions. potentials heighten this threshold and decrease the probability of a spike. The EEG is largely produced by the EPSP in the pyramidal cells of the upper layers of the cerebral cortex, with some contribution of granular and glia cell activity and, possibly, of IPSP (for reviews, see Creutzfeldt, 1974; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Speckmann & Elger, 1999; see Figure 6.1). Spike potentials that are so important for the function of the nervous system do not have any substantial contribution to the EEG, because rather than summate, they cancel each other. The EEG is characterized by three groups of different (though physiologically interrelated) phenomena: (1) oscillations, or EEG rhythms; (2) evoked potentials (EPs) and ERPs; and (3) slow potential shifts. Almost all these phenomena are generated by the cortex (exceptions are a few EP effects, such as the early auditory EP, which are not of much importance from the biofeedback point of view). However, subcortical structures (particularly the thalamus) also affect to a different extent special characteristics of these cortical phenomena. Neurophysiological Basis of the EEG and Its Behavioral Correlates The EEG results from the summation of electrical potentials of neural cells. There are three groups of such potentials: (1) axonal spike potentials generated according to the “all-or-none” principle, which serve as the main mean of long-distance information transmission in the nervous system; (2) gradual excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP), which are produced by the postsynaptic membrane on dendrites and the soma of a neuron and may, when accumulated, finally lead to generating a spike; and (3) gradual inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSP), also produced in the postsynaptic membrane when specific receptors are stimulated by an inhibitory transmitter such as gamma-aminobutyric acid. In contrast to EPSP, which lower the firing threshold of a neuron, thus increasing the probability of a spike, inhibitory Rhythmic EEG Components The EEG rhythms are defined as regularly recurring waveforms of similar shape and duration. The following EEG rhythms are distinguished by their frequency, shape, and functional meaning. 98 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback 99 FIGURE 6.1. Postsynaptic sum potentials (PSPs) as the potential source of the EEG. Afferent fibers (a, b, c) are shown with connections to an excitatory nerve cell (EC), to a large pyramidal cell (P), and to an inhibitory nerve cell (IC). The course of the potential is depicted in the area of the apical dendrites (1), the basal dendrites (2), the axon hillock (3), and the axon (4). Action potentials (APs) become more frequent with the presentation of a stimulus. At the apical dendrites, the summation of excitatory PSP leads to a negative DC shift, which often does not pass filtering in the routine EEG. From Zschocke (1995). Copyright 1995 by Springer-Verlag Berlin/Heidelberg. Reprinted by permission. Alpha activity (8–13 hertz [Hz]) is characterized by a large amplitude and sinusoidal shape. It is mostly present over posterior cortical areas and related to a wake resting state. Amplitudes of alpha waves vary considerably between individuals and wax and wane over time but are mostly around 50 µV (maximal about 100 µV) in adults. Stimulation (particularly visual) or mental effort lead to its attenuation or suppression, a phenomenon that is referred to as alpha blocking. This blocking, as a sign of cortical activation, should not be confused with “alpha dropout,” observed at the beginning drowsiness when alpha waves become more and more discontinuous, giving way to a low voltage pattern in sleep Stage I. The alpha rhythm belongs to the class of synchronized EEG activity that was originally attributed to thalamocortical circuits (Andersen & Andersson, 1968). However, more recent findings (e.g., Lopes da Silva, Van Lierop, Schrijer, & Storm van Leeuwen, 1973; Lopes da Silva, Vos, Mooibroeck, & Van Rotterdam, 1980, later reviewed by Steriade, 1999, 2006), indicated that the spread of the alpha rhythm is primarily subserved by a system of intracortical connections, whereas visual thalamic nuclei have only a moderate effect. 100 Whereas the alpha rhythm prevails over the occipital and parietal cortex, its analogue over the motor cortex (best pronounced at C3 and C4 cites, 10-20 System, see below) is referred to as mu rhythm, also called Rolandic mu rhythm or sensorimotor rhythm (Sterman, 1973, 1996; Wolpaw, McFarland, Neat, & Forneris, 1991). Its shape is more arc-like in contrast to the sine-like shape of alpha waves, and its frequency is sometimes higher than that in alpha waves (i.e., up to 15 Hz). Its individual variability is even greater than that of the alpha rhythm: It is clearly visible in only 3–14% of subjects (Niedermeyer, 1999b, p. 156), although a frequency analysis reveals it in almost all individuals (Schoppenhorst, Brauer, Freund, & Kubicki, 1980). Just as alpha oscillations reflect the resting state of the posterior sensory areas, the mu rhythm reflects the rest state of the motor areas. A phenomenon functionally very close to the mu rhythm, most apparent during Stages II and III of sleep, are sleep spindles. At the beginning of the biofeedback research, feedback of the alpha rhythm was very popular, because (1) the phenomenon is well pronounced and easy to measure and (2) it was believed that biofeedback-supported alpha increase can lead to the so-called “alpha state” regarded as a specific state of consciousness. Its popularity decreased in the following decades, particularly after Plotkin (1976, 1977) showed that alpha self-regulation is usually attained with peripheral rather than central strategies (e.g., using defocusation of the gaze). Now it is mainly used as a control condition for other kinds of EEG biofeedback or in combination with theta as “alpha–theta feedback.” This protocol rewards both alpha and theta activity in order to reduce stress, anxiety, and substance dependence (for details, see Part VII, this volume). Self-regulation of the mu rhythm is, in contrast, actively used both in treatment for several neurological disorders (epilepsy, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]) and in brain–computer interfaces (BCIs) that capitalize the fact that mu power changes with motor imagery. Beta activity (13–30 Hz) represents the desynchronized state of the EEG mainly recorded during active wakefulness, but also in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (usually accompanying dreams). It consists of a mixture of different frequencies of low amplitude (mostly 5–20 µV). Beta activity is recorded mainly over frontal and central regions. Frontal beta is common and may be very fast (~30 Hz). Central beta is mixed with the Rolandic mu rhythm (discussed earlier) and can be blocked by II. INSTRUMENTATION motor activity or tactile stimulation (Niedermeyer, 1999b). Self-regulation of beta activity is mainly employed in combination with theta (see below), in which patients learn to control the ratio between the two (for details, see Chapter 30, this volume). Gamma activity (30–80 Hz) is usually of low amplitude, thus requiring more specific analytic methods to separate it. The activity appears to have specific cognitive and behavioral correlates (e.g., Basar-Eroglu, Strüber, Schürmann, Stadler, & Basar, 1996; Pulvermüller, Birbaumer, Lutzenberger, & Mohr, 1997; Pulvermüller, Keil, & Elbert, 1999; Tallon-Baudry & Bertrand, 1999). The relation of gamma activity to conscious awareness has been widely disputed. While researchers in the 90s attempted to prove a specific link from conscious information processing to gamma activity, later researchers criticized this point and indicated a rather nonspecific relationship (e.g., Balasz et al., 2006; Steriade, 2006; Vanderwolf, 2000). It is rather gamma synchronization across cortical areas, than merely an increase of gamma activity that may possibly be regarded as a necessary (but not sufficient) precondition of consciousness (e.g., Kaiser, Birbaumer, & Lutzenberger, 2001, 2002; Kaiser, Ripper, Birbaumer, & Lutzenberger, 2003; Meador, Ray, Echauz, Loring, & Vachtsevanos, 2002; Vanderwolf, 2000). From a methodological standpoint, evoked and induced gamma responses to stimulation are distinguished (Galambos, 1992). On the one hand, like any EEG frequency, gamma oscillations can respond to a stimulus by increasing their amplitude. This has a shape of gamma oscillatory bursts whose latency fluctuates from trial to trial and is referred to as an “induced gamma response.” In contrast, an “evoked gamma response” is timelocked to external stimuli such as EPs, which we discuss below. Cortical oscillations faster than gamma are designated ripples (80–200 Hz) or fast ripples (200– 500 Hz). Because their amplitude is very low, they currently have no practical importance, though they probably indicate some aspects of information processing not manifested in other EEG measures (Eschenko, Mölle, Born, & Sara, 2006; Mölle, Yeschenko, Marshall, Sara, & Born, 2006). The delta activity band comprises frequencies from 0.5 to 4 Hz. Thalomocortical relay neurons are involved in the generation of these high-voltage, low-frequency waves (Steriade, 2006). Diffuse delta activity is associated with deep sleep in 101 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback healthy humans, as well as pathological conditions such as coma. Local delta waves are recorded over massive brain lesions. Theta activity (4–8 Hz) is rarely dominant but almost always present in the adult EEG. In early childhood (between 12 and 36 months) it is the basic rhythm of wakefulness over the posterior cortex. During the third year it moves into the alpha range, but intermingled mild-to-moderate theta activity at posterior scalp locations is still seen in young adults until the age of 30 (Niedermeyer, 1999a). There are probably two main types of theta activity (Basar, Schürmann, & Sakowitz, 2001; Klimesch, 1999; Schacter, 1977) A more diffuse theta is associated with drowsiness and is clearly discerned during transition from wakefulness to sleep, while a more frontally located theta rhythm is associated with stress and problem solving (Asada, Fukuda, Tsunoda, Yamaguchi, & Tonoike, 1999; Jensen & Tesche, 2002; Klimesch, 1999; Mizuki, Tanaka, Isozaki, Nishijima, & Inanaga, 1980). Theta activity related to active problem solving also dominates the activity in the hippocampus in mammals, but there is controversy over whether this hippocampal activity is related to frontal theta in humans (Steriade, Gloor, Llinás, Silva, & Mesulam, 1990). Some experimental results suggest that the master structure controlling at least one type of theta activity is the septohippocampal cholinergic system, driven from the brainstem reticular core (Steriade, 1999). seconds prior to movement onset; Pfurtscheller & Aranibar, 1977). After termination of the movement, the mu rhythm recovers (event-related synchronization [ERS]), indicating the immobilization or “idling” (Kuhlmann, 1978) of the pyramidal motor system. Pfurtscheller (1989) suggested that a variety of rhythms within the alpha and beta bands are attenuated when cortical structures become activated either during internal or external events. Evoked and Event‑Related EEG Changes Changes in the activity of neuronal populations time-locked to a specific event, such as a sensory stimulus, are traditionally studied with ERPs, which are deflections in the EEG that have a fixed time delay to the stimulus, while the ongoing EEG activity (i.e., the oscillations not time-locked to the event) is regarded as additive noise. To detect ERPs, averaging techniques are used (see Figure 6.2); that is, many (this number, N, can vary from dozens to thousands depending on the task) EEG segments time-locked to a particular stimulus are summated and the result is divided by N. In this way, all noise oscillations (i.e., those that are not phase-locked to the stimulus) are suppressed, while average EEG Synchronization and Desynchronization 1 Generally, the normal adult waking EEG can be classified into two main patterns. The synchronized EEG pattern is characterized by rhythmic, high-amplitude, low-frequency activity, while the desynchronized pattern comprises lower voltage and irregular higher frequency waves. Alpha, mu, theta, and delta rhythms all characterize different kinds and degrees of synchronization, whereas the beta rhythm is typical for desynchronized states. A synchronized alpha rhythm is measured in a relaxed, eyes-closed state, while desynchronization is recorded during visual stimulation and visual attention. In contrast to the alpha rhythm, the mu rhythm does not block with eye opening but with a real, imagined, or intended movement of the contralateral limb (event-related desynchronization [ERD]; Pfurtscheller & Berghold, 1989). Interestingly, the desynchronization of the mu rhythm has an anticipatory nature (i.e., it starts about 1–2 3 4 2 5 6 7 8 9 0 15 30 45 60 75 90 10 FIGURE 6.2. Illustration of the process of averaging. The lines 1 to 10 present simulated data on 10 different individual trials. As can be seen, the waveforms vary widely. A very small peak shortly before 30 ms on the time axis is present in each trial but could hardly be seen without the arrow pointing at it. The bold line on the top is the result of averaging the 10 trials. The peak before 30 ms, although almost invisible in individual trials, now becomes the most prominent, sharp, and salient component of the waveform. 102 phase-locked ERP oscillations are highlighted (Picton & Hillyard, 1988). An averaged ERP comprises a series of large, biphasic waves, in total lasting 500–1000 ms. Although the nomenclature of these waves is far from being logically clear and unified, the ERP components are usually described with a letter P or N, designating polarity (positive or negative, respectively), followed by either the serial number (components P1, N1, P2, N2, P3), or the typical peak latency in milliseconds (N270, P300, N400). The earliest portion of an ERP, usually called evoked potential (EP), is modality specific and comprises the waves, which are immediately related to the stimulus and whose amplitudes and latencies are primarily determined by the physical qualities of the stimulus (e.g., intensity and duration). This early portion can last for some 50 ms (somatosensory EP), 100–110 ms (auditory EP), or 140–150 ms (visual EP), depending on how fast the excitation in a particular sensory modality is transmitted from the receptors to the sensory cortical areas. From a practical point of view, EPs are mainly used for differential diagnosis of sensory disorders, such as deafness and blindness, as well as for the diagnosis of clinical death. With increasing latency, ERP components are less and less influenced by simple physical stimulus qualities, and more and more by the task performed by a subject. It should be noted that the boundary between the EP and the “ERP proper” is not exact, and that the primary cortical responses to stimuli, such as P1 (with a latency of about 50 and 100 ms, for auditory and visual modality, respectively) and N1 (with a latency of about 100 and 150 ms, for auditory and visual modality, respectively), are described sometimes as “EP components,” and other times as “early ERP components.” The following positive component P2 with a latency of 180–240 ms is usually the last obligatory (“exogenous”) response in the chain of oscillations elicited by any stimulus. Other components, some of them starting as early as between 50 and 200 ms, as well as all the later deflections, are referred to as “endogenous” components, because they may or may not be present, depending on the activity of the subject. In contrast to EP components indicating information transfer to the cortex, endogenous ERP components manifest information processing in the cortex and are therefore used to assess changes in this processing in neurological and psychiatric patients (e.g., Kotchoubey, 2006; Kotchoubey, Lang, Bostanov, & Birbaumer, 2002). II. INSTRUMENTATION The best known example is the parietal positive deflection P3 (Donchin & Coles, 1988; Verleger, 1988) related to most complex processing operations, perhaps even a correlate of conscious awareness of stimulus (review Kotchoubey, 2005, but see Shevrin, 2001). A nice demonstration of the purely endogenous nature of such components includes experiments in which rare irregular omissions were introduced in a sequence of otherwise regularly presented stimuli, and participants were instructed to attend to the absent stimuli (Ito, Kitagawa, & Kimura, 1997; Picton & Hillyard, 1988). The components N1 and P2 were elicited by all stimuli, but only P3 was observed 400–500 ms after omissions, although no physical stimulus at all was presented! The model assuming that an ERP can be represented by a signal added to uncorrelated noise is a simplification. Induced changes in the EEG rhythmic activity such as ERS and ERD, which are related but not phase-locked to stimuli, make the situation more complex. Thus, if EEG rhythms were additive noise, the quality of the ERP signal would be proportional to √N (N = number of averaged trials), but in the real situation, it is always somewhat worse than this. However, the signal-tonoise ratio model can be regarded as an approximation that, though theoretically wrong, is satisfactory from the practical point of view. Slow Cortical Potentials Slow cortical potentials (SCPs) are EEG changes lasting from 300 ms to several seconds. They are frequently observed as time-locked to stimuli or subjects’ movements and thus regarded as slow components of ERPs. The most important are two slow negative deflections: the bereitschaftspotential (BP; Kornhuber & Deecke, 1965), a negativity starting 500–1500 ms before a voluntary movement and reflecting movement preparation in the supplementary motor area; and the contingent negative variation (CNV; Walter, Cooper, Aldridge, McCallum, & Winter, 1964), recorded between a warning stimulus (S1) and the following imperative stimulus (S2) in an S1–S2 task in which participants should respond to S2. Whereas the BP mainly reflects the activity of the supplementary motor area related to preparation and timing of a motor action, the CNV manifests more complex processes of expectancy and general preparation to an important event (which may but does not necessarily include a movement). 103 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback It must be said, however, that SCPs do not really belong to ERPs, because the same slow shifts we record as linked to stimuli and movements also occur continuously in all functional states from active wakefulness to deep sleep, regardless of whether we are stimulated or not. They underlie all other, faster and more regular oscillations from delta to gamma (listed earlier) and reflect a mechanism of threshold regulation for local excitatory mobilization. Negative SCP shifts, such as the BP or the CNV, indicate local preparation of cortical networks to anticipated activity, whereas positive potential shifts indicate either the actual activity or the following disfacilitation. A consistent relationship between cortical negativity and reaction time, signal detection, and short-term memory performance has been found in many studies in humans and monkeys (e.g., Bauer, 1984; Birbaumer, Elbert, Lutzenberger, Rockstroh, & Schwarz, 1981; Lutzenberger, Roberts, & Birbaumer, 1993; Rockstroh, Elbert, Lutzenberger, & Birbaumer, 1982; Siegel, Sterman, & Ross, 1979). Tasks requiring attention are performed significantly better when presented during spontaneous or self-induced cortical negativity (summarized in Birbaumer, 1997; Birbaumer, Elbert, Canavan, & Rockstroh, 1990; Rockstroh, Elbert, Canavan, Lutzenberger, & Birbaumer, 1989). The possibility of SCP biofeedback was simultaneously demonstrated by Siegel et al. (1979) in animals and by Lutzenberger, Elbert, Rockstroh, and Birbaumer (1979) and Elbert, Birbaumer, Lutzenberger, and Rockstroh (1979) in humans. Since then it has been applied for treatment of epilepsy (see Chapter 37, this volume) and ADHD, reported in several controlled studies (e.g., Kotchoubey et al., 2001; Rockstroh et al., 1993; Strehl et al., 2006). Instrumentation and Recording Although procedures for recording EEG activity have improved greatly over the past decades with the incorporation of computer-controlled and digital amplifiers, there is still a need to consider carefully how EEG is recorded. Possible artifacts occur at every step of the recording procedure from the electrodes to the recording system, and interfering electrical potentials can be easily mistaken for the proper EEG signal. We describe specific problems in the corresponding sections of the chapter. Electrodes Inside the brain tissue, electric charges are transported by ions (i.e., electrically charged atoms). These charges have to be transmitted to the recording system. For this transmission, a conductive electrolyte paste is required. The electrodes are directly in contact with the electrolyte, and ions move across the boundary. Initially after fixing the electrodes, electric charges move and generate an unstable signal. When a balance is reached, a charged double layer is formed between the electrolyte and the metal surface of the electrode. It corresponds to a direct current (DC) voltage source with the electric potential difference, referred to as “electrode potential.” It is important that the electrode potential is stable after a few minutes, which is warranted by clean nonpolarizable electrodes. The lack of polarization is essential when slow EEG components such as P3 or SCP should be fed back. The electrode potential should be the same for all electrodes. Therefore, although electrodes may generally be made out of different metals, all electrodes in a recording system must be of the same material. Disturbances of the potential gradient may be caused by alterations in temperature, sweating, or mechanical displacement of the electrodes (see below). Electrode Placement The system of locating electrodes is referred to as the International 10-20 Recording System (Jasper, 1958), and originally comprised 19 electrodes (see Figure 6.3). The term “10-20” derives from the fact that electrodes are placed at sites in a 10 or 20% distance from four anatomical landmarks. There are two landmarks at the front (nasion, the bridge of the nose) and the back (inion, the bump at the back of the head), and two landmarks on the right and left sides of the head (preauricular points, depressions in front of the ears above the cheekbones). Between these landmarks, electrode positions are determined by measuring distances 10 and 20%, respectively, of the total distance between the landmarks. The standard numbering system in the 10-20 system places odd-numbered electrodes on the left and even-numbered electrodes on the right, with the letter designating the anatomical area. Such electrode placement can be replicated consistently over time, as well as between laboratories. The American EEG Society 104 ii. instruMentAtion fiGure 6.3. International 10–20 Electrode Placement System. From Jasper (1958). Copyright 1958 by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. Reprinted by permission. (1991) added electrode placement nomenclature guidelines that designate specific locations and identifications of 75 electrode positions. Which and how many electrodes are used depends very much on the research question or clinical considerations. For further information, see Reilly (1999). The Issue of Electrode Reference Many EEG handbooks distinguish between monopolar and bipolar recordings. From the physical point of view, all EEG recordings are bipolar, because EEG potentials are always recorded as the voltage difference between two sites. However, in practical use, we speak about bipolar recording when we build electrode pairs (e.g., left hemispheric leads are recorded against the symmetrical electrodes on the right side, or anterior against posterior electrodes), and we speak about monopolar recordings when using a single voltage point (i.e., the reference) against which all the remaining electrode voltages are measured. Ideally, an electrically silent position should be chosen for the reference. In fact, there is no such place. The following solutions have been proposed: 1. A single cephalic reference site remote from the brain (e.g., the nose or the chin). The problem of the latter is the presence of strong muscles and the possibility of movement artifacts. The nose is more appropriate but, unfortunately, some patients do not like having an electrode on their nose. Measured EEG voltage is always directly related to the distance between the two measurement points. Therefore, nose reference creates a small but substantial bias in that the amplitudes recorded at posterior sites are larger than those at anterior sites. 2. A cephalic reference point with a maximal activity. The idea is the opposite (i.e., if it is impossible to find a point of silence, to place the reference in the epicenter of the quake). Thus, some authors refer their recording to the vertex, but the resulting voltages do not reflect the activity under the corresponding electrodes but instead the difference between this activity and the maximal activity at vertex. 3. A single noncephalic reference (e.g., the clavicle) is attractive, because the farther from the brain, the more neutral the condition. However, noncephalic reference sites pick up more electromyographic and particularly electrocardiographic activity than electrodes placed on the head. 4. Average references. The most common are the linked ear lobe or mastoid reference. To avoid a strong asymmetry, which would appear if we use only one-side reference, the two symmetrical sites were earlier electrically linked. This technique, however, is related to some electrotechnical problems due to unequal resistance. Now these problems are avoided, because instead of wiring together the two earlobe (or mastoid) electrodes, 105 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback their voltages are online averaged during recording. The disadvantage of the ear reference, and especially the mastoid reference (the prominence behind the ear), is their contamination by the temporobasal activity of the brain. For this reason, mastoid reference should be avoided whenever one is interested in the topographical distribution of the EEG voltages; neither mastoid nor earlobe references should be used if one supposes that the activity of temporal lobes might be of importance. To avoid these disadvantages, a final option is averaging, not across two symmetrical sites, but across all recording sites: the so-called “common average references” (CARs). In the ideal case, when the entire head is covered by equally spaced electrodes and the potential on the head is generated by point sources, the CAR should result in a spatial voltage distribution with a mean of zero. In the real case, the obtained values are differences from the momentary mean over the scalp. Other average references, quite useful in EEG feedback, employ spatial filtering of the signal. Most common of these are varieties of Laplacian methods, in which the voltage at each electrode is referred to the average of the surrounding electrodes. The recorded signal is therefore the gradient indicating how much the given site differs from its environment. In a paradigmatic study, McFarland, McCane, David, and Wolpaw (1997) fed back the 8–12 Hz mu rhythm and compared a conventional ear reference, a CAR and two Laplacian methods differing in the radius of the environment used for calculating the reference. The CAR and the large Laplacian (6 cm circle of surrounding electrodes) showed a significantly better signal-to-noise ratio than did the ear reference and the smaller Laplacean method. Finally, the development of the average reference technique has led to a discovery of methods for localization of the sources of electrical activity in the brain. The best of these methods, called LOw REsolution Electromagnetic TomogrAphy (LORETA), can only be indicated here, since its detailed description would require a separate chapter (see Pascual-Marqui, 2008; Pascual-Marqui, Esslen, Kochi, & Lehmann, 2002). To date, there have already been successful attempts to combine LORETA with neurofeedback, selectively presenting to the patient the neural activity originated from a particular area in the brain (Cannon, Congedo, Lubar, & Hutchen, 2009; Congedo, Lubar, & Joffe, 2004). Amplification and Filters In modern amplifiers, only signal differences between the two inputs of the amplifier are processed. By this means, the technical noise primarily caused by 60 Hz (in Europe: 50 Hz) main power and arriving at both amplifier inputs is cancelled. The common mode rejection accomplishes the same purpose, excluding signals swinging in-phase. Thus, the common mode rejection ratio, defined as the ratio between amplification of outof-phase signals and (residual) amplification of inphase signals, constitutes a quality characteristic of amplifiers. It is important that the amplifier is able to record all desired EEG frequencies from slow waves up to 100 Hz. Many commercially available systems, however, cannot record slow potential shifts and high-frequency rhythms. The limited frequency range amplified adequately by a certain type of amplifier is called its “bandwidth.” Often, unwanted frequency ranges are additionally suppressed by filters. The high pass filter determines the lower frequency limit; it lets pass higher frequencies and attenuates lower ones. It is called the “time constant” and describes the time an alternating current (AC) signal needs to decay to twothirds of its initial amplitude (Figure 6.4). The low pass filter determines the upper frequency limit: Lower frequencies can pass and higher ones are attenuated. Notch filters set at 60 Hz (in Europe: 50 Hz) should be carefully considered, as they are not sharp and may remove frequencies of activity below and above the unwanted frequency. Artifacts Artifacts are signals that do not originate in the brain (i.e., are not a part of the EEG) but are recorded together with the EEG signal and can imitate some of the EEG phenomena described earlier (oscillations, ERPs, or SCPs). One can distinguish between technical and biological EEG artifacts. The former are caused by ambient technical devices, mostly by electromagnetic fields present in our technical environment and produced by networks and the numerous electrical devices. They are minimized or completely avoided when the impedance between EEG electrodes and the skin is low. Early EEG standards presumed impedance below 5 kOhm. This is a good value to be attained, although modern EEG amplifier are electrically shielded to such an extent that recordings free from technical artifacts can even be obtained 106 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 6.4. High-pass and low-pass filtering and its influence on a rectangular calibration signal. The high-pass filter is also referred to as time constant (tc) and determines the time an AC signal needs to decay two-thirds of its initial amplitude. From Zschocke (1995). Copyright 1995 by Springer-Verlag Berlin/Heidelberg. Reprinted by permission. with impedance between 10 and 20 kOhm. An electrode paste (usually containing sodium chloride as the electrolyte) is applied to the skin under each electrode after fat and dead skin are removed with alcohol, acetone, or an abrasive cream. A similar procedure is used with electrode helmets in which a blunted needle is used to move the hair away from the electrode and a syringe-like device is used to inject electrode paste between the electrode and the scalp. Electrodes and instruments should be disinfected after each usage. Although some decades ago all EEG examinations were performed in Faraday boxes, nowadays patients can be examined or trained at their bedside and also at home, if necessary. However, sometimes the surrounding noise from electrical devices may be so strong that artifacts remain even with very low electrode impedance. In such a case, unnecessary devices should be switched off. Noise can increase due to ground loops when different devices are connected to different grounds. Therefore, one single ground and one common socket should be used. In contrast to technical artifacts, biological artifacts result from the electrical activity of bodily organs others than the brain, most importantly (cranial) muscles and eyes. These artifacts can easily be mistaken for EEG activity. Vertical eye movements (Figure 6.5) are the most frequent artifacts occurring in EEG recording. The neurons in the retina generate electrical potentials that constitute an electric dipole, with the inner (caudal) side of the bulbus being negative. Eye movements lead to a change of the dipole, thereby influencing potential changes recorded in the EEG. Blink potentials usually occur in the absence of ocular rotation. The eyelid, like a sliding electrode, picks up a positive potential when moving over the positively charged cornea. Eye movements and blinks can be measured by electrooculography (EOG), recording potential differences from electrodes placed around the eyes. Before an EEG pattern can be interpreted or fed back, ocular influences have to be removed from it. A common method to account for ocular potentials picked up by the EEG electrodes is to subtract a fraction of the EOG. There are several EOG correction algorithms. Unfortunately, the best and most common algorithms (e.g., Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1983) are applied off line and cannot, therefore, be used to correct EEG signals during feedback. For neurofeedback purposes, therefore, there are more 107 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback FIGURE 6.5. Effects of eye movement on the EEG. Arrows indicate blinks. From Zschocke (1995). Copyright 1995 by Springer-Verlag Berlin/Heidelberg. Reprinted by permission. approximate algorithms (e.g., Kotchoubey et al., 1996) available that have demonstrated reliability. Generally, the effect of biological artifacts on the EEG is directly related to the distance between the artifact source and the EEG recording site; therefore, eye movements particularly interfere the EEG at frontal sites. Having a frequency of about 5 Hz, they can imitate theta waves or, if stimulus-locked, the P3 wave of the ERP. Muscle activity and movements also lead to artifacts in the EEG (Figure 6.6). When patients are instructed to relax and not to move, artifacts may still originate in muscles that are close to the recording electrodes (e.g., frontalis, orbicularis oculi, and temporalis muscles). Neck tensions lead to artifacts at occipital sites. On the one hand, muscle tension produces high-frequency oscillations, thus interfering with EEG beta and gamma rhythms. Sometimes, therefore, the increment of muscle tonus is interpreted as an increase in beta or gamma power (Whitham et al., 2007). On the other hand, swallowing, tongue movements, and breathing cause low-frequency artifacts. To recognize and remove muscular artifacts, it is useful to compare muscle-rich recording sites (e.g., temporal) with muscle-poor ones (e.g., vertex) (Lutzenberger, Preissl, Birbaumer, & Pulvermüller, 1997). Another source of biological artifacts is changes in skin resistance that may be caused by not only sweating but also psychological factors such as stress. In the EEG, changes of skin resistance are reflected by slow potential shifts referred to as “drifts.” Especially when slow brain activity is recorded, drifts can be misinterpreted as SCPs. Digitizing After proper recording and amplification of the bioelectric measures, the signals can be fed into the analog to digital (A/D) converter of a digital computer. The A/D converter measures the voltage at regular intervals (e.g., every 10 ms). The frequency of measurements per second is referred to as “sampling rate.” The original analog signal can be reconstructed from the digital information (i.e., numerical values) only if the original signal does not contain any frequency above half of the sampling frequency. Otherwise, the occurrence 108 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 6.6. Muscle artifacts in the EEG. From Zschocke (1995). Copyright 1995 by Springer-Verlag Berlin/ Heidelberg. Reprinted by permission. of fast frequencies would simulate oscillations on the lower frequency range. This effect is called aliasing. Therefore, an adequate low-pass filter is mandatory for the input of the signal into a digital computer (i.e., prior to A/D conversion). EEG Biofeedback: How to Choose the Right Program The effectiveness of the chosen device has to be evaluated on several dimensions: 1. Validity of the feedback parameter. The field of application (see Part VI, this volume) should be specific. The decision to change (e.g., the amount of certain spectral power frequencies) has to be substantiated theoretically and empirically, and the empirical evidence should rely on controlled studies. For example, if it appears useful to apply SCP feedback, the appropriate electrodes should be chosen, and the amplifier must have a long (at least several seconds) time constant (Bauer, Korunka, & Leodolter, 1989). Baseline data should be representative, and the feedback signal should be easily understood. 2. Reliability of the instrumentation. To reduce artifacts (discussed earlier) the filtering capacities have to be considered carefully. The susceptibility to behaviorally or physiologically induced artifacts should be checked in test sessions by the therapist. Electrical interference from the instrumentation, as well as from the environment, may distort the signal. The feedback signal should be as accurate and stable as possible, because the patient’s learning depends on this information. This holds especially for EEG feedback as compared with feedback of peripheral parameters, since humans normally cannot perceive their brain waves. To ensure accuracy, the calibration of the instrument has to be checked; for stability reasons, the power source (in the case of batteries or accumulators) has to be controlled. 3. Flexibility of the software. The software should allow adjustment to individual characteristics. If the aim of therapy is to obtain changes in frequency bands, it should be possible to define the target (e.g., the “alpha spectrum” of a specific patient). When progress is lacking, the therapist must be able to lower thresholds for positive feedback to initiate shaping (or even prompting) procedures. A selection of different screen surfaces improves the motivation in some cases; in other cases it might be important that the screen surface does not interfere with the task. Access to sophisticated data processing and data storage can be an important feature if more parameters are of interest. For instance, it might be useful to analyze the consequences of feedback of slow cortical potentials on the power spectra. Glossary A/D converter. Conversion of analog to digital signals. Aliasing. A potential error in the conversion of ana- logue to digital signals; variation in fast EEG activity may mimic slow potential changes if the original signal contains frequencies above half of the sampling frequency. 109 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback Alpha activity. EEG frequency band from 8 to 13 Hz. Beta activity. EEG frequency band from 13 to 30 Hz. Bipolar recording. EEG derivation using pairs of electri- cally active electrodes. Brain–computer interface (BCI). A system that directly connects some brain function (e.g., the amplitude of the alpha rhythm) to a computer, without an intermediate muscle-driven device such as keyboard or mouse. Rolandic mu rhythm. EEG rhythm of the sensorimotor areas around 10 Hz. Signal-to-noise ratio. Intensity of the signal of interest compared to the background noise. Slow cortical potential (SCP). Direct current shift of less than 1 Hz. Thalamus. Diencephalic brain structure; a major relay center for both sensory and motor signals. Theta activity. EEG frequency band from 4 to 7 Hz. Common mode rejection ratio. The ratio between ampli- fication of out-of-phase signals and (residual) amplification of in-phase signals. Delta activity. EEG frequency band from 0.5 to 4 Hz. Electroencephalography (EEG). Recording, amplifica- tion, and analysis of the electrical activity of the brain. Electrooculography (EOG). Recording of electrical sig- nals evoked by eye movement. desynchronization (ERD). Amplitude attenuation or blocking of rhythmic components within the alpha and beta bands time-locked (not necessarily phase-locked) to an internal or external event. Event-related Event-related potential (ERP). A series of deflections in the EEG time- and phase-locked to an internally or externally paced event. Event-related synchronization (ERS). The opposite of ERD; rhythmic, high amplitude, low-frequency brain activity time-locked to an internal or external event. Evoked potential (EP). Comprises the earlier, obligatory and exogenous ERP components. Excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP). Potential that arises behind the junctional site between two nerve cells, or between a nerve cell and an effector cell. Gamma activity. EEG frequency band from 30 to 100 Hz. Glia cell. Nonconducting cell that serves as support cell in the nervous system and helps to protect neurons. Inhibitory postsynaptic potential (ISPS). Potential that is produced by an inhibitory transmitter. International 10-20 recording system. System of locat- ing electrodes at sites in a 10 or 20% distance from four anatomical landmarks. Laplacian method. EEG derivation method; the value at each electrode location is calculated by subtracting the value at that location from the values of a set of surrounding electrodes. Monopolar recording. EEG derivation with a single ref- erence site. Pyramidal cell. Vertically oriented cell in the cerebral cortex with a pyramid-shaped soma. References American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. (1991). Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 8, 200–202. Andersen, P., & Andersson, S. A. (1968). Physiological basis of the alpha rhythm. New York: Meredith Corporation. Asada, H., Fukuda, Y., Tsunoda, S., Yamaguchi, M., & Tonoike, M. (1999). Frontal midline theta rhythm reflects alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 274, 29–32. Balasz, S., Stepan, C., Binder, H., von Gizycki, H., Avitable, M., Obersteiner, A., et al. (2006). Conjugate eye movements and gamma power modulation of the EEG in persistent vegetative state. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 246(1), 65–69. Basar, E., Schürmann, M., & Sakowitz, O. (2001). The selectively distributed theta system: Functions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 39, 197–212. Basar-Eroglu, C., Strüber, D., Schürmann, M., Stadler, M., & Basar, E. (1996). Gamma-band responses in the brain: A short review of psychophysiological correlates and functional significance. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 101–112. Bauer, H. (1984). Regulation of slow brain potentials affects task performance. In T. Elbert (Ed.), Self-regulation of brain and behaviour (pp. 216–226). Berlin: Springer. Bauer, H., Korunka, C., & Leodolter, M. (1989). Technical requirements for high-quality scalp DC recordings. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 545–547. Birbaumer, N. (1997). Slow cortical potentials: Their origin, meaning, and clinical use. In G. J. M. van Boxtel & K. B. E. Böcker (Eds.), Brain and behavior past, present and future (pp. 25–39). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press. Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Canavan, A. G. M., & Rockstroh, B. (1990). Slow potentials of the cerebral cortex and behavior. Physiological Reviews, 70(1), 1–41. Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Lutzenberger, W., Rockstroh, B., & Schwarz, J. (1981). EEG and slow cortical potentials in anticipation of mental tasks with different hemispheric involvement. Biological Psychology, 13, 251–260. Cannon, R., Congedo, M., Lubar, J., & Hutchen, T. (2009). Differentiating a network of executive atten- 110 tion: LORETA neurofeedback in anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(3), 404–441. Congedo, M., Lubar, J. F., & Joffe, D. (2004). Low-resolution electromagnetic tomography neurofeedback. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, 12(4), 387–397. Creutzfeldt, O. D. (1974). The neuronal generation of the EEG. In A. Remond (Ed.), Handbook of electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology (Vol. 2B, pp. 1–55). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. H. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 357–374. Elbert, T., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., & Rockstroh, B. (1979). Biofeedback of slow cortical potentials: Selfregulation of central-autonomic patterns. In N. Birbaumer & H. Kimmel (Eds.), Biofeedback and self-regulation (pp. 321–337). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eschenko, O., Mölle, M., Born, J., & Sara, S. J. (2006). Elevated sleep spindle density after learning or after retrieval in rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(50), 12914– 12920. Galambos, R. (1992). A comparison of certain gamma band (40-Hz) brain rhythms in cat and man. In E. Basar & T. H. Bullock (Eds.), Induced rhythms in the brain (pp. 201–216). Boston: Birkhäuser. Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifacts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484. Ito, J., Kitagawa, J., & Kimura, I. (1997). Event-related potentials (ERPs) associated with omitted somatosensory stimuli. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 37, 169–176. Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten–twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10(2), 371–375. Jensen, O., & Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1395–1399. Kaiser, J., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (2001). Event-related beta desynchronization indicates timing of response selection in a delayed-response paradigm in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 312, 149–152. Kaiser, J., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (2002). Magnetic oscillatory responses to lateralization changes of natural and artificial sounds in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 345–354. Kaiser, J., Ripper, B., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (2003). Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. NeuroImage, 20, 816–827. Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169–195. Kornhuber, H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegun- II. INSTRUMENTATION gen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pflügers Archiv für gesamte Physiologie, 284, 1–17. Kotchoubey, B. (2005). Event-related potential measures of consciousness: Two equations with three unknowns. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 427–444. Kotchoubey, B. (2006). Event-related potentials, cognition, and behavior: A biological approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 42–65. Kotchoubey, B., Lang, S., Bostanov, V., & Birbaumer, N. (2002). Is there a mind?: Psychophysiology of unconscious patients. News in Physiological Sciences, 17, 38–42. Kotchoubey, B., Schneider, D., Schleichert, H., Strehl, U., Uhlmann, C., Blankenhorn, V., et al. (1996). Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials in epilepsy: A retrial with analysis of influencing factors. Epilepsy Research, 25, 269–276. Kotchoubey, B., Strehl, U., Uhlmann, C., Holzapfel, S., König, M., Fröscher, W., et al. (2001). Modification of slow cortical potentials in patients with refractory epilepsy: A controlled outcome study. Epilepsia, 42(3), 406–416. Kuhlmann, W. (1978). Functional topography of the human mu rhythm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 44, 83–93. Lopes da Silva, F. (1991). Neural mechanisms underlying brain waves: From neural membranes to networks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 79, 81–93. Lopes da Silva, F., Van Lierop, T. H. M. T., Schrijer, C. F. M., & Storm van Leeuwen, W. (1973). Organization of thalamic and cortical alpha rhythm: Spectra and coherences. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35, 627–639. Lopes da Silva, F., Vos, J. E., Mooibroeck, J., & Van Rotterdam, A. (1980). Relative contribution of intracortical and thalamo-cortical processes in the generation of alpha rhythms, revealed by partial coherence analysis. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 50, 449–456. Lutzenberger, W., Elbert, T., Rockstroh, B., & Birbaumer, N. (1979). The effects of self-regulation of slow cortical potentials on performance in a signal detection task. International Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 175–183. Lutzenberger, W., Preissl, H., Birbaumer, N., & Pulvermüller, F. (1997). High-frequency cortical responses: Do they not exist if they are small? Electroencephalography and Clinical Electrophysiology, 102, 64–66. Lutzenberger, W., Roberts, L. E., & Birbaumer, N. (1993). Memory performance and area-specific self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: Dual-task interference. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 217–226. McFarland, D. J., McCane, L. M., David, S. V., & Wolpaw, J. R. (1997). Spatial filter selection for EEG-based communication. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 103, 386–394. Meador, K. J., Ray, P. G., Echauz, J. R., Loring, D. W., & Vachtsevanos, G. J. (2002). Gamma coherence and conscious perception. Neurology, 59, 847–854. 6. EEG Measures and Biofeedback Mizuki, Y., Tanaka, O., Isozaki, H., Nishijima, H., & Inanaga, K. (1980). Periodic appearance of theta rhythm in the frontal midline during performance of a mental task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 49, 345–351. Molle, M., Yeschenko, O., Marshall, L., Sara, S. J., & Born, J. (2006). Hippocampal sharp wave-ripples linked to slow oscillations in rat slow-wave sleep. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 62–70. Niedermeyer, E. (1999a). Maturation of the EEG: Development of waking and sleep patterns. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (4th ed., pp. 189–214). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Niedermeyer, E. (1999b). The normal EEG of the waking adult. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (4th ed., pp. 149–173). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2008). LORETA: Low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography. Retrieved from www.unizh.ch/keyinst/newloreta/loreta01.htm. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K., & Lehmann, D. (2002). Functional imaging with low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): A review. Methods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, 24(Suppl. C), 91–95. Pfurtscheller, G. (1989). Functional topography during sensorimotor activation studied with event-related desynchronization mapping. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 6, 75–84. Pfurtscheller, G., & Aranibar, A. (1977). Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, 817–826. Pfurtscheller, G., & Berghold, A. (1989). Patterns of cortical activation during planning of voluntary movement. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 250–258. Picton, T. W., & Hillyard, S. A. (1988). Endogenous eventrelated potentials. In T. W. Picton (Ed.), Human eventrelated potentials (Vol. 3, pp. 361–426). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Plotkin, W. B. (1976). On the self-regulation of the occipital alpha-rhythm: Control strategies, states of consciousness, and the role of physiological feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 195, 66–99. Plotkin, W. B. (1977). On the social psychology of experimental states associated with EEG alpha biofeedback training. In J. Beatty & H. Legewie (Eds.), Biofeedback and behavior (pp. 121–133). New York/London: Plenum Press. Pulvermüller, F., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., & Mohr, B. (1997). High-frequency brain activity: Its possible role in attention, perception and language processing. Progress in Neurobiology, 52, 427–445. Pulvermüller, F., Keil, A., & Elbert, T. (1999). High-frequency brain activity: Perception or active memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 250–252. 111 Reilly, E. L. (1999). EEG recording and operation of the apparatus. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (pp. 122–148). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Birbaumer, N., Wolf, P., Düchting-Röth, A., Reker, M., et al. (1993). Cortical selfregulation in patients with epilepsies. Epilepsy Research, 14, 63–72. Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Canavan, A., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1989). Slow cortical potentials and behavior. Baltimore, MD: Urban & Schwarzenberg. Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1982). The effects of slow cortical potentials on response speed. Psychophysiology, 19(2), 211–217. Schacter, D. L. (1977). EEG theta waves and psychological phenomena: A review and analysis. Biological Psychology, 5, 47–82. Schoppenhorst, M., Brauer, F., Freund, G., & Kubicki, S. (1980). The significance of coherence estimates in determining cerebral alpha and mu activities. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 48, 25–33. Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related markers of unconscious processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 209–218. Siegel, J. M., Sterman, M. B., & Ross, S. (1979). Automatic detection and operant reinforcement of slow potential shifts. Physiology and Behavior, 23(2), 411–413. Speckmann, E.-J., & Elger, C. E. (1999). Introduction to the neurophysiological basis of the EEG and DC potentials. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (4th ed., pp. 15–27). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Steriade, M. (1999). Cellular substrates of brain rhythms. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields (4th ed., pp. 28–75). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Steriade, M. (2006). Grouping of brain rythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience, 137, 1087–1106. Steriade, M., & Buzsáki, G. (1990). Parallel activation of thalamic and cortical neurons by brainstem and basal forebrain cholinergic systems. In M. Steriade & D. Biesold (Eds.), Brain cholinergic systems (pp. 3–64). Oxford, UK: Oxford Medical Press. Steriade, M., Gloor, P., Llinás, R. R., Lopes da Silva, F., & Mesulam, M.-M. (1990). Basic mechanisms of cerebral rhythmic activities. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 76, 481–508. Sterman, M. B. (1973). Neurophysiologic and clinical studies of sensorimotor EEG biofeedback training: Some effects on epilepsy. Seminars in Psychiatry, 5(4), 507–525. Sterman, M. B. (1996). Physiological origins and functional correlates of EEG rhythmic activities: Implication for self-regulation. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21(1), 3–33. Strehl, U., Leins, U., Goth, G., Klinger, Ch., Hinterberger, T, & Birbaumer, N. (2006). Self-regulation of slow cor- 112 tical potentials—a new treatment for children with ADHD. Pediatrics, 118, e1530–1540. Tallon-Baudry, C., & Bertrand, O. (1999). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 151–162. Vanderwolf, C. H. (2000). Are neocortical gamma waves related to consciousness? Brain Research, 855, 217–224. Verleger, R. (1988). Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 343–356. Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCallum, W. C., & Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent negative varia- II. INSTRUMENTATION tion: An electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature, 203, 380–384. Whitham, E. M., Pope, K. J., Fitzgibbon, S. P., Lewis, T., Clark, C. R., Loveless, S., et al. (2007). Scalp electrical recording during paralysis: Quantitative evidence that EEG frequencies above 20 Hz are contaminated by EMG. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(8), 1877–1888. Wolpaw, J. R., McFarland, D. J., Neat, G. W., & Forneris, C. A. (1991). An EEG-based brain-computer interface for cursor control. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78(3), 252–259. Zschocke, S. (1995). Klinische Elektroencephalographie. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Chapter 7 Quantitative Encephalography and Electroencephalographic Biofeedback/Neurofeedback Robert W. Thatcher Quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) is distinguished from visual examination of electroencephalographic (EEG)1 traces, referred to as “nonquantitative EEG,” by the fact that the latter is subjective and involves low sensitivity and low interrater reliability (Cooper, Osselton, & Shaw, 1974; Woody, 1966, 1968), whereas the former involves the use of computers and power spectral analyses, and is more objective, with higher reliability and higher sensitivity (Hughes & John, 1999). The improved sensitivity and reliability of QEEG was first recognized by Hans Berger in 1934, when he performed a QEEG analysis involving the power spectrum of the EEG with a mechanical analog computer (Niedermeyer & Lopes da Silva, 1995). QEEG at the present time clearly surpasses conventional visual examination of EEG traces, because of its high temporal and spatial resolution in the millisecond time domain and approximately 1 centimeter in the spatial domain, which gives QEEG the ability to measure network dynamics that are simply “invisible” to the naked eye. Over the last 40+ years, the accuracy, sensitivity, and resolution of QEEG have steadily increased because of the efforts of hundreds of dedicated scientists and clinicians who have produced approximately 120,000 QEEG studies cited in the National Library of Medicine database. It is recommended that the reader search the National Library of Medicine database (www.ncbi.nlm.nih. gov/sites/entrez?db=pubmed) using the key word “EEG” and the few representative citations in this chapter.2 Because of space limitations, no reviews of this vast literature are attempted here; instead, my purpose in this chapter is to briefly describe some of the most recent advances in QEEG as they relate to EEG biofeedback/neurofeedback.3 Neurological evaluation of space-occupying lesions has been correlated with the locations and frequency changes that have been observed in the EEG traces and in QEEG analyses (e.g., lesions of the visual cortex resulted in distortions of the EEG generated from the occipital scalp locations, or lesions of the frontal lobe resulted in distortions of the EEG traces arising in frontal regions). However, early neurological and neuropsychological studies indicated that function is not located in any one part of the brain (Luria, 1973). Instead, the brain is made up of complex and interconnected groupings of neurons that constitute “functional systems,” such as the “digestive system” or the “respiratory system,” in which cooperative sequencing and interactions give rise to an overall function at each moment of time (Luria, 1973). This widely accepted view of brain function as a complicated functional system, which became dominant in the 1950s and 1960s, is still the accepted view today. For example, since the 1980s, new technologies such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emis113 114 sion tomography (PET), single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) and QEEG and magnetoencephalography (MEG) have provided ample evidence for distributed functional systems involved in perception, memory, drives, emotions, voluntary and involuntary movements, executive functions, and various psychiatric and psychological dysfunctions. Modern PET, QEEG, MEG, and fMRI studies are consistent with the historical view of “functional systems” presented by Luria in the 1950s (Luria 1973); that is, there is no absolute functional localization, because a functional system of dynamically coupled subregions of the brain is operating. For example, several fMRI and MRI studies (e.g., diffusion tensor imaging [DTI]) have shown that the brain is organized by a relatively small subset of “modules” or “hubs” that represent clusters of neurons with high within-cluster connectivity and sparse long-distance connectivity (Hagmann et al., 2008; Chen, He, Rosa-Neto, Germann, & Evans, 2008; He et al., 2009). Modular organization is a common property of complex systems and “small-world” models in which maximum efficiency is achieved when local clusters of neurons rely on a small set of long-distance connections in order to minimize the “expense” of wiring and shorten time delays between modules (Buzsaki, 2006; He et al., 2009). Also, recent QEEG and MEG analyses have demonstrated that important visually invisible processes such as coherence, phase delays, phase locking and phase shifting of different frequencies is critical in cognitive functions and various clinical disorders (Buszaki, 2006; Sauseng & Klimesch, 2008; Thatcher, North, & Biver, 2009a). Phase shift and phase synchrony has been shown to be one of the fundamental processes involved in the coordination of neural activity located in spatially distributed “modules” at each moment of time (Freeman & Rogers, 2002; Freeman, Burke, & Homes, 2003; Thatcher et al., 2009a, 2009b). QEEG for Assessment and Neurofeedback for Treatment: A Parent–Child Relationship This use of the EEG changed dramatically in the 1960s, when computers were used to modify the EEG through biofeedback, referred to today as neurofeedback (NF). Studies by Fox and Rudell (1968); Kamiya (1971), and Sterman (1973) were a dramatic departure from the classical use of conventional visual EEG and QEEG in that for the first time clinicians could consider treating a II. INSTRUMENTATION disorder such as epilepsy or attention deficit disorders and other mental disorders by using operant conditioning methods to modify the EEG itself. Thus, QEEG and EEG biofeedback have a “parent–child” relationship in that EEG biofeedback necessarily uses computers and is therefore a form of QEEG that is focused on treatment based on the science and knowledge of the physiological meaning and genesis of the EEG itself. Ideally, as knowledge about brain function and the accuracy and resolution of the EEG increases, then EEG biofeedback should change in lockstep with it to better link symptoms and complaints to the brain and in this manner treat the patient based on solid science. To the extent the EEG can be linked to functional systems in the brain and to specific mental disorders, EEG biofeedback could “move” the brain toward a healthier state (i.e., “normalize” the brain; Thatcher, 1989, 1999). Clearly, the clinical efficacy of EEG biofeedback is reliant on knowledge about the genesis of the EEG and specific functions of the human brain. The parent–child relationship and interdependencies between QEEG and EEG biofeedback is active today and represents a bond that, when broken, results in reduced clinical efficacy and general criticism of the field of EEG biofeedback. The traditional and logical relationship between QEEG and NF is to use QEEG to assess, and to use NF to treat, based on a linkage between the patient’s symptoms and complaints and functional systems in the brain. This parent–child linkage requires clinical competence on the one hand and technical competence with computers and the EEG on the other. Competence in both is essential, and organizations such as the International Society for Neurofeedback Research (ISNR), the Society of Applied Neuroscience (SAN), the American Board of Electroencephalography and Neurophysiology (ABEN), the EEG and Clinical Neuroscience Society (ECNS), the Biofeedback Certification Institute of America (BCIA), the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB), and others are available to help educate and test the requisite qualifications and competence to use EEG biofeedback. The parent–child link is typically optimized by following three steps: 1. Perform a careful and thorough clinical inter- view and assessment of the patient’s symptoms and complaints (neuropsychological assessments are the most desirable). 2. Conduct a QEEG in order to link the patient’s symptoms and complaints to functional sys- 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback tems in the brain as evidenced in fMRI, PET and QEEG/MEG. 3. Devise an EEG biofeedback protocol to address the deregulations observed in the QEEG assessment that best match the patient’s symptoms and complaints. This approach reinforces the close bond between parent (QEEG) and child (NF), and allows objective evaluation of the efficacy of treatment in terms of both behavior and brain function. Figure 7.1 illustrates a common, modern quantitative EEG analysis in which conventional EEG traces are viewed and examined at the same time that quantitative analyses are displayed so as to facilitate and extend the analytical power of the EEG. Seamless integration of QEEG and NF involves two basic steps: (1) visual examination of the EEG traces and (2) spectral analyses of the EEG traces.4 Numerous studies have shown a relationship between the time and frequency domains of an EEG time series and LORETA (Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography) three-dimensional source analyses, which provide 7 mm3 maximal spatial resolution in real-time (Pascual-Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1994; Gomez & Thatcher, 2001; see Note 6). There is a verifiable correspon- 115 dence between the time series of the EEG and the power spectrum and LORETA three-dimensional source localization, for example, visual cortex source localization of hemiretinal visual stimulation, temporal lobe source localization of auditory simulation, theta source localization in the hippocampus in memory tasks, localization of theta in the anterior cingulate gyrus in attention tasks, and linkage between depression and rostral and dorsal cingulate gyrus (see Note 4). The number of clinical QEEG studies cited in the National Library of Medicine attests to the link between patient symptoms and functional systems in the brain, and protocols for treatment are commonly guided by this scientific literature. The Use of 19‑Channel Surface QEEG Z Scores and EEG Biofeedback As described by Thatcher and Lubar (2008), scientists at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) in the 1950s (Adey, Walter, & Hendrix, 1961) and later Matousek and Petersen (1973) were the first to compute means and standard deviations in different age groups, and then z-scores to compare an individual to a reference normative database of means and standard deviations. The FIGURE 7.1. Example of conventional digital EEG (left) and QEEG (right) on the same screen at the same time. The conventional EEG includes examination and marking of EEG traces and events. The QEEG (right) includes the fast Fourier transform (top right) and normative database Z scores (bottom right). 116 “Z statistic” is defined as the difference between the value from an individual and the mean of the normal reference population divided by the standard deviation of the population. John and colleagues (John, 1977; John et al., 1977; John, Prichep, & Easton, 1987) expanded on the use of the Z score and referenced normal databases for clinical evaluation, including the use of multivariate measures such as the Mahalanobis distance metric (John et al., 1987; John, Prichep, Fridman, & Easton, 1988). For purposes of assessing deviation from normal, the values of z above and below the mean, which include 95–99% of the area of the Z-score distribution, is often used as a level of confidence necessary to minimize type I and type II errors. The standard-score equation is also used to cross-validate a normative database, which again emphasizes the importance of approximation to a Gaussian for any normative QEEG database (Thatcher, Walker, Bier, North, & Curtin, 2003; Thatcher, North, & Biver, 2005). The standard concepts underlying the Z-score statistic and QEEG evaluations were recently combined to give rise to real-time EEG Z-score biofeedback, also referred to as “live Z-score biofeedback” II. INSTRUMENTATION (Thatcher 1989, 1999, 2000a, 2000b; Thatcher & Collura, 2006; Collura, Thatcher, Smith, Lambos, & Stark, 2008). The use of real-time Z-score EEG biofeedback is another method to advance the integration of QEEG and NF. Figure 7.2 illustrates the differences between raw score EEG biofeedback and real-time Z-score EEG biofeedback. There are several advantages of real-time Z-score biofeedback: (1) simplification by reducing different metrics (power, coherence, phase, asymmetry, etc.) to a single common metric of the Z score; (2) simplification by providing a threshold and direction of change (i.e., Z = 0) to move the EEG toward a normal healthy reference population of subjects5; and (3) improved linkage between patient’s complaints and symptoms and localization of functional systems in the brain. Figures 7.3, 7.4, and 7.5 show examples of how a symptom check list and QEEG evaluation are linked to give rise to a neurofeedback protocol (see Appendix 7.1). Modules or “hubs” are linked to the various basic functional systems involved in cognition and perception (Chen et al., 2008; Hagmann et al., 2008; He et al., 2009). Recent neuroimaging studies indicate that all of the various “modules” FIGURE 7.2. Diagram of the difference between standard EEG biofeedback and Z-score EEG biofeedback. The top system involves standard EEG biofeedback that relies on raw EEG measures such as power, coherence, phase, amplitude asymmetries, power ratios, and an arbitrary and subjective threshold value. The bottom system is the same as the top but with a transform of the raw scores to Z scores and thus a simplification of diverse metrics to a single metric of the Z score in which the threshold is mathematically defined as a movement toward Z = 0. The magnitude of the Z score provides real-time feedback as to the distance between the patient’s EEG and the EEG values in an age-matched sample of healthy, normal control subjects. 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback 117 FIGURE 7.3. Example of a computer-generated Symptom Check List in which the clinician first evaluates the patient’s symptoms and complaints using clinical and neuropsychological tools, then enters a score of 0 to 10 based on the severity of the symptoms. Hypotheses formation as to the most likely scalp locations and brain systems are based on the scientific literature that links symptoms and complaints to the locations of functional specialization (see Appendix 7.1). From NeuroGuide 2.5.7. FIGURE 7.4. Example of Brodmann areas as they relate to various general functions and “hubs” or “modules,” and scalp electrode locations that “sense” electrical activity generated by various functional systems. 118 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 7.5. Flow diagram of individualized protocol design based on linkage of patient's symptoms and complaints with surface QEEG Z scores and LORETA Z scores. The columns of the matrix are the 19 channels of the 10/20 International electrode sites, and the rows are symptoms and QEEG EEG features. Hypotheses are formed as to the most likely electrode site locations associated with a given symptom and complaint based on the scientific literature. The hypotheses are then tested based on QEEG and LORETA Z scores. Weak systems representing “loss of function” are identified when there is a match of QEEG Z scores, with the hypothesized scalp locations based on symptoms. Compensatory locations are identified by a mismatch between hypothesized symptoms and complaints, and the locations of observed QEEG Z scores. The suggested neurofeedback protocol that is then produced is based on the locations of the “weak” systems (see Appendix 7.1). are dynamically linked and interactive, and that subsets of neural groups in different modules “bind” together for brief periods of time to mediate a given function (Sauseng & Klimesch, 2008; Thatcher et al., 2008; Thatcher & Lubar, 2008). An illustration of Brodmann areas and electrodes as they relate to functional systems is shown in Figure 7.4. The link between a patient’s symptoms and complaints to the localization of functional systems in the brain is based on the accumulated scientific and clinical literature from QEEG, MEG, fMRI, PET, and SPECT studies conducted over the last few decades, as well as the basic literature on neurological and neuropsychological lesions. The Russian neuropsychologist Alexander Luria (1973) and the American neuropsychologist Hans-Lukas Teuber (1968) were among the leading scientists to make important links between symptoms and complaints, and localization of functional systems in the brain. The integration of QEEG and EEG biofeedback relies on such links as the initial stage in the formation of NF protocols, as illustrated in Figures 7.3, 7.4, and 7.5. The first step is to produce hypotheses about likely links between a patient’s symptoms and complaints and the location of functional systems based on the scientific literature prior to conducting a QEEG. The second step is to confirm or disconfirm the link by evaluating brain locations of deviations from normal using QEEG and LORETA three-dimensional imaging, and the third step is to produce a biofeedback protocol based on the match between hypothesized locations and the QEEG and/or LORETA evaluation (see Figure 7.5). Luria (1973) emphasized that deregulation of neural populations is reflected by reduced homeostatic balance in the brain, in which symptoms are represented as “loss of function” that is often accompanied by “compensatory” processes. One goal of the linkage of QEEG and NF is to identify and contrast the weak or “loss of function” components in the EEG with the compensatory processes in which the weak systems are the initial target of the EEG biofeedback protocol. There are six steps that must be followed to use the symptom check list and automatic Z-score protocol generator: 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback 1. Import the subject’s edited *.ng EEG file by clicking File > Open. 2. Click Report > Symptom Check List Match. 3. Click Collection > Setup & Monitor > OK. 4. Click Collection > Neurofeedback > Surface Neurofeedback. 5. Click Symptom Check List in the Surface EEG Control Panel. 6. Select the Symptom(s) that best represent the patient’s symptoms and assign a severity from 1 to 10 for each symptom. View the reduced size and number of green circles as the QEEG Z-score threshold is increased. Reduce the Z-score cutoff and view an increase in size and number of green circles. When a good fit to the patient’s symptoms by the hypothesis test is reached based on the clinical judgment of the user, click OK to generate an automatic protocol (see Figure 7.6). Figure 7.7 is an example of a 19-channel surface EEG biofeedback setup screen in Neuroguide, where users can select a wide variety of measures or metrics, all reduced to the single metric of the Z score. This includes, power, coherence, phase differences, amplitude asymmetries, power ratios, and the average reference and Laplacian montages. Nineteen channels is a minimum number needed in order to compute accurate average references and the Laplacian montage, which is an estimate of the current density vectors that course at right angles through the skull. 119 Multiple frequencies and multiple metrics may be selected in which a threshold must be reached before a visual and/or auditory reward is given (e.g., z < 2.0). The 19-channel Z-score approach provides for seamless integration of QEEG assessment and 19-channel Z-score neurofeedback or treatment. Because there are approximately 5,000 possible instantaneous Z scores, it is important to limit and structure the biofeedback protocol in a manner that best links to the patient’s symptoms and complaints. The linkage of patient’s symptoms and complaints as hypotheses that are confirmed or disconfirmed by QEEG assessment is used to develop a neurofeedback protocol. Blind and random selection of Z-score metrics runs the risk of altering “compensatory” systems and not focusing on the weak or “loss of function” systems that are linked to the patient’s symptoms and complaints. Control of the difficulty of the threshold is by (1) lengthening the event interval and (2) lowering the Z-score threshold. To make neurofeedback easier, then, shorten the event interval and raise the Z-score threshold. The event integration interval is a time window that varies from 250 ms to 1 s. In order to receive reinforcement, then, 100% of the time events within a window must reach the Z-score criteria. By lengthening the time window, one simultaneously reinforces reduced variability. Thus, the time window provides a variability feedback method. Click Sound On for the eyes-closed condition and/or use both visual and auditory feedback with eyes open. Click Symp- FIGURE 7.6. Seamless QEEG and neurofeedback: Approximately 50–60 minutes for a single session, in four steps from clinical interview to QEEG to neuropathy. 120 II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 7.7. Example of 19-channel surface EEG Z-score biofeedback setup screen inside of NeuroGuide. tom Check List if you have run a QEEG analysis and clicked Report > Create Symptom Check List Match. If one decides not to use the Symptom Check List, then manually select metrics, channels and frequencies and click OK to activate the 10/20 Reward display. If dual monitors are used, then enable the monitor in the control panel > Display Settings and depress the left mouse button over the 10/20 reward display and move it to the second monitor. If criteria are met for all time points in a window (e.g., Z < 2.0), then a reward is the color green in the 10/20 locations selected in the Z-score NF panel. The goal is to make the 10/20 head display show green as often as possible. Start with an easy reward criteria, e.g., Z < 2.0), then adjust the reward criteria to lower Z values (e.g., Z < 1.0) in order to shape the client/patient EEG features toward Z = 0. Symptom Check List Hypotheses and QEEG Z‑Score Tests As explained, one must first import the patient’s edited *.ng file and then click Report > Create Symptom Check List Match before the Symptom Check List is active (see Figure 7.8). Then identify one or more of the 49 symptom(s) exhibited by the patient/client and double-click the severity score to activate the symptom(s) (see Figure 7.9). Enter a severity score from 1 to 10. This creates a green circle on the 10/20 scalp display and the size of the circle increases as a function of the severity value. As more symptoms are selected the Neuroguide automatically weights the symptom locations and scales the size of the green circles to represent “hypotheses” of “weak systems” or “loss of function” systems (Luria, 1973). The top-right 10/20 scalp display will change depending on the symptom check list and the match of the QEEG Z scores to the hypothesized locations. The location of green circles in the “Match” 10/20 display represents a match between hypothesized scalp locations and observed QEEG Z scores. The radius of a green circle is produced by scaling with respect to the maximum average Z score greater than the threshold for a given scalp location. The larger the average Z score, the larger the radius of the circle. After the user finds an optimal link to hypothesized locations, then click OK to automatically generate a Neurofeedback Protocol. The automatic protocol is produced by the cross-product of the symptom severity and the average Z score at a given location or (S × Z)/N, where S is the sum of the severity index from the symptom check list for that location; Z is the aver- 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback 121 FIGURE 7.8. Example of Symptom Check List panel before making a symptom selection and assigning a symptom severity score. FIGURE 7.9. Example of the Symptom Check List after a symptom and severity are selected. A match of QEEG Z-score deviations from normal to the hypothesized brain locations linked to the symptom is shown. The mismatch display shows brain regions that are not linked to the symptom and possibly are compensatory. The goal is to avoid compensatory systems and target the “weak” brain systems linked to symptoms. 122 age Z scores in that location, where the absolute Z or |Z| is greater than the threshold as determined by the user (Default is |Z| = 2); and N is normalization by scaling to the maximum. The user can veto or modify the automatically produced protocol by clicking clear or by clicking metrics, frequencies, auto-spectrum and cross-spectrum selections in the Surface Neurofeedback panel. Click OK in the Symptom Check List Panel to Return to the Surface Neurofeedback Panel and View the Automatic Selections based on the Symptom Check List as Hypotheses and QEEG Z Scores as Tests of the Hypotheses. Click OK if satisfied or modify by selecting or deselecting variables, or click Clear to start over. Then click OK in the Surface EEG Neurofeedback panel to activate the Neurofeedback Reinforcement Display.6 Begin Neurofeedback Using the Automatic Symptom Match Protocol (20 Minutes) After the Symptom Check List match and mismatch has been completed then start Neurofeedback by clicking OK. When the Z-Score Neurofeedback panel again appears, then check which metrics have been selected by the automatic protocol process and edit, modify, or reject by clicking “Reset.” When ready, then click OK to begin the Neurofeedback. Move the 10/20 head display to a second monitor and/or choose a sound feedback in the Sound control. Select third- party vendor II. INSTRUMENTATION DVD/Flash and MIDI displays by clicking Display, then selecting CIS (Cybernetic Integration Systems); Brainmaster (Multimedia control) or 3D Engine (Deymed DVD/Flash and MIDI system). The latter display systems must be purchased directly from CIS, Brainmaster or Deymed. Neuroguide only provides access to already purchased display products. Figure 7.10 is the NeuroGuide 10/20 19-channel display for Neurofeedback. Green circles (shown here in gray) are the reinforcement when the Z scores are less than the threshold. If cross-spectral coherence or phase, and so forth, is selected, then only a single green circle will be present at Cz. This is because it is not possible to provide an unambiguous multiple-head location display when multiple coherence or phase channels have been selected. Therefore, all of the measures are combined into a single display (similar to what occurs under DVD/Flash control). All of the selected protocol locations must meet the threshold criteria in order to receive a reinforcement (single Cz display or DVD/Flash). Figure 7.10 shows an example of a simple 10/20 head display for feedback where the circles turn green when threshold is met (e.g., Z < 2.0) and provides feedback about the scalp locations that are meeting threshold. Figure 7.11 is an example of a progress-monitoring chart that is displayed for the clinician during the course of biofeedback. One strategy is to develop a protocol based on the linkage to the FIGURE 7.10. Example of 19-channel feedback display. The circles at a particular location turn green (but shown here in gray) when threshold is reached (e.g., Z < 2.0). 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback 123 FIGURE 7.11. Example of progress charts that a clinician views during the course of neurofeedback. The idea is to shape the patient’s brain toward the center of the normal healthy reference population, where Z = 0. Initially the threshold is set so that the patient receives a high rate of reinforcement (e.g., Z < 2.0), then to lower the threshold and make it more difficult (e.g., Z < 1.5), then, as the patient again receives a high rate of reinforcement, to again lower the threshold (e.g., Z < 1.0) so as to shape the brain dynamics using a standard operant conditioning procedure. patient’s symptoms and complaints, as discussed previously, then to set the Z-score threshold, so that it is easy for the subject to meet threshold and therefore produce a high rate of successful “Hits” or rewards. The second step is to lower the threshold and make the feedback more difficult (e.g., Z < 1.5) and as the patient or client gains control and receives a high rate of reinforcement to lower the threshold again (e.g., Z < 1.0) in a “shaping” process in which operant conditioning is used to move the patient’s brain metrics toward the center of the normal reference population, or Z = 0. Neuroimaging NF or Real‑Time LORETA Z‑Score Biofeedback Improved accuracy in the linkage between a patient’s symptoms and complaints and the localization of functional systems can be achieved by the biofeedback of real-time, three-dimensional locations or voxels in the brain. This method has been successfully implemented with fMRI for chronic pain, obsessive–compulsive disorders, and anxiety disorders (Apkarian, 1999; Bray, Shimojo, & O’Doherty, 2007; Caria et al., 2007; de Charms, 2008; de Charms et al., 2004; Weiskopf et al., 2003; Yoo et al., 2006). However, fMRI biofeedback, also referred to as “neuroimaging therapy,” has several significant limitations in comparison to LORETA 3-dimensional EEG biofeedback7: (1) a long time delay between a change in localized brain activity and the feedback signal (e.g., 20 seconds to minutes for fMRI), whereas LORETA EEG biofeedback signals involve millisecond delays; (2) fMRI only provides indirect measures of neural activity, because blood flow changes are delayed and secondary to changes in neural activity, whereas EEG biofeedback is a direct measure of neural electrical activity; and (3) expense—fMRI costs $3 million for the MRI machine plus $30,000 per month for liquid helium, whereas EEG biofeedback equipment and maintenance costs are less than $10,000. The spatial resolution of LORETA source localization is approximately 7 mm3, which is comparable to the spatial resolution of fMRI.8 fMRI, however, offers the advantage of imaging of noncortical structures such as the striatum, thalamus, cerebellum, and other brain regions, whereas 124 QEEG is limited to imaging of cortical dipoles produced by pyramidal cells. Nonetheless, even with this limitation, several studies have proven that biofeedback using LORETA real-time, threedimensional sources is feasible and results in positive clinical outcomes (Cannon, Lubar, Thornton, Wilson, & Congedo; 2005; Cannon et al., 2006, 2007; Cannon & Lubar, 2007; Cannon, Lubar, Sokhadze, & Baldwin, 2008; Cannon, Congredo, Lubar, & Hutchens, 2009; Lubar, Congredo, & Askew, 2003). Figures 7.12 and 7.13 show examples of LORETA Z-score EEG biofeedback with reinforcement toward Z = 0 as a method to reinforce stability and increased efficiency of information processing in brain networks linked to the patient’s symptoms. Future Directions of QEEG and NF Dramatic improvements in sensitivity and spatial resolution of neural sources linked to patient’s symptoms and complaints have occurred in the 80 years since EEG was first discovered. The development of EEG biofeedback occurred almost in parallel to the sudden growth of QEEG in the 1960s, and today there is ever more integration of QEEG for clinical evaluation and EEG biofeedback for treatment. QEEG assessment and EEG biofeedback treatment are inextricably bound, in that advancements in sensitivity and spatial resolution of QEEG as a clinical evaluation tool are immediately translated to treatment using biofeedback. There has been a veritable explosion of new discoveries in neuroscience related to II. INSTRUMENTATION FIGURE 7.12. Example of LORETA Z score biofeedback in which reinforcement is toward Z = 0 or increased stability in Brodmann areas linked to symptoms. A change in the protocol occurred after the first session, then a steady movement toward greater stability in the symptom linked networks occurred in five sessions. Unpublished clinical result reprinted with permission from Wesley D. Center, PhD). basic mechanisms of memory, attention, arousal and cognition, and correlations to various neurological, psychiatric, and psychological disorders. In parallel with these advancements, it is expected that EEG biofeedback will keep pace by advancing new methods of noninvasive treatment of a wide variety of clinical disorders. Today high-speed computers are available, in which 19 or more channels of EEG can be measured, and biofeedback is applied in almost the same amount of time FIGURE 7.13. Top: An example of reduced Z-score deviation from normal in the cingulate gyrus in after 10 sessions of Z-score neurofeedback. Bottom: An example of reduced Z scores in the right superior frontal gyrus after 10 sessions of Z-score neurofeedback. Unpublished clinical result reprinted with permission of William A. Lambos, PhD). 125 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback as one- or four-channel biofeedback was applied in the past. Thus, there are few practical limitations in using high-density electrode arrays for EEG biofeedback in the future. As knowledge about basic neurophysiological control measures emerge (e.g., thalamic-mediated phase shift and phase lock, coherence, cross-frequency phase synchrony, and LORETA spatial coordination of brain modules underlying perception and cognition), the clinical efficacy of EEG biofeedback will improve. Given the expense of health care and the need for noninvasive treatment, the marriage of QEEG and EEG biofeedback will continue to evolve. Notes 1. The electroencephalogram is measured from the scalp surface and is produced by the algebraic summation of cortical synaptic potentials. 2. Since approximately 1975, it has been very difficult even to publish studies that use only visual examination of EEG traces. The estimate of 90,000 arises when one uses the search term “EEG” and examines the abstracts to confirm that quantification of EEG was used. It is necessary to use the search term “EEG” and not “QEEG,” because the National Library of Medicine indexes articles based on words in the titles and most QEEG studies do not use the term “QEEG” in their titles. 3. While EEG biofeedback is sometimes referred to as “neurofeedback,” the latter term is not specific, since many treatments other than EEG may involve neurofeedback. However, in this chapter, these terms are considered synonymous and are used interchangeably. 4. Spectral analysis includes space and time sequences that are transformed, such as joint time–frequency analysis, fast Fourier transform (FFT), and all other methods that decompose EEG energies at different frequencies in space and time. 5. Simultaneous suppression of excessive theta and reinforcement of deficient beta is achieved by using a absolute Z-score threshold, which is a simplification compared to standard raw score EEG biofeedback. For example, if the threshold is set to an absolute value of Z < 2, then reduced theta amplitude and elevated beta amplitude will both be rewarded when the instantaneous EEG event exhibits a Z < 2 theta and beta power value. 6. Initially only absolute power, coherence, and phase differences are used in the automatic Symptom Check List protocol. This is because relative power is always a distortion and can be in conflict with absolute power when both are used at the same time. Because absolute power is the “mother” of relative power and unambiguously represents the degree of local synchrony of EEG generators, a limitation to absolute power is best. Amplitude asymmetry is not used because of a similar ambiguity (i.e., absolute power differences cannot be resolved without reference to the “mother” of amplitude asymmetry); therefore, amplitude asymmetry is redundant. Also, coherence and phase are limited to linked ears and are not available for the Laplacian and Average Reference montages, because these measures are not valid except with linked ears. A future release will include phase shift and phase lock duration. 7. LORETA means “low-resolution electromagnetic tomography” (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994). Since the inception of this method in 1994, there have been over 500 peer-reviewed publications (see www.uzh.ch/keyinst/newloreta/quoteloreta/papersthatquoteloreta05.htm for a partial list of this literature). 8. The voxel resolution of LORETA is 7 mm3, which is adequate spatial resolution, because the Brodmann areas are much greater in volume than 7 mm3. Also, the biological resolution of fMRI may be worse than that of LORETA, because it depends on the vascular architecture of the brain. For example, Ozcan, Baumgartner, Vucurevic, Stoeter, and Treede (2005) showed that maximal fMRI spatial resolution is > 12 mm3. APPENDIX 7.1. Symptom Check List Symptoms Problems with perception of letters Slow reader Dyslexia—letter reversal Problems with spatial perception Orientation in space problems Receptive language problems Insensitive to others’ emotional expressions Blurred vision Obsessive thoughts about self Migraine headaches Symptoms of fibromyalgia Auditory sequencing problems Short-term memory problems Face recognition problems Receptive language problems Obsessive self-examination Word-finding problems Chronic pain Poor skilled motor movements Speech articulation problems Balance problems Decreased tactile or skin sensitivity Problems recognizing objects by touch Depression (sad and blue) Problems with multitasking 126 Slowness of thought—easily confused Poor judgment Attention deficits—easily distractible Hyperactive and/or agitated Obsessive thoughts and/or hyperfocused Compulsive behaviors and/or thoughts Sequential planning problems Executive function problems Poor social skills Oppositional defiant conduct Problems concentrating Mood swings Impulsive behaviors Low threshold for anger and loss of control Self-esteem problems Failure to initiate actions Generalized anxiety Insensitive to others’ feelings Difficulty comprehending social cues Anosognosia—denial of a problem Dyscalcula—problems calculating Delusional Low motivation References Adey, W. R., Walter, D. O., & Hendrix, C. E. (1961). Computer techniques in correlation and spectral analyses of cerebral slow waves during discriminative behavior. Experimental Neurology, 3, 501–524. Apkarian, A. V. (1999). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of pain consciousness: Cortical networks of pain critically depend on what is implied by “pain.” Current Review of Pain, 3, 308–315. Berger, H. (1934). Uber das Electrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Neunte Mitteilungj. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Neverkrankheiten, 102, 538–557. Bray, S., Shimojo, S., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Direct instrumental conditioning of neural activity using functional magnetic resonance imaging-derived reward feedback. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 7498–7507. Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press. Cannon, R., Congredo, M., Lubar, J., & Hutchens, T. (2009). Differentiating a network of executive attention: LORETA neurofeedback in anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(3), 404–441. Cannon, R., & Lubar, J. [F.] (2007). EEG spectral power and coherence: Differentiating effects of Spatial-Specific Neuro-Operant Learning (SSNOL) utilizing LORETA II. INSTRUMENTATION neurofeedback training in the anterior cingulate and bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. Journal of Neurotherapy, 11(3), 25–44. Cannon, R., Lubar, J. F., Congedo, M., Gerke, A., Thornton, K., Towler, K., & Hutchens, T. (2007). The effects of neurofeedback training in the cognitive division of the anterior cingulate gyrus. International Journal of Science, 117(3), 337–357. Cannon, R., Lubar, J., Gerke, A., Thornton, K., Hutchens, T., & McCammon, V. (2006). EEG spectral-power and coherence: LORETA neurofeedback training in the anterior cingulate gyrus. Journal of Neurotherapy, 10(1), 5–31. Cannon, R., Lubar, J., Sokhadze, E., & Baldwin, D. (2008). LORETA neurofeedback for addiction and the possible neurophysiology of psychological processes influenced: A case study and region of interest analysis of LORETA neurofeedback in right anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neurotherapy, 12(4), 227–241. Cannon, R., Lubar, J., Thornton, K., Wilson, S., & Congedo, M. (2005). Limbic beta activation and LORETA: Can hippocampal and related limbic activity be recorded and changes visualized using LORETA in an affective memory condition? Journal of Neurotherapy, 8(4), 5–24. Caria, A., Veit, R., Sitaram, R., Lotze, M., Weiskopf, N., Grodd, W., & Birbaumer, N. (2007). Regulation of anterior insular cortex activity using real-time fMRI. NeuroImage, 35, 1238–1246. Chen, Z. J., He, Y., Rosa-Neto, P., Germann, J., & Evans, A. C. (2008). Revealing modular architecture of human brain structural networks by using cortical thickness from MRI. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2374–2381. Collura, T. F., Thatcher, R. W., Smith, M. L., Lambos, W. A., & Stark, C. R. (2008). EEG biofeedback training using live Z scores and a normative database. In T. Budzinsky, H. Budzinsky, J. Evans, & A. Abarbanel (Eds.), Introduction to QEEG and neurofeedback: Advanced theory and applications (pp. 103–142). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Cooper, R., Osselton, J. W., & Shaw, J. G. (1974). EEG technology. London: Butterworth. deCharms, R. C. (2008). Applications of real-time fMRI. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 720–729. deCharms, R. C., Christoff, K., Glover, G. H., Pauly, J. M., Whitfield, S., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Learned regulation of spatially localized brain activation using realtime fMRI. NeuroImage, 21, 436–443. Fox, S. S., & Rudell, A. P. (1968). Operant controlled neural event: Formal and systematic approach to electrical codifing of behavior in brain. Science, 162, 1299–1302. Freeman, W. J., Burke, B. C., & Homes, M. D. (2003). Aperiodic phase re-setting in scalp EEG of beta-gamma oscillations by state transitions at alpha-theta rates. Human Brain Mapping, 19(4), 248–272. Freeman, W. J., & Rogers, L. J. (2002). Fine temporal resolution of analytic phase reveals episodic synchronization by state transitions in gamma EEGs. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87(2), 937–945. Gomez, J., & Thatcher, R. W. (2001). Frequency domain equivalence between potentials and currents using 7. QEEG and EEG Biofeedback/Neurofeedback LORETA. International Journal of Neuroscience, 107, 161–171. Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J., Wedeen, V. J., et al. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology, 6(7), e159. He, Y., Wang, J., Wang, L., Zhang, C. J., Yan, C., Yang, H., et al. (2009). Uncovering intrinsic modular organization of spontaneous brain activity in humans. PLoS ONE 4(4), e5226. Hughes, J. R., & John, E. R. (1999). Conventional and quantitative electroencephalography in psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry, 11(2), 190–208. John, E. R. (1977). Neurometrics: Quantitative electrophysiological analyses. In E. R. John & R. W. Thatcher (Eds.), Functional neuroscience (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. John, E. R., Karmel, B., Corning, W., Easton, P., Brown, D., Ahn, H., et al. (1977). Neurometrics: Numerical taxonomy identifies different profiles of brain functions within groups of behaviorally similar people. Science, l96, 1393–1410. John, E. R., Prichep, L. S., & Easton, P. (1987). Normative data banks and neurometrics: Basic concepts, methods and results of norm construction. In A. Remond (Ed.), Handbook of electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology: Vol. III. Computer analysis of the EEG and other neurophysiological signals (pp. 449–495). Amsterdam: Elsevier. John, E. R., Prichep, L. S., Fridman, J., & Easton, P. (1988). Neurometrics: Computer assisted differential diagnosis of brain dysfunctions. Science, 293, 162–169. Kamiya, J. (1971). Biofeedback training in voluntary control of EEG alpha rhythms. California Medicine, 115(3), 44–49. Lubar, J., Congedo, M., & Askew, J. H. (2003). Low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) of cerebral activity in chronic depressive disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 49(3), 175–185. Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. London: Penguin Press. Matousek, M., & Petersen, I. (1973). Automatic evaluation of background activity by means of agee-dependent EEG quotients. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35, 603–612. Niedermeyer, E., & Lopes da Silva, F. (1995). Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications and related fields. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Ozcan, M., Baumgartner, U. L., Vucurevic, G., Stoeter, P., & Treede, R. D. (2005). Spatial resolution of fMRI in the human parasylvian cortex: Comparison of somatosensory and auditory activation. NeuroImage, 25, 877–887. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann, D. (1994). Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 49–65. Sauseng, P., & Klimesch, W. (2008). What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(5), 1001–1013. 127 Sterman, M. B. (1973). Neurophysiologic and clinical studies of sensorimotor EEG biofeedback training: Some effects on epilepsy. Seminars in Psychiatry, 5(4), 507–525. Teuber, H. (1968). Disorders of memory following penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Neurology, 18(3), 287–298. Thatcher, R. W. (1989). EEG normative databases and EEG biofeedback. Journal of Neurotherapy, 2(4), 8–39. Thatcher, R. W. (1999). EEG database guided neurotherapy. In J. R. Evans & A. Abarbanel (Eds.), Introduction to quantitative EEG and neurofeedback (pp. 26–44). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Thatcher, R. W. (2000a). EEG operant conditioning (biofeedback) and traumatic brain injury. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 31(1), 38–44. Thatcher, R. W. (2000b, September). An EEG least action model of biofeedback. Paper presented at the eighth annual ISNR conference, St. Paul, MN. Thatcher, R. W., & Collura, T. (2006, September). Z score biofeedback and new technology. Paper presented at the International Society of Neuronal Regulation annual meeting, Atlanta, GA. Thatcher, R. W., & Lubar, J. F. (2008). History of the scientific standards of QEEG normative databases. In T. Budzinsky, H. Budzinsky, J. Evans, & A. Abarbanel (Eds.), Introduction to QEEG and neurofeedback: Advanced theory and applications (pp. 29–62). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Thatcher, R. W., North, D., & Biver, C. (2005). Evaluation and validity of a LORETA normative EEG database. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 36(2), 116–122. Thatcher, R. W., North, D., & Biver, C. (2008). Intelligence and EEG phase reset: A two-compartmental model of phase shift and lock. NeuroImage, 42(4), 1639–1653. Thatcher, R. W., North, D., & Biver, C. (2009a). Self organized criticality and the development of EEG phase reset. Human Brain Mapping, 30(2), 553–574. Thatcher, R. W., North, D. M., Neurbrander, J., Biver, C. J., Cutler, S., & DeFina, P. (2009b). Autism and EEG phase reset: Deficient GABA mediated inhibition in thalamocortical circuits. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(6), 780–800. Thatcher, R. W., Walker, R. A., Biver, C., North, D., & Curtin, R. (2003). Quantitative EEG normative databases: Validation and clinical correlation, Journal of Neurotherapy, 7(3/4), 87–122. Weiskopf, N., Veit, R., Erb, M., Mathiak, K., Grodd, W., Goebel, R., et al. (2003). Physiological self-regulation of regional brain activity using real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): Methodology and exemplary data. NeuroImage, 19(3), 577–586. Woody, R. H. (1966). Intra-judge reliability in clinical EEG. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 150–159. Woody, R. H. (1968). Inter-judge reliability in clinical EEG. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 251–261. Yoo, S. S., O’Leary, H. M., Fairneny, T., Chen, N. K., Panych, L. P., Park, H., et al. (2006). Increasing cortical activity in auditory areas through neurofeedback functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroReport, 17(12), 1273–1278. Chapter 8 Introduction to Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines John G. Arena and Mark S. Schwartz We hope this chapter successfully demystifies psychophysiological assessment and enhances applied psychophysiologists’ ability to formulate clinical questions and employ psychophysiological assessments to answer them. The process of learning how to use psychophysiological assessments to answer relevant clinical questions first involves the review of some basic concepts in applied psychophysiology, namely, (1) the measures that are generally used by applied psychophysiologists, (2) the basic concepts in psychophysiology that are most relevant to the applied psychophysiologist, and (3) the general conditions that the biofeedback therapist would likely employ in such assessments. We stress what is perhaps the most important methodological issue in any type of assessment, that is, temporal stability (i.e., reliability) of psychophysiological measures. We emphasize two conditions that are of essential importance to any psychophysiological assessment: baselines and adaptation periods. response, cardiovascular activity (heart rate, blood pressure, and vasomotor activity), and respiration (generally respiration rate and depth). Measures of electroencephalography (EEG) used in neurofeedback require specialized training and are outside the expertise of the average biofeedback clinician (as well as our own); therefore, they are not discussed in this chapter. For references about how basic measures are recorded and interpreted, see Peek (2003, and Chapter 3, this volume) for SEMG, skin temperature, and electrodermal responses. For more on SEMG, see Basmajian and De Luca (1985) and Criswell (2011). For measures of cardiovascular activity, see Berntson, Quigley, and Lozano (2007). For respiration and heart rate variability, see Lorig (2007) and Sutarto, Abdul Wahab, and Mat Zin (2010). Basic Concepts in Psychophysiology Basic concepts in applied psychophysiology and the scientific method in general with which biofeedback clinicians need to be familiar are autonomic balance, individual response stereotypy, stimulus–response specificity, the law of initial values, homeostasis, orienting and defensive responses, carryover effects, and especially temporal stability of the measures (Andreassi, 2007; Arena & Schwartz, 2003). Measures Used in Psychophysiological Assessments The measures generally employed in psychophysiological assessments are surface electromyographic activity (SEMG); skin surface temperature (e.g., fingers, hands); and measures of electrodermal 128 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines Autonomic Balance “Autonomic balance” refers to response patterning of the autonomic nervous system (Abboud, 2010; Sturgis & Arena, 1984). It has long been believed that individuals who are exposed to a stimulus of some sort respond either with sympathetic or parasympathetic response activation. In 1917, Eppinger and Hess were the first to classify individuals as vagotonic (parasympathetic responders) and sympatonic (sympathetic) responders. In 1966, Wenger created a score of autonomic balance based on how an individual’s electrodermal response, heart rate, diastolic blood pressure, and salivation output responded to various stimuli. Autonomic balance scores have been used in a variety of populations, including those with anxiety-based disorders, schizophrenia, hypertension, headache, antisocial personality and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and low scores tend to be related to increased susceptibility to both physical and psychological disease, whereas high scores are associated with greater mental and physical health. Individual Response Stereotypy and Stimulus– Response Stereotypy “Individual response stereotypy” and “stimulus– response specificity” are somewhat complex categorization schemes that also involve the measurement of a number of psychophysiological measures to look for patterns in the responses. Stimulus– response specificity refers to different stimuli, such as a cognitive versus a physical stressor, producing idiosyncratic patterns of responding. Individual response stereotypy, on the other hand, is seen when an individual evidences a single, distinctive response pattern to all stimuli (Sternbach, 1966). For example, one individual may characteristically respond to stressful events with increased heart rate, whereas another might evidence lowered hand surface temperature (stereotypy); alternatively, one individual may reliably respond to a mental arithmetic task with increased respiration rate and lowered respiration depth, while responding to an ischemic pain stressor with increased frontal SEMG. The Law of Initial Values The “law of initial values” refers to the effect that prestimulus values of a particular psychophysiological measure have on that response’s magnitude of psychophysiological reactivity to a specific 129 stimulus (Wilder, 1950). The higher the level of the measure prior to presentation of a stressful stimulus, the smaller the increase in response to the stressor (often referred to as a “ceiling effect”). Conversely, the higher the level of the measure prior to presentation of a relaxing stimulus, the larger the decrease in response to the relaxing stimulus (prestimulus–response values that are low prior to the presentation of a relaxing stimulus lower the magnitude of the response and are often referred to as “floor effects”). While the law has generally been shown to hold for measures of respiration and cardiovascular activity (e.g., heart rate and the vasomotor response), measures such as salivation and electrodermal response have not been found to be influenced by prestimulus values. Homeostasis “Homeostasis” refers to the tendency of any organism to strive to maintain a state of equilibrium or rest (Baptista, 2006). Homeostasis is believed to be maintained by a negative feedback loop, a hypothesized bodily mechanism that provides information that directs the physiological system to decrease activity if levels of functioning are higher than normal, or to increase activity if levels are diminished compared to normal. Thus, all organisms strive to return to prestimulus levels of physiological arousal when presented with any stimulus. Applied psychophysiology research has demonstrated that there are limits beyond which increases and decreases in the physiological response cannot be trained. Orienting and Defensive Responses “Orienting and defensive responses” refer to the way organisms respond to unique and novel stimuli; the response is both behavioral and physiological (Campbell, Wood, & McBride, 1997; Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 2007). The orienting response can be viewed as the “What is it?” response. It typically involves “an increased sensitivity of the sensory organs, body orientation towards the stimulus, increased muscle tone with a reduction of irrelevant motor activity, EEG activation, vasoconstriction of the peripheral vascular system, vasodilatation of the cranial vascular system, increased skin conductance, respiration amplitude increase accompanied by decreased respiration rate, and a slowing of the heart rhythm” (Sturgis & Arena, 1984, p. 16). Because it is impossible to ascertain which portions of the initial responses 130 to a stimulus are orienting responses and which are actual responses to the stimulus, applied psychophysiologists generally disregard the responses to the beginning stimuli when analyzing response patterns. The orienting response generally habituates quickly, but it has been shown that responses to both psychologically and physiologically relevant stimuli habituate at much slower rates. In contrast to the increased attention toward a stimulus that is the orienting response, the defensive response is defined as a turning away of attention, usually from a painful stimulus or a stimulus that is too intense. Physiologically it is similar to the orienting response, but with increased heart rate and constriction of the cranial vascular system. It is generally believed that the orienting response habituates more quickly than the defensive response. Carryover Effects The term “carryover effect” is a basic research methodology notion that refers to the effect that prior research conditions can have on subsequent conditions (Box, Hunter, & Hunter, 2005). It is important to note that in addition to carryover effects from the actual experience of the condition, a temporal factor may also cause participants to fatigue over time (see below) and show a change in response pattern unrelated to the condition being evaluated (Sturgis & Arena, 1984). For example, a biofeedback therapist may present a variety of stressful conditions in a psychophysiological assessment, and the presentation of stressor 1 may affect stressor 2, and the presentation of stressors 1 and 2 may affect stressor 3; in addition to the specific carryover effects of stressors 1 and 2, stressor 3 may have been affected by the patient becoming fatigued over time. There are two generally acceptable solutions to the problem of carryover effects in research methodology. First, and most conservative, is to avoid the use of a repeated measures design and use only an independent groups design. The primary limitation of this design for clinical biofeedback therapists is the need for large sample sizes and, of course, the average clinician does not have the resources, the time, or the patience to conduct such studies. A second solution to the problem of carryover effects is to use a counterbalanced design, that is, to vary the order of the conditions in a random manner. This is something that the average clinician can do, since it does not involve a large number of patients. One major concern about this II. INSTRUMENTATION design is the inherent assumption that carryover effects are equivalent between the differing conditions. That is, the carryover effect of stressor 1 is exactly the same as the carryover effects of stressors 2 and 3. The possibility of interactions among the conditions and differential practice effects is not controlled. Unfortunately, there has been little research examining the possibility of carryover effects in applied psychophysiology, and the limited available data are inconclusive. Temporal Stability of Measures Temporal stability, or reliability, of measures used in psychophysiological assessment and treatment has been a topic of increasing importance in the past 30 years. If an assessment measure is not stable over time, it is a poor indicator of what is purportedly tested. Given the wide range of factors that can affect the magnitude of the various psychophysiological responses, not surprisingly, there can be difficulties in obtaining stable recordings across time. Moreover, when limits are set on the reliability of a measure, there is generally an inability to obtain a high estimate of validity (i.e., whether the measure actually records a true representation of the concept supposedly assessed). To illustrate the difficulties involved in temporal stability of psychophysiological assessment, consider the following scenario: Ms. X, a single mom who suffered from chronic headaches, came to her therapist’s office for a pretreatment psychophysiological assessment. The appointment was at 5:30 P.M. on a cold winter’s day in January. Ms. X got little sleep the night before because she was up all night taking care of her 6-year-old son, who had a stomach virus. Work was extremely difficult (she worked as a secretary in a lawyer’s office), and she was guzzling coffee all day just to stay alert. Ms. X had an argument with a coworker about who was responsible for a botched copying job just before leaving at 5:00 P.M. Traffic was horrible, and she was stuck behind a slow driver in the left lane. Ms. X arrived at the office just in time, after finding a parking spot two blocks away, where she was promptly ushered into the biofeedback and psychophysiological assessment laboratory and given a brief explanation by a therapist she had only met once. Sensors were attached to her forehead, neck, shoulders, two of her fingertips, and around her chest. She was asked by the therapist, who was speaking to her over an intercom from an adjoining room, to perform several tasks, including immersing her hand in a bucket of ice water 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines and counting backwards from 999 by 7’s. She wondered the entire time how long the session would last, because the sitter had to leave by 7:00 P.M. At the end of the assessment, she was asked if she had any questions and she responded, “No.” An appointment was scheduled for treatment and she rushed home. Halfway through treatment, on a rather warm day in mid-March, the therapist retested Ms. X to “determine if treatment has had any effects yet on your body’s responses to stressful situations.” This day, however, in contrast to the testing in January, Ms. X had a great night’s sleep. Her 6-yearold had come home the day before with all A’s on his report card. The appointment was at 9:30 A.M., and she received permission from her boss to take the morning off. She found a parking spot directly in front of the office and while waiting for 10 minutes, planned a shopping trip for later that morning. She had lost 10 pounds since beginning therapy, and her clothes were now loose. She was ushered into the biofeedback and psychophysiological assessment laboratory by her therapist, and they were both joking, since they were now well-acquainted with each other. The procedure was repeated. Ms. X’s reaction to the stressors was greatly reduced, and the therapist stated, “We now have hard evidence that the treatment has already had an effect on your body’s responses to stress.” Is this actually the case? Can we truly arrive at that conclusion? Could the reduction in magnitude of the response to the stressors merely have been a result of repeating the test? Could it have been a result of more sleep? Weight loss? Differences in time of day? Comfort with the therapist? Seasonal temperatures? Such questions underscore the vital importance of temporal stability research and the relevance of factors such as age, gender, race, weight loss, situational and trait anxiety, and so forth. Review of the Temporal Stability Literature Because of the topic’s importance, we review the essential literature concerning temporal stability of psychophysiological responses. We focus on measures of SEMG and surface skin temperature, because these are the two non-EEG responses most often used in biofeedback training. Sturgis (1980) was one of the first researchers to investigate temporal stability. She examined the frontal SEMG response, bilateral cephalic vasomotor response, and digital vasomotor responses in 10 subjects with migraine and 10 with tension 131 headaches. Overall test–retest reliability of the measures was 0.31, which, albeit statistically significant, accounted for a small proportion of the variance. We (Arena, Blanchard, Andrasik, Cotch, & Myers, 1983) began our studies of reliability using a normal population. Fifteen undergraduate subjects were assessed on multiple response measures (frontal and forearm flexor electromyography (EMG), heart rate, skin resistance level, hand surface temperature, and cephalic vasomotor response) under multiple stimulus conditions (baseline, relax whole body deeply, warm hands, relax forehead, mental arithmetic, positive imagery, stressful imagery, cold pressor) on multiple occasions (days 1, 2, 8, and 28). Subjects were screened for medical conditions, and all assessments occurred at approximately the same time of day. Three forms of reliability coefficients were computed for each response measure: coefficients on absolute scores and two coefficients on relative measures—percentage of change from baseline and change scores from baseline to stressful conditions (the term “relative measures” refers to any measure other than the actual raw value of the response; i.e., the actual raw score of the psychophysiological response has been changed or transformed in some manner. This is typically done to decrease the wide variability that is often found in psychophysiological responses, as well as to control, in SEMG studies particularly, differences between various equipment brands, etc.). Results indicated that, for absolute values of the measures, only frontal SEMG seemed consistently reliable, while hand surface temperature was reliable if sessions were repeated within 1 week. Heart rate and forearm flexor SEMG were somewhat less consistently reliable. Lower reliability coefficients were generally obtained when responses were treated as relative measures. We concluded that investigators must first ascertain the reliability of these measures on their respective subject population and subsequently employ in their research only those measures that are found to be reliable with that population. Another conclusion was that since frontal SEMG and hand surface temperature were the primary biofeedback modalities, and they were fairly reliable, clinicians should merely be wary of falsely attributing baseline hand temperature increases solely to biofeedback training, which may result partly from habituation to the clinical situation. Speckenbach and Gerber (1999) essentially replicated our results (Arena et al., 1983; Arena, 132 1984) and those of others (Sturgis, 1980; Schaffer, Sponsel, Kice, & Hollensbe, 1991) concerning the reliability of blood volume pulse. Burnham, McKinley, and Vincent (2006) measured intrasession skin temperatures bilaterally in 17 healthy subjects in the hand, forearm, shoulder, thigh, shin, and foot using a thermistor and two infrared thermometers. They found that intrasession reliability was high and similar for each device used (all r's ≥ .9), also replicating our results. Other investigators suggested examining a more complex patterning of the responses. Waters, Williamson, Bernard, Blouin, and Faulstich (1987) built on the work of Manuck and Schaefer (1978). These experimenters differentiated groups of “reactors” and “nonreactors” based on cardiovascular responses to difficult cognitive tasks, and found stability in these designations when subjects were retested a week later. Waters et al. (1987) compared 30 college students, using five stimuli and 10 psychophysiological measures over 2 weeks. The magnitude and range of correlations were similar to those in the Arena et al. (1983) study. They also analyzed individual response specificity with the Profile Similarity Index (PSI; Buco & Blouin, 1983), providing a single index of overall similarity or reliability of the two response profiles. For reactivity, with the PSI, at least 87% of the subjects showed similarity. Probably their most revealing analysis was derived from comparing subjects’ ranks (on a scale of 1 to 10) with a ranked hierarchy of standardized physiological scores for each subject and for the 10 psychophysiological measures. Waters et al. (1987) averaged the ranks across the stimulus procedures. Fifteen of 30 subjects ranking 10th in one session, and 14 ranking ninth, were ninth or 10th in the second session. Similarly, 29 of the subjects ranked first or second in the first session were first or second in the second session. Those ranked between these extremes in the first session varied considerably in the second, and some went to the other extreme. The researchers concluded that “it is thus clear that the most extreme responses in an individual's psychophysiological response hierarchy are the most stable (reliable) across experimental sessions” (p. 219). Building on the work of Waters et al. (1987), Arena, Goldberg, Saul, and Hobbs (1989a) argued that analysis of both individual response stereotypy and stimulus–response specificity might provide a perspective on reliability not available from the traditional Pearson correlational procedures commonly employed, or from an analysis of II. INSTRUMENTATION only individual response stereotypy. A multivariate response pattern approach might have some predictive validity. For example, some (Engel, 1960) might argue that clinical populations would have more stability than normals in the particular response system presumed to be abnormal (e.g., patients with low back pain in the paraspinal muscles, patients with headache in the forehead or upper trapezius muscles), whereas others (Sternbach, 1966) might argue the opposite. We therefore examined the temporal stability of three response measures (forehead EMG, hand surface temperature, heart rate) on 64 college and community volunteers during four sessions over a monthlong interval. Each session included an adaptation period, a baseline condition, a cognitive stressor (serial 7’s), and a physical stressor (a cold pressor task). Reliability coefficients on the absolute scores across conditions were, for the most part, modest and statistically significant. Treating the responses as relative measures again produced smaller and less frequently significant correlational coefficients. The data were also examined in a multidimensional manner using z-scores to determine whether each subject showed any consistencies across sessions with respect to which response system was maximally aroused. This analysis led to identification of three groups of subjects: those who responded primarily within a single system across sessions regardless of stressor (individual response stereotypy, 42%), those who responded differentially across sessions to the two stressors (stimulus–response specificity, 20%), and those with profiles not readily classifiable (38%). Results supported the notion that psychophysiological measures achieve some degree of meaningful reliability over time. We also argued that identification of clinical patients who fit the stimulus–response specificity pattern may have great clinical relevance. For example, a headache sufferer who responds to physical stressors with hand surface temperature, but to mental stressors with SEMG may require psychophysiological intervention targeting both response systems. Moreover, the clinician may need to investigate these response patterns in terms of the stimuli most readily eliciting them. This may explain why some of our headache patients fail to respond to a singlemodality biofeedback intervention. Presumably, a less complex therapeutic approach would suffice for the more common stereotypical responder. Others have also examined reliability of psychophysiological assessment from a individual response stereotypy and stimulus–response speci- 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines ficity perspective (Berman & Johnson, 1985; Foerester, 1985; Foerster, Schneider, & Walschburger, 1983; Robinson, Whitsett, & Kaplan, 1987) and have come to similar conclusions. An excellent review of temporal stability of psychophysiological response patterns can be found in an article by Heinz, Huber, Schreinicke, and Seibt (2002). Shaffer et al. (1991) studied the 1-week reliability of resting baseline psychophysiological activity for several autonomic nervous system variables assessed for 5 minutes after a 15-minute stabilization period. The 21 male and female undergraduates, ages 18–21, reclined with their legs supported and their eyes open. The stability was high for skin conductance level (r = .89), moderate and statistically significant for heart rate (r = .63), abdominal amplitude (r = .63), finger temperature (r = .54), and respiration rate (r = .49), and low and nonsignificant for blood volume pulse (r = .23). As did most of the studies examining at the temporal stability of psychophysiological measures, in our studies before 1990, we had employed the Pearson product–moment correlation coefficient as our primary correlational measure of psychophysiological intersession reliability. Statisticians would argue that the intraclass correlational coefficient (Kirk, 1995) is a more appropriate reliability statistic when employing more than two test–retest intervals. Intraclass correlations take into account changes in values, not just the relative proportion of scores. This is especially useful in psychophysiological measures on which initial intensities commonly vary. More importantly, intraclass correlations allow simultaneous incorporation of more than one set of values on the same subjects. Therefore, Arena and Hobbs (1995) reanalyzed their 1989 study data using the intraclass correlation and found that with the exception of SEMG during the physical stressor (cold pressor task), the absolute values of the responses (forehead EMG, hand surface temperature, heart rate) had quite significant reliability (.70 or greater). They concluded that statistical estimates of psychophysiological response reliability are functions of the study design and particular reliability analysis employed. Gerin and his colleagues (1998), in a seminal article, looked at the reliability of cardiovascular responses (blood pressure and heart rate) and the generalizability of these responses across various settings. Twenty-four female college students (age range 17– 26) were given a mental arithmetic task (serial 13’s) following a 12-minute baseline twice in the laboratory (to examine test–retest reli- 133 ability), once in a classroom and once at home. Adequate test–retest reliability was found for the baseline condition (.81 for systolic blood pressure, .63 for diastolic blood pressure, and .68 for heart rate). However, poor reliability was found, using change scores from baseline to mental arithmetic task (absolute values were not given in the report) for heart rate response (.09), while systolic blood pressure (.68) and diastolic blood pressure (.62) had adequate reliability. When examining for generalizability, on all three measures, smaller correlational coefficients were obtained for the nonlaboratory settings compared to the laboratory setting. The authors concluded that “this suggests that even a minor variation in procedure, such as a change in setting, can affect generalizability” (p. 209). They further state that if we are to find predictive power from the laboratory to the natural environment, there is no dimension of variability so trivial that it can be dismissed without investigation. If simply changing the location of the test site can reduce the lab-to-life associations, then altering more significant aspects of the test situation, such as the task or the subject’s motivation, is likely to do even more damage to the stability of reactivity as an individual difference. (p. 217) (For a review of the topic of heart rate variability, we refer the reader to Appelhans and Luecken [2006] and Wheat and Larkin [2010]). A number of studies have examined the reliability of heart rate variability and have for the most part found it to be reliable. To illustrate, Guijt, Sluiter, and Frings-Dresen (2007) examined the test–retest reliability of time-domain heart rate variability and respiration rate measurements in 26 normal subjects over two sessions separated by a week. Using a portable device, they took measurements during three conditions: lying down, cycling, and sleeping. Both time-domain heart rate variability (r's between .74 and .85) and respiration (r's between .75 and .98) were found to be highly reliable. Similarly, Carrasco, Gonzalez, Gaitan, and Yanez (2003) examined heart rate variability by measuring 11 normal subjects for 5 minutes three times a day over a 5-day period during a number of conditions (lying on the back, standing, diaphragmatic breathing, exercise, recovery). Most of the intraclass correlations were quite high (r's ≥ .68). Others have found a more complicated pattern. For example, Nussinovitch and colleagues (2011) recorded heart rate variability from 70 healthy volunteers for 5 minutes (the standard 134 recording time). They then recalculated heart rate variability based on randomly time-sampled 1-minute and 10-second periods. They found that “good correlations between the 5-minute electrocardiograms (ECGs) and both the 1-minute and 10-second ECGs were noted for average RR (the time elapsing between two consecutive R waves in the electrocardiogram) interval, and root mean square of successive differences in RR intervals. No correlation was noted for standard deviation of the RR interval and several other HRV parameters” (p. 117). Thus, heart rate variability is reliable depending on the time interval sampled and the type of measurement analysis employed. Veit, Brody, and Rau (1997) in an interesting study, examined the stability of cardiovascular measures (heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure) in response to a laboratory psychological stressor (mental arithmetic) in 75 adults over a 4-year test–retest interval. They found adequate reliability for both absolute and change score from baseline for heart rate (.81 absolute, .76 change score) and systolic blood pressure (.52 absolute, .66 change score) measures. However, the absolute value correlation for diastolic blood pressure was .27, and the change score coefficient was only .16. Researchers and clinicians have now begun to look at the reliability of SEMG in nontypical electrode sites, such as the back and jaw, as well as to examine the reliability of psychophysiological measures in clinical populations. Castroflorio and colleagues (2005) measured in nine healthy subjects the masseter and temporalis anterior muscles daily for 3 consecutive days. During each session, subjects sustained for 30 seconds three isometric contractions at 80% of maximal force. Excellent reliability was found for the maximal force measures. Recently, Auchincloss and McLean (2009) measured the reliability of pelvic floor muscle (PFM) SEMG using two different vaginal probes on two tasks: maximum voluntary contractions and a coughing task. They found relatively little difference between the two probes and good betweentrial reliability coefficients (ranging from .58 to .98). Between-days reliability, however, was quite poor. They concluded that “although it is acceptable to use PFM surface sEMG as a biofeedback tool for training purposes, it is not recommended for use to make between-subject comparisons or to use as an outcome measure between days when evaluating PFM function” (p. 85). Arena, Sherman, Bruno and Young (1990) made bilateral SEMG recordings of paraspinal muscle II. INSTRUMENTATION tension in 29 subjects with lower back pain and 20 normal subjects in six different positions (standing, bending from the waist, rising, sitting with back supported, sitting unsupported, prone) on two occasions. Measures were highly reliable when examined with analysis of variance procedures. As with other research, statistically significant reliability coefficients were obtained when the absolute values of the measures were examined, and when examined as relative [percent change from baseline (prone) condition] values, differences between the two groups were observed: The normal controls were statistically more reliable than subjects with lower back pain during every condition. This study demonstrates how important it is to examine reliability of SEMG measures in both normals and clinical populations. Lariviere, Arsenault, Gravel, Gagnon, and Loisel (2002) measured bilateral SEMG from four back sites in 40 subjects (half controls/half patients with chronic low back pain) during various tasks, including a dynamometer task, fatigue, and recovery, twice within 2 weeks. For the most part, measures were reliable. They found that reliability was highest in the most fatigable muscle. Callaghan, McCarthy, and Oldham (2009) measured superficial quadriceps surface SEMG in 29 health control subjects, 74 subjects with patellofemoral pain syndrome, and 55 subjects with knee osteoarthritis at 60% of maximum voluntary contraction over 3 days. They concluded that “poor between-days reliability and high measurement error suggests that surface sEMG should not be adopted to assess fatigue during multi-joint, submaximal isometric quadriceps testing” (p. 172). Netto and Burnett (2006) measured the reliability of maximal voluntary isometric contractions and submaximal (60%) isometric contractions bilaterally from eight sites in the C4–C5 level of the neck during a number of conditions (flexion, extension, left/right lateral bending) in five healthy male subjects, then retested 2 weeks later. Both maximal and submaximal contractions had excellent within-day reliability, but only maximal voluntary isometric contractions had adequate between-day reliability. Finally, a number of investigators have also looked at the temporal stability of an ambulatory monitoring device for surface SEMG levels. Arena (2010) has noted that telemedicine and ambulatory monitoring is one of the primary future directions in SEMG research and clinical work. Arena and colleagues (1994) had 26 healthy controls wear a lightweight (24 ounce) device that mea- 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines sured bilateral upper trapezius EMG, as well as peak and integral motion, for 5 consecutive days for up to 18 hours each day. Intraclass correlational coefficients for the two SEMG variables across the 5 days were both significant, with alpha levels set at .01. The two SEMG measures were highly correlated (r = .77); the two motion measures were also highly correlated (r = .60). Reliability coefficients for the SEMG measures were similar to those found in laboratory studies. They concluded that the test–retest reliability of the ambulatory monitoring device was within acceptable limits. Airaksinen and Airaksinen (1998) also examined the reliability of an SEMG device for ambulatory recording and found it to be highly reliable. 135 function of demographic factors (e.g., age, gender, and race), clinical populations, laboratory versus nonlaboratory settings, caffeine and nicotine consumption, and psychological characteristics (anger, anxiety, depression, etc.). 8. For the most part, measures of heart rate variability have been found to have adequate test– retest reliability. 9. We would urge every clinician to gather some rudimentary baseline data on the reliability of the measures routinely employed in clinical practice. This provides some general indication of equipment reliability, as well as the effect of a particular clinical setting (type of room where measures are routinely obtained, pictures, the therapist variables, quietness, etc.). Temporal Stability Conclusions There are a number of conclusions that can be drawn from the results of the studies reviewed earlier. 1. For forehead EMG, heart rate, blood pressure, and hand surface temperature, the majority of the studies indicate at least statistically significant reliability coefficients of modest magnitude. 2. The amount of variance, even in those measures with the correlations of greatest magnitude, suggests that other factors account for more variance than does the experimental manipulation (i.e., retesting). 3. Reliability is affected significantly by the statistical approach employed (i.e., analysis of variance, absolute value Pearson product– moment correlations, relative value [percent change from baseline, raw change scores from baseline], Pearson correlations, intraclass correlations, or analysis of response patterns, such as individual response stereotypy or stimulus– response specificity), with most studies suggesting that relative value correlations produce lower size correlations than absolute value correlations. 4. It is probably prudent to use multiple measures of arousal rather than rely on any one measure. 5. Hand surface temperature appears to be a very complex response that may be affected by repeated measurement. 6. More research is vitally needed, especially on the temporal stability of measures such as electrodermal response, digital blood volume pulse, respiration, and SEMG other than forehead. 7. More research is needed on reliability as a Conditions Generally Employed in Psychophysiological Assessments Adaptation Period The importance of an adaptation period in psychophysiological research has long been a topic of discussion; unfortunately, there has been little empirical research to date indicating the optimum duration of an adequate adaptation period. An “adaptation period” is defined as the time the subject spends in the experimental situation prior to the onset of baseline measures or the experimental conditions, or as Andrasik and Lords (2004) state, “adaptation refers to a client becoming comfortable and returning to a normal level of functioning” (p. 223; emphasis in original). The function of an adaptation period in psychophysiological research and clinical work is threefold: 1. It allows the subject to become familiar with the novel, experimental situation, as most people are unaccustomed to having sensors attached to various parts of their anatomy while they sit with their eyes closed in a soundand light-attenuated room. 2. It allows presession effects to dissipate, such as stress, rushing to the appointment, walking up flights of stairs, and significant temperature discrepancies between the outdoors and the office. 3. It allows habituation of the orienting response and permits the stabilization of psychophysiological responses. If these responses fluctuate prior to the experimental manipulation or the recording of tonic levels of physiological functioning, there is uncertainty whether the inde- 136 pendent variable (e.g., diagnosis, experimental instructions, biofeedback training) led to the findings, as opposed to random variations secondary to an insufficient period of stabilization. Thus, an adaptation period is especially salient in early sessions of biofeedback training. Likewise, an adaptation period is especially important if advocacy of the law of initial values yields a need to examine a patient’s physiological responses using relative rather than absolute scores (Wilder, 1950). If baseline or prestimulus levels are unstable, the relative measures—generally raw change scores pre- minus postmeasure or percent change scores from baseline—may be drastically influenced and results potentially vitiated. There have been only a few studies investigating what constitutes an adequate adaptation period; unfortunately, all were published before 1990. Meyers and Craighead (1978), in a rather confusing study, found that respiration rate, finger pulse volume, heart rate, and basal skin resistance can reach stability in an average of about 5–6 minutes, although there was a great deal of variability between subjects, with some needing almost no adaptation period and others requiring a lengthy one. Taub and School (1978), in an anecdotal study, found that some individuals required as much as 30 minutes to stabilize on hand surface temperature response. Frontal SEMG stabilization occurred in an average of 11 minutes across a very small group of 17 undergraduate nonclinical students (Sallis & Lichstein, 1979). However, among these subjects, there was “considerable idiosyncrasy of the sEMG adaptation response” (p. 339). This suggests there would probably be much variability among clinical patients, especially for those with high levels of forehead muscle tension. Lichstein, Sallis, Hill, and Young (1981) reported a gender effect for heart rate response, in which males were adapted from onset and females required 13 minutes for an adequate adaptation period. They also found that adaptation periods of 7 and 13 minutes were necessary for, respectively, skin resistance level and frontal EMG. Many factors merit consideration during planning and implementation of adaptation and baseline (see below) recordings (as well as for psychophysiological feedback). Even after making these global changes, individual differences in responses and patterns still exist, and clinical judgment must enter into the picture. One size (or adaptation) does not fit all. II. INSTRUMENTATION In practice, the clinician usually tailors the cognitive preparation and adaptation time to the patient and situation, although there is no standard. Practitioners need to be aware of the potential impact of instructional set on their patients and should consider standardizing it as much as possible and always documenting the instructions they use. Other practitioners will appreciate a clinical or research report that includes this information. It can ease replication and application to clinical practice. The total duration of the stabilization phase varies and depends on several factors, including time in the waiting room. Other practical factors include the physical condition of the patient arriving at the office, the physiological activity monitored, the therapist's purpose, and the number of prior sessions. Rashed, Leventhal, Madu, Reddy, and Cardoso (1997) suggested that cardiovascular responses (heart rate, blood pressure, and hand surface temperature) to cold pressor stress are significantly attenuated by exercise. Kim et al. (2006) note that this phenomenon does not hold for anaerobic weight-bearing exercise. Thus, an adaptation period is especially important when incorporating such a task into a psychophysiological assessment. Practitioners should monitor and record moment-by-moment physiological functioning to check the potential effects of brief orienting responses and should, for example, watch for events such as abrupt noises. Brief orienting responses are of potential clinical use as well. For example, the person who is more physiologically responsive to low-level environmental stimuli may require different procedures. Habituation is rapid after orienting responses but can affect summary scores of adaptation and other periods. Psychophysiological arousal can occur with perceived and bona fide threatening stimuli present. Habituation of this type of response is usually slow and variable (Sturgis & Grambling, 1988). Several potentially threatening factors associated with office visits can increase muscle tension and autonomic arousal. An example occurs in a person who feels threatened by physicians or mental health professionals. Sitting quietly for several minutes, reclining, and being connected to the instruments are other examples of stimuli that can be threatening for some persons. Practitioners should check these factors when planning and interpreting adaptation and other baseline periods. Repeated exposure to potentially threatening stimuli within 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines a longer initial session or repeated sessions might be necessary to obtain adequate adaptation. Additional research is needed to develop more standard guidelines for adaptation. We suggest that one commonly needs at least 5 minutes for adaptation. However, sitting quietly for up to 20 minutes may be necessary for some patients. For example, Taub and School's (1978) anecdotal report suggests that for some persons, even 30 minutes is not enough for stabilization of hand temperature. Shorter times are probably enough for most people seen in clinical practice. Adaptation often occurs between 3 and 5 minutes with instruments attached, especially for patients who have waited several minutes in a waiting room. Neutral conversation is acceptable if the goals are adjustment of body position and adjustment to the instruments. Therapists should consider omitting conversation if the goal is allowing the physiological systems of interest to settle down. During this phase, therapists typically give no specific instructions to the patient except to sit quietly and get comfortable. Arena and his colleagues (1983) usually tell their patients, “Sit quietly with your eyes closed for the next couple of minutes” (p. 450). Other therapists may wish to have patients sit with their eyes open (see discussion in baseline section, below). The basic goal is to get the patient to sit quietly and get used to the clinical or experimental situation. Another perfectly acceptable criterion is a floating adaptation period, which has no prescribed length. Rather, the clinician or investigator has a preset criterion for stabilization of each response (e.g., heart rate must remain plus or minus 3 beats/ minute for a full minute, or SEMG response cannot fluctuate by more than 5% for a full minute), advancing to another condition once that criterion is met. This saves time with those individuals who are already stable, is tailored to the patient’s physiological responding, and ensures that all patients achieve stabilization. However, disadvantages also exist: The therapist must focus deeply on a patient’s responding and risk the patient needing, as Taub and School (1978) noted, 30 minutes to stabilize. Fortunately, the latter problem is solved with a modified floating adaptation period (i.e., patients meet the floating criterion or 10-minute time limit, whichever comes first). Regardless of the adaptation period used, we urge clinicians to employ a specific strategy consistently. In psychophysiological assessment and treatment, as in most things, consistency is half the battle. 137 Psychophysiological Baselines Although the actual instructions during a baseline period are usually identical to those provided for adaptation, the two conditions serve different functions. A “baseline period” is defined as the period following adaptation, in which psychophysiological response measures have stabilized (prior to the onset of any experimental or clinical manipulation; e.g., a stressor condition or biofeedback). The purpose of this condition is to observe and measure resting basal physiological activity. We believe that this condition is essential, because, nearly always, the practitioner compares the baseline or resting condition values to the experimental or treatment conditions. As noted earlier, relative values, most generally raw change scores from baseline or percent change from baseline, are dependent on a baseline condition. A baseline is essential in cardiovascular research, in which nearly all measures of cardiovascular reactivity use relative scores. Some (Sella, 2005) have argued that for SEMG research, the term “baseline” should not be used; rather, “resting tonus” should be used in its place. We (Arena, 2005) disagree with this approach based on the fact that “baseline” is a well-established term that has been used for over a century in both behavioral and psychophysiological research/clinical practice, as well as the fact that the concept of baseline refers to a measurement, whereas “resting tonus” is the phenomenon that one is measuring. Whether a patient’s eyes are open or closed during baseline is an area of disagreement among clinicians and researchers. There are no available data on this subject, so personal preference determines choice. Here, the two authors of this chapter differ slightly. Arena conducts baselines with the patient’s eyes closed during nearly all psychophysiological assessments, and most biofeedback training sessions. He reserves an eyes-open baseline period for biofeedback when a patient expresses a preference for visual feedback or generalization training (usually, after the patient is sufficiently skilled in producing the biofeedback response, instruction advances to reproduce more challenging “real world” factors). Schwartz (1995) believes that eyes-closed baselines are suitable for conditions such as insomnia and are less realistic for assessing baseline physiology for headaches and other symptoms that occur with eyes open. For example, he asserts that using only an eyes-closed 138 baseline can lead a therapist to conclude incorrectly that there is no excess muscle activity. Commonly, more muscle activity in the head and facial muscles exists when the patient’s eyes are open. Schwartz (1995, p. 152) stated that Therapists should get baseline data with eyes open when patients' symptoms start with their eyes open and when biofeedback with eyes open is planned. Observing lower arousal with eyes open than with eyes closed is potentially useful. It provides cues about what it means for patients to close their eyes. It raises questions to answer about what patients are thinking about and doing when they close their eyes. When eyes are kept open, therapists should consider instructing patients to include time calmly gazing at an object such as a picture or plane. They should remind patients to avoid staring or examining the object they are looking at as well. Regardless of which baseline recording strategy one chooses, both Arena and Schwartz urge careful consideration and consistent application during clinical/research practice. This ensures a large database, and the larger the database is (and we would argue that clinical experience creates a very strong database, indeed), the more sure one can be about one’s observations and conclusions. As noted earlier, there are few data about what constitutes an adequate baseline, and much of the research confuses baseline with adaptation periods. For example, Hastrup (1986) reviewed the methodology and duration of baseline conditions in an exhaustive review of cardiovascular reactivity studies and found nearly no agreement among the studies in terms of methodology and duration of the baseline periods. She recommended that the baseline period be at least 15 minutes in duration for cardiovascular reactivity experiments to ensure the lowest possible baseline recordings. Jennings, Kamarck, Stewart, Eddy, and Johnson (1992) found similar results, with five of 24 studies reviewed having baseline periods of 10 minutes or more, five having between 6 and 10 minutes, and the remaining 24 with less than 5 minutes’ duration. Gerin, Pieper, and Pickering (1994) found that a 1-minute baseline was sufficient for stabilization of heart rate and systolic and diastolic blood pressure. For those interested in conducting methodologically complex psychophysiological research, Jamieson (1999) wrote an excellent article about baseline differences in psychophysiological recording, indicating that (1) relative values such as change scores are confounded with baseline II. INSTRUMENTATION whenever data are skewed, and (2) when baseline differences are real, analysis of covariance has a directional bias that magnifies differences in one direction and minimizes those in the other direction. Jamieson provides suggestions for identifying and correcting these problems. Finally, Piferi, Kline, Younger, and Lawler (2000), arguing that simply resting quietly does not ensure equivalency between individuals, present data suggesting that showing a relaxing video of the sea achieves a greater degree of relaxation and a more accurate recording of baseline measures than does the traditional baseline condition, at least for measures of cardiovascular reactivity. We would posit, however, that the baseline condition should not “obtain the lowest possible resting rates along the same point on the continuum of excitement” (p. 215), and Piferi and her colleagues are actually creating a relaxation condition (see below). Clinical Considerations in Psychophysiological Baselines For resting baselines and for office-based stressors, clinical practitioners need to be very cautious when interpreting physiological data. Comparisons of resting baselines across sessions are complex for many patients. Knowledge of this fact is important when clinicians generalize to other situations and compare data across sessions. Many practitioners view each session's resting baselines as largely new situations, at least for most autonomic-mediated variables. Marked shifts in muscle and autonomic activity often occur after the patient sits quietly for several minutes. Muscle activity can steadily or suddenly drop. Finger temperature can gradually or suddenly increase. Heart rate can plummet. Therefore, baseline periods should be considered up to 15 minutes in a very early biofeedback session and in some therapy sessions that can capture these changes. It also helps to check for physiological changes that occur in the extended relaxation periods outside the therapist's office. There is no fixed or proper time for all people and all circumstances. Realizing that these changes can occur before therapy can help to increase a patient's confidence. It is also important to document the lack of change, especially when changes begin to occur later during feedback and nonfeedback phases. This information is useful to the therapist, because such changes in an initial session do not mean that one needs therapy less. Among the important therapeutic goals are shortening the time before 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines the therapeutic changes occur and increasing the degree and replicability of such changes. Shorter baseline phases, such as 1 to 3 minutes, also are feasible and proper under some conditions. For example, if we consider the patient sitting quietly with eyes closed, his or her muscle activity may remain low and steady, with very little variability for about 1–2 minutes among multiple muscles in the head and neck. The muscle activity will probably not change significantly over the next few minutes. Cost containment and other pragmatic factors (time constraints, patient’s becoming restless, etc.) argue for the shortest baseline phases that can typically answer evaluative and therapy questions. There are circumstances in which the clinician wants or needs additional baseline data. Obtaining such data can entail more than one baseline session and extending some baseline phases even beyond 15 minutes. For example, when there is much variability within or between sessions, one can justify longer resting baselines. Another example occurs for disorders such as sleep-onset insomnia, when the relaxation sessions at home are long. Some patients show increasing roused activity, cooling hands, increasing pulse, and/or restlessness during the first few minutes of a baseline. More than several minutes of a baseline might be unnecessary and counterproductive. Even about 5 minutes may be enough. In such a case, the therapist should consider that relaxation-induced anxiety (RIA) may be present (for a detailed discussion of this topic, see Arena & Blanchard, 1996; Schwartz, Schwartz, & Monastra, 2003; or Wegner, Broome, & Blumberg, 1997). When this occurs, the clinician should consider the therapeutic goal to be gradually increasing periods of sitting quietly without increases in physiological arousal. Pretreatment and periodic physiological baselines are less practical under some circumstances. For example, there are limitations in the schedules of some patients, as in the case of the patient who lives a few hundred miles from the therapist and there is no qualified professional to whom one can refer the patient. The therapist is consulting only for one session that focuses on the intake interview and patient education for treatments thought to help reduce physical symptoms. There is time for a brief biofeedback session, but not enough time for a desired baseline. The therapist decides instead to get only a brief baseline of about 5 minutes and invest the remaining instrumentation time to providing feedback. The therapist instructs the 139 patient in relaxation techniques and provides education booklets and audiotapes. The patient then goes home and practices as instructed. The lack of physiological baseline data does not always compromise therapy. One properly adjusts priorities, maintains the patient's best interests, and can initiate therapy. If this patient returned for further therapy, one could still get a physiological baseline. Practitioners can discuss the ideal with the patient and note in their reports the reasons for proceeding differently. Conversely, there are conditions for which one can justify multiple physiological baseline segments or sessions. Such a situation occurs when one suspects that the patient has symptoms that fluctuate in intensity at different times. Examples of such times are soon after specific eliciting or emitting events (e.g., eating, upsetting discussions, physical activity, and certain times of day). Therapists should consider scheduling office sessions to coincide with or immediately follow such events. The absence of excess tension or arousal during a resting baseline does not mean the person has adequate control. This also is true for patients who show a lack of significant reactivity to a stressor. Therapists should always consider such factors as the possible effects of medications, the office environment, baseline conditions, and the limitations of simulated stressors. For example, some people do not react to office stressors. There are individual differences in how therapists present stressors. Presentation style can affect the stressor's effect. Physiological tension and arousal, reactivity, and slow recoveries in one office session often do suggest similar functioning in daily life. That is, one can generalize from the psychophysiological assessment setting to real-life settings. However, office sessions do have limits. Tension, physiological reactivity, and slow recovery in one session do not mean that the person reacts the same way in other situations. At best, these are snapshots or glimpses of a person's psychophysiological activity in daily life. Office procedures are sources of hypotheses and information for productive discussions. However, they are not always reliable evidence for a patient's daily functioning. Conditions Involving Assessment of Self‑Control Abilities Relax Deeply Condition Because many psychophysiological interventions are believed to work through the final common 140 pathway of relaxation, obtaining some measure of patients’ ability to relax on their own prior to any treatment and periodically during treatment is often useful. The instructions that Arena and colleagues have used for this condition (Andrasik, Blanchard, Arena, Saunders, & Barron, 1982; Arena, Blanchard, Andrasik, Applebaum, & Myers, 1985) are simply, “Please try to relax as deeply as you possibly can.” Schwartz (1995) uses the following instructions: “Now, rest quietly a little longer. Use whatever methods you think best to relax. Focus on the muscles of your face, head, and shoulders. Let yourself go and release the tension in different parts of your body. If you feel that you have to move, scratch, sneeze, or something else, go ahead and do it. This phase lasts a few minutes. Don’t think of problems or upsetting events and do not worry about how well you are doing. Whatever degree of relaxation achieved is all right.” (p. 154) Warm Hands Condition This condition is directly relevant to thermal biofeedback training, which involves teaching hand warming through mental means. Obtaining some measure of a patient’s ability to increase hand temperature prior to treatment is useful, particularly when assessing whether the skills have developed or been learned. There are many ways to test or prepare the patient for psychophysiological learning. The most common, by far, is a “self-control” condition that is interspersed between a baseline and a feedback segments. During the self-control period, the patient is asked to control the desired psychophysiological response (in this instance, hand temperature: “Please try to warm your hands through purely mental means”) without any feedback. If the patient can control the response, the clinician may infer that between-session learning has occurred. Such a phase can be routinely added after the second or third biofeedback session. Sometimes this condition is presented after the biofeedback portion. If a patient can control the response, then the practitioner may infer that within-session learning has occurred. “Generalization” involves preparing the patient to, or determining whether or not the patient can, apply the learning that may have occurred during the biofeedback session to the “real world.” One can partially or tentatively infer this from the previous procedures and also obtain temperature measurements in other situations. II. INSTRUMENTATION Relax Muscles Condition This condition is directly relevant to SEMG biofeedback, which involves teaching reduction of muscle activity. Obtaining some measure of a patient’s ability to decrease muscle activity prior to treatment is useful. The instructions are adapted to the muscle group recorded (e.g., “Please try to relax the muscles of your forehead for the next couple of minutes”). Personally Meaningful Positive Imagery Condition This condition is often included as a control condition that is compared to the negative imagery condition. This is frequently useful, because individuals’ abilities to imagine vary greatly, and without this phase, a practitioner may mistakenly believe that a patient has no reactivity to a negative imagery condition when, in reality, he or she is merely poor at imaging. Consider asking the patient for a vividness score of the scene after the condition is over. Prior to the assessment, consider asking him or her to describe a very pleasant scene experienced previously, with descriptors of images targeting a majority of the five senses. Also, consider requesting that the scene rate a 9 or 10 on a 1- to 10-point scale of pleasantness. Information is recorded and modified to refine the scene until consensus is reached. During the assessment, the instructions used are as follows: “I’d like you to try to imagine, to picture in your mind’s eye.” The pleasant scene is then read. In addition to variable imaging skills, another problem is the absence of control for the experimenter effect. That is, some therapists or experimenters are rather low key and likely to read the description of the image in a monotone, while others will read it with dramatic flair. Thus, some researchers and practitioners provide more standardized negative imagery tasks that use tape-recorded instructions/imagery. However, standardization, or a one-size-fits-all approach is imperfect, as we indicate below. Stressor Reactivity or the Stimulation Phase It is often useful to introduce cognitive and physical stressors to check for psychophysiological reactivity and rate of recovery. Both reactivity effects and recovery can help to identify causes and correlates of biobehavioral disorders and can potentially help to predict those persons at risk for these disorders (Haynes, Gannon, Orimoto, O’Brien, & 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines Brandt, 1991). These authors also note that assessing reactivity can help develop effective interventions. Lovallo (2005) presents a complex theory for how psychophysiological reactivity can lead to cardiovascular disease and posits a number of treatment implications. This section covers reactivity, while the next section focuses on recovery. Although many providers use stress stimuli routinely before starting therapy, some do not. However, using stress stimuli may help therapists answer some patients' questions about the ways their physiology reacts to stimuli, while other patients benefit from seeing evidence of their reactivity and recovery. For some patients there is very little, if any, excess tension or arousal while they are sitting quietly or relaxing. The therapist may suspect that this is not typical for a specific patient. Stimulation allows one to examine the patient during and following a stressor presentation, and compare and contrast him or her to normal individuals and to those with the same biobehavioral problems. There are two interdependent pathways in which physiological stress occurs (Haynes et al., 1991). One is the autonomic nervous system (ANS) pathway, especially the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) division. The other is the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical system pathway. The hypothalamus organizes the ANS pathway with input from cortical and subcortical brain structures. This tends to have a rapid onset and short equilibrium time that is the duration of maximum effect. The effects are mostly the results of nerve endings releasing epinephrine and norepinephrine and from the adrenal medulla. In the second pathway, the hypothalamus also regulates the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) from the pituitary gland. This promotes the release of cortisol from the adrenal cortex. These effects are slower and have a longer equilibrium latency or time until maximum effects. The time is longer than that resulting from epinephrine and norepinephrine. Thus, the duration of a stressor strongly influences its impact. Many studies show that shortduration stressors elevate neurotransmitters, but longer-duration stressors suppress them. Longerduration stressors deplete norepinephrine, lift the inhibition of ACTH, release cortisol, and suppress the immune system. Transient stressors often used in laboratory studies and clinical practice may not be sufficient for health-inhibiting effects. The nature of the stress—physiological or psychological—is important. For measuring primarily 141 psychological stressors, therapists should be aware that serum cortisol responds more to subjectively distressing, uncontrollable, and psychologically prominent stress (Dienstbier, 1989). In contrast, the ANS-mediated catecholamine responses, such as epinephrine and norepinephrine, respond to nearly all stimuli such as startle, cognitive, exercise, and mild electric shock (Haynes et al., 1991). See Asterita (1985), Haynes, Falkin, and SextonRadek (1989) and Kronenberg, Melmed, Polonsky, and Larsen (2008) for more detailed discussions of this topic. Stress and stimulation constitute part of evaluations and treatments for conditions other than those treated with relaxation. For example, when evaluating patients with fecal incontinence, therapists should use simulated stimulation to the lower bowel. This checks for reactivity of the internal and external anal sphincters. It also checks for ineffective tensing of the gluteal and abdominal muscles. For patients with urinary incontinence, therapists sometimes introduce fluid into the bladder to check for sphincter control. During this procedure, practitioners also check for ineffective tensing of abdominal muscles. Other examples involve patients undergoing muscle reeducation. Therapists often ask patients to hold, carry, walk, push, or bend to evaluate their muscle actity. Response magnitude such as peak reactivity during a stressor is a commonly used psychophysiological response parameter for assessing the effects of stress (Haynes et al., 1991). Researchers and practitioners use cognitive and physical stressors to examine ANS- or central nervous system (CNS)mediated reactivity. A variety of stimuli are in use in clinical practice. Practitioners assume that the stimuli are stressful. However, for some individuals, this may not be the case. In some cases, it is merely orienting or mild stimulation. The following are a few stimuli in clinical and research use and abbreviated sample instructions. For each, if instructions are not included with the description of the stressor, the therapist should assume an introductory phrase (e.g., “In a few moments I will ask you to . . . ” or “When I ask you to, please . . . ”). 1. Mental arithmetic. “When I tell you to, please start at (an arbitrarily chosen large number; e.g., 986) and count backward by 7’s (or 8’s, 9’s, or 13’s) keeping your eyes closed.” Alternatively, “Please read silently (or aloud) each math problem and write down (or call out) the answer” (e.g., 121 + 767 = ?; 326 – 74 = ?; 18 × 12 = ?; Linden, 1991). 142 This is probably the most commonly used office stressor in research and clinical practice. Advantages (Linden, 1991) of a mental math stressor are ease of administration and lack of equipment requirements (unlike video games, reaction times, or cold pressor tasks). At most, only a method to visually present equations is necessary. No ethical concerns should arise from an Institutional Review Board (IRB), Human Subjects Research Review Committee, Ethics Committee, or Clinical Practices Committee. In addition, the technique of mental arithmetic offers a wide range of variations for adaptations to specific patients and for repeated presentations. However, a potential problem for comparing studies and procedures stems from the lack of universally accepted standardized procedures, as indicated by Strike and Steptoe (2003): “It should be noted that there are no standard agreed protocols for mental stress testing. It cannot be assumed that mental arithmetic, for example, is the same challenge in different studies. Few studies have collected subjective ratings, behavioural performance measures, or other indicators of stressfulness” (p. 697). Linden (1991) provides a useful review and a series of studies of the effects of vocal versus written versions, noise distraction, and different types of math tasks. The most arousing, at least for cardiovascular reactivity, are those involving vocal responding, noise distraction, and solving visually presented equations (Linden, 1991). One may expect some attenuation of the reactivity with repeated presentations of the same math task (Sharpley, 1993). Thus, the therapist should consider different math tasks if he or she uses repeated stressor presentations involving math. 2. Tense muscles. “Make a fist.” “Clench your teeth.” “Try to open this tightly closed jar.” “Shrug your shoulders.” “Bend slightly at the waist.” “Hold this package with both hands.” 3. Personally meaningful negative imagery. Similar to personally meaningful positive imagery (discussed earlier) except the patient is asked to imagine something very unpleasant. 4. Memory tasks. “Remember this story exactly as I say it.” 5. Hyperventilation. “Inhale and exhale very quickly and deeply for 2 minutes.” Or, “Inhale and exhale through both your mouth and nose. Each time you inhale try to fill your lungs completely. Each time you exhale try to empty your lungs completely.” Or, “Inhale every time I say ‘In.’ Exhale II. INSTRUMENTATION every time I say ‘Out.’ ” (One can use an audiotape to signal inhalations and exhalations. See Gevirtz and Schwartz, 2003, for discussion of hyperventilation, Hyperventilation Provocation Test, and cautions.) 6. Prerecorded loud noises or other unpleasant sounds. Examples are a baby crying, car horns repeatedly blowing, or listening to people screaming at each other. For combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder, often sounds of war, such as helicopters and machine gun fire, are used. 7. Cold exposure and cold pressure. “When I say ‘Start,’ I’d like you to place your right hand up to your wrist in the bucket of ice water. Then close your eyes. Please keep your hand in the ice water until it hurts so badly (or becomes too uncomfortable) that you want to remove it or until I tell you to remove it. Any questions? OK. Start.” For excellent reviews of the methodology of the cold pressor test, certainly the most widely used physical stressor in psychophysiology today, as well as the physiology involved, see Mitchell, McDonald, and Brodie (2004) and Velasco, Gomez, Blanco, and Rodriguez (1997). 8. Action and challenging video games. 9. Slides of stressful scenes or videotaped trauma. “Look at these slides (or video).” 10. Difficult quizzes. “Complete this quiz. Most people get a score of at least .” 11. Your Everyday Life Pressures and Holmes– Rahe Visualizations. Rosenthal et al. (1989) developed two brief and practical stressor tasks for research that are of potential clinical use. The first involves a number of stressful vignettes based on the Holmes and Rahe (1967) Social Readjustment Scale, which ranks a number of stressful life events in terms of severity. Instructions are as follows: “Please close your eyes. Visualize yourself in the following situation(s). Try to see yourself in the situation. Feel just how this situation hits you. Really get into it! Try to make it as real and vivid as you can—include the sights, sounds, smells and emotions. Imagine how you would react as clearly as possible.” The second stressor task, which involved generally stressors of lesser intensity, was termed a YELP (Your Everyday Life Pressures) task. The YELP task involved eight selected vignettes “depicting frustrating, disappointing, or otherwise noxious” situations. Many doctoral-level clinicians selected these eight YELP tasks from among 48 potential 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines items. Interested readers should refer to Rosenthal et al. (1989) or Schwartz (1995) for additional information regarding the YELP task items, including sample vignettes. 12. Ischemic (blood pressure tourniquet) pain (e.g., Johnson & Tabasam, 2003; Pinerua-Shuhaibar et al., 1999). 13. Exercise step-up test (e.g., Feinstein et al., 1999; Lim, Shields, Anderson, & McDonald, 1999). 14. Stroop Color Test. One of the oldest reaction time tasks in psychology (MacLeod, 1991), the Stroop Color Test has been in existence since 1929. In this task, patients are asked to read the name of a color that is presented to them. If the color of the ink matches the name of the color, the naming takes less time and people are more accurate than if the name of the color does not match the color of the ink. The therapist or experimenter can make the task even more stressful by having people return to the beginning of the task if they make a mistake or by stating, “Most people are faster than you on this task,” and so forth. Obviously, one does not use all or most of these techniques with each patient. Research and clinical practice usually include from one to three stressors. Most require at least 1 minute and usually up to 4 minutes for each presentation. There are individual differences in reactivity, hence the rationale for using multiple stimuli of different types. Instructions probably have an arousal effect for at least some physiological responses, such as heart rate (Furedy, 1987; Sharpley, 1993). This probably results from several factors, such as attending to the instructions and anxiety associated with the uncertainty and challenge of the task. This additional arousal effect can confound the assessment of reactivity of the stressor. Therefore, the therapist should consider measuring the reactivity during the instructions and separating this from the stressor task data. With computer-based psychophysiological systems one can create periods or trials designated as instructions. Some practitioners insert another period of about 1 minute after the instructions and before instructing the patient to start the task. They assume that this allows the patient to relax and allows for measuring the effects of instructions and anticipation. However, some patients may prematurely start some cognitive task during this 143 period. Therapists can circumvent this easily by instructions such as “In a few moments I will ask you to. . . . Keep relaxing until I say to begin.” For counting backwards, therapists should wait until after the interspersed postinstruction period to give the numbers. One vitally important area of research that has been nearly overlooked is which stressor is better for which type of patient under what condition. Yoshida and colleagues (1999) compared the ability of cold pressor, hyperventilation, mental arithmetic, and exercise step-up stressors to induce an angina attack in 29 patients with vasospastic angina pectoris. They found that the hyperventilation task was least effective (13%), with cold pressor and mental arithmetic equally effective (27 and 28%, respectively) and the step-up test most effective (55%). Similar research is needed for other types of disorders, such as headache, panic, Raynaud’s disease, and so forth. Poststress Adaptation Periods Psychophysiological and other biobehavioral disorders often have important physiological components. Implicated as causal factors are environmental and other stressors. However, the interactions among behavioral, cognitive, and physiological factors are complex. The strength of the relationship between the magnitude of reactivity and other indices of psychological functioning is often modest (Haynes et al., 1991). Haynes et al. also observed a modest ability for reactivity to distinguish between persons with a disorder and those without it. Hence, there is interest in both the rate and degree of recovery to help explain etiology and plan clinical interventions. The goal of many clinical interventions is changing the psychophysiological response to stress (Cacioppo, Berntson, & Anderson, 1991; Haynes et al., 1989; Schwartz & Andrasik, 2003). Therefore, psychophysiological poststress recovery is of crucial importance. Implications include etiology and treatment of psychophysiological disorders. Specifically, recovery indices may help identify causal mechanisms of many biobehavioral disorders. They may help identify persons at risk for these biobehavioral disorders and help in the development of effective interventions and evaluations of treatment. For example, if a patient reacts in a normal fashion during a variety of stressors but takes much longer than normal to return to baseline levels following 144 these stressors, two reasonable inferences can be made. One is that this individual has a physiological impairment or abnormality that causes a return to baseline slower than that of normal individuals. Two, he or she needs to develop psychological and psychophysiological coping strategies immediately following a stressor to reduce physiological arousal as quickly as possible and return to a basal quiescent state. Within-study differences between stressor and poststress recovery results constitute a very important index of the importance and potential use of poststress recovery. This implies that these two indices stem from different mechanisms. Of the 180 statistical analyses reported by Haynes et. al. (1991), 81 showed nonsignificant effects of stressors. Of these 81 individuals, 74% showed significant recovery phase effects. Conversely, when stressor effects were significant in 74 analyses, recovery phases showed nonsignificant effects for 42% of the same variables. Stress effects and recovery very often differ in terms of sensitivity and potential utility. Impaired recovery or slowness of recovery after psychophysiological reactivity to one or more stressors is the focus of using poststress recovery stages. Nearly all theories of psychophysiological disorders include an impaired recovery process as one of their central tenets or in their definition of psychophysiologic abnormality. Specific definitions of poststress recovery vary in the literature. Arena et al. (1989a) defined “recovery” as a return to the quiescent baseline state after stress-induced reactivity. The SEMG recovery was a return to 5% of the mean of initial resting baseline. Hand temperature recovery was a return to within 5% of the baseline mean. Heart-rate recovery was recovery to within 2 beats/minute of the baseline. Other definitions of recovery include “changes in stressor-induced responses following stressor termination” and “the rate and degree to which a psychophysiological response approaches pre-stress levels following a stressful experience” (Haynes et al., 1991, p. 356). These definitions allow for nonlinear and bidirectional changes. It is different from a return to a prestressor quiescent baseline state. The time course of recovery is the magnitude of the response over time after stopping a stressor. It is sometimes nonlinear and may diverge from prestressor levels. For example, arousal sometimes increases or becomes unstable. Very few studies specifically address the optimal time period for assessing poststress recovery (Arena, 1984; Arena, Bruno, Brucks, & Hobbs, II. INSTRUMENTATION 1992). The physiological variables in these two studies included cephalic vasomotor response, frontal and forearm flexor EMG, hand temperature (left hand), heart rate, and skin resistance. Arena (1984) studied 15 college undergraduates (about age 20) and reported that a 3-minute poststress period was adequate to return to a basal quiescent state for most of several psychophysiological measures. Frontal EMG, however, needed more than 3 minutes to recover. This study “indicated good intrasession reliability on all measures except frontal EMG, where there was inadequate intersession reliability” (Arena, 1984, p. 247). A follow-up study (Arena et al., 1992) did not completely replicate or support the major findings of the earlier study. This study examined heart rate, hand surface temperature, and frontal SEMG following a cognitive (serial 7’s) and physical (cold pressor task) stressor, and had 6-minute poststress adaptation periods. Most subjects (about 78%) returned to baseline within 6 minutes for heart rate. Average times were 3.7 and 2.9 minutes for the two poststress periods. However, for forehead EMG, only 48% returned to baseline in the 6 minutes. The average time was 4.6 minutes and 5.1 minutes, respectively, for the two poststress periods. For hand surface temperature, 6 minutes “was clearly inadequate” for most subjects. Only about 38% returned to baseline in this period. The average time was about 4.5 minutes and 5.6 minutes, respectively, during the two poststress periods. The percentages returning to baseline appeared higher in the first poststress period. For example, hand temperatures returned to baseline in nearly 48% of subjects after the first cognitive stressor (mental arithmetic), compared to only about 26% in the second poststress period (cold pressor task). Although the earlier study indicated that about 3 minutes are required for cardiovascular and heart rate modalities to return to a prestress basal level, the latter study indicated that much longer times are necessary. Arena et al.’s (1992) speculation about the differences focuses on the larger sample size and the wider age range of the second study. The age range was 17–75, and the mean age was nearly 33. These two studies used serial 7’s from a large, random, three-digit number as the cognitive stressor and cold pressor (to the right hand) as the physical stressor. Exposure was 4 minutes for the cognitive stressor and up to that for the physical stressor. This cognitive stressor is probably milder 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines than that used by many others, and the cold pressor is a more intense physical stress than most clinicians use (and likely to evoke cardiovascular effects that prolong recovery vs. most other officebased physical stressors). The subjects were college students and community volunteers of various ages; hence, caution should be used when generalizing to patients. However, a reasonable assumption is that implications from the more recent study are more applicable to patients. Except for the limitations described earlier concerning these studies, we believe this type of research is important and useful. Findings support the need for recovery periods. They indicate differences in the durations of these periods among modalities and document the duration of the recovery periods under specified conditions. Poststress periods should be at least 6 minutes if patients must return to a baseline. This is feasible in clinical practice, using two or three stress periods during an evaluation session. However, it is typically impractical during routine therapy sessions if multiple stressors or intense stressors are used. The cognitive stress in these studies is only serial 7’s for 4 minutes. This is not universally stressful for everyone. As we have noted concerning laboratory and office-based stressors, there are many other inferences that applied psychophysiology researchers make. One is that laboratory stressful conditions are comparable to stressors found in the everyday world. Having both a) placed my hand in a bucket of ice water up to my wrist and kept it there until I couldn’t stand it any longer, and b) been in an airplane for three hours with screaming children in the seats directly in front and in back of me, I can tell you that equivalence of laboratory and “real world” stressors is a very dubious proposition, indeed. (Arena, 2000, p. 22) Clinical Vignettes of Psychophysiological Assessments We present a number of situations in which the biofeedback therapist might use psychophysiological assessments to answer clinical questions. We highlight the pain literature (headache and lower back pain), because of our expertise. First, though, we present some caveats. Arena (2000) purported that “much of the research and clinical pain work that utilize psychophysiological assessments or ‘stress profiling’— including those that employ surface sEMG measures—are based on inferences which have not 145 been empirically tested and nearly all psychophysiological assessments have not been empirically demonstrated to have any clinical utility” (p. 21). He further stated: The biggest inference that clinicians routinely proceed upon is that conducting a psychophysiological assessment or a “stress profile” will give them important information in helping to determine how to proceed in treatment. . . . This assumption has never been empirically tested. That is, no study has shown that if you have a pain patient who demonstrates one particular sEMG abnormality, compared to a different sEMG abnormality or no abnormality, that that person does better in one type of treatment compared to a different type of treatment. Such research is vitally important and must be conducted if our field is to continue to grow and flourish. (pp. 22–23) Such research is especially important when standard treatments are expected to produce very high rates of success, as is the case with psychophysiological treatments for headache (Andrasik, 2010; Arena & Blanchard, 2005). When we achieve such high rates of success by providing everyone the same standard treatment, the psychophysiological assessment must add significant predictive value, or the justification for its use is lacking. Simply because such research has not been conducted, though, does not mean that clinicians should stop conducting psychophysiological assessments. Clinicians are often making inferences and going where “no literature has gone before.” The inferences that we and other clinicians make are our best judgments, based on clinical experiences, assessment literature, and the indication that the presence of certain abnormalities or findings warrants a specific treatment direction. We should be humble and understand the limits of our interpretations. A little tentativeness goes a long way. With that very important caveat discussed, we proceed with some clinical examples. Straightforward SEMG Assessment of a Tension Headache Patient A therapist wants to ascertain in the intake whether Mrs. Smith, a tension headache patient, would likely benefit from biofeedback, and, if so, would feedback from one muscle site be more likely to achieve treatment success than feedback from another muscle site. Her reported tension and pain are primarily in the forehead and in the upper back and/or posterior neck. In a psychophys- 146 iological assessment, the therapist measures forehead SEMG and bilateral upper trapezius SEMG levels during two baseline conditions, a 5-minutes eyes-open and a 5-minute eyes-closed condition, preceded by a 10- to 15-minute adaptation period to the room and sensors with eyes open. The therapist decides to stop at this point and not continue with other aspects of an assessment, as he observes that Mrs. Smith has approximately four times the normal SEMG levels in both forehead and upper trapezius muscle groups, based on office normative data; the therapist does not notice any left–right upper-trapezius muscle differences. He tentatively concludes that (1) SEMG biofeedback would likely help Mrs. Smith due to her elevated muscle tension, and (2) he can provide feedback from either the forehead or upper trapezius regions and would most likely choose to provide feedback from the forehead as it is the standard placement for tension headache. Being a careful clinician, he conducts the same assessment prior to the first biofeedback session. Because he obtains the same results he has a greater level of certainty concerning his findings,and thus feels more confident in following his treatment plan. There is probably no need for additional assessment at this point. Throwing a Monkey Wrench into the Straightforward SEMG Assessment of a Patient with Tension Headache Let us suppose the same scenario as that presented earlier, except that the therapist on the second assessment comes up with different results than the therapist on the first assessment. Now, forehead SEMG levels remain at the same magnitude, but trapezius readings are different: The right trapezius levels are about four times higher than his normative group, but Mrs. Smith’s left trapezius is 10 times the normative levels. What can the therapist do in this situation? There are multiple possible strategies and no clearly right or wrong answers to this situation. First, the therapist could decide that he needs more information and conduct a more detailed psychophysiological assessment, including stressor conditions and assessment of various postures and positions, either immediately or at the next session. While this would give more information, it would add significant cost to the treatment regimen and, while it could simplify the treatment picture, it could also cloud it even more. Second, he could follow his original treatment plan, as forehead SEMG abnormalities were found, and forehead II. INSTRUMENTATION SEMG biofeedback is a standard treatment for tension headache (Arena & Blanchard, 2005). He is still left, however, with the nagging question of what do about the trapezius findings. He could still give trapezius feedback should Mrs. Smith prove refractory or have insufficient headache relief from forehead feedback. Third, he could continue with his plan of forehead feedback and monitor (but not have the patient attempt to control) bilateral trapezius levels. If on repeated biofeedback sessions this asymmetry continues, he may wish to change the focus of the feedback to correcting the left–right trapezius asymmetry if it does not dissipate (as, following a general relaxation theory of frontal biofeedback, it is likely to do). Fourth, he could decide to use the second assessment results and change the focus of Mrs. Smith’s feedback to correcting the left–right trapezius asymmetry. He could then give forehead feedback should Mrs. Smith prove refractory or obtain insufficient headache relief from trapezius feedback. Fifth, he could decide to give Mrs. Smith feedback from all three SEMG sites. This has the advantage of giving her a more clear picture of her psychophysiological abnormalities but might provide too much information, interfering with the psychophysiological learning process. Sixth, he might devote part of each session to each muscle area or focus on the different areas in different sessions. If the reader is getting confused by the wide variety of treatment options posed by the psychophysiological assessment results, then we have been successful in our endeavor to point out that even simple assessments often do not have simple answers. However, it is important to note that all the previous options are perfectly defensible. Dr. Arena would probably choose the third option, and Dr. Schwartz, the fifth or sixth. We urge you to document in your report the rationale for selecting a particular treatment direction so others can understand your thought processes. A More Involved SEMG Psychophysiological Assessment of a Patient with Tension Headache The psychophysiological assessment of Mrs. Smith assumes no abnormalities during the adaptation and baseline conditions. The therapist plans another assessment the next week that comprises the same adaptation and baseline conditions, three stressors (personally meaningful stressful imagery, mental arithmetic–serial 9’s, and a cold pressor task) of 4 minutes each interspersed with 3-minute poststress adaptation periods, followed 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines by assessments in six different positions (standing, bending from the waist, rising, sitting with back unsupported, sitting with back supported, and prone). Mrs. Smith again is within normal limits during both baselines, but during the personally meaningful stressful imagery, she has about six times the normal forehead SEMG levels. During both cognitive stressors, she takes longer to return to baseline on forehead SEMG, and during the standing and sitting unsupported positions she has approximately 10 times the upper trapezius SEMG levels than do normals. There are no left–right upper-trapezius muscle differences during any condition. Based on these data, one can tentatively conclude that • EMG biofeedback would likely help Mrs. Smith due to the psychophysiological assessment displaying abnormal patterns of SEMG responding. • Since forehead SEMG levels were more abnormal during the stressor and poststress recovery conditions, the therapist decides to use forehead SEMG biofeedback initially. • Following Mrs. Smith learning the forehead SEMG biofeedback response, the therapist will switch to the upper-trapezius muscle group. • When he teaches generalization of the biofeedback response, the therapist will make sure he trains Mrs. Smith to reduce her muscle tension levels when she is standing and sitting, with her back both supported and unsupported, as abnormalities in trapezius SEMG were noted during the standing and sitting unsupported positions. Given the fact that an impaired recovery process was found during both cognitive stressors for forehead EMG, the therapist will repeatedly practice with Mrs. Smith to rapidly decrease her forehead SEMG levels, and he will emphasize the importance for her to reduce forehead muscle tension levels immediately following a stressful situation in her daily living. Repeated assessment of the stressor and position conditions would be helpful, but cost-effectiveness and practicalities of clinical work probably preclude another assessment. Also, given that the portion of the assessment the therapist repeated was unchanged, there is more surety in the stability of the psychophysiological responding. One could reasonably question why the therapist did not include measures of other responses such as heart rate, respiration, and hand surface tem- 147 perature. Inclusion of these measures might have enhanced the clinical picture and allowed examination of stimulus–response specificity and individual response stereotypy. His line of reasoning was likely that he has found in his clinical practice that these measures do not add anything to assessment of patient with tension headache, and that they would be prohibitive in terms of time. The Refractory Migraineur: Scenario I A therapist conducts 12 sessions of thermal biofeedback with Mr. Jones, a 49-year-old accountant with migraines since age 16. Mr. Jones mastered hand warming in the office, as well as outside the office, but he has not experienced any headache relief. Therefore, his therapist conducts a psychophysiological assessment with Mr. Jones that comprises a variety of psychophysiological measures (forehead, upper trapezius, posterior neck and frontal–posterior neck SEMG, pulse, hand temperature from multiple sites, respiration, cephalic blood volume pulse, and electrodermal response) in a variety of conditions (adaptation, baseline, a relax body condition, and stressors such as an exercise step-up test, fists, mental arithmetic, and personally meaningful negative imagery, with poststress adaptation periods following each stressor). Mr. Jones responds generally within normal limits during all conditions; however, when examined for individual response stereotypy and stimulus–response specificity during each stressor, Mr. Jones responds with maximal arousal in some of the SEMG measures, and he is able to relax these responses the least during the relax body condition. On the basis of the psychophysiological assessment, the therapist began SEMG biofeedback from those sites, including frontal and focused daily relaxation instructions for those sites and reinforced frequent daily practice of varying durations. This is a logical and defensible treatment plan based on the psychophysiological data. One could postulate that the assessment was contaminated by the thermal biofeedback, and perhaps this was the reason Mr. Jones did not respond abnormally during the assessment. One could also advance the hypothesis that this explained his not responding stereotypically with hand surface temperature. The reader may also express concern that the therapist did not question why the treatment was ineffective sooner (although some therapists have had patients who did not receive any headache relief until 12 or more sessions). Also, 148 although repeated assessments might have shed further light on Mr. Jones’s psychophysiology, costeffectiveness and clinical practicalities probably precluded another assessment. The Refractory Migraineur: Scenario II In this scenario, the therapist found that Mr. Jones responded within normal limits in the psychophysiological assessment. There were no patterns in the examination of individual response stereotypy and stimulus–response specificity. Defensible possible responses include • Refer him because all likely possible approaches are exhausted, and he is not improving. • Repeat the psychophysiological assessment with different stressors in hope of identifying abnormalities. • Begin searching for other causes of the head pain, such as secondary gains (days off from work, children keeping quiet when headache is present, etc.), dietary factors, psychological characteristics (e.g., depression, anger, or anxiety). • Assume the stressors were insufficient, and consider other relevant stressors for another assessment (e.g., playing a tape of Mr. Jones arguing with his wife). • Attempt a psychophysiological assessment in the natural environment---through ambulatory recordings, accompanying him to work and conducting an assessment in his office, etc. • Check on and focus instructions on daily relaxation practice in terms of frequency, durations, and timing. • Consider including other muscle sites and other skin temperature sites. Routine SEMG Assessment of a Patient with Low Back Pain Some research on low back pain shows significant paraspinal SEMG differences between patiens with low back pain and nonpain controls, as well as between low back patients with pain of differing etiologies (Arena, Sherman, Bruno, & Young, 1989b, 1991; Geisser et al., 2005; Kankaanpaa, Taimela, Laaksonen, Hanninen, & Airaksinen, 1998). Arena often conducts psychophysiological assessments of his back pain patients, using rightand left-sided L4–L5 paraspinal activity and bilateral biceps femoris (a muscle located in the back II. INSTRUMENTATION of the thigh; see Arena and Blanchard, 2002, for a detailed figure depicting the sensor placement) activity in six different positions. He looks for three possible muscle tension abnormalities: 1. Unusually low muscle tension levels (perhaps from nerve damage with resultant muscle atrophy). 2. Unusually high muscle tension levels (the most frequent abnormality). 3. Asymmetry, in which one side of the back or thigh muscles has normal muscle tension levels, while the other has unusually low or high readings. He might use biofeedback to decrease muscle tension in the respective muscle groups. If an asymmetry is found, he will use biofeedback to help patients increase and/or decrease the abnormal sides. One goal is balanced bilateral values within normal ranges. For example, Mr. Doe is a 26-year-old bank clerk and an amateur weightlifter. His paraspinal SEMG levels are five to six times normal on all positions except lying prone, which does not show any abnormalities. There are also no abnormalities with biceps femoris EMG. While sitting with his back unsupported, his left paraspinal muscles are over 30 times normal, and his right side is about five times normal. This is the only time any left– right asymmetry was found during the assessment. Based on the psychophysiological assessment, the therapist conducts bilateral paraspinal SEMG biofeedback while Mr. Doe sits with his back unsupported to correct for the asymmetry. An Evaluation of a Low Back Pain Patient in the Work Setting We cannot overemphasize the importance of tailoring psychophysiological assessments and treatments to each patient. Clinicians must be creative and flexible. Take the example of Mr. Doe and assume that he went through the regimen as described and achieved a 40% reduction in his back pain, but not as much as he and his therapist had wanted and thought possible. The therapist then explored Mr. Doe’s lifestyle in greater detail. The therapist focused on the weightlifting as an obvious and probable area of inappropriate muscle usage but knows that Mr. Doe has been lifting weights for only 2 years, whereas his back problems began 4 years ago. He knows that Mr. Doe began 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines working at a bank 5 years ago. Further inquiry now about his job reveals that one of his major job functions is to assist customers in accessing their safe deposit boxes. The therapist accompanies Mr. Doe to work and notes that many of the safe deposit boxes are quite high, and although there is a ladder in the vault, Mr. Doe uses the ladder only for those boxes he cannot reach. Moreover, their are other safe deposit boxes that are nearly at floor level, and to access them, Mr. Doe must either bend, squat, kneel, or sit on the floor (the latter of which he never does). A workplace psychophysiological assessment with a portable SEMG of the paraspinal muscles when Mr. Doe was either reaching up for a safe deposit box or bending or squatting near the floor revealed massive paraspinal SEMG activity. The therapist then • Instructed Mr. Doe to use the ladder when accessing any safe deposit box that was higher than head level. • Used the portable SEMG biofeedback to instruct Mr. Doe on proper ways to access boxes on the lower wall levels. • Provided Mr. Doe with office-based paraspinal feedback of simulated access to safe deposit boxes, reinforcing what was learned during the evaluation in the vault. • Instructed Mr. Doe to become much more aware of his muscle tension levels whenever he performed his safe deposit box job function, and to conduct relaxation exercises after accessing the boxes, in addition to maintaining the correct form and posture. As a result, Mr. Doe’s back pain was reduced significantly. Although the therapist’s standard psychophysiological assessment included bending from the waist and rising conditions, it was not comprehensive enough to identify Mr. Doe’s problems. This was because bending from the waist condition meant going from straight to about 30 degrees. Generally, this is all that the typical lower back pain patient can do without experiencing significant increases in pain levels. Mr. Doe’s youth and overall physical fitness allowed him to bend much lower than the typical back pain patient, demonstrating the importance of tailoring psychophysiological assessment and treatment to the needs of each patient. (Please note that Mr. Doe is not an actual patient but a combination of three patients for illustrative purposes.) 149 Conclusion Our intent in this chapter was to present to many a reasonably detailed “how-to” on conducting psychophysiological assessments in applied psychophysiology, as well as provide an understanding of the major pitfalls and questions that arise when employing such procedures, especially with patients with headaches or other chronic pain. Our hope is to demystify somewhat the concept of psychophysiological assessment. Good clinical vvcommon sense and a basic understanding of the general concepts of psychophysiology are required. We urge clinicians to utilize psychophysiological assessments when they have questions about their patients’ treatment plans, or when they are confused about what may be causing or maintaining their symptoms. Such evaluations often shed light on complex clinical questions. At the same time that we advocate practitioners employment of such techniques, we caution readers to recognize limitations of psychophysiological assessments and avoid using them to obtain simple answers to complex questions. We are especially concerned that the applied psychophysiologist will give the same assessment to all patients, not tailoring it to the individual patient and disorder. We are even more concerned about practitioners who give every patient a psychophysiological assessment. Such a practice is neither necessary nor fruitful. We cannot rely solely on equipment for decision making and practical application. As anyone familiar with computers knows, the technology is only as good as our related abilities and knowledge. Still, after the best equipment is purchased, clinicians must formulate appropriate questions and develop valid methods to answer them. References Abboud, F. (2010). In search of autonomic balance: The good, the bad and the ugly. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 298, R1449–R1467. Airaksinen, O., & Airaksinen, K. (1998). Ambulatory device for surface EMG recordings. Acupuncture and Electro-Therapeutics Research, 23, 9–17. Andrasik, F. (2010). Biofeedback in headache: An overview of approaches and evidence. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 77, S72–S76. Andrasik, F., Blanchard, E. B., Arena, J. G., Saunders, N. L., & Barron, K. D. (1982). Psychophysiology of recur- 150 rent head pain: Methodological issues and new empirical findings. Behavior Therapy, 13, 407–429. Andrasik, F., & Lords, A. (2004). Biofeedback. In L. Freeman (Ed.), Mosby's complementary and alternative medicine (2nd ed., pp. 207–236). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Andreassi, J. L. (2007). Psychophysiology: Human behavior and physiological response. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Appelhans, B., & Luecken, L. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10, 229–240. Arena, J. G. (1984). Inter- and intra-reliability of psychophysiological post-stress adaptation periods. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 6, 247–260. Arena, J. G. (2000). Surface electromyographic psychophysiological assessment for chronic pain disorders: The necessity for clinical and research inferences to be empirically verified. Biofeedback, 28, 21–24. Arena, J. G. (2005). A rose by any other name: The case for baselines in psychophysiological research. Biofeedback, 33, 115–116. Arena, J. G. (2010). Future directions in surface electromyography. Biofeedback, 38, 78–82. Arena, J. G., & Blanchard, E. B. (1996). Biofeedback and relaxation therapy for chronic pain disorders. In R. J. Gatchel & D. C. Turk (Eds.), Chronic pain: Psychological perspectives on treatment (pp. 179–230). New York: Guilford Press. Arena, J. G., & Blanchard, E. B. (2002). Biofeedback training for chronic pain disorders: A primer. In R. J. Gatchel & D. C. Turk (Eds.), Chronic pain: Psychological perspectives on treatment (2nd ed., pp. 159–186). New York: Guilford Press. Arena, J. G., & Blanchard, E. B. (2005). Assessment and treatment of chronic benign headache in the primary care setting. In W. O’Donohue, N. Cummings, D. Henderson, & M. Byrd (Eds.), Behavioral integrative care: Treatments that work in the primary care setting (pp. 293– 313). New York: Allyn & Bacon. Arena, J. G., Blanchard, E. B., Andrasik, F., Applebaum, K., & Myers, P. E. (1985). Psychophysiological comparisons of three kinds of headache sufferers during and between headache states: Analysis of post-stress adaptation periods. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 427–441. Arena, J. G., Blanchard, E. B., Andrasik, F., Cotch, P. A., & Myers, P. E. (1983). Reliability of psychophysiological assessment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 447– 460. Arena, J. G., Bruno, G. M., Brucks, A., & Hobbs, S. H. (1992, March). What is an adequate psychophysiological poststress adaptation period? In Proceedings of the 23rd annual meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (pp. 90–92). Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Arena, J. G., Bruno, G. M., Brucks, A. G., Searle, J. D., Sherman, R. A., & Meador, K. J. (1994). Reliability of an ambulatory electromyographic activity device for musculoskeletal pain disorders. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 153–157. II. INSTRUMENTATION Arena, J. G., Goldberg, S. J., Saul, D. L., & Hobbs, S. N. (1989a). Temporal consistency of psychophysiological stress profiles: Analysis of individual response stereotypy and stimulus response specificity. Behavior Therapy, 20, 609–618. Arena, J. G., & Hobbs, S. N. (1995). Temporal stability of psychophysiological stress profiles: A re-analysis using intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Reports, 76, 171–175. Arena, J. G., & Schwartz, M. S. (2003). Psychophysiological assessment and biofeedback baselines: A primer. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 128–148). New York: Guilford Press. Arena, J. G., Sherman, R. A., Bruno, G. M., & Young, T. R. (1989b). Electromyographic recordings of five types of low back pain subjects and non-pain controls in different positions. Pain, 37, 57–65. Arena, J. G., Sherman, R. A., Bruno, G. M., & Young, T. R. (1990). Temporal stability of paraspinal electromyographic recordings in low back and non-pain subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 31–37. Arena, J. G., Sherman, R. A., Bruno, G. M., & Young, T. R. (1991). Electromyographic recordings of five types of low back pain subjects and non-pain controls in different positions: Effect of pain state. Pain, 45, 23–28. Asterita, M. F. (1985). The physiology of stress. New York: Human Sciences Press. Auchincloss, C., & McLean, L. (2009). The reliability of surface EMG recorded from the pelvic floor muscles. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 182, 85–96. Baptista, V. (2006). Starting physiology: Understanding homeostasis. Advances in Physiology Education, 30, 263–264. Basmajian, J. V., & De Luca, C. J. (1985). Muscles alive: Their functions revealed by electromyography (5th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Berman, P., & Johnson, H. (1985). A psychophysiological assessment battery. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 10, 203–221. Berntson, G. G., Quigley, K. S., & Lozano, D. (2007). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 182–210). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Box, G., Hunter, J. S., & Hunter, W. G. (2005). Statistics for experimenters: Design, innovation and discovery (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Buco, S. M., & Blouin, D. C. (1983). Use of similarity coefficients when populations parameters are unknown. Paper presented at the 29th annual meeting of the Southeastern Psychological Association, New Orleans, LA. Burnham, R., McKinley, R., & Vincent, D. (2006). Three types of skin surface thermometers: A comparison of reliability, validity and responsiveness. American ­Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85, 553– 558. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Anderson, B. L. (1991). Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines psychotherapeutic process and outcome: Contributions from social psychophysiology. Psychological Assessment, 3, 321–336. Callaghan, M., McCarthy, C., & Oldham, J. (2009). The reliability of surface electromyography to assess quadriceps fatigue during multi joint tasks in healthy and painful knees. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 19, 172–180. Campbell, B., Wood, G., & McBride, T. (1997). Origins of orienting and defensive responses: An evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simmons, & M. Balabam (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 41–68). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Carrasco, S., Gonzalez, R., Gaitan, M., & Yanez, O. (2003). Reproducibility of heart rate variability from short-term recordings during five manoeuvres in normal subjects. Journal of Medical Engineering and Technology, 27, 241– 248. Castroflorio, T., Icardi, K., Becchino, B., Merlo, E., Debernardi, C., Bracco, P., et al. (2005). Reproducibility of surface EMG variables in isometric sub-maximal contractions of jaw elevator muscles. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 16, 498–505. Criswell, E. (2011). Cram's introduction to surface electromyography (2nd ed.). Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett. Dawson, M., Schell, A., & Filion, D. (2007). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 159–181). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84–100. Engel, B. T. (1960). Stimulus–response and individual response specificity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 2, 305–313. Eppinger, H., & Hess, L. (1917). Vagotonia. New York: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company. Feinstein, R. A., Hains, C. S., Hemstreet, M. P., TurnerHenson, A., Redden, D. T., Martin, B., et al. (1999). A simple “step-test” protocol for identifying suspected unrecognized exercise-induced asthma (EIA) in children. Allergy and Asthma Proceedings, 20, 181–188. Foerster, F. (1985). Psychophysiological response specificities: A replication over a 12-month period. Biological Psychology, 21, 169–173. Foerster, F., Schneider, H., & Walschburger, P. (1983). The differentiation of individual-specific, stimulus-specific, and motivation-specific response patterns in activation processes: An inquiry investigating their stability and possible importance in psychophysiology. Biological Psychology, 17, 1–26. Furedy, J. J. (1987). Beyond heart rate in the cardiac psychophysiological assessment of mental effort: The T-wave amplitude component in the electrocardiogram. Human Factors, 29, 183–194. Geisser, M., Ranavaya, M., Haig, A., Roth, R., Zucker, R., Ambroz, C., et al. (2005). A meta-analytic review of surface electromyography among persons with low back 151 pain and normal, healthy controls. Journal of Pain, 16, 711–726. Gerin, W., Christenfeld, N., Pieper, C., DeRafael, D. A., Su, O., Stroessner, S. J., et al. (1998). The generalizability of cardiovascular responses across settings. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 209–218. Gerin, W., Pieper, C., & Pickering, T. (1994). Anticipatory and residual effects of an active coping task on pre- and post-stress baselines. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 139–149. Gevirtz, R., & Schwartz, M. K. (2003). The respiratory system in applied psychophysiology. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 212–244). New York: Guilford Press. Guijt, A., Sluiter, J., & Frings-Dresen, M. (2007). Test– retest reliability of heart rate variability and respiration rate at rest and during light physical activity in normal subjects. Archives of Medical Research, 38, 113–120. Hastrup, J. L. (1986). Duration of initial heart rate assessment in psychophysiology: Current practices and implications. Psychophysiology, 23, 5–17. Haynes, S. N., Falkin, S., & Sexton-Radek, K. (1989). Psychophysiological measurement in behavior therapy. In G. Turpin (Ed.), Handbook of clinical psychophysiology (pp. 175–214). London: Wiley. Haynes, S. N., Gannon, L. R., Orimoto, L., O’Brien, W. H., & Brandt, M. (1991). Psychophysiological assessment of poststress recovery. Psychological Assessment, 3, 356–365. Heinz, A., Huber, B., Schreinicke, J., & Seibt, R. (2002). Temporal stability of psychophysiological response patterns: Concepts and statistical tools. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44, 57–65. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213– 218. Jamieson, J. (1999). Dealing with baseline differences: Two principles and two dilemmas. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 31, 155–161. Jennings, J. R., Kamarck, R., Stewart, C., Eddy, M., & Johnson, P. (1992). Alternate cardiovascular baseline assessment techniques: Vanilla or resting baseline. Psychophysiology, 29, 730–742. Johnson, M., & Tabasam, G. (2003). An investigation into the analgesic effects of interferential currents and transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation on experimentally induced ischemic pain in otherwise pain-free volunteers. Physical Therapy, 83, 208–233. Kankaanpaa, M., Taimela, S., Laaksonen, D., Hanninen, O., & Airaksinen, O. (1998). Back and hip extensor fatigability in chronic low back pain patients and controls. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 79, 412–417. Kim, H., Sohn, D., Lee, E., Kim, Y, Oh, B., & Park, Y. (2006). Coronary blood flow after cold exposure and the effects of warm-up exercise. Journal of the American Society of Echocardiology, 19, 386–390. Kirk, R. E. (1995). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. 152 Kronenberg, H. M., Melmed, S., Polonsky, K. S., & Larsen, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Williams textbook of endocrinology (11th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier. Lariviere, C., Arsenault, A., Gravel, D., Gagnon, D., & Loisel, P. (2002). Evaluation of measurement strategies to increase the reliability of sEMG indices to access back muscle fatigue and recovery. Journal of Electromyographic Kinesiology, 12, 91–102. Lichstein, K. L., Sallis, J. F., Hill, D., & Young, M. C. (1981). Psychophysiological adaptation: An investigation of multiple parameters. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 3, 111–121. Lim, P., Shiels, P., Anderson, J., & MacDonald, T. (1999). Dundee step test: A simple method of measuring the blood pressure response to exercise. Journal of Human Hypertension, 13, 521–526. Linden, W. (1991). What do arithmetic stress tests measure?: Protocol variations and cardiovascular responses. Psychophysiology, 28, 91–102. Lorig, T. S. (2007). The respiratory system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 231–244). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Lovallo, W. R. (2005). Cardiovascular reactivity: Mechanisms and pathways to cardiovascular disease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 58, 119–132. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop Effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. Manuck, S. B., & Schaefer, B. C. (1978). Stability of individual differences in cardiovascular reactivity. Physiology and Behavior, 21, 675–678. Meyers, A. W., & Craighead, W. E. (1978). Adaptation periods in clinical psychophysiological research: A recommendation. Behavior Therapy, 9, 355–362. Mitchell, L., McDonald, R., & Brodie, E. (2004). Temperature and the cold pressor test. Journal of Pain, 5, 233–237. Netto, K., & Burnett, A. (2006). Reliability of normalization methods for sEMG analysis of neck muscles. Work, 26, 123–130. Nussinovitch, U., Elishkevitz, K., Katz, K., Nussinovitch, M., Segev, S., Volovitz, B., et al. (2011). Reliability of ultra-short ECG indices for heart rate variability. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 16, 117–122. Peek, C. J. (2003). A primer of biofeedback instrumentation. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 43–102). New York: Guilford Press. Piferi, R. L., Kline, K. A., Younger, J., & Lawler, K. A. (2000). An alternative approach for achieving cardiovascular baseline: Viewing an aquatic video. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 207–217. Pinerua-Shuhaibar L., Prieto-Rincon, D., Ferrer, A., Bonilla, E., Maixner, W., & Suarez-Roca, H. (1999). Reduced tolerance and cardiovascular response to ischemic pain in minor depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 56, 119–126. Rashed, H. M., Leventhal, G., Madu, E. C., Reddy, R., & II. INSTRUMENTATION Cardoso, S. (1997). Reproducibility of exercise-induced modulation of cardiovascular responses to cold stress. Clinical Autonomic Research, 7, 93–96. Robinson, J., Whitsett, S., & Kaplan, B. (1987). The stability of psychophysiological reactivity over multiple sessions. Biological Psychology, 24, 129–139. Rosenthal, T. L., Montgomery, L. M., Shadish, W. R., Edwards, N. B., Hutcherson, H. W., et al. (1989). Two new, brief, practical stressor tasks for research purposes. Behavior Therapy, 20, 545–562. Sallis, J. F., & Lichstein, K. L. (1979). The frontal electromyographic adaptation response: A potential source of confounding. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 4, 337–339. Schwartz, M. S. (1995). Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Schwartz, M. S., & Andrasik, F. (Eds.). (2003). Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Schwartz, M. S., Schwartz, V. J., & Monastra, V. (2003). Problems with relaxation and biofeedback-assisted relaxation, and guidelines for management. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 251–264). New York: Guilford Press. Sella, G. (2005). A rose is but a rose . . . Biofeedback, 33, 114–115. Shaffer, F., Sponsel, M., Kice, J., & Hollensbe, J. (1991, March 15–20). Test–retest reliability of resting baseline measurements. In Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, New Orleans (pp. 128–130). Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Sharpley, C. F. (1993). Effects of brief rest periods upon heart rate in multiple baseline studies of heartrate reactivity. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 18, 225–235. Speckenbach, U., & Gerber, W. D. (1999). Reliability of infrared plethysmography in BVP biofeedback therapy and the relevance for clinical application. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 24, 261–265. Sternbach, R. S. (1966). Principles of psychophysiology. New York: Academic Press. Strike, P., & Steptoe, A. (2003). Systematic review of mental stress-induced myocardial ischemia. European Heart Journal, 24, 690–703. Sturgis, E. T. (1980, November). Physiological lability and reactivity in headache activity. Paper presented at the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy 14th annual convention, New York. Sturgis, E. T., & Arena, J. G. (1984). Psychophysiological assessment. In M. Hersen, R. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (Vol. 17, pp. 1–30). New York: Academic Press. Sturgis, E. T., & Gramling, S. (1988). Psychophysiological assessment. In A. S. Bellack & M. Hersen (Eds.), Behavioral assessment: A practical handbook (3rd ed., pp. 213– 251). New York: Pergamon Press. Sutarto, A. P., Abdul Wahab, M. N., & Mat Zin, N. (2010). Heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback: A new train- 8. Psychophysiological Assessment and Biofeedback Baselines ing approach for operator’s performance enhancement. Journal of Industrial Engineering and Management, 3, 176–198. Taub, E., & School, P. J. (1978). Some methodological considerations in thermal biofeedback training. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 10, 617–622. Veit, R., Brody, S., & Rau, H. (1997). Four-year stability of cardiovascular reactivity to psychological stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 447–460. Velasco, M., Gomez, J., Blanco, M., & Rodriguez, I. (1997). The cold pressor test: Pharmacological and therapeutic aspects. American Journal of Therapeutics, 4, 34–38. Waters, W. F., Williamson, D. A., Bernard, B. A., Blouin, D. C., & Faulstich, M. E. (1987). Test–retest reliability of psychophysiological assessment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25(3), 213–221. 153 Wegner, D. M., Broome, A., & Blumberg, S. (1997). Ironic effects of trying to relax under stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 11–21. Wenger, M. A. (1966). Studies of autonomic balance: A summary. Psychophysiology, 2, 173–186. Wheat, A., & Larkin, K. (2010). Biofeedback of heart rate variability and related physiology. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 35, 229–242. Wilder, J. (1950). The law of initial values. Psychosomatic Medicine, 12, 392–400. Yoshida, K., Utsunomiya, T., Morooka, T., Yazawa, M., Kido, K., Ogawa, T., et al. (1999). Mental stress test is an effective inducer of vasospastic angina pectoris: Comparison with cold pressor, hyperventilation and master two-step exercise test. International Journal of Cardiology, 70, 155–163. Chapter 9 Consumer‑ and Home‑Based Biofeedback Mark S. Schwartz and Frank Andrasik Rationale and Caveat devices totally on their own without at least a professional’s recommendation, evaluation, and guidance. There are now several biofeedback1 instruments2 designed for consumers, and some are marketed directly to the public. Some include computerbased systems. Others label their devices other than biofeedback. The creation of these devices, mostly in the last several years, their widespread use during the last few years, increased sophistication, inclusion in many research studies, use by many practitioners, and recommendation by many practitioners for patients are among the factors involved in the rationale for reviewing them in this new chapter. Inclusion of this chapter is a major departure from the viewpoints long held by us and many other practitioners that such devices should not be marketed directly to the public. Rather, these devices were best viewed as supplements to officebased interventions when practitioners felt them to be appropriate for use. The chief rationale for this position was the complexity of biofeedback and the resultant need for supervision by appropriately trained therapists. Thus, this topic was purposely not addressed in prior editions of this book. This view is likely still shared by many practitioners and remains valid for many, perhaps most, patients. We still support the view that obtaining consultation, evaluation, and interventions by a properly credentialed professional has many advantages to and is preferable to laypersons purchasing these A Very Brief History of Consumer‑Based Biofeedback Products For many years a few devices were (and continue to be) marketed directly to the public. Other modalities (e.g., single-channel surface electromyograph [SEMG], skin temperature) have also long been available to the public and marketed to practitioners to recommend to selected patients to supplement office-based interventions. Alpha electroencephalographic (EEG) biofeedback devices were available many years ago when alpha biofeedback was part of the formative history of biofeedback. However, we have neither seen them advertised nor have they been available from major manufacturers or distributors for many years. The issues initially associated with devices available to consumers centered more on the potential for inappropriate and unsupervised intervention, presumably by persons searching for help with symptoms and conditions that might not be correctly evaluated, diagnosed, and treated by a layperson or by insufficiently educated and inappropriately credentialed professionals. Quality or accuracy of the measurements was typically much 154 155 9. Consumer‑ and Home‑Based Biofeedback less of a focus, because the early devices were far less sophisticated than the more elaborate devices and software–hardware systems available only to professionals; thus, they were not seen as being competitive. So, What Has Changed? We believe that several factors, summarized in Table 9.1, have influenced the changes. These proposed factors have impacted the availability and acceptance of biofeedback devices and systems to consumers. What Has Not Changed? Direct marketing to the public still means that we assume the following: • There is widespread unsupervised use of such devices. • Many people are buying these devices unneces- sarily. This includes people for whom biofeedback is not indicated or for whom the modality selected is incorrect. • Many people are using these devices incorrectly. • Many people do not benefit, or they have new or worsening symptoms. • However, some of the devices purchased directly by the public are done so at the specific recommendations of health care professionals who are properly credentialed in biofeedback, and who supervise the consumer or provide proper instructions before or soon after the purchase. • There is the potential that some people, failing to find benefit, become disappointed and are thereore less likely to seek professional help that could be beneficial in the future. One might argue that the patient is worse off than he or she was prior to trying the device. • Thus, we assume that some consumers for whom biofeedback is indicated have become discouraged because of misuse or insufficient use of devices. TABLE 9.1. Factors Influencing the Development and Growth of Consumer Devices Factors Sources and comments Increased awareness of biofeedback by the public Lay articles, “word of mouth,” and health care provider recommendations Increased positive attitudes about biofeedback by the public Same as above Increased positive attitudes about biofeedback by health care professionals Published research, credible biofeedback providers Increased acceptance and recommendation by major medical institutions Staff physicians and psychologists, websites Increased sophistication of devices Advances by biomedical engineers Increased information, acceptance, and recommendations by medical websites Medical advice websites that are independent of major medical institutions Increased sophistication of computer software Advances by biomedical medical programmers Increased expense of conventional health care Limited third-party coverage for biofeedback Health insurance companies are sometimes reluctant to reimburse office-based biofeedback due to multiple factors, including costs and varied types of providers Number of providers of biofeedback Growth continues but remains slow, partly due to limited educational/training opportunities Continued evolving/growth of self-help zeitgeist Widespread acceptance of complementary and alternative medicine 156 Are there exceptions, caveats, cautions, and/ or contraindications for biofeedback devices being marketed directly to the public? Yes. The most obvious ones to us involve EEG biofeedback devices. We share the serious concerns of Hammond and Kirk (2007), who go so far as to express “alarm” that “dealers, manufacturers, and trainers . . . have been supplying EEG biofeedback equipment direct to lay persons” (p. 140). Their concern is because • Some of these individuals open their own practices and advertise to the public that they can treat a variety of disorders, all of which are serious and complicated, and for which they have no advanced degrees or health care licenses for independent practice. • Purchasers in the public who attempt to selfmanage their treatment without supervision from a competent professional3 risk negative side effects and adverse reactions ranging from “very mild, transient . . . to very serious conditions” (p. 140). Everyone in the public who is considering the purchase of EEG biofeedback devices, especially without properly credentialed and licensed health care professionals supervising their therapy, needs to read this article. Devices Selected for Brief Review Consumer devices discussed in this chapter include those that are: • Marketed to or available to the public4 without the necessity of a health care professional’s recommendation or prescription. • Recommended by practitioners to supplement office-based interventions. We focus on three devices (RESPeRATE, EmWave, Wild Devine) and a cloud-based system (BFA Monitor). We intentionally do not cover this exhaustively due primarily to space limitations. Excluded from this chapter are: • Devices that are small enough to be portable or ambulatory but are solely available to practitioners. • Devices such as liquid crystal temperature devices and small thermometers attached to fingers that have been used for decades to supple- II. INSTRUMENTATION ment practitioner services. They are available to the public and are considered biofeedback devices by some professionals, although they do not meet the criteria of the official definition5 of biofeedback. • All EEG biofeedback devices. • Temperature-sensitive devices. • Exclusively electrodermal devices. Issues to Consider when Buying, Recommending, and/or Using Consumer‑Available Devices Issues include (1) advantages, (2) ease of use, (3) effectiveness for claimed/expected changes, (4) readability of instructions and explanations, (5) potential for misuse, (6) risks of negative side effects, (7) potential dangers, (8) reliability of the device, (9) accuracy, (10) research, (11) cost, and (12) likelihood of adherence to recommended use by user. Product6 Availability and Contact Information RESPeRATE Resperate, Inc. 220 Meridian Blvd., Suite 07735 Minden, NV 89423 Phone: 800-220-1925 Website: www.resperate.com EmWave HeartMath, LLC 14700 West Park Avenue Boulder Creek, CA 95006 Phone: 831-338-8700 or 800-450-9111 Website: www.heartmath.com Also available from biofeedback distributors. Wild Divine Wild Divine, Inc. 9550 South Eastern Avenue, Suite 253 Las Vegas, NV 89123 Phone: 866- 594-9453 Website: www.wilddivine.com Products include The Journey to the Wild Devine, Relaxing Rhythms, and Wisdom Quest. 157 9. Consumer‑ and Home‑Based Biofeedback Biofeedback distributors (e.g., http://bio-medical. com, www.stens-biofeedback.com) carry all these and other devices. Are These Devices Referred to as Biofeedback by Their Manufacturers? These three devices/systems are not referred to as biofeedback in their published materials and on their websites (see Note 4). For the RESPeRATE, the 2002 510(k) summary from InterCure in Lod, Israel, referred to it as a “biofeedback device . . . intended for use as a relaxation treatment for the reduction of stress by leading the user through interactively guided and monitored breathing exercises.” The device shares the same general intended use in relaxation and/or stress reduction and the same indications for use. Thus, the RESPeRATE is appropriate as a breathing biofeedback-assisted device for use with any symptoms or disorders for which this type of breathing therapy is recommended, although it is currently approved for hypertension by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA), which also notes its value for stress reduction. The Wild Divine website specifically addresses this issue on its home page. It is careful to point out that its products are different from biofeedback devices, which are considered “clinical medical instruments.” The home page emphasizes that its products provide “active feedback with rich, graphical feedback integrated into the training program (not biofeedback), emphasizing that its products “are for educational, entertainment, and leisure use” and “should not be used in place of professional medical care.” It “strongly urges” users “to discuss any and all alternative medical therapies” with their “doctor or health professional” (www.wilddivine.com home page). Advantages of These Devices • Small size. All are very portable and can be carried either in one's pocket (EmWave), briefcase (Wild Devine), or small bag (RESPeRATE). • Ease of use. All are relatively easy to learn. The RESPeRATE is probably the easiest to learn. • Time to learn. Typically, a few hours, which includes reading the manual and interacting with the devices. • Provision of readily understandable feedback. • Reasonably priced. Most cost in the range from about $150 to $350. • Durable. • Reliable. • Accurate. Some Cautions and Possible Contraindications for Using, at Least, the Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Devices • A patient who is using a pacemaker that regulates heart rate. • A patient who uses some heart-regulating medications (especially high dosages). • A patient with chronic low circulation. • A low finger temperature, because it might interfere with obtaining a pulse reading. • A patient who engages in significant movement. Additional Issues with RESPeRATE 1. Strap location. It is surprising that parts of the website instruct the user to place the strap around the chest rather than the usual and preferred abdomen. 2. Frequency of use. The manufacturer’s website and other websites and printed materials recommend 15 minutes at least three to four times per week. They then state that “typically, within 30 days of use, a sustained reduction in blood pressure can be achieved.” Like other physical exercises, regular use is required to maintain the benefits. Concern: This implies that three or four 15-minute periods of relaxed breathing are sufficient to significantly reduce blood pressures and generalize the reductions among persons with hypertension. Although not explicitly stated, the implication is that one does not need to use relaxed breathing more than a total of 1 hour a week to achieve clinically significant results. We believe that many health care professionals who treat people with hypertension consider that much more frequent relaxation, including relaxed breathing, is much preferred and probably needed. 3. The manufacturer’s website states that “ten clinical studies have proven the effectiveness of RESPeRATE on lowering blood pressure.” Concern: The disagreement here is with the term “proven.” “Supported,” yes, but research does not prove anything. 4. The manufacturer’s website highlights and implies support from the Mayo Clinic. Concern: The concern of one author (Schwartz) is the focus on using the Mayo Clinic name. The company has repeatedly used the Mayo Clinic name as an 158 implied endorsement. In prior versions of their website, the implied endorsement was even more explicit. This is inconsistent with the author’s (Schwartz) experience with Mayo Clinic. Uses and Applications In our opinion, several uses and applications are implied. These devices provide biofeedback to help users learn and to facilitate skillful and efficient slow, deep, rhythmic diaphragmatic breathing,7 a major intervention/therapy procedure for patients/clients/persons with a wide variety of symptoms, disorders, and purposes (e.g., anxiety disorders involving psychophysiological arousal— panic, general anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD], phobias, acute stress, migraine headaches, hyperventilation, functional chest pain, functional nausea/vomiting, asthma). RESPeRATE is specifically designed for respiratory biofeedback that measures breathing (e.g., rate of inhalations and exhalations, depth and location) and uses associated electronics and software, combined with a strain gauge transducer that measures the displacement of the abdomen. It is designed to help entrain slower and deeper relaxed breathing. The Iom Active Feedback Hardware measures and provides feedback for HRV and skin conductance level. EmWave2 measures HRV via pulse plethysmography (PPG) on a finger or ear lobe. Users learn to use breathing and “positive emotions” to achieve “coherence” between breathing and cardiac rhythms. Feedback includes visual and audio stimuli, and computer-based software graphic feedback. Cloud‑Based Distance Biofeedback8 This is another exciting, innovative approach with great heuristic value. This cloud-based biofeedback service could work with any kind of modality. The most common modalities currently being used in this system involve cardiac blood volume (e.g., HRV) and EEG, but other modalities are planned. The software is partly on the patient/ client/subject’s home computer and mostly on the cloud-based location. The practitioner/therapist uploads to the cloud program his or her evaluation and intervention content and program (e.g., text, photos, audio clips, video clips). The cloud-based II. INSTRUMENTATION program analyzes data, provides feedback, stores the data, sends automatic reports to the patient, and communicates the results to the practitioner. The patient logs into the website of the practitioner’s office, clinic, hospital, or research laboratory. Then, the patient follows the instructions while his or her psychophysiological data are transmitted to the cloud-based program. Clients’ identities are coded to maintain confidentiality. Each practitioner knows the code of each client, and the program provides the practitioner with frequent updates regarding the client’s adherence and psychophysiological data. Research Questions Applied research with these devices and systems is still in the nascent stage. We include a few questions that we would like to see researched in the next few years (many of which are similar to those raised by Glasgow and Rosen, 1978, in their seminal review of behavioral bibliotherapy): • Does use of one or more of these devices without professional supervision and guidance produce different results compared to those obtained with professional supervision and guidance? Are outcomes comparable to those found when the same modality is provided with more elaborate instrumentation in office sessions? • Does the use of the RESPeRATE preceding EmWave result in faster acquisition of desired HRV outcomes compared to starting with EmWave? • Are there meaningful differences in outcomes between devices? • Do clients express preferences for particular devices and, if so, what client characteristics are associated with these preferences? • How important is it to follow the manufacturer’s guidelines for use? For example, do outcomes differ if practitioners recommend a different frequency of use? • If practitioners vary their level of involvement, could certain combinations provide more favorable cost–benefit returns? For example, would differences emerge when patients use the device totally independently rather than combined with varied amounts of therapist support (prudent limited office treatment vs. multiple office sessions in conjunction with home use)? 159 9. Consumer‑ and Home‑Based Biofeedback • What characteristics of users are associated with effective and ineffective use of these devices? • How often do patients/clients actually practice relaxed breathing alone and/or HRV aside from the number of times they use the devices when treating their symptoms (e.g., hypertension)? Conclusions and Recommendations All of the biofeedback devices and systems described in this chapter are excellent and welldesigned. The devices are logical and probably useful for treating a wide variety of symptoms and disorders for which at least slow, deep, diaphragmatic breathing and/or HRV have been found clinically useful. These devices can be used in the professional’s office, loaned or “rented” for home use by the professional, or purchased as a supplement to professionally supervised intervention. In sum: • Consumer- and home-based biofeedback devices are here to stay. • There are many logical, safe, effective, and supported uses and applications of these devices. • There are limitations, cautions, and contraindications that vary across modalities and specific devices, especially when used without supervision by appropriately educated and credentialed health care professionals. • These devices will become more numerous, and, with increased software and programming, will continue to become more sophisticated. • Well-controlled research (we hope) will continue to increase, provide supportive evidence, and lead to development of guidelines for use and misuse. Notes 1. Some of these instruments are not called “biofeedback” by the manufacturers, yet they are clearly and unequivocally biofeedback instruments or devices. “A rose by any other name . . . ” 2. The terms “instrument” and “device” are used interchangeably in this chapter. Some companies use the term “device” presumably for commercial value. 3. Of course, negative side effects and adverse reactions can also occur in persons being treated by health care professionals who are properly credentialed and licensed, especially if they have less training or experience, although presumably and logically the risks are considerably less. Also, when adverse events occur within a therapeutic relationship, they are more likely to be noticed and acted upon. Further discussion of this topic, however, goes beyond the scope of this chapter. 4. These devices are also recommended by practitioners to supplement office-based interventions, and loaned to or rented to recipients to supplement office-based interventions. 5. See www.aapb.org, www.bcia.org, and www.isnr.org. 6. The order of presentation of devices in this chapter is essentially random. 7. Referred to in this chapter as “relaxed breathing.” 8. See www.bfa-global.com (BFA-GLOBAL, Biofeedback Analytics Ltd., Phone: +972-52-3334888, Email: Info@BFA-Global.com). References Glasgow, R. E., & Rosen, G. M. (1978). Behavioral bibliotherapy: A review of self-help behavior therapy manuals. Psychological Bulletin, 85(1), 1–23. Hammond, D. C., & Kirk, L. (2007). Negative effects and the need for standards of practice in neurofeedback. Biofeedback, 35(4), 139–145. P a r t III Adjunctive/Complementary Interventions Ch a p ter 10 Dietary Considerations Keith I. Block, Charlotte Gyllenhaal, and Mark S. Schwartz Many health care professionals believe, and many patients report, that certain dietary elements or patterns aggravate or trigger certain physical or psychological symptoms. Many of these symptoms are also treated with applied psychophysiological interventions such as biofeedback (e.g., migraine headaches, anxiety, irritable bowel syndrome, insomnia). The idea that specific foods act as triggers has received closer examination in recent years, and doubts have been raised about the importance of some classic migraine trigger foods. At the same time, other aspects of diet are beginning to look more relevant in migraine, and implications of diet both for intensification of migraine and other headaches, and for long-term health of migraine patients are becoming more important. We emphasize in this chapter the relevance of diet in migraine, although not to the exclusion of other conditions. brain chemicals, with irritation and inflammation of the trigeminal nerve. Among the brain chemicals involved are Substance P (a well-known pain mediator), nitric oxide, glutamate, and calcitonin gene-related peptide. Magnesium deficiency may play a role in migraine. There are two distinct types of migraines: migraines with aura (a period of visual disturbance that precedes the migraine) and those without. This further complicates our understanding of the disease. Among the processes involved is “cortical spreading depression,” a wave of abnormal activity that passes through the visual cortex and is thought to be the cause of migraine aura. The etiology of migraines is therefore complex, and one would consequently expect that multiple interventions might be necessary to manage them. The management of migraines includes several medications that are used to treat acute migraine pain, including nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs), aspirin, acetaminophen, and caffeine. All of these have undesirable side effects. Triptans can prevent migraines but should be avoided by patients with vascular disease or uncontrolled hypertension (Gilmore & Michael, 2011). Patients may also want to avoid medications due to pregnancy or other concerns with medication side effects, so biofeedback and other nonpharmacological interventions remain of great interest. Mechanisms of Migraine Models for the etiology of migraine are diverse. Taylor (2011) presents migraine as an inflammatory pain syndrome, at least in part. Both neurogenic and vascular processes are thought to contribute to migraine (Shevel, 2011). Release of serotonin or other brain chemicals may cause vasoconstriction and vasodilation, leading to further release of 163 164 Prevalence of Migraine Triggers Researchers have surveyed headache patients to determine how many of them experience migraines triggered by diet and other factors. In a study in Italy, one-third of headache patients reported susceptibility to various foods (Savi, Rainero, Valfrè, Gentile, Lo Giudice, & Pinessi, 2002). Both patients with tension headaches and those with migraines reported sensitivity to chocolate, alcoholic drinks, and cheeses. On the other hand, in a study in Denmark, Hauge, Kirchmann, and Olesen (2011) found that red wine was the only food trigger listed by a group of patients with migraines with aura, although several lifestyle factors were listed. In reviewing this literature, Wöber and Wöber-Bingöl (2010) comment that nearly every aspect of life has been named as a migraine trigger. They emphasize not only alcohol and withdrawal from caffeine as triggers but also skipping meals and possibly dehydration. Based on a survey of 120 headache patients, Wöber, Holzhammer, Zeitlhofer, Wessely, & Wöber-Bingöl, (2006) conclude that the great majority of triggers occur occasionally and inconsistently. The major triggers related to diet include allergenic foods, vasoactive amines, glutamates, specific food items (e.g., hot dogs and ice cream), alcohol, and caffeine. Migraine Diets Several proposed diets have aimed to remove possible migraine triggers from nutritional intake. The triggers investigated include allergens, vasoactive amines, caffeine, particular foods such as ice cream and hot dogs, and others. In a 50-patient randomized study, Zencirci (2010) attempted to remove multiple types of triggers. In this study, patients in Turkey were given a diet excluding the major types of foods thought to trigger migraines (caffeine sources, alcohol, cheeses, processed meats, certain beans, monosodium glutamate [MSG] and other food additives, etc.), along with a medication and supplement regimen (metropolol, vitamin B2, and naproxen at the start of headaches). A comparison group received only the medication plus supplements. Monthly numbers of migraines were similar in the two groups at the start, but by the end of the study, patients in the diet group had 2.7 migraines per month, while those in the medication-only group had 5.15 migraines per month, a significant difference. Monthly analgesic intake also decreased by about half in the diet group. The III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS presentation of results in this study was rather abbreviated, and there were no data on whether patients in the two groups adhered to their allocated programs, making the evaluation of these intriguing results difficult. The lack of dietary adherence data also applies to the other studies of whole-diet interventions in migraine and reduces the credibility of these studies. Additionally, most of these studies involve rather small numbers of patients and should be replicated with larger groups. Allergenic Foods There were no controlled studies showing that vascular headaches represent an allergic reaction until the work of Egger, Wilson, Carter, Turner, and Soothill (1983) supported an allergic pathogenesis for migraines among many children ages 3–16. Children were put on a diet low in foods commonly known to be allergenic for 2 weeks. Of the 88 children who completed the diet, 78% reported full recovery from migraines. Forty of the children then underwent double-blind challenges at weekly intervals with allergenic foods. Of these 40, 39% reported migraines from cow’s milk, 31% from wheat, 36% from eggs, and 17% from corn— all foods commonly found to be highly allergenic. Adverse reactions, however, were idiosyncratic for each child. Other commonly allergenic foods found to trigger migraines were orange, tomato, rye, fish, and soy. Some foods suspected to contain vasoactive substances also caused migraines, including chocolate, cheese, coffee, and malt (Egger, 1991). This study lacked controls in the first phase, and this work has been been criticized for other issues in experimental design, although the doubleblind challenges lend some further credibility to the results. A systematic review (Damen, Bruijn, Koes, Berger, Passchier, & Verhagen, 2006) found conflicting evidence concerning diets excluding allergens for pediatric migraines, while behavioral therapies including relaxation and biofeedback were effective. It is possible, however, that dietary manipulations in adults may be more successful than those in children. Alpay, Ertas, Orhan, Üstay, Lieners, and Baykan (2010) constructed individualized provocation and elimination diets based on immunoglobulin G (IgG) allergy testing for 30 adults with migraine. These patients each participated in a nonintervention run-in period of 6 weeks, an elimination diet period in which foods to which they were allergic were omitted, and a 165 10. Dietary Considerations provocation period in which the allergenic foods were included. Diets had the same number of calories and were nutritionally adequate. Significant reductions in migraine days (from 10.0 to 7.5 days) and number of attacks were seen in the elimination period (9.0 to 6.2). This crossover trial represented a stronger design than earlier trials. Like other trials, it lacked adherence data but at least suggested a stronger rationale for following a lowallergen diet. Vasoactive Amines and Related Compounds “Amines” are chemical substances that result from the breakdown of proteins. A number of amines are suspected of acting on blood vessels to produce migraines (termed “vasoactive” or “biogenic amines”), and foods containing these amines are often avoided by migraine patients. Biogenic amines have been studied by administering the amines in capsules, so that they can be examined in double-blind, placebo-controlled trials. However, Jansen, van Dusseldorp, Bottema, and Dubois (2003) reviewed the literature on welldesigned trials of biogenic amines and found that reliable scientific evidence for the effects of these compounds on migraines and other conditions is lacking. There were two conclusive trials of tyramine that showed no association with migraine. Phenylethylamine, a constituent of chocolate, has also been suspected of causing migraines, but Jansen et al. found no relationship between the amount of phenylethylamine in chocolate and headache. Curiously, cocoa was found to decrease expression of inflammatory molecules in the trigeminal nerve, which would argue against the migraine-triggering properties of chocolate (Cady & Durham, 2010). Holzhammer and Wöber (2006), however, also doubt the ability of amines to cause migraines. More recently, in Italy, D’Andrea, Nordera, Perini, Allais, and Granella (2007) discussed the potential for tyramine, octopamine, synephrine, and related compounds to act as migraine and cluster headache triggers based on the discovery of trace amine-associated receptors in the brain (D’Andrea, Nordera, Perini, Allais, & Granella, 2007). They recorded elevated concentrations of such amines in the blood platelets of migraine patients (D’Andrea et al., 2006). MSG and other glutamates have also been suspected of triggering migraines; however, Freeman (2006) reviewed the literature on MSG effects and did not find any consistent evidence that it produces head- aches or other symptoms. It is worth noting that by using a double-blind n-of-1 trial, a migraine patient validated occurrence of migraines due to gelatin (hydrolyzed protein) capsules used to administer potential triggers in double-blind studies (Strong, 2000), casting some doubt on the negative findings reviewed earlier. Given the complexity of migraine’s pathophysiology and this uncertainty, it is possible that some patients do genuinely react to tyramine, the most widely distributed of the biogenic amines, or to other compounds such as MSG. Table 10.1 shows tyramine contents of a variety of foods, mostly cheeses. MSG is common in many processed foods, under a variety of names, including hydrolyzed vegetable protein (HVP), natural flavor, flavoring, and kombu extract (see Block, Schwartz, & Gyllenhaal, 2003, for more information on MSG, HVP, and other substances, as well as older studies involving whole-diet interventions). Specific Foods: Ice Cream and Hot Dog Headaches In an experimental protocol devised to study icecream headaches, Selekler, Erdogan, and Budak (2004; Selekler & Budak, 2004) found that a cold stimulus could produce headaches, with migraine patients more likely to report symptoms than tension headache patients. This was attributed to irritation of the trigeminal nerve due to cold, and deficits in central pain control. Hot dog headache may be a reaction to sodium nitrite, and ingestion of nitrite by persons sensitive to it may result in headaches. Henderson and Raskin (1972) gave 10 milligrams of sodium nitrite or placebo to one patient with migraine. Headache occurred in eight of 13 trials of sodium nitrite, and no headaches occurred after placebo. Ten healthy volunteers had no headaches after nitrite or placebo. Given the possible role of nitric oxide in migraine pathophysiology, some individuals may be sensitive to hot dogs and other nitrite-containing processed meats. Alcohol In a systematic review of the role of alcohol in triggering migraines, Panconesi (2008) concluded that while about one-third of migraine patients report alcohol as an occasional headache trigger in retrospective studies, only 10% report that it frequently triggers migraine, with red wine being 166 III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS TABLE 10.1. Tyramine-Containing Foods, Beverages, and Condiments, and Tyramine Concentrations (in Micrograms per Gram or Micrograms per Milliliter, Unless Otherwise Noted) Cheeses Cheddar (Highest: old, center-cut Canadian and New York State, approx. 1500; Canadian aged in ale, approx. 1000) (Others: English, up to 953; New Zealand, 471–580; Australian, 226; other Canadian, most 120–192) Gruyère (American, 516; British, 11–1184; Finnish, 102) Stilton (American, 466; English, 2170) Emmenthal (225); Emmentaler (225–1000) Brie (American, 180; Danish, nil) Camembert (American, 86; Danish, 23–1340; Mycella [Camembert type], 1304; Cuban, 34–425) Roquefort (French, 27–520) Blue (Danish, 31–256; French, 203; Bourmandise [blue type], 216) Boursault (French, 1116) Parmesan (Italian, 65; American, 4–290) Processed (American, 50; Canadian, 26) Romano (Italian, 238) Provolone (Italian, 38) Cracker Barrel (Kraft brand, American, 214) Brick (natural Canadian, 524) Mozzarella (Canadian, 410) Gouda (Canadian, 20; Cuban, 40–280) Cream cheese and cottage cheese (nil except South African cottage, 6.6) Cuban (Fontina, 54–167; Broodkaase, 0–163; Dambom, 26–100; Carré, 52–200) Spanish (Mahon old, 369; Cáceres cured, 225; Cáceres, 102; Malaga fresh, 22) Dry/fermented sausage Salami (hard, average 210, up to 392; farmer, average 314; Genoa, average 534, up to 1237) Sausage (summer, up to 184; dry, up to 244; semidry, up to 85.5) Others (pepperoni, average, up to 195; smoked landjaeger, up to 396; dry fermented, 102–1506) Others Herring (Marinated [pickled], 3030; Canadian, 470) Caviar (estimated high, but no published analysis found) Sour cream, yogurt (variable but often nil, especially from reputable brands) Chicken liver (cooked, not kept refrigerated, 94–113; fresh cooked, nil) Beef liver (fresh or frozen, approx. 5) Raspberries, fresh (13–92) Yeast products (English: unspecified brand, 2100; Marmite, 1087–1639; Yex, 506; Befit, 419; Barmene, 152; Yeastrel, 101) (Canadian: unspecified brand, 66–84; plain, nil) Bananas (peel, 63–65; pulp, 7) Soya beans (fermented, Singapore, 713 [50 q]; Taiwan, 878 [10 ml]; Swiss soya sauce, Dr. Dunners, 293 [10 ml], Formosa soya bean condiment, 939 [20 g]; Korea soya bean paste fermented, 206 [50 g] a Red wine (highly variable, some nil) Note. For a patient-friendly summary of avoiding high-tyramine foods, see www.mc.vanderbilt.edu/documents/neurology/files/Tyramine%20Menu%20Book%2006227101.pdf. See also Block et al. (2003, Table 9.1) for many other foods and beverages, most of which have very small amounts of tyramine. All tyramine < 1 mg/portion unless specified. Portions of most meat/vegetables, 50–75 g (1.8–2.6 oz.). Normal portions of cheese are variable. As a guide, Vidaud, Chaviano, Gonzales, and Garcia Roche (1987) note that a normal portion of Camembert cheese is about 23 g (0.8 oz., avoirdupois) and of Gouda, about 30 g (1.06 oz.). The data on concentrations are from the following sources: Maxwell (1980), citing the concentrations as reported by Horowitz, Lovenberg, Engelman, and Sjoerdsma (1964), McCabe (1986), Sen (1969), Boulton, Cookson, and Paulten (1970), Coffin (1970), Hedberg, Gordon, and Glueck (1966), Marley and Blackwell (1970), Orlosky (1982), Udenfriend, Lovenberg, and Sjoerdsma (1959); Cuban and Spanish data from Vidaud, Chaviano, Gonzales, and Garcia Roche (1987), Rice, Eitenmiller, and Kohler (1975, 1976), Rice and Kohler (1976), Rivas-Gonzalo, Santos-Hernandez, and Marine-Font (1983). Vidaud et al. (1987) reviewed eight studies with ranges of 13–2000 milligrams per kilogram. All but one study (Asatoor, Levi, & Milne, 1963) reported average tyramine content of less than 211. aThe amounts given here are those consumed in a normal serving. 167 10. Dietary Considerations the most common type of alcohol selected. Prospective studies, which are more reliable, cast doubt on the hypothesis that alcohol is an important trigger, at least in adults. Hauge et al. (2011), in their survey study, reported that no migraine patients claimed that alcohol triggered more than 50% of their migraine attacks. Migraine patients in general consume less alcohol than other populations. Milde-Busch et al. (2010) found, in a cross-sectional study of over 1200 adolescents, that high consumption of cocktails was associated with both migraine and tension headaches. While such a study design is less reliable than prospective studies, this suggests that refraining from alcohol may be a useful suggestion for adolescents (and, of course, it is health-promoting for other reasons). Caffeine1 Rationale Caffeine is the world’s most popular drug and the focus of considerable attention by applied psychophysiology and health care professionals. One major rationale for discussing caffeine in this chapter is that as a powerful psychotropic stimulant, it elicits or aggravates physiological symptoms associated with many disorders: migraine headaches, anxiety, Raynaud’s disease, nausea and other gastrointestinal symptoms (irritable bowel syndrome [IBS]), hypertension, premenstrual syndrome, hot flashes, urinary incontinence, sleep-onset insomnia, and conditions associated with breathing problems, increased skeletal muscle contractions, and/or increased heart rate. Another major concern is that caffeine can interfere with developing and applying psychophysiological self-regulation. Thus, caffeine use is inconsistent with the overall goals of reducing sympathetic and general and/ or focal muscle tension. The effects of withdrawal constitute another cause for concern and rationale for including caffeine here. Metabolism and Toxicity In usual doses, caffeine stimulates the central nervous system. The effects vary but often results in excess stimulation and tension. Orally consumed caffeine spreads rapidly through the body. Body tissues and fluids quickly absorb it, resulting in peak plasma levels usually from about 15 minutes to within an hour, but longer, up to 2 hours, in some cases (Arnaud, 1993). Half-life estimates vary from about 2 to 12 hours in healthy, nonmedicated adults, with an average typically in the 4- to 6-hour range (Grosso & Bracken, 2005). Half-life is much longer in pregnant women after the first trimester, especially later stages, about 9–11 hours at about 17 weeks of gestation and longer, about 11–18 hours, by the end of pregnancy (Aldridge, Bailey, & Neims, 1981; Carrillo & Benitez, 2000). Among people with chronic liver disease the half-life of caffeine is much longer and up to 96 hours with severe liver disease (Carrillo & Benitez, 2000). Smoking nearly doubles the metabolic rate, thus shortening the half-life of caffeine. It does this via a hydrocarbon that increases liver enzyme activity (Kalow & Tang, 1991; Parsons & Neims, 1978). Interactions with many medications and dietary chemicals affect the metabolism of caffeine and hence the half-life elimination of caffeine. We suggest reading Carrillo and Benitez (2000) and Culm-Merdek, von Moltke, Harmatz, and Greenblatt (2005). Among the many interactions, note that fluvoxamine (Luvox®), a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), lengthens the half-life to an estimated 56 hours (95% confidence interval [CI] 26–76 hours; CulmMerdek et al., 2005). Eating certain vegetables, such as dill weed, celery, parsley, parsnips, and carrots (members of the Apiaceae or carrot family), slows caffeine metabolism and therefore increases half-life. Eating other vegetables, especially radish sprouts, broccoli, cauliflower, and cabbage (part of the Cruciferae or cabbage family), increases metabolism of caffeine and therefore decreases half-life (Grosso & Bracken, 2005; Lampe et al., 2000). Caffeine Effects Headaches The role of caffeine in migraine and tension headaches is paradoxical. Consuming caffeine causes vasoconstriction: It can therefore be useful in stopping headaches, and it is a constituent of several acute migraine medications. This vasoconstriction, however, is later followed by a rebound vasodilation, which may also cause headache. Researchers have recently become interested in the role of caffeine in chronic daily headache or chronic migraine, and in medication overuse headache (Aguggia & Saracco, 2010). We discuss these topics below. 168 Anxiety Moderate caffeine intake may actually be useful in some psychiatric disorders, having been associated with fewer depressive symptoms, lower suicide risk and less cognitive failure due to its stimulant and mood-elevating effects (Lara 2010). Anxiety in response to caffeine is associated with a polymorphism in the adenosine A2A receptor; thus, some persons are genetically more susceptible to caffeine effects than others. Caffeine-induced anxiety is especially noted in people with panic disorder and social anxiety disorder (Nardi et al., 2009). However, in many people, even those with susceptible genotypes, tolerance to the anxiogenic effects of caffeine occurs when they take caffeine regularly (Rogers et al., 2010). Blood Pressure Caffeine doses of 250–500 milligrams administered acutely can raise systolic and diastolic blood pressure significantly by a few to several millimeters of mercury (Lane, Phillips-Bute, & Pieper, 1998), independent of posture, activity, or perceived stress. However, with chronic intake of caffeine, the effect on blood pressure is attenuated (Geleijnse, 2008). Caffeine intake in general does not raise risks of cardiovascular disease, in part because coffee appears to protect against development of Type 2 diabetes, possibly due to chlorogenic acid content (Riksen, Rongen, & Smits, 2009). Coffee intake does appear to be associated with an increased risk of elevated blood pressure in older men who are overweight or obese (Giggey, Wendell, Zonderman, & Waldstein, 2011). Insomnia Caffeine management (e.g., elimination, substantial reduction, or at least limited use only in the early morning), is commonly recommended for all or most persons with sleep-onset insomnia (e.g., Juliano & Griffith, 2005). Caffeine is an adenosine receptor antagonist that is thought to be a factor in interfering with sleep onset, as measured by subjective sleepiness and electroencephalographic (EEG) theta activity (Landolt et al., 2004). Even individuals accustomed to caffeine may experience sleep problems from it. It is notable that the DSM-5 includes a diagnosis of caffeine-induced sleep disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Possible genetic factors are the focus of research such as that by Luciano et al. (2007), III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS who concluded that the “heritability of coffeeattributed sleep disturbance . . . was approximately 0.40, with three fourths of this genetic variance explained by genes unrelated to the general sleep disturbance factor” (p. 1378). This can partly explain the individual variations of effects of caffeine on sleep-onset insomnia and probably other symptoms. However, practitioners do not usually have genetic information available (yet); thus, the standard recommendations to manage caffeine intake are still prudent. Urinary Incontinence Approximately one-fourth of cases of urge incontinence (although not stress or mixed incontinence) appeared to be related to high (> 450 mg daily) caffeine intake in a recent prospective survey (Jura, Townsend, Curhan, Resnick, & Grodstein, 2011). Caffeine reduction is standard advice in management of urinary problems. A randomized trial in patients presenting for urinary problems found significant reductions in caffeine intake after counseling, and subsequently in urgency and frequency outcomes (Bryant, Dowell, & Fairbrother, 2002), validating this advice. Caffeine Contents of Beverages and Foods There is much variability in caffeine amounts measured in the many published studies of caffeine in coffee and tea (Bracken et al., 2002). As these investigators also note, the variability occurs even in the same participants and within the same day and the same brewing conditions when they were brewing coffee or tea. The caffeine content in coffee depends on the strain of coffee bean, the condition of the beans (whether they are green or roasted), and the type of coffee (e.g., drip, instant, espresso), brewing time,2 exposure surface area of the tea or coffee to the water (i.e., smaller tea leaves and finer coffee grounds result in more release of caffeine), and the ratio of ground coffee to water (see http://blackbearcoffee. com/resources/83 for standard and connoisseur's ratios). In view of the extensive data for caffeine contents that are available very readily on the Internet, the data and Internet links presented in Table 10.2 provide estimates of caffeine content for a few types of beverages. A practitioner need not know the exact amount of caffeine in a specific patient’s coffee. It does not pay to invest expensive professional time exploring the strain of coffee beans and brewing time 169 10. Dietary Considerations TABLE 10.2. Caffeine Contents (in Milligrams per Ounce) Coffee Generic Drip Instant Espresso Coffee shopsa Expresso McDonald’s 13–25 (est. 20) 3–22 (est. 12) 30–90 9–20 75 9 Teas Black more than green, instant, and premixed Black Green Iced (e.g., Lipton) 3–17 8–15 (est. 10) 1–4 ≤2 Soft drinksb /carbonated beverages Most “Energy” drinks (most) “Energy” shots 2–6 (est. 3 or 4) 9–50 40–50 Internet links www.cspinet.org/new/cafchart.htm www.mayoclinic.com/health/caffeine/an01211 www.energyfiend.com/the-caffeine-database Note. Content is based on most values given on websites. aFor example, Starbucks, Einstein, Dunkin’Donuts (except espresso). bFDA official limit for cola and pepper soft drinks = 71 mg in 12 oz., 6 mg/oz. used by the patient. One can estimate the caffeine intake from coffee from the general type (e.g., instant, drip) and the number of ounces drunk. One should also inquire about the size of the cup used, because most people use the term “cup” when they are actually drinking from larger-size mugs. A mug can vary from about 5 to 12 ounces, or even more. Caffeine Withdrawal The factor that motivates a substantial percentage of habitual caffeine consumers is the need to avoid withdrawal symptoms, particularly headaches, created by physical dependence on caffeine (Shapiro, 2008). The comprehensive review and literature analysis by Juliano and Griffith (2005) regarding caffeine withdrawal provided an empirical vali- dation of specific symptoms and signs (see Table 10.3) and an appraisal of the important features of caffeine withdrawal syndrome. Headaches were, as expected, the most common symptom, with an incidence of 50%. In general, regular use of as little as 100 milligrams of caffeine per day can induce a form of physical dependence, interruption of which elicits withdrawal symptoms (Juliano & Griffith, 2005). Also note the following selected symptoms noted by Block et al. (2003): increased heart rate, analgesic use, cerebral blood flow volume, EEG theta activity primarily in the occipital area, muscle pain/stiffness, and irritability. Headaches that occur after general anesthesia are often related to preoperative caffeine withdrawal after moderate daily intake of caffeine. (Fennelly, Galletly, & Purdie, 1991). Furthermore, the probability of headaches developing preoperatively and postoperatively increases with consumption of more caffeine preoperatively (e.g., increases of 100 milligrams increased headaches 16% postoperatively). A patient’s medication alone can contain enough caffeine to create or add to the problem. Even some migraine medications contain caffeine. Table 10.4 presents the caffeine content in several prescription and over-the-counter (OTC) preparations. TABLE 10.3. Caffeine Withdrawal Symptoms from a Critical Review by Juliano and Griffiths (2005) 10 caffeine withdrawal symptoms fulfilling validity criteria •• Headache •• Fatigue •• Decreased energy/activeness •• Decreased alertness •• Drowsiness •• Decreased contentedness •• Depressed mood •• Difficulty concentrating •• Irritability •• Foggy/not clearheaded Additional withdrawal symptoms judged likely to represent valid categories •• Flu-like symptoms •• Nausea/vomiting •• Muscle pain/stiffness Note. Caffeine withdrawal symptoms typically start 12–24 hours after caffeine abstinence and reach maximum intensity after 20–51 hours. Duration can be 2–9 days. 170 III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS TABLE 10.4. Caffeine Content in Milligrams per dose of Selected Prescription and Nonprescription Preparations (by Trade Names) Prescription Cafergot Ergocaf Esgic Ezol Fioricet Fiorinal Medigesic Norgesic Norgesic Forte Orphengesic Orphengesic Forte Repan tablets Synalgos-DC Tencet Triad Novartis Rugby Labs Forest; Gilbert Stewart Jackson Novartis Novartis U.S. Pharm 3M 3M Various Various 100 100 40 40 40 40 100 30 60 60 40 Everett Wyeth Roberts Forest 32 30 40 40 Whitehall Thompson Medical Thompson Medical Thompson Medical Mentholatum Chilton Bristol-Myers 32 100 200 200 32 75 65 Bristol-Myers Bristol-Myers Vita Elixer Bayer 65 65 250 32.4 Nonprescription Anacin Aqua Ban Aqua Ban Plus Caffedrine Cope Energets Excedrin Extra strength ASA free Migraine Lerton Midol Original (cramps) No Doz Quick-Pep Stay-Alert Stay Awake Summit Extra strength Vanquish Vivarin Wakespan Squibb Thompson Medical Apothecary Whiteworth Pfeiffer Bayer Smith, Kline, Beecham Weeks & Lea 100 and 200 150 200 250 65 200 200 250 Caffeine is also found in several of the popular herbal medicines and other dietary supplements for weight control, muscular development, and increased feelings of energy. The herbs that contain caffeine are kola nut, guarana, and yerba maté; green tea extracts are also sold as supplements, with and some without caffeine. “Energy drink” supplements in particular may contain high levels of caffeine. Thus, we recommend that practitioners: • Ask about OTC and prescription drugs, dietary supplements, and herbal preparations to estimate caffeine intake from these sources. • Provide information about the potential for caffeine withdrawal symptoms. • Recommend significantly reducing or often stopping caffeine consumption. • Provide guidance regarding gradual reduction of caffeine consumption. Website Link www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1g2-3403100095.html International Differences in Caffeine Content and Selected Implications International practitioners should note that beverages, especially coffee, vary in their caffeine content in different countries. This has implications also for athletes and applications of biofeedback to athletic performances. Athletes also vary in their metabolism and caffeine excretion, and may therefore vary, and exceed, regulated amounts of caffeine allowed in competition. This can occur even with only about 3–6 cups of brewed coffee of average strength. This excess is more likely to occur if the urine sample is obtained at the time that caffeine concentration is highest. Further contributing to excess caffeine in the body of an athlete or patient is the interactions of medications with caffeine (Carillo & Benitez, 2000). Meal Patterns and Headache Missing meals has commonly been recognized as a trigger for migraines. This is corroborated by the observation of more than double the migraines in Muslim patients fasting during Ramadan (AbuSalameh, Plakht, & Ifergane, 2010). In addition, Blau (2005) observed that one-third of migraine patients reported that water deprivation can trigger migraines. A small randomized trial of advice to increase water intake demonstrated a significant decrease in migraines, a result that needs further validation (Spigt et al., 2005). Other Risk Factors for Migraine Recent research has begun to investigate the role of dietary elements in two types of intensification of headache: medication overuse headache 171 10. Dietary Considerations and chronic migraine or chronic daily headache. Chronic migraine and chronic daily headache are characterized by headache at least 15 days per month. Medication overuse headache can arise in response to continuing use of various medications, including triptans, ergot alkaloids, barbiturates, and caffeine. Migraine patients are more likely than others to develop this type of chronic headache, which appears to be related to increases in calcitonin gene-related peptide or cortical spreading depression that may be triggered by these medications (Meng, Dodick, Ossipov, & Porreca, 2011). Risk factors for the transformation of episodic to chronic migraine include excessive medication, stress, sleep disorders, caffeine overuse, and obesity (Bigal & Lipton, 2009). The understanding that inflammation is important in the pathophysiology of migraine suggests a new dimension to dietary considerations. Migraine patients have higher levels than healthy controls of the biomarker C-reactive protein, which indicates chronic inflammation, as well as other biomarkers of endothelial (blood vessel) dysfunction (Tietjen et al., 2009). NSAIDs can be used to control inflammation, but they carry a high risk of gastric ulcers, so safer interventions are preferable. A trial of omega-3 fatty acid supplementation (the anti-inflammatory constituents of fish oil) in migraine patients did not reduce migraines (Pradalier et al., 2001), but the effects of these could have been counteracted if subjects’ diets were high in omega-6 fatty acids, which are common in Western diets. A Mediterranean diet supplemented with olive oil or nuts, however, was able to reduce C-reactive protein and other inflammatory biomarkers (Estruch, 2010). Growing evidence also indicates that migraine is related to disorders of glucose metabolism, including insulin resistance, the inability of insulin to deliver glucose to body tissues that is seen in Type 2 diabetes and prediabetes. Migraine patients were observed to be much more likely than normal controls to have signs of insulin resistance, and these were associated with indications of abnormalities with nitric oxide—one of the mediators of migraine (Gruber et al., 2010). Migraine patients have a higher than expected prevalence of metabolic syndrome, which is associated with insulin resistance (Guldiken et al., 2009). Both insulin resistance and inflammation place patients at elevated risk of cardiovascular disease, and migraine patients are indeed at higher than average risk for stroke. Insulin resis- tance can be treated with diet, and a variety of diets have been found effective in reducing insulin resistance, including vegetarian (Kahleova et al., 2011), high-fiber (Li et al., 2010), and other diets. Loss of body fat through diet or exercise is important in reducing insulin resistance (Camhi, Stefanick, Katzmarzyk, & Young, 2010). Therapeutic Strategies Different therapeutic strategies with regard to diet can be discussed with patients who have episodic migraines, medication overuse headaches, and chronic daily migraine. Table 10.5 summarizes dietary interventions, which are listed in order of aggressiveness. Practitioners can initially approach treatment of patients with episodic migraine using techniques of psychophysiological self-regulation. Because of the questionable evidence for the prevalence of dietary triggers of migraine, patients can be asked to avoid triggers that they already know are bothersome for them, as well as caffeine and alcohol. Issues relative to adequate hydration and regularity of meals or fasting can also be addressed. In regard to general health, it would be prudent to ask patients whether they have talked to their physicians about cardiovascular risks, and to advise them to consult a dietitian to develop a dietary plan that will decrease cardiovascular risks and either reduce or prevent overweight. With adolescents experiencing migraine or other headaches, TABLE 10.5. Dietary Interventions for Migraine •• Avoid foods patient suspects as migraine triggers. •• Avoid alcohol, especially for adolescents. •• Gradually reduce and/or eliminate caffeine. •• Remain well hydrated. •• Avoid cold foods and specific items such as hot dogs/ processed meats. •• Eat meals regularly, and approach fasting with caution. •• Address overweight (consult with dietitian). •• Address inflammation and insulin resistance (consult with integrative health professional or dietitian). •• Explore use of dietary supplements (consult with integrative health professional). •• Reduce vasoactive amine intake and intake of other chemicals such as MSG (consult with dietitian). •• Allergenic elimination diet (consult with dietitian). Note. Least aggressive interventions are listed first. 172 particular attention to caffeine and alcohol consumption is recommended. For patients with possible medication overuse headache, or patients who consume excessive caffeine-containing beverages or medications daily, a more assertive dietary intervention is wise, in addition to instruction in psychophysical self-regulation techniques. Using the tables in this chapter, the practitioner can help the patient determine dietary and medication sources of caffeine, then encourage slow reduction or elimination of caffeine. Reducing caffeine intake gradually prevents caffeine withdrawal symptoms. This strategy can also be used with patients who have other problems related to caffeine intake. With migraine patients specifically, it is important to institute nondrug therapies, so that medication overuse headache or excessive caffeine use does not lead to chronic migraine. Addition of counseling about dehydration, fasting, and known triggers may assist in establishing nondrug control. If the combined efforts are unsuccessful, consultation with other health professionals to address possible overweight, inflammation, and insulin resistance may be recommended. Internet sites offer calculation of body mass index to determine overweight, and physicians may be consulted about laboratory tests that indicate inflammation or insulin resistance. A dietitian can assist with interventions for weight reduction or laboratory-confirmed abnormalities. It is also recommended that the patient seek information about dietary supplements that show evidence of reducing migraines, including magnesium, butterbur, feverfew, coenzyme Q10 and alpha-lipoic acid (Sun-Edelstein & Mauskop, 2009); an integrative medical doctor, pharmacist, naturopath, or knowledgeable dietitian should be consulted about these, and they are more likely to be effective when used with other lifestyle changes. For patients with chronic daily headache, a vigorous intervention may be implemented, based on psychophysiological self-regulation, exploration of caffeine overuse, and elimination of known triggers (e.g., alcohol, dehydration, and fasting). The patient should also be referred to a dietitian to address overweight, inflammation, and insulin resistance as soon as possible (the practitioner may consider consulting with the dietitian to explain the significance of these conditions for migraine). Dietary supplements can be considered. If these efforts do not begin to ameliorate headache within a few months, the practitioner could consider helping the patient to analyze his or her diet for other III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS migraine triggers, such as tyramine, other amines and related compounds, allergens (consult with a dietitian), and processed meats, and encourage elimination of these on a trial basis. A full elimination diet could be attempted with support from a dietitian. It is interesting to note that many of the foods that are pointed out as migraine triggers (e.g., cheese, processed meats, chocolate candy, dairy products, fatty foods and food additives; as listed by Zencirci, 2010) are foods that contribute to inflammation, insulin resistance, and weight gain. The lack of solid high-level evidence for many of the foods commonly considered migraine triggers, and the emerging data on effects of dietary patterns on chronification of migraine suggest that a new approach to the understanding of diet and migraine is needed and appears to be evolving. Certainly, better-controlled research studies involving larger populations are needed to evaluate dietary interventions. Along with well-validated techniques of psychophysical self-regulation, however, these interventions offer the potential to assist many patients experiencing migraines to reduce the incidence or severity of headaches. These nondrug methods also may help many patients avoid the complications of medication overuse headaches and chronic daily headache. Notes 1. There are numerous credible and comprehensive websites that include or focus on caffeine. This readily available and extensive information has reduced the need and justification for including much content on caffeine in this chapter. Rather, we provide a summary of selected information and links to the websites. See www.marksschwartzphd.com for the hyperlinks (e.g., http://emedicine. medscape.com/article/821863-overview). 2. Longer brewing (e.g., 10 vs. 5 minutes) can increase caffeine by about 4–15 milligrams per 150 milliliters. References Abu-Salameh, I., Plakht, Y., & Ifergane, G. (2010). Migraine exacerbation during Ramadan fasting. Journal of Headache and Pain, 11, 513–517. Aguggia, M., & Saracco, M. G. (2010). Pathophysiology of migraine chronification. Neurological Sciences, 31(Suppl. 1), S15–S17. Aldridge, A., Bailey, J., & Neims, A. H. (2008). The disposition of caffeine during and after pregnancy. Seminars in Perinatology, 5(4), 310–314. Alpay, K., Ertas, M., Orhan, E. K., Üstay, D. K., Lieners, C., 10. Dietary Considerations & Baykan, B. (2010). Diet restriction in migraine, based on IgG against foods: A clinical, double-blind, randomized, crossover trial. Cephalagia, 30, 829–837. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington VA; Author. Arnaud, M. J. (1993). Metabolism of caffeine and other components of coffee. In S. Garattini (Ed.), Caffeine, coffee and health (pp. 43–95). New York: Raven Press. Asatoor, A. M., Levi, A. J., & Milne, M. P. (1963). Trancylpromine and cheese. Lancet, 2, 733–734. Bigal, M. E., & Lipton, R. B. (2009). What predicts the change from episodic to chronic migraine? Current Opinion in Neurology, 22, 269–276. Blau, J. N. (2005). Water deprivation: A new migraine precipitant. Headache, 45, 757–759. Block, K. I., Schwartz, M. S., & Gyllenhaal, C. (2003). Dietary considerations: Rationale, issues, substances, evaluation, and patient education. In M. S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed., pp. 181–211). New York: Guilford Press. Boulton, A. A., Cookson, B., & Paulten, R. (1970). Hypertensive crisis in a patient on MAOI antidepressants following a meal of beef liver. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 102, 1394–1395. Bracken, M., Triche, E., Grosso, L., Hellenbrand, K., Belanger, K., & Leaderer, B. (2002). Heterogeneity in assessing self-reports of caffeine exposure: Implications for studies of health effects. Epidemiology, 13, 165–171. Bryant, C. M., Dowell, C. J., & Fairbrother, G. (2002). Caffeine reduction education to improve urinary symptoms. British Journal of Nursing, 11, 560–565. Cady, R. J., & Durham, P. L. (2010). Cocoa-enriched diets enhance expression of phosphatases and decrease expression of inflammatory molecules in trigeminal ganglion neurons. Brain Research, 1323, 18–32. Camhi, S. M., Stefanick, M. L., Katzmarzyk, P. T., & Young, D. R. (2010). Metabolic syndrome and changes in body fat from a low-fat diet and/or exercise randomized controlled trial. Obesity (Silver Spring), 18, 548–554. Carillo, J. A., & Benitez, J. (2000). Clinically significant pharmacokinetic interactions between dietary caffeine and medications. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 39, 127–153. Coffin, D. C. (1970). Tyramine content of raspberries and other fruit. Journal of the Association of Official Analytic Chemists, 53, 1071–1073. Culm-Merdek, K. E., von Moltke, L. L., Harmatz, J. S., & Greenblatt, D. J. (2005). Fluvoxamine impairs singledose caffeine clearance without altering caffeine pharmacodynamics. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 60(5), 486–493. Damen, L., Bruijn, J., Koes, B. W., Berger, M. Y., Passchier, J., & Verhagen A. P. (2006). Prophylactic treatment of migraine in children. Part 1: A systematic review of nonpharmacological trials. Cephalalgia, 26, 373–383. D'Andrea, G., Granella, F., Leone, M., Perini, F., Farruggio, A., & Bussone, G. (2006). Abnormal platelet trace amine profiles in migraine with and without aura. Cephalalgia, 26, 968–972. 173 D'Andrea, G., Nordera, G. P., Perini, F., Allais, G., & Granella, F. (2007). Biochemistry of neuromodulation in primary headaches: Focus on anomalies of tyrosine metabolism. Neurological Sciences, 28(Suppl. 2), S94– S96. Egger, J. (1991). Psychoneurological aspects of food allergy. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 45(Suppl. 1), 35–45. Egger, J., Wilson, J., Carter, C. M., Turner, M. W., & Soothill, J. F. (1983). Is migraine food allergy?: A doubleblind controlled trial of oligoantigenic diet treatment. Lancet, 2, 865–869. Estruch, R. (2010). Anti-inflammatory effects of the Mediterranean diet: The experience of the PREDIMED study. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 69, 333–340. Fennelly, M., Galletly, D. C., & Purdie, G. I. (1991). Is caffeine withdrawal the mechanism of postoperative headache? Anesthesia and Analgesia, 72, 449–453. Freeman, M. (2006). Reconsidering the effects of monosodium glutamate: A literature review. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners, 18(10), 482–486. Geleijnse, J. M. (2008). Habitual coffee consumption and blood pressure: An epidemiological perspective. Vascular Health and Risk Management, 4, 963–970. Giggey, P. P., Wendell, C. R., Zonderman, A. B., & Waldstein, S. R. (2011). Greater coffee intake in men is associated with steeper age-related increases in blood pressure. American Journal of Hypertension, 24, 310–315. Gilmore, B., & Michael, M. (2011). Treatment of acute migraine headache. American Family Physician, 83(3), 271–280. Grosso, L. M., & Bracken, M. B. (2005). Caffeine metabolism, genetics, and perinatal outcomes: A review of exposure assessment considerations during pregnancy. Annals of Epidemiology, 15(6), 460–466. Gruber, H. J., Bernecker, C., Pailer, S., Fauler, G., Horejsi, R., Möller, R., et al. (2010). Hyperinsulinaemia in migraineurs is associated with nitric oxide stress. Cephalalgia, 30, 593–598. Guldiken, B., Guldiken, S., Taskiran, B., Koc, G., Turgut, N., Kabayel, L., et al. (2009). Migraine in metabolic syndrome. Neurologist, 15, 55–58. Hauge, A. W., Kirchmann, M., & Olesen, J. (2011). Characterization of consistent triggers of migraine with aura. Cephalalgia, 31, 416–438. Hedberg, D. L., Gordon, M. W., & Glueck, B. C. (1966). Six cases of hypertensive crisis in patients on tranylcypromine after eating chicken livers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 933–937. Henderson, W. R., & Raskin, N. (1972). Hot dog headache: Individual susceptibility to nitrite. Lancet, 2, 1162–1163. Holzhammer, J., & Wöber, C. (2006). [Alimentary trigger factors that provoke migraine and tension-type headache]. Schmerz (Berlin, Germany), 20, 151–159. Horowitz, D., Lovenberg, W., Engelman, K., & Sjoerdsma, A. (1964). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors, tyramine and cheese. Journal of the American Medical Association, 188(13), 1108–1110. Jansen, S. C., van Dusseldorp, M., Bottema, K. C., & 174 Dubois, A. E. (2003). Intolerance to dietary biogenic amines: A review. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology, 91, 233–240. Juliano, L. M., & Griffith, R. R. (2005). Caffeine. In J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, & R. B. Milliman (Eds.), Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook (pp. 403–421). New York: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. Jura, Y. H., Townsend, M. K., Curhan, G. C., Resnick, N. M., & Grodstein, F. (2011). Caffeine intake, and the risk of stress, urgency and mixed urinary incontinence. Journal of Urology, 188, 1775–1780. Kahleova, H., Matoulek, M., Malinska, H., Oliyarnik, O., Kazdova, L., Neskudla, T., et al. (2011). Vegetarian diet improves insulin resistance and oxidative stress markers more than conventional diet in subjects with Type 2 diabetes. Diabetic Medicine, 28, 549–559. Kalow, W., & Tang, B. K. (1991). Caffeine as a metabolic probe: Exploration of the enzyme-inducing effect of cigarette smoking. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 49, 44–48. Lampe J. W., King, I. B., Li, S., Grate, M. T., Barale, K. V., Chen, C., et al. (2000). Brassica vegetables increase and apiaceous vegetables decrease cytochrome P4501A2 activity in humans: Changes in caffeine metabolic ratios in response to controlled vegetable diets. Carcinogenesis, 21(6), 1157–1162. Landolt, H. P., Rétey, J. V., Tönz, K., Gottselig, J. M., Khatami, R., Buckelmüller, I., et al. (2004). Caffeine attenuates waking and sleep electroencephalographic markers of sleep homeostasis in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 1933–1939. Lane, J. D., Phillips-Bute, B. G., & Pieper, C. F. (1998). Caffeine raises blood pressure at work. Psychsomatic Medicine, 60, 327–330. Lara, D. R. (2010). Caffeine, mental health and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 20(Suppl. 1), S239–S248. Li, S., Guerin-Deremaux, L., Pochat, M., Wils, D., Reifer, C., & Miller, L. E. (2010). NUTRIOSE dietary fiber supplementation improves insulin resistance and determinants of metabolic syndrome in overweight men: A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. Applied Physiology, Nutrition and Metabolism, 35, 773–782. Luciano, M., Zhu, G., Kirk, K. M., Gordon, S. D., Heath, A. C., Montgomery, G. W., et al. (2007). “No thanks, it keeps me awake”: The genetics of coffee-attributed sleep disturbance. Sleep, 30, 1378–1386. Marley, E., & Blackwell, B. (1970). Interactions of monoamine oxidase inhibitors, amines, and foodstuffs. Advances in Pharmacology and Chemotherapy, 8, 185– 239. Maxwell, M. B. (1980). Reexamining the dietary restrictions with procarbazine (an MAOI). Cancer Nursing, 3, 451–457. McCabe, B. J. (1986). Dietary tyramine and other pressor amines in MAOI regimens. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 86(8), 1059–1064. Meng, I. D., Dodick, D., Ossipov, M. H., & Porreca, F. III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS (2011). Pathophysiology of medication overuse headache: Insights and hypotheses from preclinical studies. Cephalalgia, 31(7), 851–860. Milde-Busch, A., Blaschek, A., Borggräfe, I., Heinen, F., Straube, A., von Kries, R. (2010). Associations of diet and lifestyle with headache in high-school students: Results from a cross-sectional study. Headache, 50, 1104–1114. Nardi, A. E., Lopes, F. L., Freire, R. C., Veras, A. B., Nascimento, I., Valença, A. M., et al. (2009). Panic disorder and social anxiety disorder subtypes in a caffeine challenge test. Psychiatry Research, 169, 149–153. Orlosky, M. (1982). MAO inhibitors in sickness and in health. Massachusetts General Hospital Newsletter: Biological Therapies in Psychiatry, 5, 25–28. Panconesi, A. (2008). Alcohol and migraine: Trigger factor, consumption, mechanisms: A review. Journal of Headache Pain, 9(1), 19–27. Parsons, W. D., & Neims, A. H. (1978). Effect of smoking on caffeine clearance. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 24, 40–45. Peterlin, B. L., Rapoport, A. M., & Kurth, T. (2010). Migraine and obesity: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and implications. Headache, 50, 631–648. Pradalier, A., Bakouche, P., Baudesson, G., Delage, A., Cornaille-Lafage, G., Launay, J. M., et al. (2001). Failure of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in prevention of migraine: A double-blind study versus placebo. Cephalalgia, 21, 818–822. Rice, S. L., Eitenmiller, B. R., & Kohler, P. E. (1975). Histamine and tyramine content of meat products. Journal of Milk Food Technology, 38, 256–258. Rice, S. L., Eitenmiller, B. R., & Kohler, P. E. (1976). Biologically active amines in food: A review. Journal of Milk Food Technology, 39, 353–358. Rice, S. L., & Kohler, P. E. (1976). Tyrosine and histidine decarboxylase activities of Pediococcus cerevisiae and Lactobacillus species and the production of tyramine in fermented sausages. Journal of Milk Food Technology, 39, 166–169. Riksen, N. P., Rongen, G. A., & Smits, P. (2009). Acute and long-term cardiovascular effects of coffee: Implications for coronary heart disease. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 121, 185–191. Rivas-Gonzalo, J. C., Santos-Hernandez, J. G., & MarineFont, A. (1983). Study of the evolution of tyramine content during the vinification process. Journal of Food Science, 48, 417–418. Rogers, P. J., Hohoff, C., Heatherley, S. V., Mullings, E. L., Maxfield, P. J., Evershed, R. P., et al. (2010). Association of the anxiogenic and alerting effects of caffeine with ADORA2A and ADORA1 polymorphisms and habitual level of caffeine consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 1973–1983. Savi, L., Rainero, I., Valfrè, W., Gentile, S., Lo Giudice, R., & Pinessi, L. (2002). Food and headache attacks: A comparison of patients with migraine and tension-type headache. Panminerva Medica, 44, 27–31. 10. Dietary Considerations Selekler, H. M., & Budak, F. (2004). Idiopathic stabbing headache and experimental ice cream headache (shortlived headaches). European Neurology, 51, 6–9. Selekler, H. M., Erdogan, M. S., & Budak, F. (2004). Prevalence and clinical characteristics of an experimental model of “ice-cream headache” in migraine and episodic tension-type headache patients. Cephalalgia, 24, 293–297. Sen, N. P. (1969). Analysis and significance of tyramine content in foods. Journal of Food Science, 34, 22–26. Shapiro, R. E. (2008). Caffeine and headaches. Current Pain and Headache Reports, 12, 311–315. Shevel, E. (2011). The extracranial vascular theory of migraine—a great story confirmed by the facts. Headache, 3, 409–417. Spigt, M. G., Kuijper, E. C., Schayck, C. P., Troost, J., Knipschild, P. G., Linssen, V. M., et al. (2005). Increasing the daily water intake for the prophylactic treatment of headache: A pilot trial. European Journal of Neurology, 12, 715–718. Strong, F. C., III. (2000). Why do some dietary migraine patients claim they get headaches from placebos? Clinical and Experimental Allergy, 30, 739–743. Sun-Edelstein, C., & Mauskop, A. (2009). Foods and sup- 175 plements in the management of migraine headaches. Clinical Journal of Pain, 25, 446–452. Taylor, F. R. (2011). Nutraceuticals and headache: the biological basis. Headache, 51, 484–501. Tietjen, G. E., Herial, N. A., White, L., Utley, C., Kosmyna, J. M., & Khuder, S. A. (2009). Migraine and biomarkers of endothelial activation in young women. Stroke, 40, 2977–2982. Udenfriend, S. Lovenberg, W., & Sjoerdsma, A. (1959). Physiologically active amines in common fruits and vegetables. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 85, 487–490. Vidaud, Z. E., Chaviano, J., Gonzales, E., & Garcia Roche, M. O. (1987). Tyramine content of some Cuban cheeses. Die Nahrung, 31(3), 221–224. Wöber, C., Holzhammer, J., Zeitlhofer, J., Wessely, P., & Wöber-Bingöl, C. (2006). Trigger factors of migraine and tension-type headache: Experience and knowledge of the patients. Journal of Headache Pain, 7, 188–195. Wöber, C., & Wöber-Bingöl, C. (2010). Triggers of migraine and tension-type headache. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 97, 161–172. Zencirci, B. (2010). Comparison of the effects of dietary factors in the management and prophylaxis of migraine. Journal of Pain Research, 3, 125–130. C h a p t e r 11 Biofeedback and Cognitive‑Behavioral Interventions Reciprocal Contributions Daniel Hamiel and Arnon Rolnick We discuss in this chapter the integration of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) and biofeedback. Integrating these two major interventions improves the potential of each and increases their viability. The concept of a common-law marriage can figuratively describe the relationship between biofeedback and CBT. A common-law marriage is a de facto relationship that is legally recognized as a marriage even though no legal marriage ceremony was performed. Similarly, CBT and biofeedback, can “live together” de facto: Both are short-term goal-directed interventions that aim to maintain and reinforce their evidence-based status. Early in their relationship, both approaches were related to learning theory and were based partly on relaxation techniques. CBT handbooks today do not include chapters about biofeedback (cf. Dobson, 2010; Bond & Dryden, 2004). The biofeedback literature, on the other hand, continues to recognize the role of cognitive factors in biofeedback interventions. We are not aware of any clear therapeutic protocol that suggests how to integrate the two methods. We discuss in the first part of this chapter how cognitive-behavioral concepts and techniques can help practitioners using biofeedback. The second part shows how cognitive-behavioral psychotherapists may use biofeedback techniques. We conclude the chapter by considering how general attitudes and presenting problems guide selection of interventions. Biofeedback intervention was originally based on a learning theory model; however, it soon became clear that thoughts, emotions, and interpersonal processes can affect the success of biofeedback interventions. Several authors contend that from a theoretical point of view, the efficacy of biofeedback is often partially due to cognitive factors (Wickramasekera, 2002; Meichenbaum, 2007; Holroyd et al., 1984; Penzien & Holroyd, 2008). How Biofeedback Practitioners Can Integrate Cognitive‑Behavioral Elements into Their Practice The suggested model should take into account the client’s thoughts during the session and the basic assumptions (sometimes hidden) that impact the client’s views about him- or herself and the ability to self-regulate. Here, a metaphoric screen, the “cognitive–emotional screen” is introduced. Clients share their thoughts with the therapist or voice “what went on in their mind,” and therapist and client both observe this process in relation to the physiological data screen. Whereas working with the physiological screen involves repeated practice, working with the subjective experi176 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions 177 ence requires a significant amount of discussion. Indeed, the focus within this model is on the relationship between subjective content (thoughts/ images) and objective measurement (physiology). It is challenging to decide when and how to focus on the physiological feedback versus engaging in the verbal therapeutic discussion of the subjective experiences. One additional element of the integrated model is the therapeutic relationship and how it influences the client’s ability to self-regulate. According to Taub and School (1978, p. 617), “perhaps the most powerful factor influencing whether or not thermal biofeedback learning will occur is the quality of the interaction between the experimenter/therapist and the subject/patient, that is, the ‘person factor.’ ” New directions in CBT, for example, schema-focused therapy (Young, Klosko, & Weishaar, 2003), also emphasizes the importance of the therapeutic relationship. The proposed model introduces new elements to classic biofeedback training by describing ological demonstrations are crucial in establishing the client’s motivation and trust in the intervention process. Client motivation is also reinforced during the psychophysiological intake interview, when the practitioner gathers information pertaining to the client’s reactivity and recovery rate. The practitioner shares this information with the client. The process enables the therapeutic triangle of client, practitioner, and biofeedback. This enables clients to understand the cognitive–psychophysiological relationships. 1. The macro level: How to use CBT elements in the in the in the process of the training. 2. The micro level: How to use CBT elements within each session. The Macro Level: Stages Acquaintance and Educational Stage The acquaintance and educational stage is the “psychophysiological profiling” stage, in which the practitioner measures a client’s physiological responses to a variety of stimuli. We use the term “acquaintance” for this stage, because the practitioner becomes acquainted with the client’s psychological and physiological responses, and the client is introduced to the psychophysiological view of his or her disorder for the first time. The client experiences and learns the psychophysiological “dialogue” associated with their cognitions. The cognitive approach is based on the biopsychosocial model, which emphasizes the crucial role played by the client’s thinking and active involvement in treatment. The acquaintance stage also involves client education and instruction in the intervention. Much of the explanation is through psychophysiological demonstrations. The practitioner shows the client how specific external stimuli (e.g., behavior, thoughts, and imagination) influence physiological responses and symptoms. These psychophysi- Behavioral Techniques Stage: Acquiring Self‑Regulation Relaxation techniques, represent the B, behavioral, in CBT. A CBT practitioner listens to the client’s cognitions during the relaxation learning process. What is the client’s internal dialogue? Does the client believe that he or she is able to train his or her physiology? Does the client feel “betrayed” by his or her body? Failures in the selfregulation process can be used as an opportunity to learn about possible cognitions that can be discussed and modified. Cognitive Techniques Stage: Acquiring Self‑Regulation In addition to biofeedback, cognitive techniques can be very useful in teaching means to achieve relaxation. For example, Meichenbaum's (2007) stress inoculation technique (SIT) can facilitate autogenic therapy. In SIT, subjects are taught to substitute positive self-statements and expectations for negative self-statements about their ability to relax. Clients often have difficulties reducing physiological arousal. This is a time to help the client understand the automatic thoughts that can interfere with acquiring relaxation skills. According to the ABC model, the source of an individual’s emotional, physiological or behavioral Consequences is not the Actual events but the individual’s thoughts and Belief system. Using the ABC model, we can help the client identify and modify automatic thoughts and beliefs. A “third generation” of cognitive interventions, acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT; Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006), has emerged in recent years. One of the main premises of ACT is the understanding that to try too hard is unproductive. Practitioners using biofeedback can 178 use this ACT principle to help clients learn to “let go” and “not try too hard.” Generalization Stage Clients, then, must learn to implement self-regulation techniques outside the practitioner’s office, thus generalizing them. Relaxing under less comfortable conditions is challenging, and altering the conditions typically is done gradually. For example, the client starts in a reclining chair, then a sitting position in an office chair, and later standing up. The client also learns to practice with more common mental state conditions. Training proceeds with the client staying relaxed while maintaining alertness, and being involved with the environment without being overwhelmed by its demands. We ask the client what external and internal conditions help or thwart self-regulation. All of these elements aid generalization. Exposure and Desensitization Stage Exposure is a major intervention in CBT. While biofeedback procedures do not necessarily include such intervention, the proposed model suggests integrating various types of exposure into biofeedback practice. The cardinal principle to remember during this stage is to achieve a state of relaxation while simultaneously re-creating a real or imaginary stressful scenario. For example, a client who is afraid of speaking in front of an audience is asked to prepare and deliver a speech while continuing to regulate his or her physiology. An instruction to maintain physiological readings at a low level or to lower them as soon as they begin to rise, is provided. A client who is anxious about taking tests provides another example. The practitioner can create test scenarios during the therapy session and train the client to reduce stress during a test. At times it may be necessary to re-create an actual test scenario, but usually in vitro re-creation is sufficient. We can therefore train clients to implement and practice relaxation techniques during difficult situations, whether in vivo or in vitro. Clients can advance to the final stage only after they acquire and learn to implement control abilities in stressful situations as well. Weaning and Termination Stage All forms of treatment are geared toward enabling clients to separate from and leave the supportive III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS environment. Clients who sought out biofeedback because they were not able to regulate their tensions acquire the ability to do so without the intervention. It is also assumed that some of the client’s psychological problems will emerge during the transitional or separation phases. Therefore, working through this stage while the client is still in treatment provides the opportunity to re-create difficulties pertaining to this process. In biofeedback therapy, termination involves “weaning” off the device as well, thus enabling the client to gain control over his or her psychophysiological responses without direct feedback. During this stage one of the client’s task is to link internal physiological clues with psychophysiological readings, then construct a replacement in the form of habitual internal feedback. Hence, this stage deals with the ability to generate the relaxation response even without the aid of the device. To achieve this goal, the practitioner measures the physiological variable without displaying the feedback. The client is asked to “guess” his or her physiological status, as well as attempt to estimate whether the line of the graph is sloping upward or downward (“discrimination training”; Andrasik & Blanchard, 1983; Gainer, 1978). This stage is marked by the client’s deliberations over whether the time has come to terminate treatment, and whether the treatment has achieved its goals. Questions are raised pertaining to dependence and independence. Moreover, the client must cope with the loss of fantasies that “all my problems will be solved” or “I will not get tense anymore.” Indeed, the client must relinquish some of his or her basic assumptions regarding constantly being in control and feeling good. We expect that during this stage of treatment, the emphasis will be on the verbal component of the psychotherapeutic dialogue. (For a discussion of other ways to facilitate generalization and transfer of training, see Lynn & Freedman, 1979.) The Micro Level: Session Structure As cognitive and emotional factors are introduced into biofeedback training, the structure of the sessions should include various phases that allow identification and discussions of the client’s cognitions. Beck (1995) suggests that CBT sessions should be divided into a number of phases: brief update and mood check, transition from previous session, setting agenda, homework review, discussion of agenda issues, homework assignment, sum- 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions 179 mary, and feedback. The biofeedback literature is less consistent regarding this issue. Some writers describe the various sensors used to take measurements, the feedback screen, and other equipment in great detail; however, the session format is often left unmentioned. This may explain why so many laypeople believe that each biofeedback session consists of a full hour with the client sitting in front of the screen trying to control some line graphs. In contrast, Andrasik and Schwartz (see Chapter 20, this volume) describe a possible format for headache sessions: adaptation, baseline, self-regulation, stimulation, biofeedback, reassessment of self-regulation (also see Andrasik & Blanchard, 1983). We divide each session into different phases; some of them are mainly verbal phases in which we discuss the client’s internal processes (emotions, thoughts, and cognitions). Other phases use the physiological measures, with each phase using biofeedback devices differently. So we obtain measurements and provide biofeedback several times during a session. Each of these events is then accompanied by a period dedicated to verbal discussion about the subjective and objective psychophysiological data. The verbal dialogue enhances the physiological changes and vice versa. The full description of these phases is available at www. marksschwartzphd.com. third generation of CBT. This new therapy trend introduced the role of mindfulness (Hayes et al., 2006) and controlling attention in therapy (Wells, 2000). In this section we show how biofeedback is relevant to these developments, theoretically and practically. How Cognitive‑Behavioral Therapists Can Integrate Biofeedback into Their Practice In the past, physiology was neglected in psychotherapy, and to a certain extent this remains true today. This trend began when Freud abandoned his project aimed at combining neurology and psychiatry (Gay, 1998) and continued when cognitive therapists ascribed an exclusive position in therapy to thoughts (Beck, 1995). Work by Servan-Schreiber (2004) and LeDoux (1996) emphasize the special characteristics of the emotional brain. This brain is responsible for emotions and physiology but often functions separately from the cortex and from cognitive influences. Hence, the physiological component in therapy becomes very important. Under the assumption that physiology plays a major role in mental health, we focus in this section on the role of biofeedback as a therapeutic agent in a CBT setting. The role of biofeedback and physiology in therapy has recently become more natural with the reinforcing influence of the Rationale: Biofeedback as Training for Metacontrol Individuals seek out therapy because they are unable to stop or control their suffering, whether physical or mental. Naturally, they believe that regaining control will put an end to their suffering. Rather than directly trying to stop suffering and get the individual back in control, CBT challenges and changes the concept of control. CBT proposes the notion that a person's problem is not with reality (i.e., not being able to stop the suffering) but with the way he or she perceives this reality (the belief that the person should stop the suffering to go on with life—the ABC model discussed earlier). The goal of therapy is to persuade patients to give up their dysfunctional basic assumption that life should go the way they want or expect it to go. CBT seeks to provide a more sophisticated concept of control, that is, the freedom to relate to one’s situations rather than trying to control them completely (something that is not always possible). Individuals should be liberated from the (Western) addiction to achieving everything they want. According to the CBT concept, “control” is the freedom to make a decision about whether to try to change actual reality or whether to take on the more challenging task of accepting reality when necessary. This ability is referred to as “metacontrol.” Relaxation can serve as an important step toward achieving acceptance and metacontrol. Therapists like to help their patients choose between holding on (remaining tense to make a change) and letting go (relaxing and accepting). Yet this choice might be threatening for a patient. In contrast to relaxation, hypervigilance and tension serve a patient’s need to feel in control, which is very hard to abandon. Biofeedback can provide an exciting solution to this obstacle. Relaxation using biofeedback involves experiencing control (though it is actually an act of giving up) and is therefore much less threatening. Indeed, biofeedback is unique in that the individual experiences a sense of control while actually letting go. This is precisely the meaning of “metacontrol,” as mentioned earlier. 180 In CBT, patients learn to give up their basic dysfunctional assumptions related to their need for control. The therapist persuades the patient to distract him- or herself from things that used to make him or her feel more responsible and more in control (e.g., being preoccupied with worries). Feeling in control based on continuous worrying is an illusion, but letting go of the feeling is still not easy. In therapy, patients are exposed to scenarios that create the most threatening state of mind for them—relinquishing the wish for complete confidence. To do so, patients must abandon the bodily state of alertness that they use to maintain the illusion of being in control. By going through such a process, the patient eventually gives up the need for absolute control and the illusion of having it. In return, the patient achieves the best type of control possible in the actual situation. Biofeedback plays an important role in achieving this “best possible control” experience, that is, “metacontrol.” Conditioned Emotional Triggers: What CBT Cannot Resolve Dysfunctional basic assumptions can usually be addressed by CBT. However, direct and increased sensitivity to external triggers related to the emotional brain (discussed earlier) often cannot be influenced by the cortex and should therefore be treated by a system other than the cognitive one. This sensitivity is conditioned and very much connected to physiology. The integrated CBT and biofeedback (physiological) intervention relates to each individual as a whole, including both cognitive and emotional–physiological dimensions. This proposed intervention attempts to respond to the challenge posed by direct, noncognitive, emotional, and physiological sensitivity by adding a physiological dimension to therapy. As in the first part of this chapter, two different views of the intervention are presented. First, the general role of biofeedback in the different stages of CBT is discussed, then its practical application in the sessions themselves is considered. Each subsequent CBT stage is needed if the previous one proves insufficient to solve the patient's problem reasonably. Stages in Biofeedback‑Assisted CBT Intervention • Stage 1: Attempting to Stop the Suffering. At this stage of therapy, the patient has already identified the catastrophic thoughts that accompany III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS suffering, but they are not yet being effectively challenged. The therapist assumes at this stage that the patient does not really believe in the catastrophic thoughts, which serve other purposes, and therefore has no need to challenge them. The patient is aware of the poisoning effect of these thoughts and has learned not to cooperate with them. Biofeedback at this stage helps deflect the patient’s focus from the thoughts and relieves suffering by reducing the bodily stress that accompanies these thoughts. This step reflects the normal role of biofeedback as a tool for improved control over the body. By reducing bodily stress, the patient can shift his or her focus from the “thinking about” mode to a more positive and active coping mode. This is sometimes sufficient to stop the suffering. • Stage 2: Attempting to Reduce Suffering by Changing a Patient’s Way of Thinking to Change the Resultant Destructive Behavior. Changing the patient’s thinking is the hard core of most cognitive-behavioral interventions. At this stage, the therapist tries to challenge the patient’s dysfunctional basic assumptions that are the source of suffering, and to shift the focus of attention from fixed suffering to functionality. The patient learns to accept that direct efforts to stop the suffering are insufficient. Biofeedback helps patients learn and use the concept of “metacontrol” to achieve this goal. The therapist helps the patient understand the reasons he or she is stuck because of negative and catastrophic thoughts. Together they try to discover the hidden reasons (“metacognitions” according to Wells, 2000) that make the patient focus on the negative thoughts. One major hidden reason is the concomitant experience (or rather, the illusion) of taking control over one’s situation by constant thinking or worrying. The therapist and the patient attempt to discover dysfunctional basic assumptions that make the patient believe in the catastrophic thoughts. For example, if a patient believes that to feel safe he or she needs a strong supportive figure, when alone he or she might be afraid of “falling apart.” The patient may try to avoid being alone, and this effort may be expressed physiologically. CBT that works on cognition alone will not be sufficient because of the strong negative conditioning usually involved in such a case. Here, the role of biofeedback is to release the physical tension preventing the patient from thinking realistically. Bodily alertness and hypervigilance are related 181 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions to narrowed attention and a polar way of thinking, which may be effective in emergencies but not under normal circumstances. Biofeedback and physiological techniques in general help patients expand their attention and open them up to more flexible ways of thinking and behaving. They serve to release bodily tension from emergency mode. • Stage 3: Directly Experiencing Emotions and Sensations. When an individual cannot control his or her behavior due to the intensity of the negative emotions, a third stage is needed. Changing cognitive assumptions, releasing physical tension, and defocusing are not sufficient. Some forms of suffering are resistant to disappearing. At this stage, the patient is taught to overcome judgmental thinking that feeds negative uncontrolled emotions and is related to the past. The patient is directed to remain focused on the difficult emotions and sensations of bodily stress. The patient learns how to enhance skills to remain in the present (mindfulness) with the suffering (the emotions and sensations) and to deflect the focus away from negative judgmental thoughts (the past) and anticipatory anxiety (the future). Bodily sensations reflect the most pure experience of the present. Hence, biofeedback, which helps connect the patient to his or her body, plays an important role in developing this “present-orientation skill.” By teaching the patient to recognize bodily changes, partly control them, and accept the fact that control is only partial, biofeedback helps the patient to keep from returning to a focus on the negative thoughts (usually judgmental self-evaluation). Consequently, it helps to minimize the emotional and physiological impact on suffering and behavior. The amount of suffering that remains probably is necessary to illuminate some problematic issues the patient still needs to address. Integrated biofeedback-assisted CBT interventions are recommended primarily for treating people with anxiety disorders that are marked by elements of hyperarousal and “fear of loss of control” or states of uncertainty. These characteristics are associated with many other psychiatric disorders, such as eating, mood, sleep, and impulse control disorders. The following is a clinical demonstration of the potential advantage of using biofeedback techniques in CBT. First, the use of biofeedback in the psychoeducational part of the therapy session is described, followed by a demonstration of the core of therapy. The Use of Biofeedback Techniques in CBT Experiential Psychoeducation The psychoeducational part of CBT can be very influential, and, for some patients, this accounts for most of the change effected by the therapy. In simple CBT, the therapist uses the cognition itself to demonstrate some cognitive distortions and relevant information. However, this remains on the verbal level, which in turn is affected by the patient’s cognitive distortions and subjective perceptions. On the other hand, integrated therapy can help the patient directly experience various effects of their thoughts and expectations. Here are some examples: • Demonstration of the fight-or-flight response. Eliciting any surprise response from patients enables them to experience the normality of such a response, which they otherwise would have perceived as some form of personal weakness. In such a response, the electrodermal response (EDR) line graph ascends rapidly, then slowly descends. One of the major problems in dealing with anxiety is the patient’s response to his or her symptoms. The patient becomes anxious and sometimes depressed because of this anxiety. Normalizing this anxiety by demonstrating the “fight-or-flight response” and other mind–body connections can alleviate some of the patient’s worries about his or her own responses and symptoms. • Demonstration of anticipatory anxiety. The goal of this intervention is to challenge the patient’s misconception that being ready in advance will help. The therapist asks the patient to get ready for the same kind of “surprise” generated by the “fightor-flight” response demonstration but this time to try to suppress the response in advance. The therapist counts from 10 to 0, then presents the previously used stimulus, while assessing arousal level throughout the entire process. The therapist then shows the patient the high price of arousal that is paid for being prepared. This overpreparedness is a very common component of many psychopathologies. Anxious patients are constantly on the alert in order to be ready for possible future disasters. They are preoccupied with thoughts related to the past and worries about future disasters. This intervention also demonstrates very nicely the concept of “unnecessary suffering.” Relinquishing this tendency toward being prepared in advance will help the patient avoid accumulating unnecessary stress and worries. 182 • Demonstration of the thought–physiology– emotion connection. According to CBT, an event is not the reason for the consequences; rather, it is due to the person’s thoughts or the belief system. The following biofeedback demonstration can illustrate this unequivocally. The therapist says: “Try to make the line graph descend, faster . . . faster. . . . I am disappointed in you.” The therapist can also “threaten” the patient with a difficult math test. This simple exercise can also demonstrate the notion of “metacontrol.” Trying too hard will not help the patient control his or her physiology. The physiological recordings cannot be controlled until the patient gives up or relinquishes the desire to relax or achieve balance. • Understanding psychosomatic phenomena. This can be demonstrated by asking the patient something that causes an internal conflict between the wish to please the therapist and the resistance to the request. For example, the therapist might say, “May I ask you a personal question?” In a group, the therapist might say the following to one of the participants: “Will you straighten up the room at the end of the session?” The patient will probably say “yes,” even though his or her body keeps saying “no,” expressing all real feelings via physiological changes, by increased arousal level, increased muscle tension, and lowered peripheral temperature. The therapist then explains that saying “no” too often will bring about a continuous somatic response, or psychosomatic phenomena. Such a demonstration, in contrast to a purely theoretical explanation, has the potential to give the psychotherapy a real push by offering a rationale for psychosomatic symptoms and the motivation to look for the mental source of such symptoms. • Comparison between practicing biofeedback with eyes open and eyes closed. If a patient performs better with his or her eyes open, this may signal an added need for control (very often the case with children). Problems in controlling biofeedback measures with eyes open may indicate performance anxiety and sometimes a coping model of avoidance. In such cases, the therapist should train the patient in biofeedback therapy with eyes open while remaining aware of the patient’s performance. On the other hand, practicing biofeedback with eyes closed can provide exposure for a patient who has difficulties performing under conditions of uncertainty. This kind of exposure can be a particularly important experience for patients with anxiety disorders. III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS • Demonstration of the power of acceptance and distraction. As described earlier, an important element in CBT involves relinquishing the tendency to judge oneself through constant evaluations of success. Using biofeedback to distract patients from this constant evaluation can help them accept and evaluate results almost without self-judgment. During biofeedback practice, the patient is trained to distract him- or herself from the direct goal of success. This usually improves performance. The patient learns to give up on a coping model that focuses on results and learns to enjoy the process. The patient is trained to be aware of his or her performance, to accept it even if he or she is not satisfied, and to continue to do his or her best in a balanced and enjoyable way. The patient is taught the importance of distraction from judgment and learns to focus on practical evaluation of performance, as well as to decide what action to take. Both criticism and compliments of the patient’s performance during biofeedback will most probably harm performance. On the other hand, acceptance of what cannot change for now (unavoidable suffering in the present) will maximize performance. • Understanding the activity of the autonomic nervous system (ANS). Heart rate variability (HRV) as a measure of ANS balance level is shown by the ratio between the activity of the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous systems. Biofeedback software settings that provide direct feedback about the patient’s balance as reflected in the ratio of these two systems help patients learn that the goal is usually not relaxation but achieving a balanced state. The understanding that relaxation is not the goal is helpful to patients who fear losing control, because balancing the ANS allows for control, sometimes without the feature of relaxation. An increased belief in ability to control provides the patient with the needed confidence to expose him- or herself to previously avoided “risky situations.” • Demonstrating the difference between a baseline state, which usually indicates low ANS balance, and the results of short biofeedback intervention (biofeedback with slow breathing or guided imagery). Using this method, it is usually easy to generate a quick change. Low HRV usually co-occurs with a coping model of alert hypervigilance or a simplified conception of control, such as “It would be awful for things to happen against my will, so I have to remain alert.” Simple physiological intervention such as slow breathing with the help of 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions 183 biofeedback can be used to demonstrate how easy it is to relinquish this attitude and increase HRV and balance level, at least for the present time. kinds of meditation or mindfulness techniques, the patient attempts to minimize distractions, reduce bodily responses to destructive ideas, and decrease arousal level. These parameters can easily be monitored with the EDR modality (and others as well), hence making it possible to check the effectiveness of mindfulness and meditation techniques. Paying attention to the body is an important element in mindfulness techniques. Eastern philosophy and CBT share the notion that focusing on the present releases individuals from their fears and worries. An individual cannot become anxious unless past negative memories and worries about the future are combined. Focusing on experiencing the present can prevent this. Using biofeedback to increase awareness of body sensations can support this process. Awareness of bodily sensations helps distract the individual from thoughts that are not related to the present and therefore helps the person experience the present. This training is critical in the third stage of therapy described earlier. Exposure is the main goal of each stage of CBT: being exposed to life's tasks and to challenges the patient is trying to avoid. Biofeedback can support this process in several ways: • The same can be demonstrated very nicely using electromyography (EMG). Patients hooked up to EMG sensors on their extensor or flexor muscles usually exhibit high EMG levels, sometimes even above 20 mV. For many patients, however, asking them to relax their muscles and sit comfortably is sufficient to monitor a dramatic reduction in muscle tension (below 1mV)! So after a short intervention, excessively high readings (usually a low balance of ANS at baseline) can turn into much more balanced ANS readings. • The simultaneous use of two sensors (modalities) can reveal some very interesting information about an individual’s coping model. Very often we find a contradiction between measurements in two different modalities. For instance, during guided imagery, a child with encopresis describes an “accident” while in class. As predicted, his peripheral temperature decreases, but his EDR also decreases! When asked what he is doing in class during the “accident,” the child answers, “I am pretending I am not there!” The child is dissociating, or in CBT terms, using a coping model of avoidance. In this case, the child is not avoiding the situation itself but rather is dealing with the situation. Our conclusion is that as long as an individual’s physiological modalities are not working in the expected direction, he or she is still paying a physiological price for some unfinished psychological issues or a nonadaptive coping model. Biofeedback can serve as a beacon, lighting the way for psychological intervention. The Core of Therapy In Stage 1 of the therapy, the task is to stop suffering. Here, the role of the biofeedback in the core of therapy is clear. The therapist trains the patient to better control physiological elements using different biofeedback modalities. Specifically, the patient is taught to attempt to control symptoms directly, such as headache and muscle tension, breathing problems due to stress, extreme sweating, cold hands, panic attacks, and tremors. In discussing the stages of CBT therapy earlier, we described the importance of controlling attention. This skill is relevant in all stages of therapy. The easiest biofeedback modality to use for enhancing this skill is the EDR. Using different • Demonstrating the exposure principle. The patient needs to learn that avoidance is not the only way to reduce anxiety. Paradoxically, exposure does this much better, and the effect remains for a longer time. The EDR and EMG line graphs descend (and the temperature rises) with exposure (desensitization or even flooding type of exposure), demonstrating habituation, while the patient keeps a focus on the source of his or her negative emotions. The therapist can use sounds, pictures, or movies, or can create a scene related to the patient's source of negative emotions. This is appropriate for treating phobias, and can also work very well with exposure to problematic thoughts, as in obsessive–compulsive disorder. For the patient, monitoring bodily sensations during exposure is like looking inside oneself. The screen presenting the new balance in the patients’ physiological responses convinces him or her that the anxiety has been resolved even before it is noticed personally. • In vivo exposure. This can often be implemented in the clinic (e.g., when treating fears related to blood tests, injections, blood pressure measurement, or insects). The therapist prepares 184 the patients for an increase in stress and arousal level at the beginning of exposure, and later habituation. Often, however, exposure occurs without any anxiety emerging. The patient experiences the paradoxical effect of the exposure from the first moment. For example, asking the patient to focus on negative thoughts leads to a decrease in arousal level and anxiety, while asking the patient to avoid negative thoughts causes an increase in arousal level and anxiety. These phenomena, best demonstrated by the biofeedback graph, demonstrate the importance of exposure. Also demonstrated is the ineffectiveness of attempting to push thoughts out of one’s mind and the effectiveness of accepting thoughts. • In vitro exposure. Guided imagery of the actual problematic scenario the patient is trying to avoid is used in preparing for in vivo exposure. If the patient is hooked up to biofeedback sensors, it is possible to identify the more problematic points during in vitro exposure and better prepare for in vivo exposure. More importantly, implementing this imagined in vitro exposure many times makes the real exposure (in vivo) much easier. These are all examples of how biofeedback can motivate, enhance, comment on, and demonstrate a variety of cognitive techniques at different stages of therapy. III. ADJUNCTIVE/COMPLEMENTARY INTERVENTIONS representing the second difference between the models, depends on the individual’s characteristics, so it is important to explore the above characteristics in every client. The answer depends also on the presenting problem, representing yet another difference between the two models. If extreme physiological responding is involved, it makes sense to try to bring the patient some relief and use biofeedback. However, this can still be achieved by a psychotherapist familiar with biofeedback (second model or intervention). Yet there is a clear difference between the two; therefore, there are clear criteria for when to start or to switch to psychotherapy. When there is a major psychological problem (strong dysfunctional basic assumptions), biofeedback intervention is not sufficient. If this is known in advance, through experience with the patient or questionnaires, starting with the second intervention is advisable. If a major psychological problem emerges during biofeedback intervention, it is prudent to move to psychotherapy as well. Otherwise, the main criteria are personal characteristics, as described earlier. Given that most psychotherapists still refrain from the use of psychophysiological tools, we can imagine an opposite scenario in the case of extreme physiological symptoms or unbalanced ANS that ordinary psychotherapy cannot effectively address. In such cases it makes sense to refer the client to a biofeedback practitioner for a parallel intervention. Conclusion The first difference between these two models— CBT-assisted biofeedback and biofeedback-assisted CBT—is in who implements the intervention. In the first model, this is a biofeedback practitioner, while in the second, it is a psychotherapist. Naturally, the client is the first to select between the two interventions by deciding whether to pursue biofeedback intervention or psychotherapy. It is assumed that individuals who select biofeedback will have some, though not all, of the following characteristics: pay more attention to the body, become more concrete, look for control, be more scientifically oriented, be less oriented to talking about feelings, less able “to get into” thoughts and feelings, less aware of possible psychological conflicts, and looking for a quick change (Wickramaskera, 2002). Those who prefer psychotherapy will have more or less the opposite characteristics. Still, if the decision is ours, whom should we refer to which type of intervention? The answer, References Andrasik, F., & Blanchard, E. B. (1983). Applications of biofeedback to therapy. In C. E. Walker (Ed.), Clinical psychology: Theory, research, and practice (Vol. 2, pp. 1123–1164). Homewood, IL: Dow-Jones-Irwin. Beck, J. (1995). Cognitive therapy basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press. Bond, F. W., & Dryden, W. (2004). Handbook of brief cognitive behavioral therapy. New York: Wiley. Dobson, K. S. (Ed). (2010). Handbook of cognitive-behavioral therapy (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Gainer, J. C. (1978). Temperature-discrimination training in the biofeedback treatment of migraine headache. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 9, 185–188. Gay, P. (1998). Freud: A life for our time. New York: Norton. Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25. Holroyd, K. A., Penzien, D. B., Hursey, K. G., Tobin, D. L., Rogers, L., Holm, J. E., et al. (1984). Change mecha- 11. Biofeedback and Cognitive-Behavioral Interventions 185 nisms in EMG biofeedback training: Cognitive changes underlying improvements in tension headache. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1039–1053. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster. Lynn, S. J., & Freedman, R. R. (1979). Transfer and evaluation of biofeedback treatment. In A. P. Goldstein & F. Kanfer (Eds.), Maximizing treatment gains: Transfer enhancement in psychotherapy. New York: Academic Press. Meichenbaum, D. (2007). Stress inoculation training: A preventative and treatment approach. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Penzien, D. B., & Holroyd, K. A. (2008). Change mechanisms in EMG biofeedback training: Cognitive changes underlying improvements in tension headache. Headache, 48(5), 736–737. Servan-Schrieber, D. (2004). The instinct to heal: Curing stress, anxiety and depression without drugs and without talk therapy. London: Rodale International Ltd. Taub, E., & School, P. (1978). Some methodological considerations in thermal biofeedback training. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 10, 617–622. Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacognition: Innovative cognitive therapy. Chichester, UK: Wiley. Wickramasekera, I. (2002). the placebo effect and its use in biofeedback therapy. In D. Moss, A. McGrady, T. C. Davies, & I. Wickramasekera (Eds.), Handbook of mind–body medicine for primary care. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Young, J., Klosko, S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner's guide. New York: Guilford Press. Part IV Relaxation Interventions Chapter 12 Relaxation Today Self‑Stressing and Psychological Relaxation Theory Jonathan C. Smith • A client has mastered several general strategies, including concentrative meditation, visual imagery, autogenic imagery, PMR, and imagery. Should the client practice them one after another, like a mechanical fitness routine? Is there a way of more meaningfully combining or sequencing these techniques? • A client finds mindfulness and autogenics exceptionally rewarding and is highly motivated to extend the benefits of the training session to work. How might a clinician address this wish? • A client as successfully learned to control pain or reduce physiological arousal. However, he finds his routine mechanical and uninteresting and is tempted to quit. How might you alter his exercise to maintain interest and increase generalization? • A client is a devout Catholic and devotes over an hour a day to religious study and practice. She finds biofeedback relaxation mechanical and foreign. How might one incorporate her relaxation skills into a meaningful spiritual activity? We live in a new world of relaxation. In decades past, one might be proficient at a single approach, say, autogenic training or progressive muscle relaxation (PMR), mastered in an extended and costly program led by a master at a university, hospital, training clinic, or distant retreat. Today, there is a stunning profusion of freely accessible strategies from around the planet. One can find a free MP3 or YouTube presentation of just about any technique imaginable, and attempt practice anywhere the smartphone or tablet will boldly go. Although this world may well seem brave and new, it brings into clear focus a challenge that has always faced serious relaxation clinicians and biofeedback practitioners—the mindless temptation to oversimplify, to restrict oneself to one strategy, to succumb to the comfortable myth that one size fits all. Put clearly, I believe this is the core challenge: Lacking a road map, clinicians and clients can readily miss important options and opportunities. We may fail to consider some untraditional but important questions: • A client appears to master PMR. At what point might mindfulness meditation enhance the effects of training? • A client finds visual imagery too distracting. When would preparatory breathing exercises, or perhaps simple stretching, help? Which one should be tried? In this chapter I offer two road maps of selfrelaxation. I suggest a framework for making sense of the hundreds of self-relaxation exercises now available, and offer a way techniques can be systematically explored and utilized. Over the decades, I have offered various schemas. Else189 190 where I have also produced a comprehensive parallel model based on mindfulness and relaxation (Smith, 2015; http://blogs.roosevelt.edu/jsmith). Road Map 1: Self‑Stressing Theory Many relaxation experts (e.g., Lehrer, Woolfolk, & Sime, 2007) sort the myriad self-relaxation techniques into more or less six groups: yoga stretching, PMR, breathing exercises, autogenic training, imagery/positive self-statements, and meditation/ mindfulness. I propose that this differentiation is no accident but an inevitable consequence of the very nature of psychological and physiological processes that underlie stress arousal (Smith, 2006, 2007a). According to self-stressing theory, there are six ways we can trigger and sustain physiological stress arousal. Each form of self-stressing suggests a corresponding family of relaxation technique (Smith, 2006, 2007a). Let me explain. There are six ways that people trigger and maintain their physiological “fight-or-fight” stress response. 1. Stressed posture and position. When confronted with stress, people often assume a variety of defensive or aggressive postures or positions (standing, crouching, bending over a desk) for an extended time. This, combined with sustained immobility, can evoke skeletal muscle tension, joint stress, reduced blood flow, and pooling of blood, and contribute to tension, fatigue, and decreased energy. 2. Stressed skeletal muscles. When threatened, one clenches, grips, and tightens skeletal muscles to prepare for attack or escape. When chronic, such tension can contribute to pain and fatigue. 3. Stressed breathing. When stressed, one is more likely to breathe in a way that is shallow, uneven, and rapid, deploying greater use of the intercostal (rib cage) and trapezius (shoulder) muscles, and less use of the diaphragm. 4. Stressed body focus. Simply attending to and evoking thoughts and images about a specific body part or process can evoke related neurophysiologial changes. An individual facing a threat may notice her rapidly beating heart or churning stomach. Attending to and thinking about these somatic reactions can aggravate them. 5. Stressed emotion. We often motivate and energize ourselves for a stressful encounter with affect-arousing cognitions. We entertain fanta- IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS sies and repeat words and self-statements that can evoke negative affects of anxiety, anger, or depression. 6. Stressed attention. When dealing with a threat, we actively and effortfully concentrate on attacking, defending, or running. In addition, we often direct our attention to multiple targets, including competing tasks (as in multitasking), a targeted task versus worried preoccupation, or self-stressing efforts (thinking how one is breathing, maintaining a stressed posture or position, thinking about relaxed fantasies or negative emotions, etc.) rather than the task at hand. Such attentional strain maintains arousal. Self-stressing theory proposes that the current myriad popular self-relaxation techniques, used alone and often in conjunction with biofeedback, can be organized according to self-stressing. Thus, we can think of six universal family groups of selfrelaxation techniques (see Table 12.1).1 Obviously, each family group does much more than address one specific type of self-stressing. Selfstressing theory does not claim that one universal TABLE 12.1. Self-Stressing and the Six Universal Family Groups of Self-Relaxation Family group of self-relaxation Self-stressing Stretching exercises Stressed posture and position Tense–let go exercises Stressed skeletal muscles Breathing exercises Stressed breathing Autogenic training Stressed body focus Imagery and positive selfstatements Stressed emotion Meditation and mindfulness Stressed attention Note. Many popular approaches are blends of universal family groups. Thus, a variant of “hatha yoga” may blend stretching, breathing, and meditation. Jacobson’s original version of PMR in fact incorporated minimal tense–let go exercises, an occasional stretch, and breathing. What is called “mindfulness” is in fact often mindfulness and breathing with a little yoga (and occasional autogenic suggestion) sprinkled in. Unfortunately, such blends make it difficult, if not impossible, to interpret relaxation. If a popular version of mindfulness works on depression, was it the mindfulness itself, or the accompanying breathing, stretching, or autogenic exercises? Yoga research is most susceptible to such confounds. 12. Relaxation Today family group works only for one type of symptom, that PMR works only for skeletal muscle tension. Instead, the type of self-stressing associated with a type relaxation is (1) part of the training rationale, (2) incorporated in a procedural self-unstressing strategy, and (3) an initial exercise effect. So a practitioner of PMR may read that PMR targets the muscles through a tense–let go rebound effect (a typical rationale), be instructed to “focus on your shoulder muscles and let go” (a beginning procedural instruction focusing on skeletal muscles), and immediately experience shoulder relaxation after shrugging and releasing tension (initial effect). However, in time, many practitioners discover that all universal family groups of relaxation can eventually address other components of selfstressing. The student of PMR may notice that she is breathing deeper (relaxed breathing), engaging in pleasant fantasy (relaxing imagery), and focusing more easily (meditation/mindfulness). A skilled trainer may incorporate these additional effects into training by adding techniques from the universal family groups of breathing, imagery, and meditation/mindfulness exercises. Trainers often blend universal relaxation strategies. One might include breathing and imagery with PMR. Hatha yoga is often a mixture of stretching, breathing, and meditation. Mindfulness, although often defiantly presented as a pure approach, is usually a blend of mindfulness, breathing, and often a touch of imagery. Perhaps masters of relaxation intuitively recognize the value of combining family groups. However, they are usually clueless as to why various combinations work or which other combinations may work better. To give an amusing example, a few years ago I suggested that the nearly universal practice of preceding mindfulness training with breathing exercises is based on religious tradition rather than empirical evidence. The preparatory value of other approaches, perhaps autogenic training or PMR, is a question worth exploring. World-famous mindfulness researchers reacted to my observation with hostile derision (Smith, 2004). This occurred in spite of the fact that at least one famous instructor indeed unwittingly (mindlessly?) slips a few autogenic suggestions into his mindfulness training (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Self-stressing theory serves two very important purposes. First, it helps the client simplify a confusing universe of hundreds of relaxation techniques. For example, there are thousands of exercises described as “yoga.” However, they are not the same. Hundreds are part of the universal 191 family group of stretching. To this family group one might also include a wide range of Western approaches not derived from yoga. Furthermore, some exercises described as “yoga” actually belong to the “breathing” or “imagery” family group. Second, the self-stressing map gives the practitioner a powerful tool. Knowing the potential differences among relaxation strategies, one can more clearly consider how each fits relaxation goals and how exercises might be combined. For example, a biofeedback client attempting to reduce pain associated with muscle tension might explore autogenic warmth and heaviness exercises. If these happen to have limited effect in reducing muscle tension, our client may consider the map and add mindfulness meditation. Both autogenic and mindfulness strategies might then be combined with peaceful imagery. Road Map 2: Psychological Relaxation Theory It is difficult to travel the terrain of relaxation without landmarks and road signs. These let us know whether we are going in the desired direction and suggest changes we might make. In psychological relaxation theory, the landmarks and signs of relaxation are psychological states of mind, that is, client experiences. The idea is not new. For biofeedback practitioners, client self-reports often provide verbal feedback and confirmation of the effect of feedback. For example, a client may report, “My hands felt warm (self-report) the moment my monitor changed color and the tone lowered (biofeedback signal).” However, the vocabulary of selfreport used by clinicians has been limited to a few words (e.g., “Lift your finger if you feel ‘relaxed,’ ” or “Let the words ‘warm and heavy hands’ float through your mind”). I have long believed that practitioners can tell us much much more. For much of my professional life I have explored the natural language that practitioners use to report relaxation states. I began by searching over 200 core instructional textbooks for diverse traditions, East and West, old and new, spiritual and secular. These included yoga, PMR, breathing, contemplation, imagery, prayer, meditation, mindfulness, tai chi, imagery, self-hypnosis, and autogenic training. My goal was to generate a comprehensive lexicon of “relaxation,” psychological states associated with technique practice. My initial relaxation dictionary contained about 400 relaxation words, which I later reduced to about 200. At this time, it became clear that identify- 192 ing the structure of the contents of this universe would provide a psychological map of relaxation experience. Over the past 20 years, my colleagues, students, and I have subjected variations of this lexicon to nine factor-analytic studies involving 6,077 participants and over 40 relaxation techniques and activities (Smith, 2006, 2007a). Patterns and constellations of relaxation words have clearly emerged from this vast linguistic expanse. Today (Smith, 1999, 2001, 2015), I identify 19 relaxation states, or “R-states” (or alternatively “M-States” for “mindfulness states”). Twelve R-states emerged as factors. To these I identified as R-states the words that correlated uniquely and separately with various personality questionnaire variables including those from the NEO Personality Inventory— Revised (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 2012) and the 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16 PF; Cattell, Cattell, & Cattell, 1993). Also, if a set of words differentiated the effects of two techniques in a comparative pretest–posttest outcome study, those words were identified as an R-state. Two R-States (“Quick to Detect Mind Wandering” and “Easy to Let Go” are based on relaxation theory (Smith, 2015). Currently, (Smith, 2015) I group all into five levels as follows: Outline of Five Levels and 19 R‑State2 Word Groups Level 1: Basic Relaxation • R-State Disengaged (“Feeling distant, far away, detached”) • R-State Muscles Relaxed (“Body comfortable, breathing easy”) • R-State At Ease (“Peaceful, refreshed”) Level 2: Basic Mindfulness • R-State Aware (“Focused, clear”) • R-State Centered (“Absorbed, grounded”) • R-State Deepening (“Sense of ‘going deeper,’ ‘things are changing’ ”) • R-State Quiet (“Still, few thoughts”) • R-State Accepting (“Accepting what I can't have or change, ‘let it be,’ ‘it is what it is’ ”) • R-State Quick to detect mind wandering (“Easy to notice mind wandering or distraction. Catch it early”) • R-State Easy to let go and refocus (“Easy to let go of mind wandering. Not stuck or caught up in distraction”) IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS Level 3: Mindful Flow and Change • R-State Curious (“Interested. Things seem new.”) • R-State Savoring (“Enjoying each moment”) • R-State One step at a time (“Each moment comes and goes”) Level 4: Positive Emotion • R-State Happy, Optimistic, Trusting • R-State Loving, Caring • R-State Thankful, Grateful Level 5 Mindful Transcendence • R-State Awe and Wonder. Mystery. • R-State Prayerful, Reverent • R-State Timeless, Boundless, Infinite, At One Clinical Applications I find this map useful for comparing relaxation techniques and assessing client progress. Research has already identified several useful patterns (Smith, 1999). For example, a client who reports the R-states Disengaged and Muscles Relaxed apparently has accessed a limited range of R-states, a pattern I find typical for novice students of relaxation and those who benefit from PMR. Anxious and depressed clients, or those under severe stress, consistently report Disengaged as an R-state they seek when practicing relaxation. R-states are not restricted to one or two groups of relaxation. One who reports the R-states Quiet and Accepting (perhaps while practicing autogenic exercises) is describing states often reported by practitioners of meditation or mindfulness. Here one might augment autogenic training with meditation or mindfulness exercises. Similarly, one who reports feeling surprisingly Happy, or Loving, is describing a cluster of R-states that are common among those who benefit from vivid visual imagery, suggesting another strategy that might be explored. Schwartz (2003) provides a strong case for home practice of relaxation using recorded exercise instructions. I use a variation of this in my self-guided online relaxation program (Smith, 2015; http://sites.roosevevelt.edu/jsmith). Clients can download 20-minute MP3 instructions for each of the six universal family groups of relaxation (stretching, PMR, breathing, autogenic exercises, 12. Relaxation Today 193 imagery, meditation and mindfulness). In several ways these exercises differ from others that are available. Most important, I present “pure” versions so clients can identify R-states associated with each strategy. For example, PMR exercises emphasize “tense–release” cycles, with a minimum of breathing, autogenic suggestion (“Feel the warmth as your hands relax”), or imagery. This way, one knows whether any resulting R-states are the result of tensing and releasing rather than some other approach. In recent books (Smith, 2006, 2015) I help users explore the six family groups of exercises in the context of mindfulness. My structured programs provide essential rationales and step-by-step instructions. Short validated daily questionnaires and diaries for assessing R-states are offered for each approach. These sensitize clients to subtle effects and provide an ongoing record whereby client and clinician can compare and contrast techniques. • I try to present a relatively “pure” version of each approach, so that clients can discover its unique effects independently of other approaches. For example, I present PMR tense–let go cycles, minimizing breathing and imagery, so clients can clearly note the relaxation effect of tensing up and letting go. • Once a client is trained in a variety of approaches, we select those that work best and construct an individualized script and tape. Rather than present exercises as a mechanical fitness routine (as one might find in a gym), they are artfully integrated into a sequence with internal structure and meaning that expresses a client’s relaxation goals and aspirations. • The goal of relaxation training goes beyond the relaxation response of lowered arousal. Additional objectives are cultivating appropriate R-states and acquiring beliefs and personal philosophies conducive to deepening relaxation and extending its rewards to all of life. Relaxation Scripting My approach is to craft an individualized relaxation recording based on a mutually developed, verbatim script of exercise instructions. There are several advantages to such script writing: Over the last two decades, I have introduced relaxation to thousands of individuals. They have taught me one very unexpected lesson. I used to think that most clients have very specific relaxation preferences and respond best to just one general strategy, whether it be PMR, stretching, meditation, or the like. I spent many years of research attempting to identify “PMR,” “yoga,” or “meditation” types of people. So far, I have found few patterns. All this time, my clients were teaching me something very different; virtually all of them preferred highly individualized mixtures of many approaches. Very few people prefer just one or two strategies alone. This discovery has led me to develop a new approach to teaching relaxation: scripting (Smith, 2007b). I find scripting to be a powerful strategy for clients, although it is a bit time-consuming. Perhaps more important, it is an excellent tool for teaching health professionals the diversity of relaxation strategies, as well as the practical side of our two road maps of relaxation and mindfulness. “Scripting” is based on the following ideas: • Different approaches to relaxation have different effects and work differently for different people. • The best way to teach relaxation is not to impose one or two approaches on everyone, but to introduce a variety of approaches. • Since the client is inventing his or her own relaxation exercise, he or she is more likely to take it seriously and practice it regularly. Indeed, a client may well treasure his or her script as a truly personal possession, and practice it very seriously. • Given that training is varied and changing, interest and motivation are maintained, reducing premature quitting. • Finally, relaxation can be used as a reminder of personal philosophies conducive to living a life of peace and calm. Relaxation scripting takes at least 10 sessions (although I offer variations that can be offered in as few as one or two sessions in both individual and group formats—even online or over the phone). The first seven sessions are devoted to an orientation and training each of the six families of relaxation. In sessions eight through —10, one creates a script based on the exercise elements that worked best. A script is not simply a hodgepodge of exercises, or a workout sequence. Instead it is an artfully crafted blend of exercises guided by a unifying idea and targeted to client needs and interests. Client-selected R-states are inserted to enhance the effects of training (e.g., example, 194 using the words “warm and heavy” with PMR, or “focused and clear” with meditation). Care is taken to create a package of exercises that flow, ranging from those that are complex, active, and energized to those that are simple, passive, and calm. Thus, one might begin with active PMR or stretching, move to breathing, and end with autogenic suggestion and imagery, and eventually meditation. Similarly, the onset of a script may incorporate R-state words such as “alive,” “tingling,” and “release.” Mid-sequence words may include “distant,” “far away,” and “limp.” Passive and focused concluding words might include “focused,” “peaceful,” or even “reverent.” Once client and trainer have agreed on a script, both collaborate on an audio recording. This is a unique feature of relaxation scripting, one that departs from the common practice of using a generic calm “relaxation voice” whenever speaking relaxation instructions. Specifically, I pay particular attention to using a “relaxation voice” appropriate for each family group of techniques; that is, each approach to relaxation has its own suggestive voice tone and pattern. Briefly, the “tense up” components of PMR are spoken in a tone of voice that is somewhat raised, like that of a gym coach. In contrast, the following “let go” sequence of words is voiced in a quiet and slow monotone. The voice appropriate for yoga stretching is slow and somewhat energized, emphasizing the act of stretching (as if one were “stretching” with one’s voice). The breathing voice emphasizes the flow of breath, with phrases paced with the slow and deep pace of the trainer’s flow of breath. For example, as a trainer says, “Slowly let the air flow out through your lips,” he or she actually gently exhales while speaking the instruction. The voice for imagery is somewhat colorful, a gentle “storytelling voice” as opposed to a monotone. In contrast, a meditative voice is clear, calm, slow, and simple. It has little inflection. When teaching relaxation to clinicians, I devote several weeks to cultivating the voice pattern most appropriate for each family of relaxation. Often trainers have to overcome the mistaken tendency to use the same generic “soft relaxation voice” for all relaxation techniques. Some voice patterns are particularly difficult to master, for example, the slow and colorless monotone I suggest for PMR “let go” phrases, as well as autogenic training. For more on suggestions for relaxation voicing see my CD Relaxation Voice Training Program (http:// drsmith.deltalprinting.com). All of the free MP3 relaxation downloads I offer (http://blogs.roosevelt. edu/jsmith) utilize my voice pattern suggestions. IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS Conclusion Standardized relaxation recordings, MP3 downloads, and YouTube presentations provide an exciting new world of opportunities for streamlining and enhancing relaxation and biofeedback. However, I think it is a mistake to give all clients the same set of instructions and hope they practice until it works. Until now, the alternative was often to invite clients to wander untrained and unequipped into uncharted lands of confusing claims and techniques. Self-stressing and psychological relaxation theory suggest a new way. Give your client what you truly believe is the best initial approach, say, PMR. Feel free to use a CD, a DVD, a download, a book, or a group workshop. Then provide the road maps so your client can intelligently enhance and extend what you have taught. Self-stressing theory simplifies thousands of techniques into six universal family groups. For example, if you began with PMR, your client might discover that the family group meditation/mindfulness, or maybe imagery, helps it work better and contributes to generalization to life at large. Psychological relaxation theory teaches that relaxation training can go beyond the relaxation response. Clients may discover that PMR can do more than relieve stress and physical symptoms. Creatively enhanced, it can be a source of energy and insight, a productivity tool, something creative and fun, and indeed even a form of spiritual expression. Indeed, these additional goals may reinforce the impact of technique on stress and symptoms, as well as contribute to compliance and generalization to life at large. In summary, I suggest starting with what you do best. Instruction does not end when your sessions are over. Properly trained, your client is ready to step forth into a new world. Notes 1. In response to the inevitable question, “What about biofeedback? Jogging? Massage? Listening to music? Aren’t they all approaches to relaxation,” I refer the reader to Smith (2006), where I explore the distinction between self-relaxation, assisted relaxation, and casual relaxation. “Self-relaxation” includes techniques one can do by oneself, without outside mechanical, electrical, human, or animal assistance (petting pets). I would include relaxation techniques that can be learned by reading a book or listening to or watching recordings. “Assisted relaxation” requires the initial or continued support of assists (biofeedback, massage, saunas, etc.). “Casual relaxation” includes everyday activities that require neither assistance nor professional 12. Relaxation Today training and may have relaxation as a side effect. These include exercise, listening to music, taking walks, chanting, group prayer, having sex, or reading my books on the paranormal (Smith, 2010). 2. Formal R-states are capitalized. References Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., & Cattell, H. E. (1993). The 16PF Questionnaire (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Costa, P. T., & McCraw, R. R. (2012). NEO Personality Inventory—Revised (NEO PI-R). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York: Delta. Lehrer, P. M., Woolfolk, R. L., & Sime, W. E. (Eds). (2007). Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Schwartz, M. S. (2003). The use of audiotapes for patient education and relaxation. In M. S. Schwartz & F. 195 Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Smith, J. C. (1999). ABC relaxation theory: An evidencebased approach. New York: Springer. Smith, J. C. (2001). Advances in ABC Relaxation: Applications and inventories. New York: Springer. Smith, J. C. (2004). Alternations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation: Three caveats. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 148–152. Smith, J. C. (2006). Relaxation, meditation and mindfulness: A guide for health professionals. New York: Springer. Smith, J. C. (2007a). The psychology of relaxation. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed., pp. 38–52). New York: Guilford Press. Smith, J. C. (2007b). The relaxation, meditation and mindfulness essential self-training manual. Charlotte, NC: Lulu Press. Smith, J. C. (2010). Pseudoscience and extraordinary claims of the paranormal: A critical thinker’s toolkit. New York: Wiley-Blackwell. Smith, J. C. (2015). Mindfulness reinvented and the M-Tracker Method. Charleston, SC: Createspace. CH hA aPT tER r 13 Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback Richard N. Gevirtz, Paul M. Lehrer, and Mark S. Schwartz In this chapter we describe the biofeedback, or applied psychophysiological, methods that have been using cardiorespiratory signals. Although this chapter is not intended as a literature review, we cite research evidence as a guide. breathing therapy with good patient education, other relaxation procedures, and cognitive- or mindfulness-based approaches. The specific need for biofeedback-assisted breathing therapy (e.g., respiration rate, diaphragmatic breathing, CO2 feedback, and volumetric feedback) also remains logical, although unsubstantiated. Psychophysiological measurements and feedback are valuable at least for therapist information, documentation, and information and for patient motivation and confirmation. RESPIRATORY FEEDBACK Using feedback from various instruments, or just instructing clients on breathing techniques, can be helpful for treating a number of disorders. Hyperventilation Syndrome Functional Chest Pain and Functional “Cardiac” Symptoms There is a surprising paucity of studies using breathing therapies alone for hyperventilation syndrome (HVS). The available reports and studies of breathing therapy alone for HVS do show clinically significant reductions of HVS symptoms (Fried & Grimaldi, 1993; Grossman, De Swart, & Defares, 1985; Timmons & Ley, 1994). However, methodological problems and equivocal results of studies detract from firm conclusions. Studies of cognitive restructuring, relaxation, feedback for respiration rate, and patient education indicate that these approaches also lead to significant improvements (Bass, 1994; Timmons & Ley, 1994). Although using breathing therapies alone for HVS remains a logical and sound approach, practitioners probably will continue to combine There are many patients with chest pain, without positive cardiological findings. Often there is no objective or probable organic cardiac pathology explaining these symptoms. One correctly assumes that psychophysiological factors play a role in these symptoms for many patients (Clouse, 1992; Hegel, Abel, Etscheidt, Cohen-Cole, & Wilmer, 1989). However, chest pain associated with symptomatic hyperventilation is not “psychogenic” in the sense of being purely cognitive in origin. There are physical reasons, albeit often psychophysiological ones, for the symptoms. Muscle tension, spasm, and fatigue in the intercostal muscles constitute one such mechanism (Bass, Gardner, & Jackson, 1994). Many physicians refer to this musculoskeletal explanation as “chest wall pain.” 196 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback Some physicians suggest that a distended stomach caused by aerophagia (air swallowing) places excess pressure on the diaphragm and can cause chest pain (Bass et al., 1994). Diaphragmatic spasms may create chest symptoms. For some, dysfunctional esophageal involvement contributes to these symptoms (Clouse, 1992). Here, too, psychophysiological factors often play a role, as noted by Drossman et al. (1990). An international panel of clinical investigators provided a preliminary consensus report of functional gastrointestinal disorders. Included among these disorders was “functional chest pain of presumed oesophageal origin” (Drossman et al., 1990, p. 163; i.e., “midline chest pain with or without dysphagia for at least three months; and no evidence for oesophagitis, cardiac or other disease to explain symptoms” [p. 163]). Although esophageal disorders are common in these “chest” symptoms, the authors recognized the etiological potential for psychological factors. In assessing chest pain, one also must consider reduced blood flow to the heart (Fried & Grimaldi, 1993); hyperventilation (HV) can trigger paroxysmal vasospasms in the heart (and the brain). The effects of HV on cardiac functioning are not in question. They are real and are accepted by almost all experts. In fact, HV and high arousal contribute to many occurrences of cardiac symptoms of organic origin, such as angina pectoris and infarction (Nixon, 1989; Fried & Grimaldi, 1993). However, a question of clinical significance for practitioners is whether the cardiac changes with HV indicate an organic cardiac diagnosis. They often do not; just as saying that breathing changes and thoughts affect various parts of the brain does not mean that a patient has an organic brain disorder. Another question of clinical significance is whether there are noncardiac reasons to explain functional chest pain that may appear to patients (and practitioners) as having a cardiac origin. The answer is “yes.” There are musculoskeletal, diaphragm-, and esophagus-related causes of chest pain that are often stimulated and provoked by psychophysiological factors. Many people are hypervigilant and focus on their bodily sensations more keenly than is needed, or desired. These people may catastrophize these sensations and attribute dire causes to sensations and symptoms that are not at all dangerous, or that are within the range of normal physiological sensations and events. Such attributions can result in cognitive anxiety and worry, as well as physical musculoskeletal tension and autonomic nervous system (ANS) 197 arousal. These, in turn, can lead to, or accompany, the physical changes that produce the chest symptoms. Treatments of potential value for functional chest pain include psychopharmacological therapy, such as low-dose antidepressants (Clouse, 1992); cognitive therapy (Salkovskis, 1992); behavioral therapies similar to those for chronic pain (Bradley, Richter, Scarinci, Haile, & Schan, 1992); and breathing therapies (DeGuire, Gevirtz, Kawahara, & Maguire, 1992; DeGuire, Gevirtz, Hawkinson, & Dixon, 1996; see also the review by Garssen, De Ruiter, and van Dyck, 1992). To date, there are no studies using the combination of these therapies, and no studies comparing these therapies. Here we focus on the study by DeGuire et al. (1992), in which breathing therapies and patient education led to significantly reduced symptoms among patients with functional cardiac symptoms who showed signs of HV. Patients received one of three breathing therapies: (1) without physiological feedback, (2) with visual biofeedback from thoracic and abdominal strain gauges, or (3) with ETCO2 capnometer feedback. A control group receiving no therapy was also studied. Therapy that was common to all patients in the three treated groups included the following: 1. Verbal patient education about respiratory physiology and the hypothesized effect of HV on functional cardiac symptoms. 2. Instructions for diaphragmatic breathing, and the therapist’s demonstration of diaphragmatic breathing. 3. Office practice and correction of errors. 4. Encouragement to persist and reassurance about the expected decrease in patients’ often reported discomfort caused by slow diaphragmatic breathing. 5. Encouragement to avoid increasing tidal volume, or amount of air inhaled, to compensate for changes in rate of respiration. 6. Encouragement of a slow-paced rate of respiration (less than 14 beats per minute). 7. Encouragement to practice this slow pace during conversations and while visualizing situations in which patients were having problems maintaining the new breathing. In the two conditions that included feedback, physiological monitoring and feedback typically occurred in later sessions. Several criteria for improvement included the number of days with symptoms and the fre- 198 quency of cardiac symptoms. Patients completed self-reported ratings of symptom frequency and severity. There were six treatment sessions over 3 weeks. The study partly based improvement on symptom changes between a 2-week baseline and the 2 weeks after treatment. The three breathing therapy groups reduced the days on which symptoms occurred from 8–to 10 of 14 days down to 4–5 of 14 days. In contrast, the control group showed no drop from 10 of 14 days with symptoms. The treated groups reduced their frequency of cardiac symptoms from an average of 21 symptoms (range 15–25) down to about 9 (range 4–15). All three groups showed reduced symptoms compared to the control group, which did not change. After treatment, the three treatment groups decreased reported symptoms from 23 to four (without physiological feedback), from 15 to nine (with visual biofeedback from thoracic and abdominal strain gauges), and from 25 to 15 (with ETCO2 capnometer feedback), compared to the control group, which only went from 27 to 26 symptoms. The authors note that they did not measure duration of symptoms, and that this omission resulted in underestimation of the effect. For example, some patients recorded one episode of chest pain all day; others reported only episodic mild pain. Those particular patients thus appeared to show increased symptoms rather than improvement, which would be a more accurate interpretation. ETCO2 increased significantly and respiratory rate dropped (from 16.5 to 8.5 beats per minute) for the three active treatment groups compared to the control group, which showed no change from 15 beats per minute. The breathing therapies led to ETCO2 increases. They started with 34–38 torr and increased to 39 to 41 torr, showing a more normal level of CO2. Reduction of cardiac symptoms and increased ETCO2 occurred to a greater extent among those with reduced breathing rate. The reduced respiration rate correlated with reduced frequency of symptoms (r = .53, p < .001), number of days with symptoms (r = .59, p < .001), and increased ETCO2 (r = .38, p = .018). DeGuire et al. (1992) noted that with “the use of end-tidal CO2, abdominal and thoracic strain gauge monitors did not add significantly to either reducing the frequency of cardiac symptoms or facilitating changes in physiology” (p. 676). However, there was a trend for patients using strain gauge monitors with computer-based visual biofeedback to produce the largest effect. The authors caution that the extent to which physi- IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS ological feedback information added to the new attributions about symptoms is unclear. In a follow-up study, 40% of the patients (representative of the entire sample) were contacted 3 years later. Reported symptom reductions were maintained, or improved. Similarly, ETCO2 and respiration rate measures remained in the normal range (DeGuire et al., 1996). Practitioners should consider including breathing therapies for patients diagnosed with functional chest pain and functional cardiac symptoms. However, functional chest symptoms and HVS are not synonymous (Bass, 1994). These authors also remind us that HV only provokes chest pain in less than half of the patients assessed. Even so, using breathing therapy for these patients is logical, considering the successful use of this therapy for many patients with panic symptoms and the frequency of panic symptoms among these patients (see below). We can also note the potential benefit of relaxation therapies with breathing therapy during cardiac rehabilitation for patients after myocardial infarctions (van Dixhoorn, Duivenvoorden, Staal, Pool, & Verhage, 1987; van Dixhoorn, Duivenvoorden, Staal, & Pool, 1989; Duivenvoorden & van Dixhoorn, 1991). These authors report fewer second coronary events, rehospitalizations, unstable angina episodes, and other serious cardiac events. van Dixhoorn (2007) presented variations of breathing therapy that emphasize attentional states and total body involvement. van Dixhoorn and Duivenvoorden (1999) randomly assigned 156 patients with myocardial infarction to a standard physical training or to the same training with an additional component of breathing awareness and retraining therapy (six sessions). The breathing group failed physical testing less often, returned to work more often, lowered respiration rates, increased cardiac variability, and had fewer cardiac events at a 2-year follow-up. Seventeen of 76 patients had a significant reinfarction (five patients died) in the breathing group, while 29 of 80 patients in the exercise-only group experienced similar poor outcomes (seven findings are consistent with other trials around the world (Blumenthal et al., 1997; Patel et al., 1985). Panic Breathing therapy constitutes a basic part of current treatment for many patients with panic symptoms and panic disorder. The debate continues as to whether HV causes panic or merely accompa- 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback nies it, in some patients with panic symptoms. Ley (1993) is a prolific, tenacious, and persuasive advocate of the role of HV in panic and the necessity of breathing therapy for these patients (see Ley, 1991, 1992). Contrary views by Garssen et al. (1992) and Clark and his colleagues (Salkovskis & Clark, 1990) provide balance and perspective. Ley’s (1993) proposal helps resolve some differences in opinion. Some researchers report more benefit from respiratory control with progressive exposure to situations in which symptoms occur, compared to only exposure, without breathing therapy. However, breathing therapy often leads to mixed results, whether or not one includes cognitive components. This contrary view is that “recent studies do not support the idea that HV is an important causal mechanism in producing panic attacks” (Garssen et al., 1992, p. 149). Rather, according to this view, HV accompanies panic in some patients with panic. Garssen et al. concluded that methodology problems make it impossible to derive interpretations and final conclusions on the specific role of breathing therapy. Examples of these problems include combining therapies, very small samples, and lack of controls. The specificity and mechanism of breathing therapy are unclear and elusive. The majority of the studies point to a therapeutic effect of breathing retraining and cognitive reattribution of physical symptoms to HV for patients with HVS and the closely related panic disorder, with or without agoraphobia. This conclusion seems warranted in that both help alleviate anxiety in patients with HVS or related disorders (Garssen et al., 1992). However, Ley (1992, 1993) provides a studious resolution to the disagreement about the relationship of HV and panic. He has proposed three types of panic attacks (PAs; Ley, 1992). He calls the classic PA “Type I” or “classic PA.” He distinguishes this from “anticipatory PA (Type II)” and “cognitive PA (Type III).” The classic Type I PA has distinctive and objective physiological features, especially compared to cognitive PA, Type III. These features include (1) sharp drops in pCO2 (> 10 mm Hg), (2) sharp increases in respiration rate and/or tidal volume, (3) sharp increases in heart rate (> 10 beats per minute), (4) sharp increases in electrodermal activity, and (5) low finger temperature (< 80°F). Two more studies support Ley’s position. Biber and Alkin (1999) divided 51 patients diagnosed with panic disorder into two groups: those with 199 predominantly respiratory symptoms and others. The respiratory group had more sensitivity to inhaled CO2, scored higher on panic and anxiety scales, and had longer duration of illness. Moynihan and Gevirtz (2001) divided patients with panic disorder by symptoms as described earlier and compared the respiratory group with the cognitive group on a number of respiratory parameters during various conditions. As expected, the respiratory group had lower ETCO2 (especially during a stressor) and more rapid respiratory rates, both of which are risk factors for panic. Ley (1992, 1993) suggests that practitioners should expect the most benefit from breathing therapy for patients with Type I symptoms and expect the most benefit from cognitive therapies for Type III. One should read at least these two references to appreciate his position. More recently, a group of researchers that originated at Stanford University produced studies that support the importance of dysregulated breathing in panic and the use of breathing therapies in treatment (Roth, Wilhelm, & Trabert, 1998; Meuret, Wilhelm, Ritz, & Roth, 2003, 2008; Meuret, Ritz, Wilhelm, & Roth, 2005; Meuret, Rosenfield, Hofmann, Suvak, & Roth, 2009; Meuret, Rosenfield, Seidel, Bhaskara, & Hofmann, 2010). Meuret et al. (2009) completed a study comparing five sessions of biofeedback in a 1-month period, using an ambulatory capnometer, to a delayed treatment wait-list group. Twenty participants were assigned to the biofeedback treatment and 17 to the delayed treatment. The treatment had five major components: (a) educating patients about the role of breathing in the etiology and maintenance of PD [panic disorder], (b) directing their attention to potentially problematic respiratory patterns, particularly those observed during the extended physiological monitoring, (c) having them perform different breathing maneuvers with capnometer feedback to experience how changes in breathing affect physiology, symptoms, and mood, (d) teaching them ways to simultaneously control pCO2 level and RR [respiration rate], (e) and having them practice breathing exercises daily. The weekly sessions were aimed at reviewing changes in pCO2 and RR along with changes in symptoms and emotions. Individual training exercises, to be performed twice-daily for 17-min, at home or elsewhere, consisted of three parts: (a) a baseline period (baseline), during which patients sat quietly with their eyes closed for 2-min, (b) a 10-min paced breathing period (paced) during which patients breathed in synchrony with tones while occasionally checking their pCO2 and RR on a feedback device, and (c) a 5-min 200 breathing period without pacing tones during which patients were to maintain their previously paced RR and pCO2 level using the feedback device (transfer). The paced breathing was used as a guiding tool to gradually shape slower breathing across treatment weeks. Patients were instructed to gradually adjust their breathing patterns (RR, rhythm, and depth) to reach or maintain pCO2 in a normocapnic range (pCO2 > 35 mmHg; (Oakes, 1996). In the first two weeks the emphasis was on stabilization of breathing patterns (RR and rhythm), while in the last two weeks the emphasis shifted to normalizing pCO2. For the minority of patients (n = 11) with normocapnic pCO2 levels, treatment focused on regularity of breathing (regular rate, avoiding of sigh breaths). (Meuret et al., 2008, p. 4, emphasis in original) Compliance and attendance were excellent (91% of homework completed), with only a few dropouts at the follow-ups. Results suggested that treated participants could raise pCO2 levels (32–37 mmHg) and dramatically reduce panic severity scores (PDSS, 2–4 on a 4-point scale), depression, and anxiety sensitivity. Effect sizes ranged up to d = 2.2, a large effect compared to cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) effects (Hofmann & Smits, 2008). In a subsequent study, Meuret, Hofmann, and Rosenfield (2010) and Meuret and Ritz (2010) indicated that respiratory factors mediate symptom improvement as well as (if not better than) cognitive changes in therapy. Taken as a whole, this literature justifies the use of biofeedback in panic treatment. In subsequent studies, Meuret and colleagues have replicated the efficacy of the previous results (Meuret et al., 2008) and showed that decrease in panic symptoms were mediated by respiratory changes to a greater degree than changes in dysfunctional cognitions (Meuret, Rosenfield, et al., 2010). Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback The Importance of ANS Parameters in Health, Illness, and Performance Note: Excellent papers describing the phenomenon of HRV are available (see for example: Bernardi et al., 1994; Berntson et al., 1997; Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, 1993; Berntson, Cacioppo, Quigley, & Fabro, 1994; Grossman & Taylor, 2007). A theoretical perspective is provided by Stephen Porges (2011). IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS Heart rate variability (HRV) feedback has received a lot of attention in recent years because of the emerging realization that many disorders found in Western health care settings may be mediated by autonomic processes. Table 13.1 presents some of these disorders and lists the probable or suspected autonomic mediator. Since psychological processes (stress, anger, depression, worry, rumination, etc.) are known to affect autonomic processes, psychological etiology is often implied for many of these disorders (usually ignoring the psychophysiological pathways; Gevirtz, 2007). Table 13.1 summarizes some of these relationships. As can be seen, a number of prominent disorders that are treated by biofeedback practitioners have autonomic mediators. For this reason, HRV analysis can be useful in assessing the psychophysiological profile of the patients. Low HRV can be seen in both “time domain” and “frequency domain” measures, based on the time intervals between R-spikes in the electrocardiogram. Each of these measures is calculated exclusively from “normal” cardiac interbeat intervals (those produced by sinus rhythms, under neural control, but excluding particularly long or short intervals caused by valve malfunctions, abnormal cardiac arrhythmias, etc.). Time domain measures include standard deviation of normal to normal R-wave duration (SDNN) or, for 5-minute intervals, the standard deviation of the average NN intervals (SDANN), the percent of successive normal interbeat intervals differing by 50 milliseconds or more (pNN50), or the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD). Frequency domain measures are calculated from spectral analysis of the electrocardiogram signal and include high-frequency (HF) variability, 0.15– 0.4 Hz, reflecting respiratory influences on heart rate and vagal (parasympathetic) control, low-frequency (LF) activity, 0.05–0.15 Hz, reflecting control of blood pressure by the baroreflex and probably influenced by both the parasympathetic and sympathetic systems, and the LF/HF ratio, which is often interpreted as reflecting autonomic balance. Note that various measures of HRV reflect two separate processes in the body, and, although the effects of these may overlap, they probably are not one and the same. One process is autonomic balance. Thus, abnormal processes associated with either sympathetic or parasympathetic hyperarousal or hyperreactivity tend to be reflected in increases or decreases in various HRV parameters. The other is the activity of various regulatory reflexes. Two of these are respiratory sinus 201 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback TABLE 13.1. Explaining “Unexplained” Medical Symptoms Symptom cluster Typical diagnoses Potential mediators Level of evidence Local muscle pain Lower back pain (LBP), cervical strain, tension headache, repetitive strain injury (RSI), etc. Sympathetically modulated trigger points Herbs, Gevirtz, & Jacobs (1994) Abdominal pain, diarrhea, constipation, bloating Irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), recurrent abdominal pain (RAP) Autonomic imbalance with excessive sympathetic tone and prolonged vagal withdrawal Sowder, Gevirtz, Shapiro, & Ebert (2010); Mayer et al. (2001) Posttrauma: arousal, reexperiencing dissociation Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Cortical overload, vegetative vagus, limbic reshaping van der Kolk (2001, 2006); Lanius et al. (2002, 2006); Zucker et al. (2009) Constant worry, inability to relax Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) Low vagal tone, weak inhibitory circuits Thayer, Friedman, & Borkovec (1996) Nonrestorative sleep, allodynia, fatigue Fibromyalgia (FM), chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) Dopamine depletion in hippocampal limbic circuits, substance P in dorsal horn Wood et al. (2007); Russell (2000) Labile blood pressure readings Labile or white-coat hypertension Weak baroreflex Joseph et al. (2005); Schein et al. (2001); Parati & Steptoe (2004) Asthma symptoms Asthma Pulmonary smooth muscle hyperreactivity, parasympathetic overreactivity, airway inflammation Lehrer et al. (2000a, 2004) arrhythmia and the heart rate component in the baroreflex system. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) refers to the rhythms in heart rate that correspond with breathing, such that increases in heart rate correspond with inhalation and decreases with exhalation. RSA reflects processes controlling respiratory and gas exchange control (Hayano, Yasuma, Okada, Mukai, & Fujinami, 1996; Yasuma & Hayano, 2004), and affects many other regulatory processes throughout the body (Eckberg, 2003). The baroreflex modulates blood pressure variability through changes in heart rate. When blood pressure rises, heart rate falls; when blood pressure falls, heart rate rises. The changes in blood flow associated with heart rate produce mechanical changes in blood pressure. As we will see in the following sections early research on HRV biofeedback showed that the technique alters autonomic function, can restore homeostatic autonomic balance, increases homeostatic regulation (ability to recover to normal values after a perturbation), and improves various emotional and somatic symptoms affected by the autonomic nervous system. Indirect effect may influence inflammatory and emotional processes, through vagal-inflammatory pathways and stimulation of brainstem structures involved in emotional control, and directly affecting respiratory gas exchange efficiency by controlling phase relationships between heart rate and breathing. If these data hold up over time, this type HRV feedback will prove valuable in treating a variety of disorders (Gevirtz, 2006; Mayer, Naliboff, & Chang, 2001a; Mayer, Nalibott, Chang, & Coutinho, 2001b; van der Kolk, 2001, 2006; Frewen, Pain, Dozois, & Lanius, 2006; Hopper, Frewen, van der Kolk, & Lanius, 2007; Lanius et al., 2002; Lanius, Bluhm, Lanius, & Pain, 2006; Thayer, Friedman, & Borkovec, 1996; Thayer & Siegle, 2002; Thayer & Friedman, 2002; Wood, 2006; Wood et al., 2007; Russell, 2000; Parati & Steptoe, 2004; Schein et al., 2001; Joseph et al., 2005; Lehrer et al., 2004; Hassett et al., 2007; Karavidas et al., 2007; Zucker, Samuelson, Muench, Greenberg, & Gevirtz, 2009; Siepmann, Aykac, Unterdorfer, Petrowski, & Mueck-Weymann, 2008; Siepmann et al., 2014; Grossman, van Beck, & Wientjes, 1990). 202 Resonance Frequency Stimulation as the Mechanism of HRV Biofeedback The mechanism by which HRV biofeedback stimulates very high-amplitude oscillations in heart rate is the interaction between two important control reflexes: respiratory sinus arrhythmia and the baroreflex. The particular patterns by which these reflexes are stimulated influence both respiratory gas exchange (Hayano et al., 1996; Ito et al., 2006) and the baroreflex (Lehrer et al., 2003; Vaschillo, Lehrer, Rishe, & Konstantinov, 2002), which modulates blood pressure (Fisher, Kim, Young, & Fadel,2010; Gisolf, Imminck, van Lieshout, Stok, & Karemaker, 2005; Liu et al., 2002). The baroreflex induces a specific rhythm in HRV, averaging about 5.5 times/minute (Vaschillo, Vaschillo, & Lehrer, 2006). When blood pressure rises, the baroreflex causes heart rate to slow. The subsequent reduction in heart rate then mechanically causes a decrease in blood pressure, because less blood flows through the vessels. When blood pressure decreases, the baroreflex then causes heart rate to rise again. Because of plasticity in the blood vessels and inertia in blood flow through the system, the change in blood flow (and pressure) caused by heart rate changes is delayed by about 4–5 seconds. This causes a low-frequency rhythm in heart rate, at about 5.5 times/minute, on average. Because respiration can be slowed to approximately this rate, an interaction can occur between respiratory sinus arrhythmia and the baroreflex. In each person, there is a specific oscillation frequency at which heart rate varies perfectly in phase (0° phase relationship) with breathing (Vaschillo, Vaschillo, & Lehrer, 2004), and perfectly out of phase (180o phase relationship) with blood pressure (Vaschillo et al., 2002). This frequency happens to coincide with the frequency in which the baroreflex affects heart rate. Thus, when people breathe at this frequency, the increases in heart rate accompanying inhalation compound the increases in heart rate caused by the baroreflex (induced by decreases in blood pressure at that same time), causing simultaneous effects of (1) increased baroreflex gain, (2) increased respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and 3) improved respiratory gas exchange (Yasuma & Havano, 2004). The baroreflex is an important mechanism by which the body modulates blood pressure changes. Actually, the rhythms produced by the baroreflex create resonance characteristics in the cardiovascular system, such that stimulation (by breathing) at baroreflex frequency causes effects that IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS are characteristic of resonance: an endogamous rhythm in heart rate that is always present and easily stimulated, and, when this rhythm is stimulated, very high-amplitude oscillations at a the resonance frequency that recruit and obliterate other sources of variation. Analogous examples of resonance effects can be seen when a microphone is placed in front of a speaker, or when a child is pushed in a swing, in rhythm with every oscillation. An example of resonance effects in heart rate during this process is shown in Figure 13.1, in which the smaller oscillations represent resting HRV, and the high-amplitude oscillations reflect heart rate during resonance-frequency breathing. Conversely, resonance frequency breathing can be shaped and reinforced through HRV biofeedback. When people try to produce maximal increases in heart rate during inhalation and maximal decreases during exhalation, they must be breathing at the resonance frequency produced by the baroreflex system. Lehrer et al. (2003) have shown that daily practice of this “resonance frequency training” increases the total amount of HRV, increases baroreflex gain, and improves pulmonary function, even among healthy people. The overlapping of respiratory and baroreflex effects on HRV during slow respiratory rates presents some treatment opportunities. Based on experimental work by Vaschillo et al. (2002), Lehrer, Vaschillo, and Vaschillo (2000b) have theorized that practicing HRV biofeedback stimulates resonance characteristics in the cardiorespiratory system caused by the baroreflex system. They have shown that daily practice of this “resonance frequency training” increases the total amount of HRV, with almost all of the oscillations occurring at a single frequency, thus increasing baroreflex FIGURE 13.1. Example of resonance. 203 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback gain and, at the same time, improving pulmonary function and increasing cardiac vagal tone, as reflected in RSA (Lehrer et al., 2003), even among healthy people. Profound changes in these measures are found both immediately, while practicing the biofeedback technique, and, over time, in resting measures, when the individual is not practicing biofeedback. Practice of the technique improves both autonomic balance and autonomic regulation. This has been found even when autonomic function (both sympathetic and parasympathetic) has been almost completely blocked by experimental exposure to an inflammatory drug (Lehrer et al., 2010). Repeated stimulation of the baroreflex “exercises” is hypothesized to increase the efficiency of the reflex (Lehrer et al., 2003), thus leading to improved overall control of blood pressure, autonomic reactivity, and through baroreflex-associated brainstem activity (centered at the nucleus tractus solitarius) that impacts on the limbic system’s control of emotional reactivity (Gray et al., 2009). HRV biofeedback may also affect central nervous system functioning by way of the vagal afferents. The vagal afferent system has recently emerged as an area of interest due to the use of vagal nerve stimulation for (1) severe depression (George et al., 1994, 2000; Goodnick, Rush, George, Marangell, & Sackheim, 2001; Marangell et al., 2002; Mu et al., 2004; Rush et al., 2000; Sackeim et al., 2001), (2) seizures (Hsiang, Wong, Kay, & Poon, 1998; Sahin, Ilbay, Imal, Bozdogan, & Ates, 2009; Shahwan, Bailey, Maxiner, & Harvey, 2009; Sherman et al., 2008), (3) congestive heart failure (Sabbah et al., 2011; Zhang, Ilsar, Sabbah, Ben David, & Magzgalev, 2009), and (4) autoimmune disorders (Zitnik, 2011). Brown and Gerbarg (2005) have speculated that slow breathing techniques (similar to those used in HRV biofeedback) may be stimulating vagal afferents in a way that is similar to the invasive medical devices. This would open up many other applications for this technique. Application Areas Consistent with its important physiological effects, many groups around the world have been reporting that “RSA” biofeedback is a viable feedback modality. Wheat and Larkin (2010), reviewed the literature that existed until about 2009 and concluded: Results revealed that HRV biofeedback consistently effectuates acute improvements during biofeedback practice, whereas the presence of short-term and long-term carry-over effects is less clear. Some evidence suggests HRV biofeedback may result in longterm carry-over effects on baroreflex gain, which is an area most promising for future investigations. (p. 229) Practicing HRV biofeedback has produced dramatic improvements in asthma, including apparent 100% effectiveness in eliminating asthma exacerbations, while improving pulmonary function, decreasing symptomatology, and allowing decreases in consumption of asthma medication (Lehrer et al., 2004). Herbs, Gevirtz, and Jacobs (1994) have shown that HRV biofeedback reduces blood pressure in hypertensives. Anecdotal reports from Russia suggest that it may be helpful in treating a variety of psychosomatic and stress-related physical disorders (Chernigovskaya, Vaschillo, Petrash, & Rusanovsky, 1990). A similar technique involving biofeedback training in slow breathing (without assessing resonance frequency, but reducing breathing to the range where resonance effects should be expected) has been deemed by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) to be an effective adjunctive method for controlling hypertension (Schein et al., 2001). This is the only biofeedback method that has received an FDA indication for treating any disease. Tables 13.2 and 13.3 list the various clinical conditions for which there is evidence that HRV biofeedback has beneficial effects. Procedure for Performing Resonance Frequency Biofeedback Training In the HRV biofeedback technique, the client is instructed to maximize the peak–valley amplitude based on a cardiotachometer line graph. Over time, almost all participants achieve this by slowing and deepening breath, sometimes also by practicing a “mindful” mental state and using other relaxation techniques. Although the method is still too new for a codified procedure, a suggested procedural method has been outlined by Lehrer et al. (2000b, 2007). The first clinical session is usually devoted to determining the trainee’s resonance frequency. This is accomplished by having the individual breathe at various frequencies near 0.1 Hz, and finding the frequency that yields the highest amplitude of heart rate oscillations. The trainee is then advised to practice breathing at this frequency daily until the next session (approximately a week later). In subsequent sessions, bio- 204 HRVB + oxymeter feedback Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease HRVB Recurrent abdominal pain HRVB HRVB HRVB + CBT HRVB + stress management Congestive heart failure Coronary artery disease Coronary artery disease Congestive heart failure Cardiac rehabilitation HRVB HRVB Cyclic vomiting Fibromyalgia HRVB integrated into other therapies HRVB IBS Recurrent abdominal pain HRVB Recurrent abdominal pain Slow breathing + finger temperature feedback HRVB Asthma Functional gastrointestinal disorders Recurrent abdominal pain Intervention Disorder Case studies HRVB + CBT vs. stress management Vs. TAU Vs. sham EEG Vs. TAU TAU Case study Case study Vs. hypnosis Vs. control Vs. TAU Vs. TAU Vs. sham EEG Design (control) Harvested heart tissue viability HRV measures + adjustment scales HRV measures (SDNN) 6-minute walk Standard fibromyalgia scales IBS symptom measures Vomiting frequency Symptom log IBS Symptom Severity Scale, HADS Symptom ratings and HRV measures Parent and child symptom ratings 6-minute walk Symptoms, lung function, medication Measures Training group equal to LVAD HRVB + CBT > stress management HRVB > TAU HRVB > sham EEG if LVEF > 31 HRVB > TAU HRVB > TAU Greatly improved Greatly improved Both groups improved equally (HRVB slightly better) Symptom improvement associated with SDNN gains Breathing > control HRVB > TAU HRVB > control Results Moravec (2008); Moravec & McKee (2013) Nolan et al. (2005) Del Pozo, Gevirtz, Scher, & Guarneri (2004) Swanson, Gevirtz, Spira, & Guarneri (2006) Hassett et al. (2007) Ebert (2013) Slutsker, Konichezky, & Gothelf (2010) Masters (2006) Dobbin, Dobbin, Ross, Graham, & Ford (2013) Sowder, Gevirtz, Shapiro, & Ebert (2010) Humphreys & Gevirtz (2000) Giardino, Chan, & Borson (2004) Lehrer et al. (2000a, 2004) Reference TABLE 13.2. Disorders Treated with HRV Biofeedback That Are Hypothesized to Have Restoration of Autonomic Function as the Primary Mediator 205 Breathing and temperature HRVB (stress eraser) PIH Postpartum depression Vs. TAU (but random assignment) Vs. activity management vs. TAU Vs. matched case histories Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale BP levels logged daily BP, birthweight, gestation length Preterm stress, preterm delivery Measures of pain and function Measures of pain, vitality and social functioning Trigger point pain Pain and function measures BP, HRV BP, HRV, BRS Medication adjustment and BP HRVB had less anxiety, sleep disturbance at 1 month than controls Biofeedback group halted rising BP levels vs. other groups HRVB > controls for birthweight and gestation length HRVB > control for stress; 13% vs. 33% preterm delivery (n.s.) HRVB > TAU HRVB > TAU HRVB combined with physical release relieves pain Combination superior to other interventions Slow breathing = EMG feedback > slow breathing alone HRVB > either control, improved on BP, HRV, and BRS measures HRVB maintained BP with fewer medications Kudo, Shinohara, & Kodama (2014) Sommers, Gevirtz, Jasin, & Chin (1989) Cullin et al. (2013) Siepmann et al. (2014) Berry et al. (2013) Hallman, Olsson, von Scheele, Melin, & Lyskov (2003) Gevirtz (2006) Gordon & Gevirtz (2006); Vagades et al. (2011) Wang et al. (2009) Lin et al. (2012) Reinke, Gevirtz, & Mussgay (2007) HRVB, heart rate variability biofeedback; CBT, cognitive-behavioral therapy; EMG, electromyogram; EEG, electroencephalogram; TAU, treatment as usual; IBS, irritable bowel syndrome; HADS, Hamilton Anxiety and Depression Scale; SDNN, standard deviation of normal-to-normal heartbeats; BP, blood pressure; BRS, baroreflex sensitivity; LVEF, left ventricular ejection fraction; LVAD, left ventricular assist device; 6-minute walk, maximum distance walked in 6 minutes; PIH, pregnancy-induced hypertension. From Gevirtz (2013). Copyright by the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Reprinted by permission. HRVB (stress eraser) PIH Vs. TAU HRVB Vs. control sessions Vs. TAU HRVB HRVB Case studies HRVB Preterm labor OB/Gyn Four groups: stabilization exercises, HRVB alone, myofascial release alone, or combination Vs. slow breathing alone Vs. slow breathing and control, 3-month follow-up Vs. sham EEG HRVB and myofascial release Slow abdominal breathing + EMG biofeedback Prehypertensives Chronic muscle pain HRVB HRVB Pre-hypertensives Hypertension 206 Depressed patients vs. healthy controls Vs. TAU after cardiac surgery Vs. relaxation HRVB HRVB HRVB HRVB Vs. delayed treatment Anxiety HRVB Anxiety and mood Vs. matched controls Case example Case study HRVB + TAU HRVB Vs. relaxation Vs. control Vs. TAU HRVB + DBT Phobia PTSD HRVB Anxiety disorders PTSD HRVB Vs. Zoloft Vs. DBT + relaxation HRVB with stress eraser + DBT HRVB + DBT + Zoloft No control, singlegroup trial HRVB Depression Design (control) Intervention Disorder HRVB > control Somatic symptoms Approach phobic object PCL PCL Information processing CAPS, Trauma Symptom Checklist BDI and HDS BDI and HDS CES-D BDI BDI and HDS BDI and HDS Measures Henriques, Keffer, Abrahamson, & Horst (2011) HRVB using heartmath > control Improved phobic avoidance PCL improved 21% HRVB = relaxation HRVB > information processing HRVB > TAU HRVB > Zoloft alone HRVB > relaxation HRVB > TAU Depressed patients reduced on BDI, no changes in controls HRVB group superior Depression reduced markedly Results Nada (2009) Prigatano (1973) Reyes (2014) Zucker, Samuelson, Muench, Greenberg, & Gevirtz (2009) Ginsberg, Berry, & Powell (2010) Tan, Dao, Farmer, Sutherland, & Gevirtz (2011) Rene et al. (2011) Rene, Gevirtz, Muench, & Birkhead (2011) Patron et al. (2013) Siepmann, Aykac, Unterdorfer, Petrowski, & Mueck-Weymann (2008) Zucker, Samuelson, Muench, Greenberg, & Gevirtz (2009) Karavidas et al. (2007) References TABLE 13.3. Disorders Treated with HRV Biofeedback That Are Hypothesized to Involve Central Nervous System Mediators 207 HRVB HRVB HRVB HRVB HRVB HRVB (Em wave) Sleep Performance Baseball Golf Dance Dance Music Vs. control Vs. neurofeedback vs. control Vs. neurofeedback vs. control Case study Vs. sports psychology control Vs. autogenics (AT), vs. control Vs. control Performance anxiety measures Refereed dance ratings Refereed dance ratings Golf performance Hitting performance HF amplitude during sleep Sleep disturbance scale + actigraphy Sleep log Anxiety measures Cholesterol, glucose, heart rate, BP, positive outlook, and overall psychological distress HRVB > control No effect on dance, HRVB reduced anxiety HRVB and neurofeedback > control Reduced anxiety, improved performance HRVB > controls HRVB > AT > control HRVB > controls Insomnia improvement, long-term maintenance Improvement HRVB > control on all measues, projected cost savings Thurber (2006) Gruzelier, Thompson, Brandt, & Steffert (in press) Raymond, Sajid, Parkinson, & Gruzelier (2005) Lagos, Vaschillo, Vaschillo, Lehrer, & Bates (2008) Strack & Gevirtz (2011) Sakakibara, Hayano, Oikawa, Katsamanis, & Lehrer (2013) Ebben, Kurbatov, & Pollak (2009) McLay & Spira (2009) Reiner (2008) McCraty, Atkinson, Lipsenthal, & Arguelles (2009) DBT, dialectical behavior therapy; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; AT, autogenic training; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; HDS, Hamilton Depression Scale; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; PCL, PTSD Checklist. Other abbreviations as in Table 13.2. From Gevirtz (2013). Copyright by the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Reprinted by permission. HRVB Case report Single-group study HRVB + therapy HRVB (stress eraser) Vs. control HRVB + stress management Sleep lab insomnia Sleep Sleep Stress 208 feedback is provided in the form of a cardiotachometer display or an online Fourier analysis of heart rate, updated every few seconds. The trainee is instructed to maximize the amplitude of heart rate variability at his or her resonance frequency, and to fine-tune the estimation of his or her resonance frequency by observing the respiration rate that continues to yield the highest amplitude of heart rate variability. To enhance the effect, the individual also is sometimes instructed to breathe abdominally and to exhale through pursed lips. The trainee is also advised to breathe shallowly in order to avoid hyperventilating during practice of this method. Although most people are able to produce high amplitudes of HRV within just a few minutes of training, it usually takes several sessions to learn to stabilize HRV at the resonance frequency. The number of sessions required to maximize effects is not yet known. Data from our laboratories suggest the curve representing physiological effects has not yet leveled off after 10 sessions of training. Conclusion HRV biofeedback has become a popular modality in the professional community and has been widely disseminated to the public. Thanks to products such as the emWave (heartmath.com), the Stress Eraser (stresseraser.com), Journey to the Wild Divine (wilddivine.com), MyCalmBeat (mybrainsolutions.com), Resp-e-Rate (resperate.com), and others, a worldwide audience is now using versions of this biofeedback technique. It remains to be determined how the actual biofeedback can be combined with other therapies or meditative disciplines. Additionally, the actual mechanisms of change need to be identified (Giardino, Lehrer, & Feldman, 2000). Lehrer and Gevirtz (2014) have recently reviewed possible mechanisms. Glossary Oscillators. Devices or physiological systems that pro- duces to and fro rhythmic activity. R-wave. Electrocardiographic wave represented by the peak during electrical stimulation of the ventricles. It is used in biofeedback settings to trigger a timer to measure “interbeat interval” (the time between R-wave peaks). Spectral analysis. A mathematical and graphical tech- nique used to decompose complex wave forms into their constituent components (frequency bins). The IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS mathematical formulae to do this were first worked out by Fourier; therefore, the analysis is often called a fast Fourier transform. References Bass, C. (1994). Management of patients with hyperventilation-related disorders. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Bass, C., Gardner, W. N., & Jackson, G. (1994). Psychiatric and respiratory aspects of functional cardiovascular syndromes. In B. H. Timmons & R. Ley (Eds.), Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders (pp. 125–138). New York: Springer. Bernardi, L., Leuzzi, S., Radaelli, A., Passino, C., Johnston, J. A., & Sleight, P. (1994). Low-frequency spontaneous fluctuations of R-R interval and blood pressure in conscious humans: A baroreceptor or central phenomenon? Clinical Science, 87(6), 649–654. Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Jr., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretative caveats. Psychophysiology, 34, 623–648. Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Cardiac psychophysiology and autonomic space in humans: Empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 296–322. Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., Quigley, K. S., & Fabro, V. T. (1994). Autonomic space and psychophysiological response. Psychophysiology, 31(1), 44–61. Berry, M. E., Chapple, I. T., Ginsberg, J. P., Gleichauf, K. J., Meyer, J. A., & Nagpal, M. L. (2013) Non-pharmacological intervention for chronic pain in veterans: A pilot study of heart rate variability biofeedback. Global Advice Health Medicine, 3(2), 28–33. Biber, B., & Alkin, T. (1999). Panic disorder subtypes: Differential responses to CO2 challenge. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(5), 739–744. Blumenthal, J. A., Jiang, W., Babyak, M. A., Krantz, D. S., Frid, D. J., Coleman, R. E., et al. (1997). Stress management and exercise training in cardiac patients with myocardial ischemia: Effects on prognosis and evaluation of mechanisms. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157(19), 2213–2223. Bradley, L. A., Richter, J. E., Scarinci, I. C., Haile, J. M., & Schan, C. A. (1992). Psychosocial and psychophysical assessments of patients with unexplained chest pain. American Journal of Medicine, 92(5A), 65S–73S. Brown, R. P., & Gerbarg, P. L. (2005). Sudarshan Kriya yogic breathing in the treatment of stress, anxiety, and depression: Part II. Clinical applications and guidelines. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 11(4), 711–717. Chernigovskaya, N. V., Vaschillo, E. G., Petrash, V. V., & Rusanovsky, V. V. (1990). Voluntary regulation of the heart rate as a method of functional condition correction in neurotics. Human Physiology, 16, 58–64. 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback Clouse, R. E. (1992). Psychopharmacologic approaches to therapy for chest pain of presumed esophageal origin. American Journal of Medicine, 92(Suppl 5A), 106–113. Cullins, S. W., Gevirtz, R. N., Poeltler, D. M., Cousins, L. M., Edward Harpin, R., & Muench, F. (2013). An exploratory analysis of the utility of adding cardiorespiratory biofeedback in the standard care of pregnancyinduced hypertension. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(3), 161–170. DeGuire, S., Gevirtz, R., Hawkinson, D., & Dixon, K. (1996). Breathing retraining: A three-year follow-up study of treatment for hyperventilation syndrome and associated functional cardiac symptoms. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21(2), 191–198. DeGuire, S., Gevirtz, R., Kawahara, Y., & Maguire, W. (1992). Hyperventilation syndrome and the assessment of treatment for functional cardiac symptoms. American Journal of Cardiology, 70(6), 673–677. Del Pozo, J. M., Gevirtz, R. N., Scher, B., & Guarneri, E. (2004). Biofeedback treatment increases heart rate variability in patients with known coronary artery disease. American Heart Journal, 147(3), E11. Dobbin, A., Dobbin, J., Ross, S. C., Graham, C., & Ford, M. J. (2013). Randomised controlled trial of brief intervention with biofeedback and hypnotherapy in patients with refractory irritable bowel syndrome. Journal of the Royal Colleege of Physicians of Edinburgh, 43(1), 15–23. Duivenvoorden, H. J., & van dixhoorn, J. (1991). Predictability of psychic outcome for exercise training and exercise training including relaation therapy after myocardial infarction. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 35, 569–578. Ebben, M. R., Kurbatov, V., & Pollak, C. P. (2009). Moderating laboratory adaptation with the use of a heart-rate variability biofeedback device (StressEraser). Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(4), 245–249. Ebert, C. (2013). The use of HRVB for the treatment of FGID in children and adolescents. Paper presented at the AAPB, Portland, OR. Eckberg, D. L. (2003). The human respiratory gate. Journal of Physiology, 548(Pt. 2), 339–352. Fisher, J. P., Kim, A., Young, C. N., & Fadel, P. J. (2010). Carotid baroreflex control of arterial blood pressure at rest and during dynamic exercise in aging humans. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 299(5), R1241–R1247. Frewen, P. A., Pain, C., Dozois, D. J., & Lanius, R. A. (2006). Alexithymia in PTSD: Psychometric and fMRI studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 397–400. Fried, R., & Grimaldi, J. (1993). The psychology and physiology of breathing. New York: Springer. Garssen, B., De Ruiter, C., & van Dyck, R. (1992). Breathing retraining: A rationale placebo. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 141–153. George, M. S., Sackeim, H. A., Marangell, L. B., Husain, M. M., Nahas, Z., Lisanby, S. H., et al. (2000). Vagus nerve stimulation: A potential therapy for resistant 209 depression? Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(4), 757–783. George, R., Salinsky, M., Kuzniecky, R., Rosenfeld, W., Bergen, D., Tarver, W. B., et al. (1994). Vagus nerve stimulation for treatment of partial seizures: 3. Long-term follow-up on first 67 patients exiting a controlled study: First International Vagus Nerve Stimulation Study Group. Epilepsia, 35(3), 637–643. Gevirtz, R. (2006). The muscle spindle trigger point model of chronic pain. Biofeedback, 34(2), 53–57. Gevirtz, R. N. (2006). The muscle spindle trigger point model of chronic pain. Biofeedback, 34(2), 53–57. Gevirtz, R. N. (2007). Psychophysiological perspectives on stress related and anxiety disorders. In P. Lehrer & W. Sime (Eds.), Stress management: Principles and practice (2nd ed., pp. 209–226). New York: Guilford Press. Giardino, N. D., Chan, L., & Borson, S. (2004). Combined heart rate variability and pulse oximetry biofeedback for chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: Preliminary findings. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 29(2), 121–133. Giardino, N. D., Lehrer, P., & Feldman, J. M. (2000). The role of oscillations in self-regulation: A revision of the classical model of homeostasis. In J. G. Carlson, D. Kenny, F. J. McGuigan, & J. L. Sheppard (Eds.), Stress and health: Research and clinical applications (pp. 27–52). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harwood. Ginsberg, J. P., Berry, M. E., & Powell, D. A. (2010). Cardiac coherence and posttraumatic stress disorder in combat veterans. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 16(4), 52–60. Gisolf, J., Immink, R. V., van Lieshout, J. J., Stok, W. J., & Karemaker, J. M. (2005). Orthostatic blood pressure control before and after spaceflight, determined by timedomain baroreflex method. Journal of Applied Physiology, 98(5), 1682–1690. Goodnick, P. J., Rush, A. J., George, M. S., Marangell, L. B., & Sackeim, H. A. (2001). Vagus nerve stimulation in depression. Expert Opinion in Pharmacotherapy, 2(7), 1061–1063. Gordon, C., & Gevirtz, R. (2006). Trigger point pain theory and practice: Recent developments [Abstract]. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 29(4), 299. Gray, M. A., Rylander, K., Harrison, N. A., Wallin, B., Gunnar, B., & Critchley, H. D. (2009). Following one’s heart: Cardiac rhythms gate central initiation of sympathetic reflexes. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(6), 1817–1825. Grossman, P., de Swart, J. C., & Defares, P. B. (1985). A controlled study of a breathing therapy for treatment of hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29(1), 49–58. Grossman, P., & Taylor, E. W. (2007). Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 263–285. Grossman, P., van Beek, J., & Wientjes, C. (1990). A comparison of three quantification methods for estimation of respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Psychophysiology, 27(6), 702–714. 210 Gruzelier, J., Thompson, T., Brandt, R., & Steffert, T. (in press). Application of alpha/theta neurofeedback and heart rate variabilty training to contemporary dancers: State anxiety and creativity. International Journal of Psychophysiology. Hallman, D. M., Olsson, E. M., von Scheele, B., Melin, L., & Lyskov, E. (2003). Effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in subjects with stress-related chronic neck pain: A pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36(2), 71–80. Hassett, A. L., Radvanski, D. C., Vaschillo, E. G., Vaschillo, B., Sigal, L. H., Karavidas, M. K., et al. (2007). A pilot study of the efficacy of heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback in patients with fibromyalgia. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 32(1), 1–10. Hayano, J., Yasuma, F., Okada, A., Mukai, S., & Fujinami, T. (1996). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A phenomenon improving pulmonary gas exchange and circulatory efficiency. Circulation, 94(4), 842–847. Hegel, M. T., Abel, G. G., Etscheidt, M., Cohen-Cole, S., & Wilmer, C. I. (1989). Behavioral treatment of anginalike chest pain in patients with hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 20(1), 31–39. Henriques, G., Keffer, S., Abrahamson, C., & Horst, S. J. (2011). Exploring the effectiveness of a computer-based heart rate variability biofeedback program in reducing anxiety in college students. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36(2), 101–112. Herbs, D., Gevirtz, R. N., & Jacobs, D. (1994). The effect of heart rate pattern biofeedback for the treatment of essential hypertension [Abstract]. Biofeedback and SelfRegulation, 19(3), 281. Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(4), 621–632. Hopper, J. W., Frewen, P. A., van der Kolk, B. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2007). Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: Symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(5), 713– 725. Hsiang, J. N., Wong, L. K., Kay, R., & Poon, W. S. (1998). Vagus nerve stimulation for seizure control: Local ­experience. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 5(3), 294– 297. Humphreys, P. A., & Gevirtz, R. N. (2000). Treatment of recurrent abdominal pain: Components analysis of four treatment protocols. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 31(1), 47–51. Ito, S., Sasano, H., Sasano, N., Hayano, J., Fisher, J. A., & Katsuya, H. (2006). Vagal nerve activity contributes to improve the efficiency of pulmonary gas exchange in hypoxic humans. Experimental Physiology, 91(5), 935– 941. Joseph, C. N., Porta, C., Casucci, G., Casiraghi, N., Maffeis, M., Rossi, M., et al. (2005). Slow breathing improves arterial baroreflex sensitivity and decreases blood pres- IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS sure in essential hypertension. Hypertension, 46(4), 714–718. Karavidas, M. K., Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Marin, H., Buyske, S., et al. (2007). Preliminary results of an open label study of heart rate variability biofeedback for the treatment of major depression. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 32(1), 19–30. Kudo, N., Shinohara, H., & Kodama, H. (2014). Heart rate variability biofeedback intervention for reduction of psychological stress during the early postpartum period. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 39(3–4), 203– 211. Lagos, L., Vaschillo, B., Vaschillo, E., Lehrer, P., & Bates, M. (2008). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a strategy for dealing with competitive anxiety: A case study. Biofeedback.36(3), 109–115. Lanius, R. A., Bluhm, R., Lanius, U., & Pain, C. (2006). A review of neuroimaging studies in PTSD: Heterogeneity of response to symptom provocation. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40(8), 709–729. Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore, M., Gupta, M., Neufeld, R. W., et al. (2002). Brain activation during script-driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biological Psychiatry, 52(4), 305– 311. Lehrer, P. M. (2007). Biofeedback training to increase heart rate variability. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practices of stress management (pp. 227–248). New York: Guilford Press. Lehrer, P. M., & Gevirtz, R. N. (2014, July). Heart rate variability biofeedback: How and why does it work? Frontiers of Psychology. Lehrer, P. M., Karavidas, M. K., Lu, S. E., Coyle, S. M., Oikawa, L. O., Macor, M., et al. (2010). Voluntarily produced increases in heart rate variability modulate autonomic effects of endotoxin induced systemic inflammation: An exploratory study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 35(4), 303–315. Lehrer, P. M., Smetankin, A., & Potapova, T. (2000a). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback therapy for asthma: A report of 20 unmedicated pediatric cases using the Smetankin method. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 25, 193–200. Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., & Vaschillo, B. (2000b). Resonant frequency biofeedback training to increase cardiac variability: Rationale and manual for training. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 25(3), 177–191. Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Edelberg, R., Lu, S. E., Eckberg, D., et al. (2003). Heart rate variability biofeedback increases baroreflex gain and peak expiratory flow. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(5), 796–805. Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Lu, S. E., Scardella, A., Siddique, M., et al. (2004). Biofeedback treatment for asthma. Chest, 126(2), 352–361. Ley, R. (1991). The efficacy of breathing retraining and the centrality of hyperventilation in panic disorder: A reinterpretation of experimental findings [comment] [see comments]. Behavior Research Therapy, 29(3), 301–304. 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback Ley, R. (1992). The many faces of Pan: Psychological and physiological differences among three types of panic attacks. Behavior Research Therapy, 30(4), 347–357. Ley, R. (1993). Breathing retraining in the treatment of hyperventilatory complaints and panic disorder: A reply to Garssen, de Ruiter, & van Dyck. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(5), 393–408. Lin, G., Xiang, Q., Fu, X., Wang, S., Chen, S., Shao, L., et al. (2012). Heart rate variability biofeedback decreases blood pressure in prehypertensive subjects by improving autonomic function and baroreflex. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 18(2), 143–152. Liu, H.-K., Guild, S.-J., Ringwood, J. V., Barrett, C. J., Leonard, B. L., Nguang, S.-K., et al. (2002). Dynamic baroreflex control of blood pressure: Influence of the heart vs. peripheral resistance. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 283(2), R533–R542. Marangell, L. B., Rush, A. J., George, M. S., Sackeim, H. A., Johnson, C. R., Husain, M. M., et al. (2002). Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for major depressive episodes: One year outcomes. Biological Psychiatry, 51(4), 280–287. Masters, K. S. (2006). Recurrent abdominal pain, medical intervention, and biofeedback: What happened to the biopsychosocial model? Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 31(2), 155–165. Mayer, E. A., Naliboff, B. D., & Chang, L. (2001a). Basic pathophysiologic mechanisms in irritable bowel syndrome. Digestive Diseases, 19(3), 212–218. Mayer, E. A., Naliboff, B. D., Chang, L., & Coutinho, S. V. (2001b). V. Stress and irritable bowel syndrome. American Journal of Physiology. Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology, 280(4), G519–G524. McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Lipsenthal, L., & Arguelles, L. (2009). New hope for correctional officers: An innovative program for reducing stress and health risks. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(4), 251–272. McLay, R. N., & Spira, J. L. (2009). Use of a portable biofeedback device to improve insomnia in a combat zone: A case report. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(4), 319–321. Meuret, A. E., Hofmann, S. G., & Rosenfield, D. (2010). Catastrophic appraisal and perceived control as moderators of treatment response in panic disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3, 262–277. Meuret, A. E., & Ritz, T. (2010). Hyperventilation in panic disorder and asthma: Empirical evidence and clinical strategies. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78(1), 68–79. Meuret, A. E., Ritz, T., Wilhelm, F. H., & Roth, W. T. (2005). Voluntary hyperventilation in the treatment of panic disorder—functions of hyperventilation, their implications for breathing training, and recommendations for standardization. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(3), 285–306. Meuret, A. E., Rosenfield, D., Hofmann, S. G., Suvak, M. K., & Roth, W. T. (2009). Changes in respiration mediate changes in fear of bodily sensations in panic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(6), 634–641. 211 Meuret, A. E., Rosenfield, D., Seidel, A., Bhaskara, L., & Hofmann, S. G. (2010). Respiratory and cognitive mediators of treatment change in panic disorder: Evidence for intervention specificity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(5), 691–704. Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., Ritz, T., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Breathing training for treating panic disorder: Useful intervention or impediment? Behavior Modification, 27(5), 731–754. Meuret, A. E., Wilhelm, F. H., Ritz, T., & Roth, W. T. (2008). Feedback of end-tidal pCO2 as a therapeutic approach for panic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(7), 560–568. Moravec, C. S. (2008). Biofeedback therapy in cardiovascular disease: Rationale and research overview. Cleveland Clinical Journal of Medicine, 75(Suppl. 2), S35–S38. Moravec, C. S., & McKee, M. G. (2008). Biofeedback in the treatment of heart disease. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 78(Suppl. 1), S20–S23. Moravec, C. S., & McKee, M. G. (2013). Psychophysiologic remodeling of the failing heart. Biofeedback, 41(1), 7–12. Moynihan, J. E., & Gevirtz, R. N. (2001). Respiratory and cognitive subtypes of panic. Preliminary validation of Ley's model. Behavior Modification, 25(4), 555–583. Mu, Q., Bohning, D. E., Nahas, Z., Walker, J., Anderson, B., Johnson, K. A., et al. (2004). Acute vagus nerve stimulation using different pulse widths produces varying brain effects. Biological Psychiatry, 55(8), 816–825. Nada, P. J. (2009). Heart rate variability in the assessment and biofeedback training of common mental health problems in children. Medicinski Archives, 63(5), 244– 248. Nixon, P. G. (1989). Human functions and the heart. In D. S. A. Cribb (Ed.), Changing ideas in health care. Chichester, UK: Wiley. Nolan, R. P., Kamath, M. V., Floras, J. S., Stanley, J., Pang, C., Picton, P., et al. (2005). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a behavioral neurocardiac intervention to enhance vagal heart rate control. American Heart Journal, 149(6), 1137. Oakes, D. F. (1996). Clinical practitioner's guide to respiratory care (4th ed.). Old Town, MN: Health Educator. Parati, G., & Steptoe, A. (2004). Stress reduction and blood pressure control in hypertension: A role for transcendental meditation? Journal of Hypertension, 22(11), 2057–2060. Patel, C., Marmot, M. G., Terry, D. J., Carruthers, M., Hunt, B., & Patel, M. (1985). Trial of relaxation in reducing coronary risk: Four year follow up. British Medical Journal (Clin Res Ed), 290(6475), 1103–1106. Patron, E., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Favretto, G., Valfre, C., Bonfa, C., Gasparotto, R., et al. (2013). Biofeedback assisted control of respiratory sinus arrhythmia as a biobehavioral intervention for depressive symptoms in patients after cardiac surgery: A preliminary study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(1), 1–9. Porges, S. (2011). The polyvagal theory. New York: Norton. Prigatano, G. P. (1973). Spider phobia: Autonomic reactions and biofeedback control of heart rate variability. Unpub- 212 lished doctoral dissertation, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, KY. Raymond, J., Sajid, I., Parkinson, L. A., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2005). Biofeedback and dance performance: A preliminary investigation. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30(1), 64–73. Reiner, R. (2008). Integrating a portable biofeedback device into clinical practice for patients with anxiety disorders: Results of a pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 33(1), 55–61. Reinke, A., Gevirtz, R., & Mussgay, L. (2007). Effects of heart rate variability feedback in reducing blood pressure. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 32, 134. Rene, R., Gevirtz, R., Muench, F., & Birkhead, E. (2011). The efficacy of a portable heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback device in conjunction with mental health treatment of clients with major depressive disorder enrolled in a county welfare-to-work program. Unpublished dissertation, Alliant University, San Diego, CA. Reyes, F. J. (2014). Implementing heart rate variability biofeedback groups for veterans with PTSD. Biofeedback, 42(4), 137–142. Roth, W. T., Wilhelm, F. H., & Trabert, W. (1998). Autonomic instability during relaxation in panic disorder. Psychiatry Research, 80(2), 155–164. Rush, A. J., George, M. S., Sackeim, H. A., Marangell, L. B., Husain, M. M., Giller, C., et al. (2000). Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment-resistant depressions: A multicenter study. Biological Psychiatry, 47(4), 276– 286. Russell, I. J. (2000). Fibromyalgia syndrome. In S. Mense, D. Simons, & I. J. Russell (Eds.), Muscle pain: Understanding its nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 289–337). New York: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. Sabbah, H. N., Ilsar, I., Zaretsky, A., Rastogi, S., Wang, M., & Gupta, R. C. (2011). Vagus nerve stimulation in experimental heart failure. Heart Failure Reviews, 16(2), 171–178. Sackeim, H. A., Rush, A. J., George, M. S., Marangell, L. B., Husain, M. M., Nahas, Z., et al. (2001). Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment-resistant depression: Efficacy, side effects, and predictors of outcome. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(5), 713–728. Sahin, D., Ilbay, G., Imal, M., Bozdogan, O., & Ates, N. (2009). Vagus nerve stimulation suppresses generalized seizure activity and seizure-triggered postictal cardiac rhythm changes in rats. Physiology Research, 58(3), 345–350. Sakakibara, M., Hayano, J., Oikawa, L. O., Katsamanis, M., & Lehrer, P. (2013). Heart rate variability biofeedback improves cardiorespiratory resting function during sleep. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 38(4), 265–271. Salkovskis, P. M. (1992). Psychological treatment of noncardiac chest pain: The cognitive approach. American Journal of Medicine, 92(5A), 114S–121S. Salkovskis, P. M., & Clark, D. M. (1990). Affective responses to hyperventilation: A test of the cognitive model of panic. Behavior Research Therapy, 28(1), 51–61. IV. RELAXATION INTERVENTIONS Schein, M. H., Gavish, B., Herz, M., Rosner-Kahana, D., Naveh, P., Knishkowy, B., et al. (2001). Treating hypertension with a device that slows and regularises breathing: A randomised, double-blind controlled study. Journal of Human Hypertension, 15(4), 271–278. Shahwan, A., Bailey, C., Maxiner, W., & Harvey, A. S. (2009). Vagus nerve stimulation for refractory epilepsy in children: More to VNS than seizure frequency reduction. Epilepsia, 50(5), 1220–1228. Sherman, E. M., Connolly, M. B., Slick, D. J., Eyrl, K. L., Steinbok, P., & Farrell, K. (2008). Quality of life and seizure outcome after vagus nerve stimulation in children with intractable epilepsy. Journal of Child Neurology, 23(9), 991–998. Siepmann, M., Aykac, V., Unterdorfer, J., Petrowski, K., & Mueck-Weymann, M. (2008). A pilot study on the effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with depression and in healthy subjects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 33(4), 195–201. Siepmann, M., Hennig, U., Siepmann, T., Nitzsche, K., Muck-Weymann, M., Petrowski, K., et al. (2014). The effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with preterm labour. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 39(1), 27–35. Slutsker, B., Konichezky, A., & Gothelf, D. (2010). Breaking the cycle: Cognitive behavioral therapy and biofeedback training in a case of cyclic vomiting syndrome. Psychology, Health, and Medicine, 15(6), 625–631. Sommers, P., Gevirtz, R., Jasin, S., & Chin, H. (1989). The efficacy of biobehavioral and compliance enhancement interventions in the adjunctive treatment of mild pregnancy induced hypertension. Biofeedback and Self Regulation, 14(4), 309–317. Sowder, E., Gevirtz, R., Shapiro, W., & Ebert, C. (2010). Restoration of vagal tone: A possible mechanism for functional abdominal pain. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 35(3), 199–206. Strack, B., & Gevirtz, R. (2011). Getting to the heart of the matter: Heart rate variability biofeedback for enhanced performance In B. Strack, M. Linden & V. Wilson (Eds.), Biofeedback and neurofeedback applications in sport psychology (pp. 145–174). Wheat Ridge, CO: AAPB. Swanson, K., Gevirtz, R., Spira, J., & Guarneri, E. (2006). Does biofeedback increase heart rate variability in patents with known New York Heart Association class I-III heart failure [Abstract]. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 2, 71–91. Tan, G., Dao, T. K., Farmer, L., Sutherland, R. J., & Gevirtz, R. (2011). Heart rate variability (HRV) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): A pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36(1), 27–35. Thayer, J. F., & Friedman, B. H. (2002). Stop that!: Inhibition, sensitization, and their neurovisceral concomitants. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43(2), 123– 130. Thayer, J. F., Friedman, B. H., & Borkovec, T. D. (1996). Autonomic characteristics of generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry, 39(4), 255–266. Thayer, J. F., & Siegle, G. J. (2002). Neurovisceral integra- 13. Cardiorespiratory Biofeedback tion in cardiac and emotional regulation. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, 21(4), 24–29. Thurber, M. R. (2006). Effects of heart-rate variability biofeedback training and emotional regulation on music performance anxiety in university students. Denton: University of North Texas Press. Timmons, B. H., & Ley, R. (Eds.). (1994). Behavioral and psychological approaches to breathing disorders. New York: Plenum Press. Vagades, J., Gordon, C., Schwaemmle, M., Andrasik, F., Gevirtz, R. N., Hautzinger, M., et al. (2011). Does deep breathing training improve myofascial release in comination with trigger point therapy for patients with low back pain? . Paper presented at the 15th annual meeting of the Biofeedback Foundation of Europe. van der Kolk, B. A. (2001). The psychobiology and psychopharmacology of PTSD. Human Psychopharmacology, 16(Suppl. 1), S49–S64. van der Kolk, B. A. (2006). Clinical implications of neuroscience research in PTSD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 277–293. van Dixhoorn, J. (2007). Whole body breathing. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practices of stress management (pp. 291–332). New York: Guilford Press. van Dixhoorn, J. J., & Duivenvoorden, H. J. (1999). Effect of relaxation therapy on cardiac events after myocardial infarction: A 5-year follow-up study. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 19(3), 178–185. van Dixhoorn, J., Duivenvoorden, H. J., Staal, H. A., & Pool, J. (1989). Physical training and relaxation therapy in cardiac rehabilitation assessed through a composite criterion for training outcome. American Heart Journal, 118(3), 545–552. van Dixhoorn, J., Duivenvoorden, H. J., Staal, J. A., Pool, J., & Verhage, F. (1987). Cardiac events after myocardial infarction: Possible effect of relaxation therapy. European Heart Journal, 8(11), 1210–1214. Vaschillo, E. G. (1984). Dynamics of slow-wave cardiac rhythm structure as an index of the functional state of an operant. Doctoral dissertation, Leningrad State University, Leningrad, Russia. Vaschillo, E., Lehrer, P., Rishe, N., & Konstantinov, M. 213 (2002). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a method for assessing baroreflex function: A preliminary study of resonance in the cardiovascular system. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 27(1), 1–27. Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., & Lehrer, P. (2004). Heartbeat synchronizes with respiratory rhythm only under specific circumstances. Chest, 126(4), 1385–1386; author reply 1386–1387. Vaschillo, E. G., Vaschillo, B., & Lehrer, P. M. (2006). Characteristics of resonance in heart rate variability stimulated by biofeedback. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 31(2), 129–142. Wang, S. Z., Li, S., Xu, X. Y., Lin, G. P., Shao, L., Zhao, Y., et al. (2009). Effect of slow abdominal breathing combined with biofeedback on blood pressure and heart rate variability in prehypertension. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(10), 1039–1045. Wheat, A. L., & Larkin, K. T. (2010). Biofeedback of heart rate variability and related physiology: A critical review. Applied Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 35(3), 229–242. Wood, P. B. (2006). Mesolimbic dopaminergic mechanisms and pain control. Pain, 120(3), 230–234. Wood, P. B., Schweinhardt, P., Jaeger, E., Dagher, A., Hakyemez, H., Rabiner, E. A., et al. (2007). Fibromyalgia patients show an abnormal dopamine response to pain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(12), 3576–3582. Yasuma, F., & Hayano, J. (2004). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Why does the heartbeat synchronize with respiratory rhythm? Chest, 125(2), 683–690. Zhang, Y., Ilsar, I., Sabbah, H. N., Ben David, T., & Mazgalev, T. N. (2009). Relationship between right cervical vagus nerve stimulation and atrial fibrillation inducibility: Therapeutic intensities do not increase arrhythmogenesis. Heart Rhythm, 6(2), 244–250. Zitnik, R. J. (2011). Treatment of chronic inflammatory diseases with implantable medical devices. Annals of the Rheumatic Diseases, 70(Suppl. 1), i67–i70. Zucker, T. L., Samuelson, K. W., Muench, F., Greenberg, M. A., & Gevirtz, R. N. (2009). The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: Aa pilot study. Applied Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 34(2), 135–143. Part V Practice Issues Chap ter 14 Intake and Preparation for Intervention Mark S. Schwartz This chapter is about intake decisions and the preparation of persons1 for interventions.2 It includes considerations and guidelines for making decisions about selection of persons, planning of appropriate interventions, discussion of tailoring intervention goals and cognitive preparations of individuals, and sections that focus on interviewing, history taking, and self-report measures. One basic intake decision in the therapeutic setting is whether to use biofeedback3 with a specific person. This chapter includes discussion of many factors in this decision-making process. It is my intention in this chapter that the content, although not exhaustive, will be sufficient for most of the situations practitioners encounter, and that practitioners will benefit from this information and guidelines. Readers who want more information about intake and considerations about specific disorders are referred to other chapters in this volume. titioners first consider the published literature and current practice. Published Literature and Current Practice Good research is one cornerstone of practice and a basis for deciding which symptoms, conditions, and disorders to consider, and the persons for whom biofeedback-related interventions are indicated. However, research often does not capture the essence of applied applications, and high-quality research is lacking for many conditions and behaviors. Therefore, practitioners need not wait for well-controlled research to support all procedures and applications before using biofeedback and associated therapies. However, they do need to base decisions on sound logical and responsible criteria. Other considerations become more important when adequate research is lacking. Prudent professionals know their limitations. They also recognize the limits of published research and the limits of other practitioners. As practitioners, we preferably guard against the problem of “not knowing what we do not know.” We ideally places ourselves in the role of the person with whom we are providing interventions: “Suppose I were that person” or “Suppose someone asked me to pay for the intervention for this person. What questions would I ask? What compromises and accommodations would I ask for and Conditions for Which Biofeedback and Related Interventions Are Appropriate When considering the conditions4 for which biofeedback and other applied psychophysiological treatments are appropriate, one considers the individual for whom the interventions are intended, the correctness of the diagnosis, and the specific features and stages of the condition. Prudent prac217 218 regard as proper?” Prudent practitioners consider several sources of data. Conditions and Disorders Agreement on a list of conditions and disorders for which biofeedback is appropriate presents a challenge considering the many issues, options, and diversity of providers. Practitioners consider many sources when selecting conditions for which to recommend using biofeedback, and for which persons. Unanimous or near-unanimous agreement is unlikely. Whether a condition is on an indication list depends on the selection criteria, the individual persons for whom biofeedback is being considered, and the degree of caution preferred or adopted by the involved parties developing the list. In prior editions of this volume, I was tempted to avoid lists, because lists are subjective, somewhat arbitrary, subject to criticism, and may quickly become obsolete. However, lists were included in this chapter in prior editions. The three lists conveyed my various degrees of confidence in the literature and practice. The lists were merely guidelines. Furthermore, I offered additional cautions. “Mindful practitioners never automatically accept or reject for treatment all or most patients with a disorder on any list.” I decided to forgo the A, B, and C lists of the prior editions and instead refer readers to efficacy documents from the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB; Yucha & Montgomery, 2008), which review the efficacy for 41 types of disorders (a summary of these disorders may be found at my website, www.marksschwartzphd. com), in addition to the chapters in this book, of course, and especially the literature found in the journal Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, the AAPB “magazine” Biofeedback, and the International Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR) Journal of Neurotherapy. However, this chapter provides some reminders, guidelines, caveats, and cautions to use when reviewing the conclusions and reviews from “official” sources. • Even consideration of symptoms, conditions, and disorders that are considered to have the “best” support does not imply that biofeedback alone is always the treatment of choice or the sole or primary treatment. One exception is nocturnal enuresis, for which forms of biofeedback can be the primary or sole treatment. • Even for some symptoms, conditions, and disorders for which there is less than ideal support for V. PRACTICE ISSUES biofeedback used alone or as a major intervention component, it is still reasonable to consider biofeedback as a legitimate part of an intervention plan. In many cases, other interventions often play a larger role. • For other symptoms, conditions, and disorders for which there is weak support (e.g., case studies), one can consider including biofeedback within a stepped-care approach, assuming that the practitioner can clearly justify and document the rationale. • All symptoms, conditions, and diagnoses require careful patient selection and tailoring of treatment combinations to the individual. Inclusion of a disorder in any listing is not intended to imply that biofeedback is suitable for all or most patients with this diagnosis. One also can include symptoms, conditions, and disorders for which relaxation and other applied psychophysiological techniques are effective and one uses biofeedback instruments to obtain a more complete assessment. Biofeedback can help some of these persons change their beliefs about themselves, including their self-confidence about making changes. Biofeedback helps some persons to improve their self-regulation. Instrumentation also allows practitioners to assess and document progress. One might also include other symptoms and conditions from the field of physical medicine and rehabilitation Cautions and Contraindications Prudent practitioners consider various cautions and contraindications before using biofeedback and other applied psychophysiological interventions. Experts generally agree about many of these; however, there is no single, agreed-upon document listing or discussing all cautions and contraindications. Adler and Adler (1984, 1989a, 1989b) offered sage opinions regarding limitations of biofeedback, and I still recommend reading of those chapters. Consider the following disorders and conditions as outright contraindications to biofeedback, or at least as indicating the need for much caution. These include severe depression; acute agitation; acute or fragile schizophrenia (or a strong potential for psychotic decompensation); mania; paranoid disorders with delusions of influence; severe obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD); delirium; acute medical decompensation; or a strong potential for 219 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention a dissociative reaction, fugue state, or depersonalization. However, there is very little or no literature on biofeedback or other applied psychophysiological interventions for patients with these disorders, because logic has precluded such interventions with these patients. In the rare cases when a practitioner can justify using relaxation and biofeedback with a patient who has one of these diagnoses or conditions, prudent standards of practice dictate using special assessment and treatment procedures. For example, one might treat tension or migraine headaches in a patient with OCD. Caution must also be employed in using some forms of biofeedback and relaxation therapies for patients with certain other conditions. These are not contraindications; however, providers must be very knowledgeable and experienced with these conditions, and well versed in using special approaches. These conditions include moderate to severely impaired attention or memory (as in dementia and various forms of intellectual disability), as well as some seizure disorders. One also needs to be cautious with patients with significant “secondary gain” from symptoms, as would be true for any psychological-based intervention. Practitioners should inform patients taking medications for certain medical disorders (e.g., diabetes mellitis, hypothyroidism, seizure disorders, hypertension, glaucoma, and asthma) that relaxation therapies might result in a need for reduced dosage, and should discuss this possibility with the patients’ physicians. However, it should be noted that reports documenting adverse effects or altered medication requirements associated with relaxation therapies and biofeedback are very rare. have already ruled out medical causes and diagnoses, and who will reevaluate patients as indicated. The scope of this chapter does not permit a detailed discussion of this subject, which has been well treated by others elsewhere. Readers are referred to Chuang & Forman (2006), Maldonado (2009), Taylor (2007), and to my website (www. marksschwartzphd.com) for excellent, detailed information. A variety of types of medical disorders produce psychological/psychiatric symptoms and signs, as noted in Table 14.1. It is in the early stages of many of these disorders that persons present with the symptoms and practitioners are more likely to be misled. Note examples of classes of prescription medications that can induce psychological/psychiatric-like symptoms: chemotherapy, immunosuppressants (e.g., cyclosporine), antivirals (e.g., interferons), antiparkinsonian, cardiovascular, thyroid, anticholinergic, corticosteroids, psychostimulants, sympathomimetics, sedative and central nervous system (CNS) depressants (e.g., barbiturates, benzodiazepines), opioids, hormones, diuretics, antihistamines, vitamin B complex, and nicotinic acid (Hall, 1980; Maldonado, 2009; Othmer & Othmer, 2002). Examples of substances that can induce psychological/psychiatric-like symptoms include alcohol, cocaine, marijuana, phencyclidine (PCP), lysergic acid diethylamine (LSD), heroin, amphetamines, Jimson weed (street names are thornapple, stinkweed, locoweed), gamma-hydroxybutyric acid (GHB; e.g., medical applications as prescription medication Xyrem for cataplexy and excessive daytime sleepiness associated with narcolepsy). Medical Conditions Masquerading as Psychological Symptoms Recommendations All practitioners need to know and understand that many symptoms “masquerade” as psychological or functional symptoms but actually are caused by an organic medical disorder requiring different interventions (Rosse, Deutsch, & Deutsch, 2000; Othmer & Othmer, 2002; Hall, 1980). Practitioners need to be familiar with the symptoms and other manifestations of diagnosed medical conditions, and other possible diagnoses and conditions. Nonmedical practitioners particularly need to exercise extra caution and consult medical specialists. Prudent practice standards dictate working closely with competent physicians and other medical professionals in appropriate specialties who The following recommendations are intended to help practitioners avoid important mistakes: • Be familiar with the special features of the many medical conditions/disorders and the symptoms/signs for disorders known or suspected among the practitioner’s patients/clients/subjects (Maldonado, 2009; Taylor, 2007). • Be familiar with the features of psychological/ psychiatric conditions/disorders for those persons with one or more of these conditions/disorders. • Make sure that patients/clients/subjects will soon have or recently have had a competent medical examination. 220 V. PRACTICE ISSUES TABLE 14.1. Selected Symptoms and Signs from Selected Medical Disorders That May Masquerade as Psychological/Psychiatric Disorders Medical disorders Symptoms/signsa may masquerade as . . . Hypothyroidism Tiredness, depression, myalgia, hypertension, bradycardia, cold periphery, anxiety, mania, dementia, schizophrenia Hyperthyroidism (Grave’s disease) Irritability, restlessness, malaise, muscle weakness, tremor, breathlessness, palpitations, anxiety (e.g., panic disorder or generalized anxiety disorder), mood disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children Hypoadrenalism Depression, nausea/vomiting, abdominal pain, constipation, joint or back pain, fatigue, apathy, anorexia Hyperadrenalism (Cushing’s disease) Fatigue, mood lability, decreased mood, sleep disturbance Pheochomocytoma Anxiety resembling panic attacks Hypoparathyroidism Anxiety and panic Pancreatic cancer Anxiety, depression, dementia Oat-cell lung cancer Anxiety and panic Systemic lupus erythematosus Anxiety, depression, multiple symptoms in many body areasb Wilson’s disease, multiple sclerosis, cerebral ischemia Anxiety and/or depression Supraventricular tachycardia Multiple panic-like symptoms (e.g., rapid heartbeats, often above 140 to about 200 beats per minute) aThe basis for selecting these few of the possible symptoms was the types of symptoms presented by persons most commonly seeking, or referred to practitioners for, biofeedback and applied psychophysiology. bDevelops over a long time, often many years. • Maintain a close relationship with patients’/ clients’ physicians and/or other physicians who can help. • Recommend to patients/clients/subjects that they maintain regular or periodic contact with their physicians. • Encourage patients/clients/subjects to report any new symptoms, new and unusual behaviors, and changes in existing symptoms. • Know the drugs (prescription and over the counter [OTC]) and substances your patients/ clients/subjects take to understand possible symptoms and side effects. Other Considerations in Choosing Treatments Stepped Care The stepped-care approach usually involves first using less complicated, less expensive, and sometimes less time-consuming treatments. There is a strong precedent for this approach in medicine and other health care fields. For example, practitioners consider less potent medications or lower doses before choosing more potent or higher doses, and changes in diet and exercise before medication. The stepped-care model is very consistent with the cost containment zeitgeist. There are situations in which an even more conservative approach than biofeedback is appropriate and preferable to try first. Patients and referral sources will usually be grateful and respectful when practitioners are conservative and successful with “less” rather than “more” treatment. One can consider dietary changes (e.g., cessation of caffeine use) before relaxation and biofeedback. All patients need not stop caffeine use before starting biofeedback and related therapies. However, consider this in particular with patients with headaches, anxiety, Raynaud’s disease, irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), psychophysiological insomnia, and essential hypertension. Consider 221 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention stopping or changing other substances, and/or changing medications that might be contributing to symptoms (in consultation with the treating physician). At a minimum, discuss with such patients the rationale for eliminating caffeine to reduce interference with relaxation and biofeedback. If stopping caffeine before other treatments leads to improvement, there will be a credible demonstration of the effects of caffeine. However, even when eliminating caffeine does not result in symptom relief, it still allows for a more meaningful symptom baseline. Patients also may be more motivated to comply with later recommendations, and better able to do so, if they have first made indicated dietary changes (see Block, Gylenhaal, & Schwartz, Chapter 10, this volume). Stepped care also includes other factors that are often simple to change. For example, to stop gum chewing is sometimes proper before relaxation and biofeedback for tension-type headaches, temporomandibular (TMD) symptoms, or tinnitus. Consider the patient who chews gum a few hours per day and has headaches in the temporalis muscles or facial pain from probable daytime bruxism. This simple change is a necessary first step that may be sufficient. Another disorder that may lend itself to this approach is psychophysiological insomnia. Tried and/or Available Alternative Treatments Among the important initial factors to consider are the prior therapies that have been attempted and their outcomes. If relaxation or biofeedback interventions were previously unsuccessful, consider the following: • Find out exactly which interventions were used, to avoid providing the same or similar interventions. • Determine the patient’s/client’s understanding of the rationale and procedures, and his or her compliance with these. • Determine the patient’s comfort level with the prior practitioner. • Ascertain whether the prior practitioner was present or absent during sessions. • Find out the instrumentation and modalities used, and the placement sites of electrodes and sensors. • Find out the types of relaxation instructions and body positions assumed during relaxation and biofeedback. • Determine the patient’s or client’s understanding and have him or her demonstrate desired breathing techniques. • Determine the degree of the patient’s past generalization and transfer of training procedures. A therapist can usually obtain answers to these and related questions within a few minutes. A prior unsuccessful trial need not prevent another trial with biofeedback, assuming an adequate rationale, competent provider, and the presence of other necessary criteria that are noted and discussed in this chapter and others in this volume. Of course, there need to be indications that another trial could improve upon important therapy procedures. An adequate trial of proper medication might offer a less expensive yet effective option that is acceptable to many persons. Furthermore, biofeedback and associated therapies are often more easily justified after an unsuccessful trial of other proper therapies. Severity and/or Seriousness of Symptoms or Disorders The practitioner also needs to consider the seriousness or severity of the patient’s symptoms and disorder when deciding whether to offer biofeedback. Consider physiological self-regulatory therapies, including biofeedback, for some persons with serious or severe symptoms even when there is insufficient research and/or clinical experience to support its use, especially when options are nonexistent, more risky, or far more expensive. Again, this assumes an adequate rationale for biofeedback, a competent provider, and the presence of other necessary criteria for such therapies. Consider for example, a patient of mine who presented with hyperemesis gravidarum, a serious disorder involving unrelenting nausea and vomiting during pregnancy. Antiemetic medications were either contraindicated because of her pregnancy or were no longer available in the United States. Research studies on the use of biofeedback and relaxation therapies for this disorder did not exist. However, the clinical rationale was sound. Practitioners and research support the successful use of such therapies for patients with functional emesis not associated with pregnancy (e.g., patients with anticipatory emesis associated with chemotherapy for cancer). Alternative therapies for this patient were far less practical. The only other option was hospitalization and intravenous 222 nutritional therapy for the remaining 6 months of her pregnancy or until the hyperemesis stopped. A brief and intensive therapy program led to a substantial reduction in the emesis. A practitioner might reasonably consider avoiding or deferring treatment if a patient’s symptoms are mild or infrequent enough that he or she can live comfortably with minimal or no treatment. This category, for example, might include patients with vascular headaches once every 2–3 months that last a few hours and respond well to a safe medication; patients with very mild tension-type headaches two or three times a month, each lasting a few hours; or patients with bruxism without pain or damage to the teeth or other oral structures or tissues. Some practitioners encourage such people to live with their symptoms unless a brief and inexpensive therapy program has a good chance of success, or they may devote a session or two teaching them procedures for successful coping. Geographical (or Other) Distance between Patients and Treatment Facilities Some patients live beyond a reasonable driving distance from treatment facilities. Often a suitable referral in a patient’s home area is not available. Other patients prefer treatment away from their home area, because they have had bad experiences there. Still others prefer or need to maintain strict confidentiality away from their home area. Some prefer the type of therapy, professional care, and credibility of a specific facility. In some large medical centers, a patient can only stay for a short time. When there are good indications for biofeedback and related therapies, and more regularly spaced treatment options are impractical, inappropriate, or nonexistent, a practitioner might consider a “massed-practice” therapy program. Such a program involves one or two daily office sessions for a few or several days. There are advantages to this schedule, and sometimes it is the only or the best option available. It can also serve to encourage the patient to continue therapy when he or she returns home. A limitation of massed-practice therapy away from the home environment and usual routine is that the patient might be experiencing less stress, and there are no opportunities to practice techniques in real-world settings and to troubleshoot with the therapist, and so forth. There also may be self-imposed or implied pressure to accomplish more than is reasonable in a short period of time. V. PRACTICE ISSUES The Professional’s Confidence and Competence with Biofeedback The practitioner’s confidence and competence in using necessary instrumentation during evaluation and therapy are obviously important. Whether other professionals believe they can attain the same results without instrumentation may be a moot point. If a professional uses instrumentation competently and prefers to do so, then doing so is acceptable, regardless of whether noninstrumentation-based procedures might produce similar results. There are considerable precedents for this philosophy of practice in medicine and psychology. For example, some psychologists and psychiatrists prefer to rely usually or solely on detailed interviews and direct observations to make diagnoses and recommendations. This is a common and acceptable approach. In contrast, many professionals prefer to add and often rely on psychological assessments and testing, which can add significant costs to evaluations. However, the additional assessments are common practice, and many believe that they add to the quality of the diagnostic and therapy plan. Similarly, many neurologists and other physicians request neuropsychological assessments to help confirm a diagnosis or add information about a patient’s functioning. Sometimes these merely confirm practitioners’ clinical impressions. This is acceptable and common clinical practice. Other neurologists and physicians believe such assessments are unnecessary. This, too, is acceptable clinical practice. Alternative therapies and procedures may be equally effective. Some studies show one treatment to be better, whereas other studies show that a second treatment is better than the first; still other studies may show them to be equal. Practitioners have the right to choose among evaluative and therapeutic approaches that are consistent with their interest and confidence given that their interest and confidence are crucial to the therapeutic outcome. Of course, practitioners must also keep in mind risks, costs, and efficacy. Initial Physiological Evaluation and Baseline Session(s) The results of the psychophysiological baseline assessment constitute an important source of information for making therapy decisions. For example, suppose that a practitioner observes con- 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention sistently low levels of muscle activity from multiple muscle areas during rest and stressor segments. In this situation, the practitioner should consider deferring biofeedback, particularly surface electromyography (SEMG) biofeedback, or omitting biofeedback from the treatment plan. In such a case, the instrumentation-based monitoring serves a very useful purpose. It reveals that the patient can relax within a therapeutic range. The practitioner then clearly explains the meaning of that finding and instructs, encourages, and supervises the patient in the use of relaxation. This includes using relaxation frequently enough, long enough, and at the right times for it to be of therapeutic value. It may be that selected muscular tension is occurring in the patient’s daily life but not in the practitioner’s office. Without demonstration of tension in the office, it may still be proper to use non-instrumentation-based physiological self-regulatory procedures in the patient’s daily activities. Another question that arises in this context concerns the criteria for being sufficiently relaxed. There are no hard and fast rules, and professionals disagree except at the extremes. The level and duration of physiological activity needed for a positive therapeutic effect differ among patients. Practitioners often do not agree on necessary physiological criteria for relaxation to result in positive results for the symptoms and disorders treated. Reaching clear or ideal criteria during biofeedback-assisted relaxation office sessions is probably often unnecessary. Patients often improve despite somewhat tense levels in early and later baselines. Practitioners consider recommending avoidance of excessive tension, especially for sustained periods. A person might benefit from reproducing the reduced tension observed in the baselines, even if he or she has not reached the ideally relaxed range. Indeed, the therapist should suggest this and help the patient learn to do it frequently, rapidly, for various durations, and at the right times. Baselines need to be long enough to observe increasing or decreasing physiological activity over several minutes. Baselines that are too short or those integrated over long periods can obscure such trends. Also consider varying the conditions under which baselines are conducted. Baselines achieved with the person’s eyes closed can be very misleading and inadequate, because many people show much higher tension with their eyes open. In addition, monitoring only during resting conditions without stressors provides unrealistic results. Such recordings often show little or no tension or arousal, whereas with anticipation of or dur- 223 ing stressful stimuli, tension and arousal are often greater. It should also be noted that psychophysiological measurements can be unreliable across sessions. Thus, the activity in one session does not reliably occur in other sessions. In the first session, the person may show more tension because of the novelty of the situation. This is one reason for sometimes considering additional baseline segments in making decisions about the need for biofeedback. Furthermore, some persons relax adequately at home and in their real-life situations; however, they have difficulty relaxing in a professional’s office. No matter how a practitioner may present it, there is an implied evaluative atmosphere in an office that some people find difficult to overcome. This is sometimes called “office hypertension” or “white coat hypertension.” The practitioner who suspects this should consider evaluating how the person views the session and how he or she feels during office sessions. The practitioner should then attempt to check the physiological activity during practice periods outside the office. Other useful data include the physiological responses during and after feedback segments. The questions to be asked include the following: • Is there significantly lower tension and arousal with the feedback? • Is the lowered arousal and tension maintained after the feedback? • Does the feedback lead to increased arousal and tension? • Do stressors increase the arousal or precipitate arousal? There are several questions and types of information on which one may base the decision to provide more biofeedback. Here are a few examples: • Is there much excess tension and arousal during resting baseline and self-regulation segments without feedback? • Is much tension and arousal precipitated or worsened with office stressors? • Does feedback result in significantly lowered tension and arousal? • Does the person return to baseline tension and arousal after feedback? It is instructive to remember that such logical questions and criteria have not yet been clearly shown to predict the necessity of biofeedbackassisted therapies for achieving positive therapeu- 224 tic outcomes. Until research supports such evidence, practitioners need to remain cautious and conservative in making such decisions and recommendations. Symptom Changes in the First Weeks Symptom changes during the first weeks of therapy are important for deciding whether to continue sessions and determining which therapy procedures to pursue. Such improvements often occur early, before reaching ideal physiological mastery. If the initial intervention results in a clinically significant reduction of symptoms, practitioners need to justify more office-based intervention. The primary goal is to decrease or stop symptoms, not just to reduce microvolts, increase hand temperatures, or change the electroencephalographic (EEG) frequency or pattern. When there are clinically significant changes in the first few weeks, some prudent practitioners consider deferring more biofeedback sessions, even if they assume that some persons need specific physiological mastery for reliable therapeutic changes. In addition to cost considerations, another reason for this determination is credibility. Consider preparing the patient for the possibility that more office interventions may be needed later. As an example, here is a statement that might be made to a person who has shown clinically significant improvement of symptoms in the early weeks of therapy. “I am happy for your rapid improvement with significantly reduced hours of severe headache, increased hours without headaches. You also are showing improved ability to reduce muscle tension. However, the muscle tension in your head is still there when we measure it in the office. It might be better to relax these muscles deeper and faster, and maintain the lower levels longer. More biofeedback sessions could speed up improvement, but I cannot predict that with certainty. Let’s consider you continuing to apply the therapy and keep records of your symptoms for another few weeks and then review the situation.” Patient Characteristics Even when studies indicate that person characteristics significantly differentiate more successful from less successful subjects, there is usually much overlap between groups. There are many variables V. PRACTICE ISSUES that influence the relationships. These studies need replications in a variety of clinical settings and with potential “moderator variables” before we conclude that some persons should not receive an intervention. A question to ask here is what to do with persons with different characteristics: Do we practitioners need to provide improved education? Will more frequent or more closely spaced sessions work better? Will procedural variations help? Patient’s Motivation and Compliance: Enhancement with Biofeedback Maintaining motivation for practice and application of physiological self-regulatory therapies are needed and include persons who can benefit from non-instrumentation-based intervention (e.g., relaxation only). For many persons, biofeedback is confirming and encouraging. It helps the patient maintain or gain confidence in the intervention and abilities. Some question and dispute the idea that they are physically tense or sympathetically aroused. Others doubt that their thoughts affect their physiological tension and arousal. Still others doubt that they can control their physiology. They all need concrete and credible evidence. Using instrumentation with physiological self-regulation procedures is not a rejection of the value of relaxation alone. It is not an either–or choice, as some portray it. The Patient’s Choice and Cost of Therapy Respect each person’s needs and treatment choices, and realistically discuss their options, time needed, desired results, prognosis, and costs. As practitioners, we want to see much improvement. We often measure success against research criteria such as symptom reductions of 50, 70, or 90%, along with reduced medications when indicated. Indeed, we strive for these ideal goals. However, such goals may not match the patient’s goals, and they are sometimes unrealistic. Some people welcome an improvement of 20–50%, especially after years of very little change. Other people have learned to be less distressed by the symptoms, although the intensity and duration have not decreased. For example, patients with chronic pain sometimes report improvement in “affective/ reactive” aspects rather than “sensory/intensity” features. A person may decide that additional improvement is not worth the additional financial and 225 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention time investments, and time away from other priorities. Also consider the costs and inconvenience of transportation and child care. Allow people to participate in choosing how much benefit they desire or will accept. An Introduction to Patient Education Rationale Some people are skeptical, critical, and resistant to accepting the potential benefits of relaxation, biofeedback, and other applied psychophysiological interventions. That should not lead us to become defensive or dismiss them as unsuitable candidates. Many have already seen other practitioners who were optimistic about interventions, yet the results were unsuccessful. We are now asking them to accept a different approach, often perceived as the last one. People are very often unfamiliar with behavioral and self-regulatory strategies. They often need information they can understand and accept about the rationale for these interventions. Furthermore, some people are skeptical about some practitioner specialties (e.g., nonmedical health care professionals). Relaxation and biofeedback interventions can appear to be rather simplistic. In contrast, explanations can appear very complex (e.g., some patients may think to themselves something like the following): “You mean I have had these symptoms for years, went to several good doctors, took lots of medication, and continue to suffer. Now you tell me that this relaxation (and/or biofeedback) is all that I need? I would like to believe that . . . but convince me!” Do not assume that others understand and accept the rationale for intervention, that explanations are sufficient, or that patients spontaneously ask questions or directly tell us their concerns. They usually do not! Furthermore, we should not assume that people accept and remember explanations and recommendations. Thus, seriously consider well-planned and well-executed presentations to increase attention, understanding, recall, confidence, satisfaction, and compliance. Devote adequate time early in the relationship and thereafter to prepare and teach adequately. The values of good education are often underestimated or neglected. Good education and information can also reduce anxiety, increase the credibility of the professional and the intervention procedures, and facilitate positive expectations. As Shaw and Blanchard (1983, p. 564) concluded, giving participants a high initial expectation of therapeutic benefit . . . has significant benefit in terms of self report of change and reduced physiological reactivity, and . . . these improvements are mediated at least in part by increased compliance with home practice instructions. . . . The procedures, per se, are not especially powerful without the appropriate set. They also noted that “a certain degree of salesmanship and trainer enthusiasm certainly can make a difference in outcome” (p. 564). Good educational presentations can improve patients’ knowledge and attitudes about the causes of their functioning and about intervention. It can enhance their perceptions of a practitioner (e.g., as being credible and trustworthy). Adherence and intervention effectiveness partly depend on such knowledge, beliefs, perceptions, and a positive alliance. Metaphors Tailoring educational presentations to a specific person depends on that person’s intelligence, education, reading ability, sophistication, and psychological mindedness. Professionals commonly use metaphors to communicate and educate. One of the major reasons for using metaphors is to help simplify information, concepts, and procedures. Metaphors are excellent for presenting the rationale, concepts, and ideas that people need to understand. This can make the ideas easier to accept and use (Combs & Freedman, 1990). One definition of “metaphor” is that it “is a way of . . . describing something in terms of something else” (Morris & Morris, 1985, p. 387). Metaphors are much more than simply analogies (Black, 1962; Richards, 1936). Muran and DiGiuseppe (1990) and Boyd (1979) describe metaphors as “cognitive instruments” by which similarities are created that previously were not known to exist. Metaphorical communication is highly persuasive as a means of conveying and altering thought and is a vehicle of change (Petrie, 1979). Siegelman (1990) starts her book by stating, “Most of us, and our patients . . . find ourselves cleaving to metaphor to communicate experience that is hard to convey in any other way” (p. 1). Practitioners of cognitive-behavioral therapies also advocate analogies and metaphors (Stott, Mansell, Salkoskis, Lavender, Cartwright-Hatton, 226 2010). McMullin (1986) describes many examples of perceptual shift techniques for cognitive restructuring therapy. Although professionals use many metaphors, further discussion or examples is beyond the scope of this chapter. Cautions in the Use of Metaphor Many linguists and psychologists warn about the potential misuse and misleading potential of metaphors. For example, the use of metaphor can foster careless thought “by acting as a substitute for the hard, analytic work of determining precisely what to say, a point previously raised by Aristotle . . . when he warned of the ambiguity and obscurity inherent in metaphor” (Muran & DiGiuseppe, 1990, p. 72). Practitioners need to be careful about why they are using a metaphor, with whom, and in what context. One premise of this section is that metaphors can improve cognitive preparation and education of people, and that improved cognitive preparation improves adherence for effective intervention results (Levy, 1987). Evaluation/Assessment: Interviewing, History Taking, and Self‑Report Measures Psychological Evaluation Deciding where to begin history taking, and whether (and, if so, how soon) to include psychological inquiries, depends on practitioner judgment, circumstances, and the patient/client. With many medical patients, mental health practitioners are often wise to begin with a history of physical symptoms. However, exceptions abound. Often all or most of the symptom history information is available in the recorded history. With medical patients, there are often practical constraints for psychological evaluations (e.g., schedules, resistance due to limited psychological mindedness). However, even a brief psychological evaluation can be helpful. Asking a few psychological questions can help build rapport and assess receptiveness or resistance to this type of question and intervention. It also can help with deciding whether or not more evaluation is needed. The following list of psychological factors is based on one by Adler and Adler (1987), who provide an erudite, insightful, refined, and skilled commentary on history taking. One must read their original text to appreciate their style and V. PRACTICE ISSUES clinical wisdom. Although it was written as a guide for interviewing people with headaches, their list and discussion are useful for other disorders. The Adlers suggest considering evaluation of many factors: • Patients’ expectations of themselves. • Perceived expectations by others. • Existence of past or present family conflicts. • Sensitivity to criticism and to emotional expressions. • Comfort with and skills in being assertive. • Illnesses and hospitalizations. • Past or present grief, or anticipated grief. • Medication misuse. • Perceptions of health care professionals. • Perceived emotional triggers or factors increasing the risk of a symptom. Do patients’ personality features and psychopathology worsen or maintain current symptoms, or are they the effects of chronic symptoms? How necessary is it for practitioners to assess and treat psychopathology to reduce current physical symptoms significantly? Also, do life stressors in the past cause or contribute to current symptoms? For example, what is the role of past sexual abuse or grief in current symptoms? How necessary is it for practitioners to assess and treat these factors to reduce current physical symptoms significantly? History Taking and Interviewing There are many resources for history taking and interviewing (Brannon, 2011; Hersen & Turner, 1985; Othmer & Othmer, 2002; Sommers-Flanagan & Sommers-Flanagan, 2008). Practitioners often use interview outlines as guides for specific conditions (e.g., consider Lacks [1987] and Morin and Espie [2003] for insomnia). The topics and specific items covered and the time invested for each depend on many factors: professional setting, professional specialty, referral source, referral information available, whether there will be continuing care by another professional, results from screening measures, stepped-care considerations, cost consideration for the patient, time available by the patient and the practitioner, and purpose of the consultation or evaluation.5 Practitioners responsible for assessment and treatment are wise to obtain at least some of their own history information rather than to rely exclusively on information from others. This is true even when the other sources are competent profes- 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention sionals. A practitioner needing specific information often must obtain it directly from the patient. The practitioner can review the prior reports aloud with the patient for his or her confirmation and elaboration. Even seemingly clear information, such as onset, location, frequency, and duration, can differ when one asks the questions and listens carefully to the answers. Even competent and experienced professionals can overlook potentially important items. This does not mean that they are careless or incompetent. Patients give different professionals different information and provide different answers to the same types of questions (e.g., Blanchard, O”Keefe, Neff, Jurish, & Andrasik, 1981). Practitioners may also misunderstand patients’ statements. Furthermore, practitioners, including physicians, sometimes obtain only the information needed for the purpose of their consultation—which may be to make a diagnosis; rule out serious organic pathology; prescribe medication; and/or make referrals for psychological evaluations, biofeedback, physical therapy, or other treatments. Physicians and other practitioners with special interests and expertise in specific symptoms and disorders often collect more detailed information than do other professionals. For example, for headaches, these areas of information include dietary factors, gum-chewing habits, use of bed pillows, sleep habits, stress, work postures and other ergonomic factors, driving habits, beliefs, and sexual and physical abuse. The practitioner who observes discrepancies between the recorded history and the information he or she now receives from the patient should address these discrepancies tactfully. Sexual Abuse Research reveals that sexual abuse is often part of the history of patients presenting with multiple somatic disorders (e.g., functional6 gastrointestinal disorders, nonspecific chronic pain, psychogenic seizures, chronic pelvic pain, fibromyalgia (when sexual abuse was defined as rape); Paras et al., 2009). There is separate support for the relationship between emotional, physical, and sexual abuse in persons with fibromyalgia syndrome (Häuser, Kosseva, Üceyler, Klose, & Sommer, 2011). Sexual abuse history also is associated with later risks for multiple psychiatric/psychological disorders (i.e., anxiety disorder, depression, eating disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD], sleep disorders, and suicide attempts; Chen et al., 227 2010). These results are independent of gender or age when the abuse happened. One conclusion and implication is that practitioners need to inquire about a history of possible sexual and physical abuse at least among persons presenting with the types of symptoms and disorders noted here. The interview questions from Drossman et al. (1990) comprise one set of possible questions. Practitioners should review other sources and guidelines for self-report and interview questions, and consult with specialists in this field before deciding how to assess this complex and delicate topic. Evaluating this topic of abuse and considering proper intervention are of potential value to biofeedback practitioners. One potential advantage of knowing about abuse is the opportunity to consider its possible influence on the development and/or maintenance of medical and psychophysiological problems. There are now substantial legal concerns for health care professionals when questioning patients about a history of sexual abuse, and when providing psychotherapy for patients based on the history (Cannell, Hudson, & Pope, 2001; Scheflin & Spiegel, 1998). This concern is particularly important if a professional is inquiring about and/or suspects a history of chronic sexual abuse (CSA) that the patient is not reporting. Prudent health care professionals guard against overzealousness, are well informed about the topic of CSA and so-called “repressed memories,” the needs for careful informed consent, and the other legal issues and recommendations (see www.apa.org/ topics/trauma/memories.aspx). Clinicians are well-advised to practice defensively in cases involving memory or dissociative disorders, especially by keeping extracautious notes and more frequent use of informed consent forms. Books on legal risk management are important preventive guides that could help avoid costly and unnecessary lawsuits. (Scheflin & Spiegel, 1998). However, the history of abuse in some patients may sometimes be very significant. “Failing to inquire about a history of trauma, and therefore assuming that it did not occur or if it did is unimportant, can be as damaging as insisting that a trauma history must lurk behind any symptom” (Scheflin & Spiegel, 1998, p. 861). Does it perhaps influence tolerance for symptoms? Does it contribute to those factors that motivate people with these symptoms to seek medical help? Does 228 V. PRACTICE ISSUES it perhaps contribute to the factors that motivate people to avoid treatment? Self-blame, poor selfimage, control issues, shame, trust, vulnerability, dependency conflicts, sexual dysfunction, and suppressed anger could affect all of these. Interview Outline The following outline or checklist offers a guideline to consider and from which to glean ideas for interviewing and other intake procedures. Most of these questions and items are useful in the general clinical practice of applied psychophysiology, and especially in the treatment of headache and anxiety disorders. Some items and questions do not apply to other disorders, such as incontinence. (See Andrasik & Schwartz, Chapter 20, for a more detailed discussion of taking a headache history.) 1. Symptom(s). (Note: A patient’s highest-pri- ority symptoms are not always the reason for referral.) a. Description. “What are the symptoms like?” [Offer choices.] b. Location. “Where does it begin? Show me. Does it move around?” c. Frequency. “How often does it occur? When does it increase–decrease?” d. Timing. “When do the symptoms occur? What time of day do they occur? Do they always or usually occur?” e. Duration. “How long do the symptoms last? Do they last for . . . ? What are the shortest, longest, and usual durations?” f. Intensity. [Consider rating scales.] “Are the symptoms slight, mild, moderate, severe, or very severe?” g. Origin. “When did the symptoms originally begin?” h. Development. “Has it changed over weeks, months, or years?” i. Course/progression. “Does it change over minutes/hours after it starts?” j. Precipitants/antecedents. [Look for dietary, environmental, postural, hormonal, emotional, work/family stress, and time factors.] “What do you think causes or starts the symptoms? Do you suspect that anything might be triggering it? Is there anything that often seems to precede it?” k. Aggravating/worsening factors. “Does anything increase the severity? What makes it worse?” l. Alleviating/helping factors. “Does any- thing decrease the severity? What makes it better? What do you do that reduces the symptoms?” m. Medications. (Note when and why medications are taken. Are medications taken when the patient is anticipating situations? Does the patient take medications with minimal symptoms?) “When do you take medications? How soon do you get relief after taking . . . ?” n. Nonrelief. “What has not worked for you? When was it taken?” o. Periods of remission. “Are you ever totally free of symptoms for days, weeks, longer?” p. Associated symptoms. “What other symptoms do you get with the main one? Do you get . . . ?” (Ask specific questions about specific symptoms.) q. Reactions of others. “What does your family do when you have symptoms?” r. Behaviors before, during, and after onset, including behaviors and attitudes on days without the symptoms. “On days when you are feeling much better or have no symptoms, do you try and catch up with house/ yard work, and other activities? Do you typically have worsening or resumption of symptoms soon after or the next day? Do you feel you need to fulfill your responsibilities on good days?” s. Limitations in life due to symptoms. t. Family members with similar or the same symptoms (optional). 2. Prior treatments. a. Prior psychological treatments. (Ask when, where, with whom; number of sessions, duration, results; patient’s reactions and views.) b. Prior experience with relaxation therapies. (Ask about prior relaxation therapies, what was done, how long this was used, what is still done, when it is done, what seems to work, what does not work. Ask for demonstration, especially breathing.) c. Prior experience with biofeedback. (Ask who provided it, where on body sensors were placed, whether eyes were open or closed, what body positions were used, what was done during sessions, and whether patient was alone or with therapist. Ask about perceptions and attitudes about this treatment.) d. Other treatments. (Ask about other thera- 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention pies, including other applied psychophysiological therapies; when, number, and what helped; and patient’s attitudes about these treatments, including either desperation or open-mindedness.) 3. Current treatments. (Psychological and medical. Obtain names and addresses of professionals seen, and ask about attitudes, comfort, expectancies, content, preferences, questions, and plans.) 4. Attitudes about health care professionals, treatments, and symptoms. a. Symptoms. “What do you think is causing your symptoms? Do you think anything has been overlooked?” [This is when one learns of a patient’s beliefs and fears about a cause not yet found.] “What are your thoughts when symptoms start and worsen?” [Cognitive factors.] “How would your life be different without these symptoms or with greatly reduced symptoms?” b. Treatments. “What do you expect from treatments? What have you heard or read about this treatment? What did your physician tell you?” c. Professionals. “What do you think/feel about coming to a [e.g., psychologist]?” 5. Reasons for seeking treatment now. “Why have you come for treatment now? Are your symptoms worse? Have new features? Is your depression worse, or is your job or marriage at risk?” (Is the patient planning life changes, such as pregnancy, that entail a need to stop medications? Is the patient returning to school, changing jobs, getting married, or making other major changes calling for better treatment for the symptoms? Is there another agenda, such as secondary gain, or seeking help as a socially acceptable means of access to the practitioner?) 6. Stressors. (Check past and current stressors in interview and/or questionnaires. Stressor areas include interpersonal, work, schedule overload, perfectionism, procrastination, disorganization, inefficient time use, lack of goals and priorities, family, financial, health, sexual, living conditions, legal, existential.) 7. Emotions. (Observe, ask, and consider measures for depression, anxiety, anger.) 8. Neurocognitive factors. (Observe, review records, ask, and consider assessing for longterm limitations or acquired impairments in memory, attention/concentration, intellec- 229 tual, language, academic achievement. Check for head/brain injuries and surgeries with residual effects.) 9. Physical factors. (Observe, check records. Ask about hearing, vision, and physical limitations.) 10. Dietary and chemical intake. (Check records and ask for past and current use of caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, other vasoactive substances [e.g., tyramine and monosodium glutamate, other stimulants and depressants, gum chewing, other foods and dietary substances, and so-called “street” or “recreational” substances/ drugs; see Block et al., Chapter 10, this volume].) 11. Medications. (Check records. Ask about all prescription and OTC medications, results, and side effects.) 12. Health-promoting behaviors. (Check records. Ask about exercise, time use management, dietary, vacation.) 13. Social support systems. (Check on family and friends, church/synagogue activities, volunteer and other organizational activities. Ask where children and other family members live, their relationships with the patient, frequency of visits with them.) 14. Education and work history (recent and current). 15. Sleep. (Check for at least basics, such as bedtime and awake time, sleep-onset latency, sleep interruptions and durations, sleeping partner’s observations [e.g., snoring, breath stopping, teeth grinding]. Check for feelings after morning awakening and daytime sleepiness. Consider Epworth Daytime Sleepiness questions [Johns, 1991; http://epworthsleepinessscale.com].) 16. Abuse. (Check for physical and sexual abuse in childhood, adulthood. If recorded history or other sources provide insufficient information, consider asking. The intake interview is usually not the time for detailed discussion of this topic or probing unless the patient wants to talk about it then. Gaining this information is a very delicate matter fraught with complex subtleties. Factors guiding whether to obtain this information and how much include presenting symptoms; purposes of the interview; time available; likelihood of seeing the patient again; and your own experience, skills, and comfort. Consider asking, at least, “Did you experience any abuse, sexual or physical, as a child or adolescent?” or “Is it 230 possible that you experienced anything as a child or adolescent that one might consider sexual or physical abuse?” A patient’s equivocal response, such as “Not that I remember,” is a cue to a history of possible abuse. Consider inquiring further, or wait for or create chances for further inquiry in later sessions—for example, “When I asked you about abuse, you said you didn’t think so as far as you could remember. Is there anything you want to tell me or do you have any questions?” [If this leads to the possibility, probability, or confirmation of abuse, consider a consultation with or referral to specially trained and experienced professionals].) 17. Recommendations: Considerations and discussions. (Further evaluation with interview, inventories, self-report measures, or neurocognitive assessment. Referral, psychophysiological assessment, multiple types of relaxation therapies and demonstration, symptom log, biofeedback-assisted therapies. Consider “prudent limited office treatment” (PLOT), stepped-care options, and so forth. Discussion/patient education on varied topics tailored to patient.) Self‑Report Measures as Part of Intake Rationale, Uses, and Issues The usefulness of self-report measures in clinical practice is well established among most practitioners (e.g., Turk & Melzack, 2010). Their use has many advantages, summarized in the list below. These measures can provide information about topics not obtained during interviews and observations. They can shed light on unclear behaviors and provide hypotheses to explain these. They provide quantification and documentation of many variables of interest and are often necessary for reports to other professionals and third-party payers. The usual issues are selection of measures, when and how to use them, interpretation, and costs. Some professionals argue persuasively that there are situations in which it is prudent to administer sets of such measures routinely. Bradley, McDonald-Haile, and Jaworski (1992) state that in their inpatient program, they educate, prepare, and reassure patients before the evaluation that the psychological evaluation [is] part of the medical diagnostic process. In order to reduce patients’ concerns that their symptoms are not viewed as V. PRACTICE ISSUES legitimate . . . they are informed that the psychological assessment is mandatory for all patients . . . performed . . . prior to completion of the medical diagnostic procedures. . . . required to identify interactions between pathophysiological and psychologic[al] processes that affect patients’ physical symptoms, disabilities, and social and familial activities . . . [and] also may suggest interventions that might help to reduce the patients’ suffering.” (p. 194) However, there are clinical situations in which such measures are unnecessary and not costefficient. They sometimes do not add enough to clinical decision making and treatment plans to justify the required time and expense. They also sometimes can interfere with desired rapport and the therapeutic alliance between practitioner and patient. Many practitioners are skilled interviewers and highly experienced clinicians; self-report paper-and-pencil measures often do not provide much more information than such practitioners can gain in a good interview. Let us consider, for example, a consultation to decide the appropriateness of biofeedback for tension-type headaches for a probable work-posturerelated tension myalgia. Let us further assume that this is a consultation with a patient who is resistant to seeing a psychologist. Now consider the potential perceptions and reaction of this patient to a series of self-report mood and personality measures. The decision to proceed or not to proceed with biofeedback and related therapies will be the same, regardless of information gained from paper-andpencil self-report measures. Skilled practitioners can often base such decisions on prior recorded information and an interview. This sounds like an argument against the use of the measures. That is not my point at all; however, I maintain that one must use them prudently and not routinely in all clinical situations. The merits of self-report measures include their ability to do the following: • Document symptoms, personality, beliefs, and behaviors. • Document changes or lack of changes. • Direct practitioners to areas needing more time and effort. • Increase patients’ awareness of their beliefs, behaviors, and personality factors. • Provide a basis for feedback to patients about attending to their beliefs, behaviors, and personality factors. • Correct some self-misperceptions by patients. • Provide cautions for practitioners. 231 14. Intake and Preparation for Intervention • Confirm or disconfirm impressions from interviews. • Correct practitioners’ misperceptions of some patients. • Generate hypotheses about possible problems and treatments. • Assist less experienced practitioners. • Potentially save interviewing and treatment time. • Raise topics, beliefs, and behaviors for discussion. • Select patients needing special attention. The selection of measures is the prerogative of individual practitioners and depends on many factors. A detailed discussion of these factors is beyond the scope of this chapter; a brief list will suffice: • Availability of the measures. • A patient’s motivation and availability for the time needed. • A practitioner’s experience with the measures. • Brevity of the measures and ease of administration and scoring. • Reading level of the measures and reading ability of the patient. • Useful and/or important clinical and treatment plan questions needing information obtainable from the measures. Conclusion This chapter has covered topics and guidelines for selecting whom to treat with biofeedback or other applied psychophysiological interventions (including other physiological self-regulation therapies). It includes topics and guidelines for intake interviewing and patient education. Notes 1. Terms such as “persons” and “individuals” are often used in this chapter rather than relying solely on the terms “patients’ and “clients.” (See Note 4.) 2. The term “intervention” is used in this chapter rather than “therapy,” which was used in prior versions of this chapter in previous editions of this volume. “Intervention” is used commonly in multiple fields (e.g., interventional cardiology), so it is appropriate for those practitioners working in medical and mental health fields, and it works adequately for those working with sports and performing artists, as well as other applications. 3. In this chapter, the term “biofeedback” subsumes both biofeedback and neurofeedback or EEG biofeedback. This convention was adopted in part for practical reasons (i.e., space and readability). 4. Conditions include diagnoses, behavioral habits and patterns of athletes and performing artists, subjects in studies, students, and others. In prior editions of this volume, this chapter referred to “diagnoses” rather than “conditions” because I worked in a medical setting and the vast majority of applications involved diagnoses of patients/ clients, and the applications for sports and performing artists were not so far along in their development. This is no longer the case. 5. Practitioners’ evaluations of patients for therapy with other professionals will differ from their evaluations of patients they themselves intend to see for therapy. 6. “Functional” here signifies the absence of a structural, infectious, or metabolic cause (Berkowitz, 1998). References Adler, C. S., & Adler, S. M. (1984). Biofeedback. In T. B. Karasu (Ed.), The psychiatric therapies: The American Psychiatric Association Commission on Psychiatric Therapies. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Adler, C. S., & Adler, S. M. (1987). Evaluating the psychological factors in headache. In C. S. Adler, S. M. Adler, & R. C. Packard (Eds.), Psychiatric aspects of headache. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Adler, C. S., & Adler, S. M. (1989a). Biofeedback and psychosomatic disorders. In J. V. Basmajian (Ed.), Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Adler, C. S., & Adler, S. M. (1989b). Strategies in general psychiatry. In J. V. Basmajian (Ed.), Biofeedback: Principles and practice for clinicians (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Berkowitz, C. D. (1998). Medical consequences of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22(6), 541–550. Black, M. (1962). Models and metaphor. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Blanchard, E. B., O’Keefe, D., Neff, D., Jurish, S., & Andrasik, F. (1981). Inter-disciplinary agreement in the diagnosis of headache types. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 3, 5–9. Boyd, R. (1979). Metaphor and theory change: What is metaphor for? In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bradley, L. A., McDonald-Haile, J., & Jaworski, T. M. (1992). Assessment of psychological status using interviews and self-report instruments. In D. C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of pain assessment. New York: Guilford Press. Brannon, G. (2011). History and Mental Status Examination. Retrieved from http://emedicine.medscape.com/ article/293402-overview#aw2aab6b3. 232 Cahill, C., Llewelyn, S. P., & Pearson, C. (1991). Treatment of sexual abuse which occurred in childhood: A review. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 1–12. Cannell, J., Hudson, J. I., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2001). Standards for informed consent in recovered memory therapy. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 29, 138–147. Chen, L. P., Hassan, M., Paras, M. L., Colbenson, K. M., Sattler, A. L., Goranson, E. N., et al. (2010). Sexual abuse and lifetime diagnosis of psychiatric disorders: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 85(7), 618–629. Chuang, L., & Forman, N. (2006, April 13). Psychiatric presentation of medical illness: Mental disorders secondary to general medical conditions. Retrieved May 11, 2015, from http://www.nepsychotherapy.com/id3.html. Combs, G., & Freedman, J. (1990). Symbol, story, and ceremony: Using metaphors in individual and family therapy. New York: Norton. Drossman, D., Lagerman, J., Nachman, G., Li, Z., Gluck, H., Toomey T., et al. (1990). Sexual and physical abuse among women with functional and organic gastrointestinal disorders. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 113, 828–833. Hall, R. C. W. (Ed.). (1980). Psychiatric presentation of medical illness. New York: Spectrum. Häuser, W., Kosseva, M., Üceyler, N., Klose, P., & Sommer, C. (2011). Emotional, physical and sexual abuse in fibromyalgia syndrome—a systematic review with metaanalysis. Arthritis Care and Research, 63(6), 808–820. Hersen, M., & Turner, S. M. (1985). Diagnostic interviewing. New York: Plenum Press. Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep, 14(6), 540–545. Lacks, P. (1987). Behavioral treatment for persistent insomnia. New York: Pergamon Press. Levy, R. L. (1987). Compliance and clinical practice. In J. A. Blumenthal & D. C. McKee (Eds.), Application in behavioral medicine and health psychology: A clinician’s source book. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange. Maldonado, J. R. (2009). Neuropsychiatric masquerades: Medical and neurological disorders that present with psychiatric symptoms: Part 1. Presented at 22nd Annual U.S. Psychiatric and Mental Health Congress, Las Vegas, NV. McMullin, R. E. (1986). Handbook of cognitive therapy techniques. New York: Norton. Morin, C. H., & Espie, C. A. (2003). Insomnia: A clinician's guide to assessment and treatment. New York: Springer. V. PRACTICE ISSUES Morris, W., & Morris, M. (1985). Harper dictionary of contemporary usage (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row. Muran, J. C., & DiGiuseppe, R. A. (1990). Towards a cognitive formulation of metaphor use in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology Reviews, 10, 69–85. Othmer, E., & Othmer, S. C. (2002). The clinical interview using DSM-IV-TR (Vol. 1). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Paras, M. L., Murad, M. H., Chen, L. P., Goranson, E. N., Sattler, A. L., Colbenson, K. M., et al. (2009). Sexual abuse and lifetime diagnosis of somatic disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 302(5), 550–561. Petrie, H. G. (1979). Metaphor and learning. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press. Richards, I. A. (1936). The philosophy of rhetoric. London: Oxford University Press. Rosse, R. B., Deutsch, L. H., & Deutsch, S. I. (2000). Medical assessment and laboratory testing in psychiatry. In B. J. Sadock & V. A. Sadock (Eds.), Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (7th ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott/Williams & Wilkins. Scheflin, A. W., & Spiegel, D. (1998). From courtroom to couch: Working with repressed memory and avoiding lawsuits. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 21(4), 847–867. Schwartz, M. S. (1995). Intake decisions and preparations of patients for therapy. In M. S. Schwartz & Associates, Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Shaw, E. R., & Blanchard, E. B. (1983). The effects of instructional set on the outcome of a stress management program. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 8(4), 555–565. Siegelman, E. Y. (1990). Metaphor and meaning in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press. Sommers-Flanagan, J., & Sommers-Flanagan, R. (2008). Clinical interviewing (4th ed.). New York: Wiley. Stott, D., Mansell, W., Salkoskis, P., Lavender, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2010). Oxford guide to metaphors in CBT. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Taylor, R. (2007). Psychological masquerade: Distinguishing psychological from organic disorder (3rd ed.). New York: Springer. Turk, D. C., & Melzack, R. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of pain assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Yucha, C., & Montgomery, D. (2008). Evidence-based practice in biofeedback and neurofeedback. Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Chapter 15 Adherence Jeanetta C. Rains and Mark S. Schwartz “Adherence” generally refers to the concordance between a patient’s behavior and a recommended treatment plan, and is increasingly recognized as moderator of outcome with all forms of treatment. Certainly, patients must cooperate with recommendations if the recommendations are to be effective. In most cases, treatments are validated with specific protocols that maximize effectiveness and minimize complications. Failure to adhere to the recommended protocol is likely to yield an inferior outcome. Evidence from a wealth of medical and psychological literature confirms that nonadherence is almost invariably associated with poorer outcomes across a wide range of disorders and therapies. The World Health Organization (2003) proclaimed that improving patient adherence to existing treatments would have a far greater impact on the health of the population than development of new treatments. Thus, nonadherence acts as a ceiling on effectiveness of all treatments and warrants specific attention in administering and evaluating any form of treatment. In biofeedback therapies, nonadherence may take the form of failure to follow through with initial referral, missed appointments, failure to complete homework or enact lifestyle changes related to treatment, and premature termination or dropout. Nonadherence wastes health care resource (e.g., time, money), portends a poorer therapeutic prognosis for patients, and may be demoralizing for practitioners. It seems counterintuitive that patients with significant and often distressing symptoms would not avail themselves of available treatment. Recent attention has been devoted to development of health behavior models that help explain the paradox of nonadherence. Some of the more widely accepted and compelling models take into account psychological factors such as patient’s self-efficacy and readiness for change, as well as external barriers. Rather than merely carrying out “doctor’s orders,” patients are believed to follow recommendations based on their own implicit cost–benefit analyses in which the necessity of treatment is weighed against concerns about the perceived negative effects or costs. Health behavior theory helps account for the myriad biopsychosocial determinants of adherence and provides a guiding framework for strategies to facilitate adherence. Appreciation of such concepts may assist in optimally tailoring interventions to patient needs through educational, motivational, and behavioral adherence-enhancing strategies. We discuss briefly in this chapter the scope and impact of nonadherence and factors that affect adherence, and we summarize ideas and conclusions from the extensive literature on adherence with medical and psychological treatments. We also presents considerations about conducting a professional practice. This chapter’s guidelines and considerations apply to most settings in which practitioners provide biofeedback. 233 234 Scope of Nonadherence Across chronic conditions such as asthma, arthritis, headache, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and so forth adherence to pharmacological and psychological therapies is known to be poor. A review of the medical literature indicated that nonadherence impacts virtually every aspect of health care (Dunbar-Jacob et al., 2000; LaGreca, Bearman, & Roberts, 2003). Conservatively, 30% of prescriptions are never filled, and only half of prescribed agents are taken sufficiently to achieve a therapeutic effect. Not surprisingly, compliance is poorer for chronic than for acute conditions, and 50 to 60% of persons with chronic conditions are nonadherent with prescribed medication regimens. Failure rates for appointment keeping range from 8 to 63%. Up to 50% of patients on chronic medical regimens drop out of care entirely within 1 year of beginning treatment. Compliance with lifestyle recommendations such as dietary modifications, weight loss, exercise, and smoking cessation occurs less often than adherence to prescribed medication regimens. Nonadherence with Psychological Interventions Though fewer studies have examined adherence to psychological therapies, evidence confirms that adherence to psychological treatment is at least as poor as adherence to pharmacological treatment. A review of three decades of psychotherapy research found that 30–60% of patients drop out of treatment (Reis & Brown, 1999). The modal number of psychotherapy sessions is one single session, and 20–57% of patients do not return after the initial session. The early phase of treatment appears crucial for continuation insofar as dropout rates tend to decline as the number of sessions increases. There is a dose–response relationship between number of sessions and clinical improvement with psychological interventions (Lambert, Hansen, & Finch, 2001) and preliminary evidence for such a relationship in biofeedback. Reiner (2008) observed a dose–response effect between compliance with daily practice with respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) biofeedback and improvement in anxiety, anger, and sleep measures among 20 patients completing 3–4 weeks of treatment for a psychophysiological disorder (e.g., anxiety, irritable bowel syndrome, insomnia). Homework V. PRACTICE ISSUES included daily practice with a portable, handheld RSA biofeedback device. Patients were instructed to practice periodically throughout the day using the device, which awarded points for the desired response. The goal involved achieving the desired response for 20 minutes total per day, or 100 points. A significant positive correlation was observed for adherence (number of points per week) with measures of sleep quality, trait anxiety, and trait anger (p < .01). Likewise, Byrne, Solomon, Young, Rex, and Merlino (2007) found that completion of the full course of treatment (sixsessions) predicted improvement with biofeedback for fecal incontinence in a large sample of 513 patients, although authors did not analyze dose–response effects; compared to those who did not complete treatment, completers exhibited significant improvements in incontinence scores, objective measures of sphincter function, and quality of life. Patients’ reasons for discontinuing treatment have been examined. In a study of 233 patients referred for cognitive-behavioral therapy (Bados, Balaguer, & Saldana, 2007), half of the patients (50.7%) completed the recommended number of sessions. The majority of dropouts occurred in the earlier sessions—a cumulative total 40.4% of patients dropped out after the first or second session (28.1% after the first session). Patients’ reported reasons for dropping out were consistent with earlier research. The most common reasons for leaving therapy were dissatisfaction with therapist or treatment and/or low motivation (46.7%); external factors such as transportation problems, timetables, and competing responsibilities (40%); and the belief that symptoms had improved (13.3%). Thus, establishing a positive therapeutic relationship with patients and enhancing motivation within the initial sessions of treatment (especially the first two sessions) appears essential for continuation and success of treatment. Predictors of Nonadherence Barriers to adherence may be environmental, psychological, interpersonal, and financial. Predictors of dropout from a wide range of psychological interventions have been reviewed elsewhere (DiMatteo, Lepper, & Croghan, 2000; DunbarJacob & Mortimer-Stephens, 2001; Dunbar-Jacob et al., 2000; Lambert et al., 2001; O’Donohue & Levensky, 2006; Reis & Brown, 1999) and presumably generalize to biofeedback. Delays in health care delivery, such as time spent waiting 235 15. Adherence for consultation, tend to decrease engagement in treatment and increase dropout, whereas reminding and re-calling patients have been shown to increase retention. Patient demographic characteristics do not consistently predict dropout, but lower education and income, poorer interpersonal relationships, and lack of social support appear to increase dropout. Experienced therapists appear to have lower rates of dropout. Patient dissatisfaction with therapist/therapy and poor patient–therapist communication appears to increase dropout. Comorbid psychopathology, especially depression, is one of the most important predictors of dropout and nonadherence in the medical literature (e.g., chronic pain, asthma, diabetes). Substance abuse is associated with poorer adherence to both psychological and medical treatment. “Self-efficacy,” or the degree to which a patient believes his or her actions can achieve the desired outcome, has been shown to predict treatment adherence in a wide variety of chronic conditions and is believed to be one of the most potent psychological determinants of adherence identified to date. Model for Health Behavior Change Grounded in the social learning theory of Albert Bandura (1977, 1986), adherence with recommendations for health behavior change is believed to involve three essential constructs: patient’s readiness for change, perceived importance of change, and confidence in one’s ability to change (self-efficacy) (Elder, Ayala, & Harris, 1999; Kinzie, 2005; Miller & Rollick, 2002). Interventions addressing self-efficacy and readiness for change have proven effective in improving adherence. Self‑Efficacy Access to treatment, and skills to carry out such treatment, are not sufficient to ensure that such skills will be performed. Rather, patients must have confidence in their ability to perform the behavior, as well as the expectation that performance of that behavior will result in the desirable outcome. Here, self-efficacy represents the degree to which a person believes he or she can carry out the desired behavior (e.g., attending biofeedback session, completing homework) and that acquiring biofeedback skills will in fact relieve presenting symptoms (e.g., pain, incontinence, anxiety). Patients who doubt the efficacy of treatment or lack confidence in their ability to carry out treatments requirements are less likely to comply. Self-efficacy can be augmented through strategies that draw on past successful experiences and build new skills through modeling and reinforcement of successively more complex adherence behaviors. Four methods are encouraged to augment self-efficacy (Prochaska & Lorig, 2001), generally beginning with verbal persuasion in describing the expected benefits of the treatment, as well as encouraging patients to perform the desired behavior. Biofeedback skills simulated by therapist or model (vicarious experience) or delivered to the patient in small, manageable steps of guaranteed success (performance accomplishment) build selfefficacy. By succeeding with smaller immediate tasks, patients develop a sense of competence to continue and persist through challenges. Finally, teaching patients to expect, identify, and interpret physiological states as part of the change process helps them to persist through symptom fluctuations and exacerbations. Providing a realistic expectation of time and course of change using biofeedback may help patients anticipate plateaus and setbacks as a necessary part of the process and may facilitate perseverance through adverse experiences. Examples of strategies to enhance self-efficacy are embedded in subsequent recommendations. Readiness for Change Patients’ motivations for treatment are dynamic and vary over time. A useful framework for understanding shifting motivations in the behavior change process is the transtheoretical model (Prochaska, Redding, & Evers, 1997), which purports that patients move along a continuum of motivational readiness to change until the behavior ultimately becomes a habit. Change does not necessarily progress in a linear fashion; rather, patients cycle through five stages of change: precontemplation (not thinking about changing behavior), contemplation (actively thinking about changing behavior but not trying to change), preparation (beginning to make changes slowly), action (actively engaged in regular behavior change), and maintenance (maintaining changes). In the maintenance stage, behaviors are performed habitually and require little thought. Relapse is movement from one stage back to a prior stage. Although no published studies to date have been reported with biofeedback, low contemplative ratings on the 236 Stages of Change Scale were associated with premature termination from psychotherapy (Derisley & Reynolds, 2000), and interventions directed at each of these processes have proven effective in improving adherence. Providers may recognize patients’ stage of readiness for change and tailor their educational, motivational, and behavioral strategies accordingly. For example, patients referred for biofeedback may be in the stage of precontemplation and have no consideration of altering their own behaviors. Here, education linking patients own behaviors with symptoms is needed, and any efforts at introducing biofeedback skills acquisition are likely to be lost in this stage. Patients in the contemplation stage of change warrant education about options and also may benefit from social support and encouragement. Here biofeedback can be introduced with education describing the manner in which learned control over physiological responses can be gained through training. Those in preparation for change stage warrant clear, step-by-step behavioral instruction, modeling, discussion of barriers, and close follow-up. The action stage while carrying out biofeedback warrants the closest assessment of appointment and homework adherence. Reinforcement is useful in maintaining the behavior through barriers until the benefits of treatment (symptom relief or functional improvement) can emerge to maintain the behavior. Maintenance of treatment gains includes continued enhancement of self-efficacy and relapse prevention. Adherence Facilitation The ability to engage and motivate patients enhances the effectiveness of health care professionals, regardless of the treatment’s efficacy and the professional’s knowledge or good intentions. Patients do not comply for a variety of reasons, and it is useful to attempt to understand their unique cost–benefit perception for requirements of treatment and tailor efforts to address misunderstandings, competing motivations, and barriers. The following recommendations are based on evidence primarily gleaned from parallel literatures and clinical experience. Strategies for adherence facilitation generally involve preparatory techniques and motivational enhancement. Recommendations are divided into three major categories addressing (1) the professional, (2) the patient, and (3) evaluation and intervention. V. PRACTICE ISSUES The Professional Professional Setting, and Nontherapy and Therapy Personnel Personnel who are friendly, efficient, and professional in their appearance and behaviors affect patients’ impressions, comfort, confidence, satisfaction, and compliance. Comfortable, neat, and uncluttered office rooms, with comfortable temperatures, also probably help. Short waiting times and consistency of therapists promote adherence. Likewise, reminding and re-calling patients should be a integrated in clinic administration, since failure to keep appointments is significant in psychological interventions and acts as a ceiling on all future treatment and adherence efforts. With up to one-half of patients failing to return for treatment after consultation (Reis & Brown, 1999), this would appear to be the single greatest opportunity to improve adherence. Calling patients to remind them of appointments and re-calling those who miss a scheduled appointment are fundamentally the most cost-effective, adherence-enhancing strategies. Although not studied in biofeedback, additional simple strategies have been shown across other chronic disorders to improve appointment keeping and include reminders (mail, telephone), clinic orientations, and contracting with patients (Macharia, Leon, Rowe, Stephenson, & Haynes, 1992). The Referral Source’s Attitudes and Behaviors A well-informed referral source can be an ally for treatment. Practitioners should consider discussing with referral sources their viewpoints about biofeedback and related therapies. This helps a practitioner know how much attention to direct toward building a patient’s confidence in the therapy rationale, recommendations, and procedures. Formal feedback to referral sources is likely to increase continuity of care and reinforce the referral process. Therapists should consider sending educational reading materials and including useful, informative, and sensible content in letters to referral sources. Well-written letters to referral sources, and well-written notes in patients’ records, influence referral sources’ attitudes and behaviors. One can avoid both writing a long report or letter and repeating what the reader already knows about the patient. A sample letter to a referral source (Figure 15.1) may be supplemented with data and graphs. Another option is to send a copy of the intake evaluation and therapy session notes. 237 15. Adherence Dear Dr. , Thank you for referring Ms. for treatment of her tension headaches. I will not repeat her history, of which you are aware. My evaluation of her head and neck muscle activity used four sites: bifrontal, bilateral posterior neck, and right and left frontal posterior neck. Baseline measures were taken with her eyes open and closed while she was sitting with her back and head supported, and while she was standing. The assessment involved rest and mild office stressors. The procedures then included feedback to measure her response and begin to develop her physiological self-regulation. Muscle activity from the neck while she was sitting with her eyes open showed excess muscle activity, mostly 4 to 6 microvolts (100- to 200-hertz bandpass). While standing and trying to relax, she showed higher excessive muscle activity, mostly 9 to 12 microvolts. Muscle activity from the other sites was only slightly tense for resting muscles while she was sitting, and only slightly higher while she was standing. Visual feedback helped her reduce muscle activity, especially while standing. Without the feedback, muscle activity increased and remained elevated. I discussed the rationale for therapy and the procedures. Evaluation of psychosocial factors did not suggest enough to warrant other forms of stress management, and she was not receptive to other forms of stress management. I provided audiotapes and patient education booklets to her. She was seen for five more sessions. She is working on weaving physiological selfregulation into her daily activities. She continues to increase her ability to lower muscle activity during resting conditions, and to maintain lower muscle activity after feedback stops. In the last two sessions, her muscle activity during some phases was in a therapeutic range below 2 microvolts. Her symptom log for the last 4 weeks shows a 75% reduction in severe headaches, a 50% reduction in total hours of headaches, and an 80% decrease in medication use compared to her initial reports. Thank you for referring this pleasant lady. I am happy to be of help to her. Contact me with any questions. Sincerely yours, FIGURE 15.1. Sample letter for referral source. The Practitioner’s Characteristics and Behaviors Patient satisfaction with the therapist/treatment and the therapist experience have been associated with improved adherence. Credibility of practitioners is enhanced by professional presentation, as well as the amount and quality of time spent with patients. Our appearance, behaviors, and personality all affect credibility and trustworthiness. Practitioners increase and maintain patients’ trust in them by being on time and maintaining confidentiality and consistency. They realistically discuss therapy goals and expectations. They also discuss expected and possible changes in therapy well in advance. Effective communication and the appearance of a relaxed therapist, or one who effectively shows relaxation when needed, conveys an important teaching model. The therapist might consider self-disclosure about how he or she has used physiological self-regulation to prevent and manage symptoms. Communication should be logical and coherent in order to be understood, accepted, and remembered. Effective communication must also be within the patient’s “latitude of acceptance.” A provider with a rigid conceptual framework and a rigid approach to therapy may not evoke compliance from patients whose attitudes are outside the limits of the information and therapy plan. Patients’ attitudes must be taken into account when therapists are framing explanations about the causes of their symptoms, as well as descriptions of the therapy rationale, procedures, and recommendations. Both the wording and the content of these explanations and descriptions must be considered. For example, therapists must consider how such phrases as “letting go” and a “nonstriving 238 attitude” sounds to many patients. Similarly, some recommendations (e.g., changing work schedules) are outside the acceptance range for some patients. A collaborative relationship and enlisting patients active participation in setting goals is likely to result in a more positive interaction and realistic treatment plan. The therapeutic relationship is the cornerstone for effective therapy. Table 15.1 outlines recommendations for interaction between practitioner and patient. The professional should clearly explain the role of each factor and develop a flexible stepped-care approach. Flexibility, laced with clear communication and empathy, is often more likely to lead to acceptance than rigid insistence. Consider the following sample explanation to a patient: “Mrs. , we talked about the potential advantages of stopping caffeine, managing your time use more effectively, and using relaxation therapies. Caffeine interTABLE 15.1. Recommendations for Interaction between the Practitioner and Patient 1. Attend to your personal characteristics and behaviors. 2. Spend enough time with the patient. 3. Provide an active interaction. 4. Acknowledge the legitimacy of the patient’s complaints. 5. Present an organized, systematic, and flexible approach. 6. Include appropriate, but limited, social conversation. 7. Provide reassurance, support, and encouragement 8. Provide and reinforce realistic positive expectations. 9. Provide choices for the patient. 10. Allow the patient to question recommendations. 11. Tailor therapy whenever indicated and practical. 12. Demonstrate and model selected procedures. 13. Provide appropriate self-disclosure. 14. Show attention and interest in the patient through tone of voice, facial expression, and physical posture. 15. Convey appropriate affect. 16. Maintain frequent eye contact. 17. Touch the patient appropriately. 18. Observe for signs of anxiety, resistance, and confusion. V. PRACTICE ISSUES feres with effective relaxation. More effective use of time will help you make time for enough relaxation to help you reduce anxiety and tension in your life. I know you might not feel ready for some of these changes. I am not saying that all of them are completely necessary for you to reduce or stop your symptoms. However, some are necessary. Seriously consider these recommendations. The decision about what to do, both now and later, is up to you. You can start with for a trial, and then see how far you get.” Tailoring Therapy: Providing Choices for the Patient Although there may be a place for predesigned therapy programs, tailoring therapy to patients’ individual situations and preferences is almost invariably needed. Tailoring also includes consideration of the patient’s attitudes, schedule, and finances. In addition, the therapist considers the symptoms during the first days and weeks of therapy and the first few biofeedback sessions. Having choices, and knowing the potential advantages and disadvantages of each, gives a patient the sense of actively participating in the design of his or her program. Such an approach also conveys that the practitioner is considering the patient’s situation, preferences, and needs. Consider these factors: 1. Symptoms sometimes improve after a physician reassures a patient about the nonserious nature of his or her symptoms. 2. Starting a new medication or changing dosage can improve symptoms before physiological effects of the change may be expected. 3. Making changes in lifestyle, work, exercises, and/or diet can result in a significant decrease in symptoms. (Note. In these first three examples, compliance with a time-consuming treatment will often be less than ideal, especially if the patient believes that the symptom changes result from these factors. In such circumstances, tailoring the therapy plan can involve deferring more biofeedback.) 4. Another factor that alerts practitioners to the need for tailoring is a lack of physiological tension and arousal, and/or a rapid return to a “therapeutic range”1 after intentional arousal during office sessions. This suggests that the patient can relax but needs to apply his or her ability to do so. 239 15. Adherence 5. Some patients show significant reduction of symptoms in the first weeks of therapy, regardless of the physiological self-regulation shown in the office. It is sometimes difficult to explain rapid improvement of symptoms. The scientist in each of us wants to know why and how this is occurring; the skeptical and cautious part of us is suspicious. However, the clinician in us, especially the cost-conscious and pragmatic part, accepts the progress and may defer more office therapy. If the symptoms increase later, patients may better accept the role of the therapy and the need for compliance. 6. Therapists should consider deferring or stopping office-based biofeedback sessions with patients who are nonadherent with necessary parts of therapy so as not to associate the failure experience with therapy and inoculate the patient against future treatments. 7. Assessment and feedback sites may need tailoring. For example, a patient may perceive repeated sessions semireclined, with eyes closed, and with feedback from only one area as meaningless. Tailoring the sites, body positions, and conditions can make the sessions more sensible to the patient and increase the patient’s confidence and compliance. 8. Physiological arousal in response to cognitive stressors and activities offers data with which to tailor therapy. For example, let us suppose that a patient shows good relaxation in multiple muscle areas, warm hands, and low skin conductance during resting baselines. Furthermore, suppose that the patient can do this during a standard cognitive stressor and in different positions. Now let us suppose that this patient has significantly decreased finger temperatures while imagining or talking about work or family stress. The therapist may consider repeating the arousal scenes several times, with and without feedback, and exploring the content. Or the therapist may encourage relaxation before, during, and immediately after work situations. 9. Long geographic distance between the patient and the treatment office require tailoring of the therapy. Office sessions twice a day for 2 or more consecutive days, with a few weeks between such phases, may be preferable for some patients who live too far away for weekly sessions. Such a “massed-practice” schedule conveys that the therapist is willing to extend him- or herself, and may help increase patient compliance. Providing Appropriate Self‑Disclosure Limited, appropriate self-disclosure (Table 15.1, item 13) includes brief and proper descriptions of how practitioners effectively use applied psychophysiology in their own lives. In this manner, therapists can communicate that they know firsthand that this works and potentially enhance patients’ self-efficacy through vicarious experience. Professional Verbal and Nonverbal Behavior The practitioner’s voice, facial expressions, body posture, and nonverbal behaviors all convey interest, trust, sincerity, experience, and confidence (Table 15.1, items 14 and 17). Likewise, touch is important. Psychologists and others providing biofeedback do not typically use physical contact in their other contacts with patients. Even those with experience may not know how to use touch to convey sincerity, support, and encouragement. There are several obvious chances to use touch properly, aside from the initial handshake. These include times when the therapist is attaching electrodes and other transducers to a patient, and when he or she is directly helping in relaxation or muscle reeducation. For example, the therapist might consider how he or she moves a patient’s hair and grasps the patient’s arm. It can be enlightening for the practitioner to ask him- or herself, “How would this contact feel if I were the patient?” The practitioner can gently but firmly place a hand on the patient’s forearm, and may give a mild squeeze or brief pat on the arm, but for no longer than about 2 seconds. A therapist must always be sensitive about contact; some patients may not like or may distrust such contact. However, if not overdone in intensity or frequency, the technique can have positive effects. Other chances for reassuring with touch include occasions when patients express frustration, fear, life stress, and difficulty with physiological self-regulation. Notably, touch should not be overdone, insofar as touch can have a negative effect. For example, consider the possible impressions of female patients touched by male practitioners. I am not suggesting that one should avoid such contacts altogether. However, the duration and frequency of the contacts can convey the wrong message. Consider the difference between a possible undesirable message from a touch of about 240 5 seconds, and a desirable one of about 2 seconds. If the practitioner’s hands are cold, moist, or both, he or she should consider touching clothed parts of the arm and not bare skin. Cognitive Preparation of the Patient Cognitive preparation includes (1) the rationale for physiological self-regulation, (2) therapy process, (3) therapy goals, (4) use of medications, (5) generalization, (6) therapy options, (7) stepped care, and (8) a symptom log. It may be helpful to tailor this information to patient’s apparent stage of readiness for change. A therapist should try to avoid overloading a patient during one session. Some information may need presentation during the first few sessions, then be repeated later for emphasis and to help patients recall. Carefully developed patient education scripts, booklets, checklists, and audiotapes help. Because patients often forget much of what they hear, therapists should consider repeating key points. Most patients will have at least some compliance problems. Shaping acceptance of self-responsibility is one of the challenging aspects of clinical practice. Cognitive preparation also includes expecting slow progress, plateaus, and setbacks. Practitioners want to communicate realistic positive expectations. It can be useful to show patients graphs of developing physiological self-regulation and symptom changes from prior patients. In addition, it can be helpful to tell a patient something like the following: “You have a good chance of making progress in reducing or stopping your symptoms. I do not know how long this will take. Some patients show much improvement within days or weeks. Others progress gradually over several weeks or a few months. Plateaus and even temporary reversals happen. If they occur, remember that they are normal and a natural part of learning. You need not feel discouraged. You know athletes and musicians expect unevenness in developing and keeping their skills. Also, keep in mind that even accomplished athletes have off days. Not even a great baseball player, golfer, or tennis player always hits the ball well.” Patients often display resistance, skepticism, and pessimism. Often they have gone to several doctors and have tried various treatments. They have had positive expectations but have been disappointed with the results. For some patients, our treatment V. PRACTICE ISSUES approach appears too simplistic, despite careful explanations and professional reputations. If their healthy skepticism leads to our being defensive or rejecting of them, then we unnecessarily risk dropout. Checking and discussing patient’s perceptions may be needed to explain the rationale for a therapeutic trial, discuss options, and gently and empathetically overcome patient resistance. It is too easy to give up and label such patients as unsuitable for therapy. For such patients, the greater challenge to their therapists is to establish a therapeutic alliance and mobilize these patients realistically. In addition, the challenge is to shape their attitudes and perceptions of the therapy. Thus, therapists shape patients’ self-confidence, their optimism, and their willingness to engage in a realistic therapy trial. A therapist should consider an empathetic and nondefensive response: “If I put myself in your place, I would be skeptical too. I know you went to several doctors and tried several treatments without success. I understand that you were hopeful and then disappointed. Part of you is asking yourself, ‘Why is this going to be any different?’ You do not want to get your hopes up, because you do not want that disappointment again. I understand that, and I think it is perfectly normal. You know, I cannot promise that you will improve or tell you how much improvement you will have. However, I can tell you that many patients treated did well, despite being unsuccessful with past therapies. Thousands of professionals all over the country report the same experiences with their patients. There are several approaches we can take. If the first is less than ideal, then there are variations and other approaches. Biofeedback and related therapies involve many therapies. Ask me any questions you wish, or express any concerns and doubts you may have. Some parts of the therapy may appear to you as too simple to work. I do not want them to appear complicated. They are not as simple as they appear, but neither are they very complicated. Even long-term symptoms often do not require complex solutions. For example, I treated many patients with [this patient’s symptoms] for years. [Practitioners can also insert their personal experiences.] These treatments helped most of them. Many of those patients thought these treatments were probably not enough for them, yet they got better. Relaxation and biofeedback treatments are often enough alone. However, sometimes we also need other therapies.” 241 15. Adherence The Patient A patient’s perceptions, expectations, and mood impact therapy. The list below provides a catalogue of many specific patient perceptions, expectations, affective and symptom-related factors, and other factors. All of these can affect compliance and therapy results. This list should be considered during evaluation, cognitive preparation, and treatment. Perceptions of Biofeedback/Relaxation Patients May Perceive Biofeedback/ Relaxation as Psychological Treatment Because mental health professionals commonly provide biofeedback-assisted relaxation, it is understandable that many patients perceive biofeedback as a psychological approach. Many health care professionals also consider biofeedback a psychological technique. However, these therapies are multidisciplinary and not uniquely or exclusively within the province of psychology. When patients reject or fear stigma of “psychological” therapies, the therapist should explain that many professionals do not view biofeedback and relaxation as psychological treatments. Such a discussion is pertinent when treating patients with medical disorders such as migraine. One may not need to discuss the multidisciplinary nature of biofeedback with patients who voluntarily seek mental health help. Furthermore, nurses, physical therapists, occupational therapists, and other non-mentalhealth professionals providing or supervising these therapies need not have this type of discussion with patients. A mental health professional might consider portraying physiological self-regulation and biofeedback as having many unique features, in addition to any psychological aspects. Patients May Perceive Biofeedback/ Relaxation as Insufficient, Useless, or a Waste of Time Prior unsuccessful therapies or exposure to diluted forms of therapy (e.g., self-administered biofeedback tools, relaxation tapes) can diminish the credibility of self-regulation therapies. Patients may harbor these perceptions yet not openly express them. A corollary of the insufficiency/ uselessness perception is that relaxation is simply a waste of time. Such a perception is especially common among medical patients who have chronic physical symptoms. A therapist should consider showing awareness of these perceptions, for example, by saying: “You may be thinking, ‘How is this therapy going to help me? I have had these symptoms so long and tried so many treatments. I got my hopes up before and the treatments did not help. How is this going to be different? I have a busy schedule. I am having trouble believing this is not a waste of time.’ If you are having thoughts like this, let’s discuss them.” The therapist can then explain the rationale for therapy and discuss how it can help the patient, despite the chronicity of his or her symptoms and any prior unsuccessful treatments. Time spent in the present treatment can be described as an investment. Compliance with homework assignments is often crucial within many therapies, including applied psychophysiology. Reviews are available elsewhere (Kazantzis, 2000; Scheel, Hanson, & Razzhavaikina, 2004). Most studies support the role of homework adherence and improvement in therapy, including the dose–response relationship observed between RSA biofeedback homework and outcome describer earlier (Reiner, 2008), whereas other studies have not found such support. Generally, homework in biofeedback facilitates generalization of skills, and many therapists consider completion of homework to be a barometer of the patient’s motivation and commitment to therapy. Homework assignments should have a dedicated purpose and not be burdensome to the patient. When homework assignments are made, the documents should be reviewed in session and patients reinforced for adherence. Homework is discussed in a later section of this chapter. Patients May Perceive Biofeedback/ Relaxation as the Last Hope Patients who have tried several therapies without success may perceive relaxation and biofeedback as therapies of “last resort.” This perception is especially common among patients seen in tertiary medical centers and considered refractory. As with other misperceptions, patients are usually hesitant to report this perception spontaneously. Such a perception is an added source of anxiety. It can interfere with patients’ attending to what professionals present and complying with recommendations. The increased tension and symptoms that sometimes result can lead to more frustra- 242 tion and discouragement. Some patients then consider giving up entirely and dropping out of therapy. Providers should emphasize that different therapies and approaches are available. There are lifestyle changes, dietary changes, cognitive stress management therapies, other stress management approaches, and various combinations of these. This information can significantly alter the perception of biofeedback as the therapy of last resort. Patients May Perceive Therapy as Preprogrammed and May Resist Such a Program Practitioners sometimes provide biofeedback in preprogrammed packages, with a standard number of sessions, placements of transducers, body positions, and treatment conditions. There are often specific physiological criteria for proceeding to the next stage or changing to a different strategy. Some patients (and practitioners) resist preprogrammed packages and prefer tailored therapy. Therapists need to be aware of these potential perceptions and adjust their treatments accordingly. Patients May Perceive That Other Therapies Are Needed, Based on Prior Medical Consultations Physicians with whom a patient has consulted have probably discussed and recommended therapy options. These include new or different medications, dosage changes, surgery, or psychotherapy. Before pursuing these other therapies, the patient has come to a provider who uses biofeedback and related therapies. However, the patient often perceives the other options as viable and possibly effective. The biofeedback therapist should uncover these perceptions and deal with them as soon as it is practical to do so. Patients May Perceive Aspects of Biofeedback/Relaxation as Silly or Embarrassing Patients may fail to understand certain aspects of relaxation, such as tensing and releasing facial muscles, diaphragmatic breathing, listening to audiotaped relaxation, and relaxing in public places. Patients rarely volunteer such perceptions and feelings. The next question is how to revise them. Some practitioners and therapists model some aspects of biofeedback and relaxation. A therapist’s self-disclosure that he or she uses these V. PRACTICE ISSUES procedures in daily life can be reassuring. Some patients also may feel more comfortable once they know that many professionals, executives, athletes, entertainers, and others also often use these procedures. Providing a credible rationale for, and explaining the application of, each procedure can help to put it in better perspective as well. Patients May Perceive the Therapy as Too Costly, Impractical, or Time Consuming Some patients’ perceptions that the costs and duration of treatment are beyond their capabilities may undermine adherence. Solving this problem is not easy, but a stepped-care model would adjust the number of office sessions. The therapeutic relationship can help the patient accept the costs and duration of therapy. A practitioner can support reduced fees when standard fees are a hardship for a patient, when there is a clear need for therapy, and when fee reduction is allowable. Such humane and generous adjustments result in appreciation and can increase compliance. However, fee adjustments have become more difficult in recent years because of reimbursement problems throughout the health care system. Patients May Perceive the Therapy Procedures, Logs, and Practice as Burdensome Patients must relate treatment demands and procedures to symptom reduction. Otherwise, they may perceive treatment as being too complex or irrelevant for them. Self-monitoring and practice should be tailored to patients’ symptoms and lifestyle. Impractical and complex logs can lead to a lack of records, contrived data, or withdrawal from therapy. Biofeedback/relaxation time commitments can take time away from patients’ other activities. Taking an hour or more daily for applying recommendations—which therapists often suggest—is more than some patients can do. Of course, when patients’ schedules are very full, there is often a greater need for balance and therapy. However, patients may not perceive their situations in this way. Examples of people whose schedules do not permit time for ideally applying treatments are farmers, tax accountants, and other seasonal workers whose schedules change significantly. In such cases, therapists need to be flexible and wait rather than “beating their [professional] heads against a stone wall.” Shorter daily relaxation sessions, and/or instructions for blend- 243 15. Adherence ing therapy activities into daily activities, should be considered. Another strategy would be to defer some treatment and focus therapy on altering the patient’s schedule and priorities—in other words, to practice “time use therapy.” For example, some people need to delegate, limit, or learn to do some tasks with less perfection. Patients May Mistrust Health Care Professionals Some patients have learned to mistrust health care providers. Such distrust often stems from negative personal or vicarious experiences. Many such patients have experienced mistreatment, have been misled, or have received treatment from insensitive providers. Professionals who have added to the negative perceptions of patients may have provided inadequate relaxation, biofeedback, and associated therapies. For example, many patients react negatively to being alone for most or all sessions. These patients report feeling abandoned, anxious, confused by what to do, and frustrated. These feelings have added to their negative perception of biofeedback and of professionals offering such services. Professionals can modulate such experiences by the quality of interactions with their patients. They should inquire about prior experiences if the patients do not volunteer the information. Some patients candidly describe their negative experiences. Others either sit quietly with distrustful looks on their faces or provide no obvious clues of their distrust. Still others simply do not want to be in therapists’ offices or follow therapists’ advice—no matter who the therapists are, what they say, and how pleasant they are. Patients May Not Perceive Their Practitioners as Allies Even patients without negative experiences may not perceive practitioners as allies in the battle against symptoms. We may be credible and even highly competent. However, patients may see us as too formal, too distant, not devoted enough, or too busy to provide the time they perceive they need. We need to get out from behind our desks and our formality, and convey that we do care about patients as people, not just as patients or cases. It is difficult for some professionals to adopt Will Rogers’s dictum, “I never met a man [or woman] I didn’t like.” However, it helps to keep striving to show our liking for all patients . . . perhaps with a few exceptions! Patients May Perceive Their Symptoms as Organic and Requiring Solely Biological Therapy A patient’s perception may be that “if only my doctors believed me and did more tests, they would find the cause of my problems.” Such patients continue to believe that an organic cause is the major factor explaining their symptoms. They may hold this perception despite the fact that highly expert medical examinations and laboratory tests have ruled out an organic explanation for the symptoms. Patients often limit compliance in such cases. Adherence necessitates that patients accept the rationale and believe that stress, tension, or arousal may cause or worsen their symptoms. They need to believe that organic factors are minor or nonexistent, or that they can be overcome. Superficial compliance with the mechanics of therapy may still occur; however, these patients only “go through the motions.” They often wait for another chance to get more medical tests. They may even view such compliance as a chance to show that the symptoms indeed are organic. They do this by failing to improve. Such a situation is typically very difficult to manage. Such patients often go to highly credible tertiary medical centers. Resulting examinations and tests sometimes do find organic disease explanations for some patients’ symptoms. Ruling out organic causes by the best credible medical examinations can help patients accept physiological self-regulation and related therapies. It is often wiser to defer applied psychophysiological therapies if a patient does not yet sufficiently accept these therapies. Otherwise, one risks souring the patient’s experience with failure from a treatment that might later be successful when the patient is more accepting. However, the presence of some patient skepticism does not prevent starting applied psychophysiological therapies. Patients May Perceive Symptoms as Out of Their Control Patients may believe that their symptoms are beyond their control, even if they accept a functional or psychophysiological explanation and believe that psychophysiological therapies can ameliorate their symptoms. Such patients have been described as “precontemplative,” because they do not perceive a relationship between their behavior and symptoms and therefore have not even contemplated changing their behavior. Other patients may lack sufficient self-efficacy; while 244 they do see the relationship between behavior and symptoms, they lack the confidence to change. They also may perceive the intensity or chronicity of their symptoms to be so severe that these therapies cannot possibly work for them. A skilled clinician can convince patients of the potential for help, despite the chronicity and severity of their symptoms, and can mobilize these patients to comply enough to achieve successful results. Patients May Fear Using Passive Therapies or Loss of Control with Relaxation Therapy Some patients perceive relaxation therapies as tantamount to becoming passive or losing control; this is a threatening perception for certain patients. Therapists should look for fear as the cause when patients are avoiding relaxation practice. Such patients need to be reassured that relaxation is in fact an active response that increases rather than decreases self-control. Therapists can try briefer periods of relaxation, or suggest that patients periodically raise themselves out of the relaxed state. The latter can reassure patients that they can do this anytime the need arises. Patients May Perceive a Lack of Cooperation from Significant Others Social support is known to facilitate adherence, and participation of significant others may facilitate treatment. Some patients believe that these people will not be understanding, accepting, and cooperative. Often, such perceptions are accurate. Therapists should consider four options. First, give patient education booklets and/or tapes to patients and encourage them to share these with significant others to gain their understanding, acceptance, and cooperation. Second, consider directly contacting the other people, with the patient’s permission. This option can be pursued when cooperation from the other people is necessary but the patient is not getting it. Third, suggest that the patient use relaxation procedures only when other people are not around. This is the least desirable option. Fourth, consider enlisting others to give direct cooperation and help to the patient. For example, others can take care of some household or work responsibilities, reduce noise, and answer phone calls for a few minutes. Others can remind the patient about body postures and relaxation. However, cooperation and help may threaten some patients’ perceptions of self-suffi- V. PRACTICE ISSUES ciency. Some patients prefer to conduct their therapy without help from other people. Others have a strong preference and capability for independence and a good history of self-discipline. Patients May Have Unrealistic Positive or Negative Expectations Unrealistic negative expectations often interfere with compliance, as noted in several of the contexts discussed earlier. However, unrealistic positive expectations are also common and involve expecting greater or faster benefit than usual. Unrealistic positive expectations can result in disappointment when the expectations do not match reality. Cognitive preparation includes promoting realistic positive expectations. Patients May Have Had a Prior Inadequate Biofeedback/Relaxation Trial and Expect Failure Patients may perceive that biofeedback/relaxation therapy is inadequate because they did not have successful results with it in the past. However, the prior therapy may have been less than ideal. Reversing negative expectations requires extra care, especially if one is to avoid disparaging the prior practitioner. A therapist should consider saying something like the following: “The treatment you received in the past sounds like what many professionals provide. Some professionals devote more time to developing specialized knowledge, skills, and procedures. Others are less specialized. Some professionals are not in places that allow them to get preferred instruments. Most practitioners mean well, but often they do not know what they do not know. There is much we can add to the therapy you had in the past. This can be important and helpful for you.” Patients May Be Reluctant to Speak Candidly about Psychological, Interpersonal, and Other Stressful Matters Some patients view a referral for relaxation and biofeedback therapy as more “face-saving” than a referral for psychological evaluation. These patients sometimes expect that psychological topics will not be a focus of the evaluation and treatment. They are often reluctant to speak candidly about psychological matters that are of potential 245 15. Adherence importance in the formulation of an effective treatment program. For some of them, practitioners can skirt such topics, at least in the early sessions. It is often sufficient to focus on physiological self-regulation therapies, reducing or stopping the use of chemical stressors, and making related changes. Symptoms May Be Reinforcing and Symptom Relief May Be Perceived as Threatening Symptoms can provide reinforcement or secondary gain, despite the discomfort and impairment they cause. Some patients may find significant symptom relief to be threatening. Practitioners often struggle with ways to discuss this delicate topic with patients. Discreet practitioners consider using examples when discussing this topic and checking for the possible role of this factor in maintaining patients’ symptoms and in compliance problems. For instance, persons with chronic obesity (often from their adolescence onward) may encounter resistance from significant others or find themselves in unfamiliar heterosexual situations when they lose considerable weight and their figures approach normal size. They may not believe that they have the interpersonal skills and stress management strategies to adjust to such conditions. They may or may not be aware of the approach–avoidance conflict; in either case, they will probably be reluctant to discuss their feelings spontaneously. A common result is nonadherence with a weight loss program. Patients May Not Want to Stop Using Vasoactive Dietary Chemicals, Chewing Gum, and/or Taking Unnecessary Medications Many patients accept recommendations to eliminate caffeine, chewing gum, and other vasoactive chemicals. However, they may resist stopping nicotine, alcohol, and unnecessary medications. Practitioners can advise patients of these substances’ potential negative effects on treatment, and explain the physiological effects of caffeine, nicotine, and other chemicals. They try to persuade patients to avoid such chemicals during at least the hour or so before relaxation and biofeedback sessions. The effects of gum chewing on temporalis muscles are not clear to most patients; hence, there is a need for patient education and demonstration. A general statement to patients about these substances may be something like the following: “I know your symptoms are very distressing to you, and you want to reduce or stop them. I’ve explained how interferes with therapies and self-regulation of your body. I know you want to make progress as fast as is practical. You probably want to limit the number of office sessions you need. Continuing to consume these chemicals can detract from your progress and prolong therapy. The decision about whether you stop them or not is yours. However, I do not want you to waste your time and money. I will do everything I can to help you withdraw from and stop these chemicals. Please give this some thought, and we can discuss this further.” Patients with Impaired Neurocognitive Functioning Impaired neurocognitive functioning often results in patients’ inability to remember instructions and therapy recommendations, and to attend to therapy tasks. Practitioners can sometimes successfully treat such patients with help from cooperative persons living with the patients. Evaluation and Intervention Patient engaging in biofeedback should be encouraged to self-monitor progress. Recommended self-report outcome measures are listed in Table 15.2. Self-monitoring homework conveys the importance of the information requested, as well as the intent of the therapist to review this information. However, requesting complicated record keeping can be counterproductive. Patients’ records need not be 100% accurate for practitioners to obtain useful information. A log is often enough to allow practitioners to check adherence and stimulate discussion. Some portion of each office session should be devoted to reviewing the log. Clinicians may utilize simple questions to elicit useful information,. For example, consider the value of the response “yes” to the following questions: • “Are you practicing your relaxation?” • “Are you practicing your relaxation as instructed?” • “Are you feeling relaxed when you use the relaxation?” • “Are you practicing your relaxation daily?” • “Are you practicing your relaxation at various times each day?” 246 TABLE 15.2. Recommended Methods for Assessing Adherence with Biofeedback Homework 1. Patient self-monitoring of frequency and duration of relaxation practice, and times when relaxation is used. 2. Self-report log of symptoms. 3. Self-monitoring of subjective physiological sensations associated with relaxation. 4. Self-monitoring of cognitions associated with relaxation. 5. Self-monitoring of caffeine, nicotine, and other chemical use. 6. Self-monitoring of physiological parameters (e.g., skin temperature, pulse, blood pressure) before, during, and after relaxation sessions. 7. Practitioner interviews. 8. Periodic psychophysiological assessments. 9. Practitioner observation of a patient’s breathing and body postures. 10. Reports from other people in a patient’s daily life. 11. Patients’ motor functioning. 12. Patients’ daily activities. These and similar questions are inadequate alone to elicit useful information. Patients can answer such questions with a “yes” or “no,” but they may be interpreting the questions in ways they prefer, which are not always the ways the practitioner intends. For instance, when patients say “yes” to “practicing daily,” does this really mean “daily,” or does it mean “most days” or just “some days”? Limited time to question patients in detail, or limited interviewing skills, can result in not enough information for assessing compliance and progress. If there is no self-report log or if it is incomplete, the therapist might consider the following examples of interview questions. Open-ended questions may yield greater information. One might start by saying, “Let’s review your relaxation practice. As accurately as you can recall, please tell me how often and when you are using relaxation procedures [or other procedures].” Depending on the completeness of the patient’s response to such a question, the therapist might consider more specific questions: • “How many times each day, during the past week, did you relax for 15–20 minutes each? How many brief relaxations of 2–10 minutes each?” V. PRACTICE ISSUES • “Are there days on which you were not been able to relax? How many days?” • “Let’s talk about those days. What are the problems?” • “How do you feel during your relaxation sessions? What sensations are you experiencing during relaxation? What do you experience after your relaxation sessions? How long do those sensations and feelings last?” • “In what situations did you use relaxation during the past week?” • “When could you benefit from relaxation but are not using it?” Therapists should avoid misusing and misinterpreting physiological measures during office sessions to assess adherence. It is reasonable to assume that warmer baseline skin temperatures, faster increases in skin temperature, lower baseline muscle activity, and/or faster drops of muscle activity reflect good relaxation experiences between office sessions. However, a therapist cannot assume that these are reliable indicators of compliance. Improved baseline psychophysiological functioning in office sessions can reflect increasing comfort or habituation to the office, instrumentation, and the therapist. Psychophysiological data are often consistent with verbal reports of adherence with relaxation, reduction of undesired chemicals, and/or other lifestyle changes. In such cases, the therapist can use the data as a valuable basis for positive verbal reinforcement of the patient. Conversely, nonadherence should not be assumed from the lack of psychophysiological progress during baselines. Observations of significant others may help therapists assess and reinforce adherence with observation of patients’ restlessness, posture, breathing, facial muscles, and other visible cues of tension and relaxation. Increasing use of technologies, such as portable biofeedback devices for home use, electronic diaries, and Internet-based interventions enhance objectivity of adherence monitoring (Clough & Casey, 2011). Summary and Recommendations Nonadherence with regard to referrals, keeping appointments, homework, and treatment recommendations undermines the effectiveness of psychological interventions and adherence should be recognized a moderator of effectiveness in biofeedback treatment. Practitioners are encouraged to take into account patients’ risk factors for non- 247 15. Adherence adherence, such as dissatisfaction with therapist or treatment, low self-efficacy, and other barriers. Therapists are encouraged to adopt adherence facilitation strategies as a mode of practice. There are many tools that promise to increase adherence, including clinic structure, practitioner communication, the therapeutic relationship, and behavioral homework. Patients present with diverse experiences and beliefs, and providers may wish to consider the patient’s individual cost–benefit analysis for treatment. Likewise, it should be noted that there is no cure for low adherence, and adherence facilitation should be maintained as long as treatment is needed. This chapter helps to answer a common question asked by practitioners: “What can I do to ensure, or increase the likelihood, that my patients will do what I recommend?” The most pertinent recommendations are summarized in Table 15.3. It is useful for us as practitioners to remind ourselves that we are here to serve our patients—not the reverse. We fulfill many of our responsibilities when we provide patients a rationale for our recommendations and present practical and achievable methods to reach therapy goals. We fulfill other responsibilities by maintaining our credibility in the views of our patients and preserving positive rapport with them. It is abundantly clear that adherence is a complex and many-sided concept that requires great care, preparation, ingenuity, persistence, and patience by practitioners. It requires professionals to review their own professional behaviors, setting, and procedures. It requires tailoring interventions and giving patients adequate time to apply recommendations. Practitioners also need to tolerate and function with ambiguity, and within the less than ideal world of clinical practice. Providers should strive toward cultivation and growth of skills to help patients cultivate compliance, healthy attitudes, and health-promoting behaviors. TABLE 15.3. Summary of Adherence Recommendations References Persons and factors that impact adherence: •• Professional setting and office personnel. •• Professional’s characteristics and behaviors. •• Interaction between the professional and patient. •• Cognitive preparation of the patient. •• Patient’s perceptions, expectations, and affect. •• Patient’s family members and other significant individuals. Specific adherence facilitation interventions: •• Use readily accessible, easy-to-use self-report record systems. •• Ask patients to record readily observable and meaningful behaviors. •• Instruct patients why and how to self-monitor. •• Reinforce patients’ accuracy and completeness. •• Convey that the patient’s records will be reviewed. •• Encourage patients to record behaviors, experiences, and symptoms when they occur. •• Establish subgoals, and review and revise them as needed. Important general considerations: •• Be willing to accept less than ideal compliance and therapeutic progress. •• Successively approximate and shape compliance. •• Allow patients to set their own goals and subgoals, and discuss cost–benefit considerations. Note 1. We prefer the term “therapeutic range,” which may or may not be the same as a “relaxed range.” The criteria for a relaxed range differ among practitioners; they also differ at different stages of therapy and for different therapeutic goals. Bados, A., Balaguer, G., & Saldana, C. (2007). The efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy and the problem of drop out. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(6), 585–592. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Byrne, C. M., Solomon, M. J., Young, J. M., Rex, J., & Merlino, C. L. (2007). Biofeedback for fecal incontinence: Short-Term outcomes of 513 consecutive patients and predictors of successful treatment. Diseases of the Colon and Rectum, 50(4), 417–427. Clough, B. A., & Casey, L. M. (2011). Technological adjuncts to enhance current psychotherapy practices: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 279 –292. Derisley, J., & Reynolds, S. (2000). The transtheoretical stages of change as a predictor of premature termination, attendance and alliance in psychotherapy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39(4), 371–382. DiMatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., & Croghan, T. W. (2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment: Meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2101–2107. Dunbar-Jacob, J., Erlen, J. A., Schlenk, E. A., Ryan, C. M., Sereika, S. M., & Doswell, W. M. (2000). Adherence in chronic disease. Annual Review Nursing Research, 18, 48–90. 248 Dunbar-Jacob, J., & Mortimer-Stephens, M. K. (2001). Treatment adherence in chronic disease. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 54(Suppl. 1), S57–S60. Elder, J. P., Ayala, G. X., & Harris, S. (1999). Theories and intervention approaches to health-behavior change in primary care. American Journal Preventative Medicine, 17, 275–284. Kazantzis, N. (2000). Power to detect homework effects in psychotherapy outcome research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(1), 166–170. Kinzie, M. B. (2005). Instructional design strategies for health behavior change. Patient Education and Counseling, 56, 3–15. LaGreca, A. M., Bearman, K. J., & Roberts, M. C. (2003). Adherence to pediatric treatment regimens. In M. C. Roberts (Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology (3rd ed., pp. 119–140). New York: Guilford Press. Lambert, M. J., Hansen, N. B., & Finch, A. E. (2001). Patient-focused research: Using patient outcome data to enhance treatment effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(2), 159–172. Macharia, W. M., Leon, G., Rowe, B. H., Stephenson, B. J., & Haynes, R. B.(1992). An overview of interventions to improve compliance with appointment keeping for medical services. Journal of the American Medical Association, 267(13), 1813–1817. Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. V. PRACTICE ISSUES O’Donohue, W. T., & Levensky, E. R. (2006). Promoting treatment adherence: A practical handbook for health care providers. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Prochaska, J. O., & Lorig, K. (2001). What do we know about what works: The role of theory in patient education. In K. Lorig (Ed.), Patient education: A practical approach (3rd ed., pp. 21–55). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Prochaska, J. O., Redding, A., & Evers, K. E. (1997). The transtheoretical model and stages of change. In K. Glanz, F. M. Lewis, & B. K. Rimer (Eds.), Health behavior and health education (pp. 60–84). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Reiner, R. (2008). Integrating a portable biofeedback device into clinical practice for patients with anxiety disorders: Results of a pilot study. Journal of Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 33, 55–61. Reis, B. F., & Brown, L. G. (1999). Reducing psychotherapy dropouts: Maximizing perspective convergence in the psychotherapy dyad. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 36(2), 123–136. Scheel, M. J., Hanson, W. E., & Razzhavaikina, T. I. (2004). The process of recommending homework in psychotherapy: A review of therapist delivery methods, client acceptability, and factors that affect compliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41(1), 38–55. World Health Organization. (2003). Adherence to longterm therapies: Evidence for action. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Ch a p ter 16 Problems Associated with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Guidelines for Management Mark S. Schwartz, Nancy M. Schwartz, and Vincent J. Monastra • Meditation (Carrington, 2007) • Behavioral relaxation training (Poppen, 1998) • Electroencephalographic (EEG) biofeedback (Thompson & Thompson, 2007; Hammond, 2010) Most people use relaxation therapies and biofeedback-assisted relaxation without problems. Nevertheless, a few people do experience negative reactions and other problems. These difficulties can seem alarming to the subject1 (e.g., patients, clients) and professionals, and can result in stopping potentially useful interventions. Even if subjects experiencing such difficulties continue interventions, they might reduce adherence with recommended relaxation procedures and therefore reduce their chances for improvement (Borkovec et al., 1987). Fortunately, significant negative reactions are uncommon and can usually be avoided or easily managed. Practitioners who are aware of these and other potential difficulties and their possible causes can often prevent or lessen these effects. Readers are referred to selected published discussions and some suggested solutions: The reader is referred to these excellent discussions and their suggested solutions. Negative Reactions A partial list of the potential negative reactions reported includes the following: 1. Musculoskeletal activity. Examples of such activ- ities are tics, cramps, myoclonic jerks, spasms, and restlessness. 2. Disturbing sensory experiences. These experiences include sensations of heaviness, warmth, or cooling; feelings of depersonalization, misperceived body size, or floating; and a variety of visual, auditory, gustatory, and olfactory experiences. 3. Sympathetic nervous system activity. These reactions include increased heart rate and electrodermal activity. • Progressive muscle relaxation (Bernstein, Carlson, Schmidt, 2007; Bernstein & Borkovec, 1973; Bernstein & Carlson, 1993; McGuigan & Lehrer, 2007) • Autogenic therapy (Linden, 2007; Schultz & Luthe, 1969). • Dysfunctional2 breathing (van Dixhoorn, 1997, 2007) 249 250 4. Disturbing cognitive and/or emotional reactions. Examples include feelings of sadness, anger, and depression; disturbing and intrusive thoughts or mind wandering; tearfulness; and increased anxiety and fears (e.g., losing control). 5. Other possible negative side effects. These include hypotensive reactions, headache, sexual arousal, and psychotic symptoms. Incidence of Relaxation‑Associated3 Negative Reactions The very few studies about these problems involve very few subjects. Available reports are mostly anecdotal surveys of mental health professionals, observations of subjects or patients in studies, and small samples of patients with anxiety disorders. Survey studies are helpful, but they are fraught with methodological problems. See www. marksschwartzphd.com for a summary of papers by Jacobson and Edinger (1962), Edinger and Jacobson (1982), Blanchard, Cornish, Wittrock, and Fahrion (1990), and Braith, McCullough, and Bush (1988). In Blanchard et al. (1990), among 73 hypertension patients receiving temperature biofeedback and relaxation, there were 4–9% who reported minor negative sensations or experiences and none reported relaxation-induced anxiety (RIA)-type reactions. In another study, among 30 undergraduates with chronic anxiety (Braith et al., 1988) 5 or 17% reported increased anxiety during a taped session of progressive relaxation. Relaxation‑Associated Negative Reactions The term “relaxation-induced anxiety” (RIA)4 is sometimes still used to denote a variety of negative reactions associated with relaxation procedures. However, there has been cogent criticism of the term and its connotations (van Dixhoorn, 2007; Lehrer, Woolfolk, & Sime, 2007; McGuigan & Lehrer, 2007; Bernstein et al., 2007), as summarized in this chapter. Heide and Borkovec (1983) defined RIA as “paradoxical increases in cognitive, physiological, or behavioral components of anxiety as a consequence of engaging in systematic relaxation training” (p. 171). Carrington (1977) described intense restlessness, profuse perspiration, shivering, trembling, pounding heart, and rapid breathing associated with a type of meditation. Others refer to subjects being frightened by some of the sensations associated with relaxation and having fears about V. PRACTICE ISSUES losing control and experiencing anxiety and worrisome cognitions. van Dixhoorn (2007) focuses on fear of losing control, especially associated with relaxation methods such as autogenic procedures that have suggestive instructions in comparison with procedures focused on muscle tension and movement (e.g., progressive muscle relaxation). When anxiety/panic does occur during relaxation procedures, van Dixhoorn (2007) and Ley (1988) attributes it to unintended hyperventilation. This is consistent with the view that Jacobson’s progressive relaxation, in contrast to the “post-Jacobsonian . . . techniques” or “briefer methods” (Bernstein et al., 2007, pp. 70–71), does not cause anxiety. Proposed Causes of Relaxation‑Induced Negative Reactions and Risk Factors There are several hypothesized causes of relaxation-induced negative reactions (RANRs). Any one or a combination of them might apply to a specific individual. The first five were suggested by Heide and Borkovec (1984) discussing RIA. 1. Cognitive fear of unfamiliar sensations. Some people have cognitive fear of the sensations associated with relaxation, such as tingling, heaviness, warmth, and muscle jerks. Patients may view these physiological–behavioral reactions and the related cognitive–affective reactions as uncomfortable or unfamiliar rather than as positive signs that relaxation is occurring. This may be more common in people who rarely or never attend to body sensations, or among those who interpret these sensations as negative. A related hypothesis (Denny, 1976) suggests that stimuli produced by relaxation may become conditioned to fear when paired with a history of punishment during relaxation or times of safety. 2. Fear of losing control. Some people are preoccupied with maintaining control over their physical and psychological processes (Braith et al., 1988; Lehrer, 1982) and fear losing control (van Dixhoorn, 2007). Western culture assumes that exercising control requires active effort. Seligman (1975) defined “control” as being able to change outcomes by voluntary actions. Such patients may display a pattern of trying too hard. They maintain a high degree of activity out of fear that without it, they will waste time and accomplish nothing. This fear of inactivity may lead to more anxiety. 16. Problems with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Management Guidelines The fear of losing control may be more common among people who avoid rest and reflection, and among those intent on maintaining control with active and effortful activity. For people with generalized anxiety, “daily maintenance of higherthen-normal tension may be a learned avoidance response to relaxation” (Borkovec et al., 1987, p. 887). Associated with the loss of control is the perception that relaxation “signifies vulnerability, lack of control over anger and sexual desire, overpassivity, etc.” (Lehrer, 1982, p. 424). This may have been the cause of the angry feelings evoked in five patients who were asked to relax, as reported by Abramowitz and Wieselberg (1978). 3. Fear of experiencing anxiety. RIA is more common in persons who are chronically anxious. Relaxation methods often direct people to focus away from external stimuli and on body sensations or thoughts. This may increase their awareness of current internal cues, which are often associated with higher levels of anxiety. In the past, these cues were distressing: The person viewed them as meaning “out of my control,” or associated the cues with heightened anxiety or even panic. For example, specific thoughts about anxiety may result in cognitive anxiety (Norton, Rhodes, Hauch, & Kaprowy, 1985). 4. Fear of encountering oneself. This is the hypothesized fear of attending to the heightened awareness of internal experience in general. Some professionals view this phenomenon as one resulting from dissatisfaction with oneself or from fearing the increased awareness of inner conflicts. 5. Situation-produced worry, or intrusive thoughts and worries. Patients may find, as they reduce their focus on external stimuli, that their own thoughts and worries arise and become more dominant. This phenomenon is similar to cognitive intrusions’ interference with sleep onset. Note that these thoughts do not relate to relaxation but become associated with the relaxation experience (see also Lichstein, 1988, p. 138). 6. Breathing-related physical changes. Another hypothesis with support is that of van Dixhoorn (2007), with similarities to that of Ley (1988). Physical changes from breathing occur during relaxation and cause or increase the chance of having RANR. Chronic hyperventilation alters the amount of carbon dioxide and other body chemicals. See van Dixhoorn (2007, p. 310) for his summary. Briefly, he refers to persons exerting 251 excessive effort, breathing too deeply, with pauses that are too short after inhalations and/or exhalations, and/or using regulated breathing for excessively long durations. 7. Parasympathetic or trophotropic responses. An old hypothesis is the parasympathetic or trophotropic response hypothesis. According to this view, some people tend to have more parasympathetic responses. RIA is a compensatory ergotropic sympathetic nervous system response. This is similar to theories such as the one proposed by Stampler (1982), suggesting that relaxation may directly stimulate a “complex interplay of psychological and physiological factors” (Cohen, Barlow, & Blanchard, 1985, p. 99) that can lead to RIA in susceptible people. For example, DeGood and Williams (1982) reported the case of a 40-year-old female patient treated with autogenic training and electromyographic biofeedback for low back pain and leg pain. They monitored her finger temperature and skin conductance. She developed acute headaches with nausea soon after each of the first two sessions. Revising the training procedure to having the patient sit upright with her eyes open helped to stop the postsession symptoms. DeGood and Williams speculated that the negative symptoms were caused by “vagal rebound” or “parasympathetic overcompensation” (p. 464) after physiological deactivation during the relaxation. This explanation focuses on the possible role of the anterior hypothalamus (Gellhorn, 1965, 1967; Mefford, 1979). According to this view, “lowered somatic activity . . . tends to be accompanied by increased activation of the trophotropically dominant anterior hypothalamus and related structures (DeGood & Williams, 1982, p. 464). 8. Switching from passive to active coping. Another possible factor involves switching from a passive, immobilized, nonpreparatory and relaxed state to anticipation of or preparation for action (Elliott, 1974; Obrist, 1976, 1981; Cohen et al., 1985). Heart rate is slower during passive coping. The person switching from a passive to an active coping method could experience large accelerations of heart rate, according to this explanation (Cohen et al., 1985). Such accelerated heart rate is presumably not because of increased anxiety, but because of cardiac–somatic coupling. This is the close relationship between heart rate and striated muscle activity. Thus, preparation for action may increase somatic arousal and heart rate. 252 9. Other explanations. Some patients who experience RANR are competitive with themselves and fear failure. In other individuals, relaxation may arouse thoughts and feelings of sexual arousal. Still others take certain medications and may confuse the side effects or interactions with the feelings induced by relaxation; this confusion may result in RANR. Another useful categorization of negative effects was provided by van Dixhoorn (2007). Although his work focused on breathing, the categorization is broader, with implications for other relaxation methods. He addressed the negative effects topic under six categories: hyperventilation, increased unpleasant awareness, relaxation overdose, RIA, cathartic responses, and cardiac arrhythmia. See van Dixhoorn (2007) for details. A summary follows: Increased Unpleasant Awareness. Unpleasant bodily experiences occur in patients with hyperventilation symptoms, including during intervention, in addition to desired and pleasant experience, thus lead Dixhoorn (2007) to recommend including individual evaluation of unpleasant experiences. Relaxation Overdose. This refers to practicing too long in an upright position, standing or sitting, or getting up too quickly from a reclined position. The result can be dizziness, lightheadedness, or even faintness associated with an excessive drop in blood pressure. The faintness typically lasts a few minutes but can last longer. Risk factors include, but are not limited to, being tired or after an illness. The original discussion by van Dixhoorn (2007) is worth reviewing. Relaxation-Induced Anxiety. For van Dixhoorn (2007) the focus is on fear of losing control and is associated with relaxation techniques involving suggestion rather than movement of muscles. One implication is for breathing interventions to include movements that affect breathing, rather than sitting or lying quietly for too long. He also refers to the potential for unplanned hyperventilation during any type of relaxation procedure, which is also discussed by Ley (1988). Cathartic Responses. The negative responses include spontaneous movements (e.g., shivering, yawning, movements of legs or arms, whole body jerks) or emotionally charged reactions (e.g., experiencing historical traumas with associated emotions). The movements might dissipate naturally, resulting in the patient feeling refreshed and relieved. However, if perceived as distressing, V. PRACTICE ISSUES solutions recommended are voluntary movements or deep inhalation and breath holding for a few seconds, either of which typically stops the movements. Cardiac Arrhythmia. This refers to patients with cardiac arrhythmias for whom certain breathing patterns may provoke the arrhythmia. We direct interested readers to discussion of this by van Dixhoorn (2007, pp. 312–313). In summary, based on his very extensive experience, research, and publications regarding use of breathing therapy for cardiac patients, he stated that “sometimes breathing is too slow and exhalation pauses too long in comparison with the state of agitation of the whole system. Also, exhalation constricts the intrathoracic space and may stimulate the heart mechanically. The heart responds with extra beats, mostly supraventricular; but also premature ventricular contractions (PVSs) or even ventricular tachycardia may occur” (p. 313). For more discussion and solutions, see van Dixhoorn (2007) and his other publications at www.methodevandixhoorn.com/centrum/index.htm for more discussion and solutions. Guidelines for Avoiding, Minimizing, and Managing RANR People who experience RANR are among those who often most need applied psychophysiological interventions The implication for practitioners is not to avoid relaxation but to be aware of and anticipate reactions. Practitioners should provide understandable and realistic patient education regarding the specifics of the relaxation procedures, and should select types of relaxation that are less likely to result in these reactions with particular patients. A positive therapeutic alliance can help practitioners manage these reactions. A practitioner’s preparation of subjects for relaxation includes an explanation that subjects might experience certain sensations and thoughts during relaxation, including the normal signs that relaxation is taking place. This explanation is especially important for persons with chronic anxiety. Moreover, he or she should caution some persons to expect intrusive thoughts in early sessions of relaxation and expect that these will diminish as their skills and confidence increase. In addition, the practitioner should explain that relaxation increases rather than diminishes control. He or she explains that people often achieve relaxation proficiency and increased autonomic nervous system control through less rather than more effort. 16. Problems with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Management Guidelines The practitioner can also consider a switch to a different type of relaxation method. For example, if a person is having difficulty with a bodily focus type, consider a switch to a more cognitive approach. If a person is having trouble with a cognitive method, consider a switch to an active, external attentional focus or to a bodily focus type. An example of this would be focusing on external sounds in or outside the office rather than a mental focus on body awareness (Wells, 1990; see also the discussion of distraction and intrusive thoughts below). Other Reactions and Problems, and Guidelines for Avoiding, Reducing, and Managing Them 1. Embarrassment. Some people feel embarrassed or self-conscious about selected relaxation procedures (e.g., tensing facial muscles, closing one’s eyes). Solutions: Model the procedures and offer reassurance and supportive statements. In earlier sessions, the practitioner can look away from the subject during part of the procedure. 2. Gender-related or sexual problems. Some subjects feel sexually aroused, self-conscious, or threatened during relaxation. Reclining in a darkened room and using suggestive or other relaxation terms with double meanings can add to the potential for subjective discomfort. Self-consciousness and similar discomfort can also occur with people (especially males) who are unaccustomed to the passive role in any situation. Now they find themselves asked to recline passively. This can be psychologically uncomfortable for them. We assume that many, or most, of these people are either not aware of these reasons or they will not explain because of their discomfort. Solutions: Be sensitive to this potential and gender-related factors, and adjust language and procedures accordingly (e.g., consider starting selected subjects in a comfortable upright posture to which they are more accustomed). Maintain appropriate boundaries and be careful about touching subjects. 3. Script content problems. The contents of relaxation scripts are comfortable for some patients but uncomfortable for others. This depends on patients’ perceptions, attitudes, and fantasies. For example, the practitioner might consider the potential effect of using the term “feelings of 253 heaviness” with patients who have actual or perceived weight problems. Solutions: Tailor scripts to the subject as indicated. 4. Distraction and intrusive thoughts. There are many sources of distraction from the concentration needed during relaxation and body awareness procedures. These include associations to the contents of the relaxation script. Some people think about their life and responsibilities at these times. Solutions: Discuss this with subjects and reassure them that these distracting thoughts and images are normal. Then provide assistance to lessen them (e.g., consider shorter sessions, more breaks, or having subjects start with eyes open or partially open). Practitioners also might consider using active sensory awareness exercises, such as the example by Wells (1990) of external attentional focus on sounds in the environment. One can expand this to training to focus on sensory awareness of the environment rather than to strive for internal awareness and a self-attentive focus. For example, provide guidance or direction to attend to the texture of the armchair, sounds in the office, and the color of a wall. Then provide guidance by switching from one to another to increase the subject’s ability to choose and to control his or her mental focus. Do these slowly, but focus only a few seconds on each, as in this example: “Right now, think about the color of the wall. Right now, think about the texture of the chair. Right now, think about the sound of _________. Now, switch your attention from one to another.” Another suggestion is for patients to think of the distracting thoughts as words or pictures on a television or movie screen. Patients can then imagine the screen becoming smaller and smaller, until it becomes tiny and distant or disappears entirely. Patients can imagine that they are moving farther away or that the screen is moving farther away. Sitting close to a big color screen is more distressing than seeing the same words and images on a 3-inch black-and-white screen several feet away. 5. Restlessness and related problems. Being silent and being motionless are paradoxically uncomfortable for some people. These people become restless with longer sessions. Some may have features of the syndrome sometimes known as adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Problems with laughing, talking, coughing, sneezing, and other body movements are related. Solutions: Assess and prepare for restlessness early. Ask whether a subject has any concern about 254 sitting quietly for the planned amount of time. If a practitioner anticipates subject discomfort, discuss this early, reassure the patient, and suggest adjusted body positions and durations as indicated. Patients should be given choices about physical positions, lighting, and time to make adjustments. Sessions can be shortened or interspersed with breaks. Subjects may keep their eyes open or partially open. Avoid long silences without verbal instructions, discussion, changes in feedback displays and tasks, or physiological changes that are obvious to patients. 6. Low self-efficacy and fear of failure. People often do not have the needed self-confidence in their abilities to develop effective relaxation skills. The theme in their self-statements is “I cannot do it.” They also may not have realistic goals; they may expect the goal of therapy to be mastery. Similarly, fear of failure is a common problem. Such people say, at least to themselves, “Am I doing this right?”; “Am I doing this better than the last time?”; or “I will never get the feelings and benefits I need!” Solutions: Provide explanation and reminders that learning any new physical or mental skill is a process with peaks, valleys, and plateaus. Moreover, developing or cultivating low or lower tension and arousal is often a gradual process. Using examples from the acquisition of athletic, musical, or other skills is often helpful. Provide encouragement and reminders to avoid hurrying and to apply the “three P’s”—patience, practice, and persistence. Discussing fear of failure early and periodically can help to replace negative thoughts with positive ones. Provide reminders that most people can make progress, and guide subjects away from viewing physiological self-regulation as something that they “pass or fail.” In addition, encourage people to allow relaxation to happen, or to let go, rather than trying to make it happen. Focus on increasing awareness of feelings associated with physiological self-regulation. Finally, shift the goals away from specific numbers and using performance competition as a model. 7. Increased awareness of tension. People, typically patients, sometimes report increased symptoms or other signs of dysfunction during the early stages of relaxation therapies. Because general relaxation permits more awareness of tension of selected body areas, people may perceive themselves as more tense than they were before. This does not mean that patients are actually more V. PRACTICE ISSUES tense, simply that they are more body-aware. An increased awareness of tension can also result from increased focus on symptoms through the use of self-report symptom logs. Some relaxation procedures, such as tensing muscles, can increase some symptoms as well. Some body areas remain tense during some relaxation procedures, including tense–release procedures. Solutions: Discuss this phenomenon and reassure patients that such perceptions are common and normal. By noticing tension earlier, one can reduce the tension and prevent symptoms. Patients can reframe the belief in increased tension as increased awareness. 8. Problems with significant others. Family members and other significant people in a subject’s life during relaxation therapy may not be understanding and cooperative. Solutions: If such people are not present during office sessions, provide education materials explaining the rationale, procedures, and need for cooperation from others. Practitioners may need to provide counseling on how to discuss recommended relaxation with others. and how to increase cooperation from family members. 9. Factors not related to starting relaxation. Factors other than starting relaxation therapy can increase symptoms. If a person is not cognitively prepared for the intervention, he or she can experience increased concern, emotional arousal, and tension, therefore increasing symptoms. Another factor is the presence of continuing or even increasing stress that is added to the symptoms. Solutions: Discussion of current life events and counseling, adjustments, and reassurance are appropriate. Rule out other causes, such as an incorrect diagnosis and/or intervention strategy. 10. Viewing treatment as stressful. There are patients who view some aspects of treatment as stressful, and this can add to symptoms. Consider the time allotments and other arrangements required for patients to attend office sessions. These include time away from other duties and responsibilities, explanations to employers and supervisors, and often extra work when patients get back from appointments. People invest time and effort in carrying out homework assignments, maintaining self-report records, and completing questionnaires. All of these pressures are stressful, and added to them are the expenses for intervention. Thoughts about any or all of these added sources of stress can intrude. 16. Problems with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Management Guidelines Solutions: Sensitivity and flexibility on the practitioner’s part about scheduling and assignments can help to decrease the effects of this stress. 11. Disregarding instructions. Subjects (e.g., patients) sometimes disregard instructions during biofeedback and other relaxation procedures in the practitioner’s office and elsewhere. For example, they may not imagine the stress stimuli the practitioner presents, or they may imagine the stress stimuli only part of the time. In addition, some subjects intentionally think of topics other than the biofeedback signal or verbal relaxation instructions. People are unlikely to admit to such diversions without careful questioning. Be careful in discussing this, in order to avoid giving the impression of being critical. Solutions: Sensitivity and flexibility on the practitioner’s part about assignments and instructions can help. Once again, provide adequate information. A practitioner can change the content of the relaxation script and procedures, or consider saying something like the following: “Sometimes you might be thinking about other topics during relaxation and biofeedback. It is normal for that to occur at times, and I understand. I need to know if this is happening. Please share it with me when it is happening.” 12. Not focusing on physiology. Some people listen to prerecorded relaxation but do not focus on their physiology. Similarly, some just listen to or watch feedback signals, but with minimal or no focus on their physiology. It is as if they expect or hope that the relaxation instructions and biofeedback signals themselves will induce the desired outcome. Solutions: Anticipate this potential problem and discuss it. Provide guidance away from a scenario of passively expecting the prerecorded relaxation procedures or feedback signal to be therapeutic by themselves. 13. Falling asleep. Some people doze or start sleeping during relaxation procedures. Solutions: Be aware of this potential. Schedule sessions earlier in the day, or avoid relaxation after meals, unless it is needed for postprandial symptoms. In persistent cases, consider a sleep disorder evaluation to check for sleep apnea, psychophysiological insomnia, sleep–wake schedule disorders, and narcolepsy. 14. Misuse of relaxation audiotapes. Some patients are dependent on prerecorded relaxation methods and use them too often and too long. 255 They rely on them and do not learn to relax without them. Solutions: Clarify the proper role of prerecorded scripts and encourage people to avoid dependence on them. If dependency does occur, taper via fading and related behavioral techniques. Consider using progressively briefer scripts. People can turn the recording off progressively earlier in the script, or lower the volume gradually and continue the relaxation. 15. Not having or taking enough time to practice and apply relaxation. This is a very common problem. Substantial time use problems are common. Some people do not know how to or have not applied effective time use management in their lives. Solutions: Practitioners can conduct, or refer such people for an evaluation of and education about, time use management. For example, help such people learn to set goals and priorities, to delegate responsibility appropriately, to avoid or reduce activities that waste time, to reduce perfectionism, and to manage procrastination. Encourage scheduling relaxation and making practice and application a high priority. 16. Specific problems with biofeedback procedures. Problems specific to biofeedback can occur during biofeedback-assisted relaxation sessions (Gaarder & Montgomery, 1981, p. 94). We include some of these problems here and suggest a few solutions. Experienced practitioners can develop their own repertoires. a. Very small changes in a physiological parameter, or no patient perception the feedback signal is changing. Solutions: Use the threshold or change it to ease the task. Increase the gain, so that the visual display feedback or audio feedback changes are more obvious, with smaller physiological changes. Encourage shifting attention to other sites or cognitions, as well as switching to a different task or feedback site. Use varied verbal relaxation instructions or change the visual display. Ask the subjects to close their eyes for a few moments, then freeze the visual display if it changes in the desired direction. Then ask subjects to open their eyes gently to see the change. Repeat as indicated. b. Movement of the feedback signal in the undesired direction. 256 V. PRACTICE ISSUES Solutions: Stop instrumentation-based feedback. Discuss “trying too hard” and provide quiet, brief, and clear verbal feedback when the signal moves in the desired direction. Observe the person’s posture, breathing, and movements, and suggest adjustments as needed. Shift the focus to other sites, tasks, or techniques. Use varied verbal relaxation instructions, ask about cognitions, and discuss and suggest changes as needed. Increased control is the ability to move the signal in either direction. The experienced practitioner will notice what makes it go in the wrong direction, as this can give clues for moving it in the desired direction. c. Patient fatigue with the feedback signal or task. Solutions: Change the feedback displays. With computer-based systems, there are a wide variety of feedback options. Conduct shorter sessions or adjust the goals of a session to increase the chance of obvious successes and reinforce changes. 17. Inability to exhibit a relaxation posture due to physical limitations. Poppen (1998) discusses the specialized procedures he developed and calls behavioral relaxation training (BRT).5 Physical limitations include scoliosis, arthritis, or unequal leg length that can interfere with the patient’s ability to implement BRT. Solutions: Be alert to these limitations and tailor adjustments in the relaxation process and criteria. 18. Breathing with open mouth while relaxing results in dry oral cavity. Solutions: Breathe nasally and close mouth. 19. Frustration from feeling that performance of specific procedures is impossible. Examples are being unable to keep eyes closed without twitching of the eyelids, breathing at a slower rate, or swallowing less often. Solutions: Provide reassurance that 100% perfection is not expected. Also provide positive feedback anytime the relaxed behavior occurs. Side Effects of EEG Biofeedback As Thompson and Thompson (2007) discussed, there are a few reports of side effects and some theoretically possible side effects associated with EEG biofeedback. Alpha–Theta “Abreaction,” or spontaneously occurring images, as observed and reported by Peniston and Kulkosky (1989), occurred in some of their patients diagnosed with alcoholism and being treated with alpha and theta enhancement. This recollection of traumatic events is possible in patients with a history of abuse or psychological trauma and is presumably associated with increased theta as it is a “state in which the unconscious becomes more accessible,” as noted by Thompson and Thompson (2007, p. 265). They also note that there are psychoanalysts “who use EEG feedback to help their patients get into a hypnagogic state . . . in which the person in analysis can access free associations and dream-like states . . . [allowing] memories and fantasies to emerge, and therein lies its potential for use in psychotherapy, and work with positive replacement imaging (Peniston & Kulkowsky, 1990) as well as its danger” (p. 265). The risk is that “any suggestions occurring in this state can solidify the mixture of fantasy and memory, with the unfortunate production of ‘false memories’ ” (Thompson & Thompson, 2007, p. 265). Precipitating a seizure is theoretically possible in vulnerable persons, with increased drowsiness associated with increased theta. Neurotherapy with added techniques, for example, audiovisual stimulation or HEG (hemoencephalography or brain blood flow biofeedback) theoretically might be associated with more side effects, although none were noted by Thompson and Thompson (2007). Beta training has been reported by some professionals to increase overactivity after a session (Thompson & Thompson, 2007), although the authors observed this in only one of about 2000 clients in which inadvertent training occurred. Greater hypnotizability among clients with ADHD is associated with their excess theta, as noted by Thompson and Thompson (2007). This might theoretically result in unwanted and unplanned expectations if clients are asked questions about agitation or excitability from the training. These authors do not report any such cases in their own experience or that of others. Training the wrong (intentionally or unintentionally) EEG frequencies at specific sites might, theoretically, result in undesired side effects. For example, Lubar (1991) intentionally reversed EEG feedback reinforcement conditions. After subjects successfully learned to suppress theta activity (4–8 Hz) and increase beta (16–20 Hz), the experimenters intentionally reversed this to increase 16. Problems with Relaxation Procedures and Biofeedback, and Management Guidelines theta and inhibit beta. In contrast to the previously achieved desired improvement of attention and behaviors, the reversal resulted in the patients becoming more disorganized, inattentive, and hyperactive. Resumption of the initial protocol resulted in a return to improved functioning. This reversal illustrates the dramatic effect that operant conditioning of specified EEG frequency bands can exert on human functioning. Monastra, Monastra, and George (2002) noted certain adverse side effects associated with combining EEG biofeedback with medication. Stimulant therapy (Ritalin) was administered to all 100 patients with ADHD in their study, and a titrated dose was maintained for a 1-year period. During that time, 51 of the patients received EEG biofeedback; the other 49 did not. Only patients receiving the EEG biofeedback developed increased cortical arousal over the central midline region, which was associated with sustained improvement on behavioral and neuropsychological measures. By the conclusion of the first year, evidence of increased irritability was noted in approximately 20% of the patients who had received EEG biofeedback. This irritability was eliminated once stimulant therapy was reduced or discontinued. Consequently, clinicians treating patients diagnosed with ADHD with a combination of stimulant therapy and EEG biofeedback may find it useful to monitor patients as treatment progresses, and to consider a reduction of stimulant dose should irritability emerge. Side Effects Associated with the Use of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback Lehrer (2007) discussed some people’s tendency to hyperventilate when they start relaxed breathing during the early sessions of heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback. Thus, his training manual (Lehrer, Vaschillo, & Vaschillo, 2000; Lehrer, 2007) guides clients/patients toward shallow breathing initially, and educates and sensitizes them regarding hyperventilation symptoms. He suggests using a capnometer to monitor endtidal CO2 in early sessions, if available, although Thompson and Thompson (2007) note that only a “small number of practitioners measure CO2. We do not know the percentage, but we, too, suspect that it is only a small percentage and typically is not practical. However, if available, and assuming practitioner training and expertise with a capnometer, it could be useful for selected situations such as these. 257 Research on Negative Reactions and Other Problems More research is needed on the incidence and mechanisms of negative reactions and other problems associated with relaxation therapies, including biofeedback-assisted relaxation (Poppen, 1984; Edinger, 1984). Practitioners also need to conduct more research on patient variables associated with these difficulties and on preventive and management procedures. Retrospective survey research is fraught with enough methodological problems to preclude its value for estimating the incidence of these difficulties. (For more on this, see www. marksschwartzphd.com.) Prospective research needs to control for many variables, including relaxation procedures, biofeedback modality and procedures, presence or absence of therapist, subject preparation, duration of sessions, details of the negative reactions, whether eyes are open or closed, antecedent events, postural information, lighting information, therapist characteristics, types of physical and mental symptoms and disorders, patients’ prior experience with these and other therapies, and medications and doses. Cautions and Contraindications There are other, more serious factors to consider when practitioners are providing physiological self-regulatory therapies. These constitute the cautions and contraindications (see M. S. Schwartz, Chapter 14, this volume, for a discussion of these). Therapists can expect potentially serious problems to occur if they provide these therapies to patients for whom such cautions and contraindications apply. However, they can use special approaches for carefully selected patients with some of these disorders and conditions, if the practitioners are knowledgeable about and experienced with these disorders. Conclusion Various negative effects and other problems can occur as part of some relaxation therapies and some types of biofeedback. The experience of significant negative reactions can interfere with interventions and reduce adherence with recommendations. However, very few people are at risk, and very few experience negative reactions. Among those who do experience various prob- 258 lems, there are good solutions. Prudent practitioners use available information, wisdom from experience, skills, precautions, patient education, and good judgment in patient selection and implementation of treatments. Interventions with relaxation procedures and biofeedback should be provided by professionals with appropriate credentials, training, and experience, or by someone under the direct and personal supervision of a professional so qualified. Notes 1. For convenience in this chapter, we use the term “subject” to denote patients, clients, trainees, performers, athletes, and research subjects. 2. van Dixhoorn (1997)) recommended the concept and term “dysfunctional breathing” rather than “hyperventilation syndrome” (HVS). The replacement term is more encompassing and includes the hypocapnia associated with HVS. 3. In prior editions, the term “relaxation-induced” was used. The term used here, “relaxation-associated,” indicates that the reactions are thought to be due to multiple factors (e.g., procedural, relaxation, breathing, cognitive, past history, diagnoses). This also is more consistent with Poppen (1998), who suggested the term “training-induced arousal.” 4. Poppen (1998) also questioned the accuracy of the term “relaxation-induced anxiety” for describing the phenomenon. He suggested a more general and descriptive term such as “training-induced arousal.” The rationale he gives is “studies of the phenomenon have not indicated that any degree of relaxation occurs prior to the trainee’s upset; instead arousal occurs before training or right at the beginning” (p. 86). The second reason is that the term “anxiety” implies a specific set of behaviors, but that term does not always accurately describe the discomfort experienced by some persons. The examples given by Poppen (1998) involve nausea or falling sensations, which he described as “not properly labeled anxiety.” 5. BRT is useful for various populations, but it is particularly useful for special populations, such as those with intellectual disabilities (i.e., severe retardation, acquired brain injury, hyperactivity disorder, and schizophrenia). Poppen uses the terms “trainee” and “client” rather than “patient.” Items 17 through 20 are gleaned from Poppen (1998). References Abramowitz, S. I., & Wieselberg, N. (1978). Reaction to relaxation and desensitization outcome: Five angry treatment failures. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 1418–1419. V. PRACTICE ISSUES Bernstein, D. A., & Borkovec, T. D. (1973). Progressive relaxation training. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Bernstein, D. A., & Carlson, C. R. (1993). Progressive relaxation: Abbreviated methods. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (2nd ed., pp 53–87). New York: Guilford Press. Bernstein, D. A., Carlson, C. R., & Schmnidt, J. E. (2007). Progressive relaxation: Abbreviated methods. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Blanchard, E. B., Cornish, P. J., Wittrock, D. A., & Fahrion, S. (1990). Subjective experiences associated with thermal biofeedback treatment of hypertension. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 15(2), 145–159. Borkovec, T. D., Mathews, A. M., Chambers, A., Ebrahimi, S., Lytle, R., & Nelson, R. (1987). The effects of relaxation training with cognitive or nondirective therapy and the role of relaxation-induced anxiety in the treatment of generalized anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(6), 883–888. Braith, J. A., McCullough, J. P., & Bush, J. P. (1988). Relaxation-induced anxiety in a subclinical sample of chronically anxious subjects. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 19(3), 193–198. Carrington, P. (1977). Freedom in meditation. Garden City, NY: Doubleday/Anchor. Carrington, P. (2007). Modern forms of mantra meditation. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L. Woolfolk, & W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Cohen, A. S., Barlow, D. H., & Blanchard, E. B. (1985). Psychophysiology of relaxation-associated panic attacks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94(1), 96–101. DeGood, D. E., & Williams, E. M. (1982). Parasympathetic rebound following EMG biofeedback training: A case study. Biofeedback an