See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/339739872 Current Theory Chapter · May 2019 CITATIONS READS 0 1,081 4 authors: Tindara Caprì Rosa Angela Fabio Link Campus University Università degli Studi di Messina 97 PUBLICATIONS 1,035 CITATIONS 484 PUBLICATIONS 2,537 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE Giulia Emma Towey Alessandro Antonietti Università degli Studi di Messina Catholic University of the Sacred Heart 15 PUBLICATIONS 186 CITATIONS 504 PUBLICATIONS 4,699 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Rosa Angela Fabio on 13 May 2022. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE Chapter 1 CURRENT THEORY Tindara Caprì*, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey and Alessandro Antonietti Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina, Messina, Italy INTRODUCTION Attention is a research field that has produced, since its beginnings, a growing interest involving areas of apparently different investigations, among which: philosophy; branches of psychology, such as developmental psychology, social psychology, experimental psychology; areas of cognitive science, such as artificial intelligence and the simulation of neural networks; and, in general, neurosciences. In the last 30 years, several theories have been elaborated to explain the attention processes. Consequently, today, a rich body of research on the attentive processes is present in literature. This theoretical and methodological richness is due to the multifactoriality of attention. It is a complex concept, so when one * Corresponding Author’s Email: tcapri@unime.it. Complimentary Contributor Copy 2 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. studies attention one thinks of referring to a single construct, but in fact, the process of attention requires different stages of information processing, connected to them. It is also a process that does not involve a single brain area, but a network of brain areas closely connected to each other. Although we may want to refer to a single construct, for example, selective attention, the paradigms of research are different. This violates the idea, now consolidated, that attention is a multiple process both from a theoretical and a methodological point of view. Contemporary cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience have sought to deepen the mechanisms of attention, taking into account the inseparable link between attention and other aspects of cognition such as perception, action, memory, language. For these reasons, it is very difficult to write a chapter aimed to outline a historical and recent theory that can fully explain a complex cognitive process, such as attention. In the present chapter, the classical definitions of attention and related functions are outlined. In a second section of this chapter, an overview of historical theories of attention is provided, and, in addition, automatic and controlled processes of attention are described. These processes have given rise to a long debate in attention literature. In the final section, the current theories of attention are shown. 1. DEFINITIONS OF ATTENTION The concept of attention has often been interpreted in a very broad sense, despite what William James, already in 1860, in his most important work “Principles of Psychology,” stated: “Everyone knows what attention is …” (James, 1890), this means considering attention as an “anticipated thought.” The behavioral psychologist Hebb, in 1949, stated that attention should be understood as “a kind of process that is not completely controlled by environmental stimulation” (Hebb, 1949). In 1908, Titchener argued that “the doctrine of attention is the nerve of the whole Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 3 psychological system” (Titchner, 1908), assuming that the study of attentional processes embraces a fairly broad field of inquiry. In attentional literature, there has been much discussion about the definition of attention. Attention has conceptualized as a binding glue which aids in object perception (Treisman & Gelade, 1980); as a biasing mechanism which helps in selection of the task-relevant target (Desimone & Duncan, 1995); as related to the action system (Norman & Shallice, 1986), as a contributing agent in consciousness (De Brigard & Prinz, 2010); in terms of space and time as well as modes of deployment, i.e., exogenous and endogenous (Theeuwes, 1991). Alan Baddeley (1992), argued that “any attempt to review the literature on attention immediately leads those who undertake it to the conclusion that the concept of attention is not at all unitary” (Baddeley, 1992). Umiltà (2001), an Italian psychologist, said that human cognitive activity can be described as information processing and there are numerous processes and internal mechanisms that allow this elaboration, among these all those processes that go under the term of attention. In fact, man needs “the aid of the attentive mechanisms” since the elaboration process has several limits of time and space, as it is almost impossible to perform multiple activities simultaneously, to elaborate two stimuli or to recover information other than memory. We are aware of the spatial-temporal limits of the human processing system, during our daily life, using quite simple and common examples, as it is almost impossible to carry out an activity like typing and at the same time be able to hold a conversation, or follow a program on television and simultaneously talk to another person, or dance a tango and at the same time whistle a waltz. “We can therefore consider attention as a function that regulates the activity of mental processes, filtering and organizing information coming from the environment, in order to issue an adequate response” (Umiltà, 2001). Providing a definition in order to explain the construct of attention exactly, beyond the known equation “attention = selection,” is considered a very complex action. Even specialists have always been particularly reluctant to provide a clear and precise definition of this concept. Complimentary Contributor Copy 4 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. The term attention derives from the Latin term “attendi”, from “attentus,” past participle of the verb to wait and it means literally “pay attention”, an expression that is also commonly used in our everyday language to describe certain experiences or situations. Therefore, this term means “the attempt to devote skills of concentration, memory, or generically mental effort, to the elaboration of an event (object or person) relevant at a certain moment” (Turatto, 2008). In the classical account of attention, the common view is that attention is selective and has a limited capacity. Another feature of attention is that it can be strategically allocated. Attention, therefore, can be considered both as a psychic function that allows to select stimuli or tasks, either as a component of consciousness, given that to be conscious and aware of a specific element we must pay attention to the space in which it is inserted. This can happen when our attention is so focused on a certain thought that we do not hear the events happening around us. However, it may seem that this cognitive function is only inherent in sensation and perception in the light of the strong link of interaction, but it is possible to pay attention to things other than sensory inputs, that is, attention can be directed to internal mental processes, such as rethinking certain memories or mentally adding numbers. In conclusion, the definition considered still today the most eloquent is, as previously stated, that proposed by William James. In The Principles of Psychology, James discussed attention in terms of several dimensions, as following: “Everyone knows what attention is (…) It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains [p. 404] of thought. Focalization, concentration, of consciousness are of its essence. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively with others and is a condition which has a real opposite in the confused, dazed, scatter brained state which in French is called distraction, and Zerstreutheit in German.” Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 5 1.1. The Functions of Attention Figure 1. Functions of attention. From a cognitive prospective, attention has been considered as a multifactorial process, as it is made up of many functions. In classical view, the main functions of attention are: 1. Focused attention: respond to specific stimuli (focus on a precise task); 2. Sustained attention: maintain a consistent response during longer continuous activity (stay attentive a long period of time and follow the same topic); 3. Selective attention: selectively maintain the cognitive resource on specific stimuli (focus only on a given object while ignoring distractors); 4. Alternating attention: switch between multiple tasks (stop reading to watch something); 5. Divided attention: deal simultaneously with multiple tasks (talking while driving) (Matei, Ferrera, Riche & Taylor, 2016). According to some authors, two sub-categories are closely related to them (to what?): • • attention shifting: consists in the alternation between two focuses. It is connected to divided attention. generalized attention or arousal: predisposes the body to receive input from the environment, implying behavioral and physiological Complimentary Contributor Copy 6 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. changes in response to new stimuli. It is strongly interconnected to sustained attention. 1.1.1. Divided Attention and Attention Shifting Helmholtz provided the first demonstration that attention can be shifted covertly and consequently, independently of the direction of eyegaze. When control is purely endogenous, (Klein, 1980; Klein & Pontefract, 1994) and others (Hunt & Kingstone, 2003) have demonstrated that such shifts of attention are not accomplished via sub-threshold programming of the oculomotor system. The term divided attention refers to the ability to keep two or more focus, or tasks, simultaneously, as in the case of listening to a message, writing and answering questions; it happens in the daily life of carrying out an activity, for example cooking and at the same time one realizes that one is humming a song, this is a typical case in which divided attention becomes active. Umiltà (2001) argued that “the divided attention is the mirror image of selective attention, because in the latter the subject is asked to filter and attenuate as much information as possible from the non-relevant source, while in the former the task requires to process information coming from multiple sources at the same time.” The so-called “attentive resources” are activated, according to which there is a common and limited source of processing capacity that a normal subject can either intentionally distribute among various tasks, or concentrate on one, “the ability - therefore - to perform more tasks at the same time, without exceeding the limit of the capacity of the processing system, depends on the difficulty of the task and the habit of carrying it out. Moreover, we are talking about a primary task, when for example the task is carried out in an optimal way, and of a secondary task, when, instead, a task receives a remaining portion of attention. An essential component in defining divided attention is attentive shifting, which consists of the ability to shift attention alternately between two focuses, which need not be paid attention to at the same time, i.e., the Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 7 subject must be able to move from one task to another when the situation requires it. The speed and mode of movement of attentional focus change according to age. Focus in newborns is conditioned by the novelty of the stimulus, however, afterwards, it is the subject himself who decides which direction to assume in relation to their experiences or to the desire to achieve certain goals. A typical example of attentive shifting can happen when we are driving a car; while we are behind the wheel we slow down near a red light, and knowing that the traffic lights are always placed on our right we focus attention on the color that tells us to stop, suddenly stopping because a pedestrian crosses the road, not on the pedestrian crossing, from the left. To avoid running them over, our attention focus moves immediately to the side in which the person comes up to us, “forgetting” almost completely about the traffic light. A shift of the attentional focus includes some steps: detachment of the attentional focus from the first selected information; displacement of the focus towards the new information; anchoring of the attentive focus to the second information (Marzocchi, Molin, & Poli, 2000). The tasks of shifting attention, that is the passage from one target to another, together with the planning and initiation of strategies concerning the execution of “double tasks,” that is, multiple information that must be processed at the same time, together with the inhibition of inappropriate responses, which allow to deal with any interference, are the main components of attention that imply executive functions. Executive functions mean those “cognitive processes that are involved in a targeted way in the organization, selection and temporal structuring of our cognitive abilities, to achieve a particular purpose.” Executive functions are manifested through numerous, dissimilar phenomena, for this reason many situations require the aid of an executive control, for example those that require the correction of errors, the production of little learned or new action sequences, the overcoming of habitual responses, the formation of an intention. Complimentary Contributor Copy 8 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Studies (Noudoost & Moore, 2011; Stevens, & Bavelier, 2012) have also highlighted that the inhibitory component of selective attention can be considered an executive function. It has been found that most control processes are based on selective inhibition systems that inhibit any execution by potentially interfering events, for example, the phenomenon of negative priming, introduced by Tipper in 1985 that is “definable as the effect of interference that irrelevant information, in a test, causes when it is identical or semantically associated with the relevant information in the next trial” (Tipper, 1985). The negative priming effect is not found only when the irrelevant information in a test is identical to the relevant information in the next test, but also when the two pieces of information are semantically associated. It is also believed that the different phenomena are due to a central system whose neural substrate mainly involves the frontal lobes; this apparatus over the course of time has assumed different denominations in the scientific field: central executive, operating system, supervisory system, supervisor, central processor or even anterior attentive system. Despite the various appellations, the studies carried out on this system agree that there is a clear distinction between a system that controls and a multiplicity of processes that can be controlled, since, at the beginning, they can act autonomously, but they are always activated by external stimuli or by control processes; on the contrary, the processes of voluntary control do not depend on external stimulation. 1.1.2. Sustained Attention And “Arousal” Sustained attention has been defined as the ability to maintain attention on a specific task over an extended period of time (Betts, McKay, Maruff, & Anderson, 2007; Coull, 1998). Hence, this type of attention refers to the fact that attentional performance varies as a function of the temporal characteristics of the task (Cohen, 2014). Beginning with the experiments carried out by Mackworth around 1950, the assessment of sustained attention performance, also known as the supervisory term, required situations in which an observer is urged to watch inconspicuous signals for prolonged periods of time. The state of Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 9 readiness to respond to the rare and unpredictable signals that occur is characterized by an overall ability to detect signals, defined, “level of supervision” and, above all, by a decrease in terms of performance over time, defined, instead, “supervisory decrease”(Parasuraman, 2000). The supervisory level describes the general performance, i.e., false alarms or the proportion of successes, while a decrease in supervision is triggered by an increase in errors and reaction times that slow down over time. Psychological research on vigilance, or sustained attention, has interested well-known scholars in recent decades, allowing them to detect the development and validity of the various tasks administered, through sustained attention tests carried out on both humans and animals in order also to further research neuronal circuits that study the performance of sustained attention in humans and in laboratory animals (Martino et al, 2018; Fabio, Caprì, Lotan, Towey, & Martino, 2018; Fabio et al, 2018; Gangemi et al, 2018; Sarter, Givens, & Bruno, 2001). It is therefore important to take into account the state of supervision, which together with the attention paid, oversees both how attentive performances vary over time and how a state of alert can be reached and above all supported through the various skills. Surveillance corresponds therefore - to the ability to monitor events with a low frequency of occurrence over time. Umiltà (2001) claimed that the typical situation - to record the activation of sustained attention - is that of an observer placed in front of a screen in which weak light stimuli constitute the background noise, while a more intense stimulus constitutes the signal to be detected. By means of an explicit, verbal or motor response the subject is asked to be able to signal when a target stimulus appears in front of him, despite activating a background noise that acts as a distractor. A marked deterioration in the observer’s performance was observed over time, as the subject was distracted by the detection of a non-existent signal. During a test to detect a state of vigilance, it is necessary to monitor a series of stimuli to identify a critical element. The experimental subjects are observed for a fairly long period of time and the stimulus they have to Complimentary Contributor Copy 10 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. identify appear a few times and according to predictable modalities. The possibility that the target stimulus appears ranges between 3% and 5%. In the initial phase of the test it was recorded that the experimental subjects maintain a fairly fast performance and a good level of accuracy, while as the hours pass, there is a progressive decline in performance, as reaction times, false alarms and omissions increase; all this is due to a gradual decline in the detection of signals and an increase in the number of errors. Moreover, the performance turns out to be worse when the critical signals are not very salient, that is when their duration is just above the perceptive threshold, when the target stimuli are very rare and when they require a complex elaboration. In support of this, some studies have shown that sustained attention is, therefore, constituted by three stages (De Gangi & Porges, 1990): • • • the activation of attention: it is as a reflex of orientation or “initial alert orientation to a stimulus,” and an individual reaction to sensory stimulation. the maintenance of attention: it happens when activation is due to a stimulus that is new or complex for the subject so much so as to motivate the individual to elaborate it. the fall of attention: it represents the detachment of attention from a stimulus; it can occur due to physical and mental fatigue or because attention is paid to new or different stimuli. Plays a role in supporting attention: the arousal. Arousal, or generalized attention, “predisposes the human organism to receive inputs. It can be described as an orientation reaction that involves behavioral and physiological changes in response to new stimuli” (Fabio, 2009). Umiltà (2001) identifies arousal as a level of activation meaning it as “physiological alertness to respond to internal and external stimuli,” therefore, it can be understood as a degree of generalized activation proper to each individual and is connected to potential intensity of attention. Consequently, when activated, it generates a general alert reaction of both Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 11 receptors and effectors so that the body is prepared to concentrate on a set of information that comes from the environment and that are considered suitable. Once the information is acquired, what is considered relevant undergoes reinforcement, while the remaining information, even if it is considered irrelevant in that precise context, is either marginally accepted or completely rejected. Between arousal and attention there is a close relationship that finds explanation in the Law of Yerkes and Dodson, dating back to 1908, according to which performances, in a specific task, undergo an optimal course assuming the form of an inverted U function. Low levels of performance correspond to levels of low activation, since both the selectivity and the irrelevant indices are accepted in an elementary way, but then, while the activation gradually increases and both the selectivity and the capacity to process the stimuli increases, it is noted that the performance improves until, once the optimal growth ratio between arousal and performance has been reached, it stops at the highest point of the curve, from which the performance progressively deteriorates (Figure 2). Figure 2. Law of Yerkes and Dodson (1908). Complimentary Contributor Copy 12 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Furthermore, it should be emphasized that the level of organism activation and vigilance are independent, even if it is still true that, generally, a good level of activation is accompanied by good supervision and vice versa (Mazer, 2011; Parasuraman, 2000; Theeuwes, Kramer, & Irwin, 2011). The level of vigilance is sensitive to the level of arousal at the beginning of the task, while, supervisory performance is usually poor during the task to be performed if the level of arousal is low: physiological arousal tends to decrease in each monotonous environment or in conditions of prolonged performance, although this decrease is not always associated with a decline in performance. All this may depend on the ability to detect the signal or the response criterion of the observer. During the tests of detection on the degree of relevance, the experimental subject must be able to tell if a certain target is present or absent, and his answer will depend on both the perceptual factors and decision-making criteria he uses. In any case, the subject is able to modify the response criteria based on the goals, expectations or consequences that may be associated with correct and/or incorrect answers. Sustained attention influences the decrease in the supervisory status as both changes in signal sensitivity and in the response criterion are detected, i.e., during the test the subjects hypothesize the criterion of response based on the probability of the signal appearing, which will fade if the subject does not detect signals within a certain period of time. Therefore, the lowering of vigilance becomes a consequence of changes in the response parameter of the subject, that is, the longer the time between the appearance of the target-stimulus and the other increases, the more the subject will tend to adopt a more stringent and conservative criterion. Some research, conducted by Colquhoun and Baddeley, around 1967, focused on vigilance by modifying the sequence of stimuli and critical events that occurred, noting that the correctness of the response remains stable over time when the appearance of stimuli and critical events are high. On the contrary, the lowering of the task reaches its maximum when the rhythm of stimulus presentation is sustained and critical events are infrequent. These studies have shown that expectation factors and anticipatory factors play a first order role in establishing the order of Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 13 response, and that the decrease in supervision is certainly determined both by a decrease in sensitivity, strongly influenced by arousal, and from changes in the answer criterion. 1.1.3. Selective Attention and Focused Attention Attention literature focused, for a long time, on selective attention. This type of attention was considered as the most important function of attention. Selective attention can be defined as ‘The mental ability to select stimuli, responses, memory or thought that are behaviourally relevant among the many others that are behaviourally irrelevant’ (Corbetta, 1998). Selective attention concerns the use of one source of information rather than another. Therefore, it is linked to selection. Selection is the process by which some informational elements are given priority over others (Cohen, 2014). Selection is necessary because there are severe limits to our capacity to process visual information. These limits are likely imposed by the fixed amount of overall energy available to the brain and by the high energy cost of neuronal activity involved in cortical computation. Furthermore, our attention system, based on needs, is able to select information through different sensorial modalities, as it allows us to decide which visual stimulus is to be classified and ignored, for example, the auditory stimulus, or vice versa. The factors that determine good selective attention “depend on the effectiveness of the cognitive system and on the characteristics of external stimuli” (Marzocchi, Molin, & Poli, 2000), as efficient mnemonic mechanisms facilitate the selection of all the information from part of the attention. Moreover, a good surveillance system allows to filter the stimuli adequately and, at the same time, the inputs coming from the outside world are able to better access the attentive process if they are new and interesting. Baddeley argued that “the function of selective attention is to protect the human cognitive system, as a limited capacity system, from information overload” (Baddeley, 1992). However, more recent cognitive research has failed to empirically validate the existence of this limit, since it has shown that our cognitive system is able to preserve independent Complimentary Contributor Copy 14 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. representations of very complex and continuously evolving messages without causing any interference and, therefore, in light of this it is assumed that our capacity is not limited (Allport, 1989). In order to perform actions it is necessary, thus, that they are selected. Although our senses are able to record and process several inputs simultaneously coming from different directions, our ability to perform allows us to perform one task at a time, so it is important to trigger an action control mechanism that aims to select the most useful piece of information by separating it from the rest. When we enter a room, for example, the representation we make of it depends on the purpose we have in mind, that is, if we intend to modify the furnishings, the type of information we need will be based on the knowledge of the dimensions, of the materials used for the renovation or the type of furniture we will need, the element that we will ignore is certainly people who are present at that moment. The situation changes radically instead if the room is a classroom in which we have to follow a lesson, the relevant information will be: the number of people present, the available vacancies, completely ignoring the color of the walls or if the floor is covered in ceramic or parquet. These are banal examples that allow us to better explain how it is necessary, in order to carry out an action, for the information to undergo a selection. Another function of attention, closely related to selective attention, is “focused attention.” It is defined as “the set of information selected in a given situation taking into account the spatio-temporal limits of that moment” (Mazzocchi, Molin, & Poli, 2000). Since the two dimensions always act in a synergistic and coordinated way, it is inseparable from selective attention. Attention is focused when it is specifically directed to a single target or task, without being involved in the simultaneous processing of multiple sets of information. At the same time, it is important to take into account two assumptions, when we talk about this subcategory: 1) the selectivity of the attention induces a narrowing of the field of analysis to a few elements; Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 15 2) the degree of commitment exercised in the analysis of these stimuli is increased. When we had to solve a mathematical problem at school, our goal was to solve it, so we concentrated on a single solution, or when playing chess or checkers we are intent on both looking at the board and predicting the opponent’s moves: these are typical examples in order to better understand what is focused and what is needed. 1.1.4. Automatic and Controlled Processing In the last 25 years, a large body of experimental studies has investigated the control of attentive deployment, demonstrating that a subject’s attention for an input scene arises from both stimuli driven factors referred to as bottom-up attention, and task-driven factors referred to as top-down attention (Cheal & Lyon, 1991; Fabio, 2017; Fabio & Antonietti; 2012; Fabio & Caprì, 2015; 2017; Fabio, Castriciano, & Rondanini; 2015; Fabio & Urso, 2014; Fabio, Caprì, Mohammadhasani, Gangemi, Gagliano, & Martino, 2018; Hawkins, Hillyard, Luck, Mouloua, Downing, & Woodward, 1990; Hickey, McDonald, & Theeuwes, 2006; Hikosaka, Miyauch, & Shimojo, 1993; Martino, Caprì, Castriciano, & Fabio, 2017; Mohammadhasani, Fabio, Fardanesh, & Hatami, 2015; Mohammadhasani, Fardanesh, Hatami, Mozayani, & Fabio, 2018; Nakayama & Mackeben, 1989; Pestilli & Carrasco, 2005; Posner, 1980; Posner & Cohen,1984; Yantis, 1996; Yantis & Johnson, 1990; Yeshurun & Carrasco, 1998). Overall, bottom-up attention is based on salient features of the input image such as orientation, colour, intensity and motion. It is operated in the pre-attention stage and is the outcome of simple feature extraction. Topdown attention refers to the set of processes used to bias visual perception based on task or intention. It is driven by the mental state of the observer or cues they have received. From 1970, an extended effort to develop an empirical and theoretical understanding of automatic and controlled processing has been made. The first distinction between automatic and controlled processing was Complimentary Contributor Copy 16 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. introduced by Schneider and Shiffrin (1977). The authors proposed that there are two distinctive cognitive processes, namely, controlled processes and automatic processes. Automatic processing is fast, effortless, autonomous, stereotypic, unavailable to conscious awareness and fairly error-free. It can be accomplished simultaneously with other cognitive processes without interference, it is not limited by attentional capacity and it can be unconscious or involuntary (Moors & De Houwer, 2006; Fabio, 2009). Moreover, automatic processes are learned, sequential activation of nodes in which (1) the same sequence is always activated by a particular input and (2) the sequence is activated and run with little or no attention required. Once learned, an automatic sequence is difficult to suppress, or ignore. Automatic processes, because they make minimal demands on the attention of the short-term storage capacity, can run in parallel (Cohen, 2014). Controlled processing is effortful, slow, and prone to errors, but at the same time, flexible, useful to deal with novel situations and limited by the capacity of the short-term storage (Moors & De Houwer, 2006; Fabio, 2009). Because of this capacity limitation, controlled processes must be executed serially. Moreover, control processes (decisions, rehearsal, coding, searching of storage) manipulate the input and output of information from short-term storage (Cohen, 2014). Currently, two opposed theoretical frameworks predominate in the debate on the control of attentive deployment: stimulus-driven theories (Theeuwes, 2004, 2010; Yants, 2000;) and goal-driven theories (Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992). According to stimulus-driven theories, salient stimuli automatically capture visuospatial attention, regardless of a viewer’s goals (Gaspelin, Leonard, & Luck, 2017). Theeuwes and colleagues (1994, 2004, 2010; Hickey, MacDonald, & Theeuwes, 2006; Theeuwes, Kramer, & Kingstone, 2004; Theeuwes & Chen, 2005; Theeuwes & van der Burg, 2008) argued that attentional capture is basically bottom-up and not subject to top-down control. Processing in early vision is driven exclusively by bottom-up factors such as salience, Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 17 and only later do top-down factors play a role in processing. In addition, stimulus-driven theories explain cases where salient stimuli produce no observable attentional capture effects, throughout the rapid disengagement hypothesis (Theeuwes, De Vries, & Godiijn, 2003; Theeuwes, 2010). According to this interpretation, attention initially is oriented to the most salient stimulus in the display, but the salient stimulus can then be rapidly rejected so that the target is attended with little delay (Gaspelin, Leonard, & Luck, 2017). In contrast to stimulus-driven framework, goal-driven theories propose that goals and experiences of individuals determine whether the salient stimulus can capture the attention (Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992). In other words, visual attention is involuntarily guided to objects that have features matching what the observer is looking for (called an attentional set) (Gaspelin, Leonard, & Luck, 2017). Folk, Remington and Johnston (1992) also argued that attentional capture requires task relevancy, and therefore top-down attentional control tends to overwhelm bottom-up automatic attentional capture. According to their prospective, the so-called “contingent capture account,” attentional capture is completely subject to top-down control. Precisely, the ability of a stimulus to capture attention is contingent on whether an attentionalcapturing stimulus is consistent with the top-down settings, which are established “off-line” on the basis of current attentional goals. Only stimuli that match the top-down control settings will capture attention; stimuli that do not match the top-down settings will be ignored (Fabio, Piran, & Antonietti, 2005; Folk, Remington & Johnston, 1992). In summary, deployment of attention has been viewed as goal directed or stimulus driven (Corbetta, & Shulman, 2002). This distinction refers to top-down guidance of attention specific to cognitive goals of the agent, or bottom-up capture of attention by external stimuli. However, this dichotomous distinction of attentional control has been revised suggesting that top-down and bottom-up attentional capture operates simultaneously (Fabio, Caprì & Romano, in press). Complimentary Contributor Copy 18 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. 1.2. Historical Theories of Attention Figure 3. Historical theories of attention. The development of selective attention studies has had three phases: biological studies, computational models and then applications. Consequently, there are many theories of selective attention, and the most important are: the Attenuation Theory propounded by Anne Treisman, Deutsch and Deutsch Model, Donald Broadbent’s Filter Theory, Kahneman’s Capacity Model, Cherry’s Cocktail Party Theory, Multimode Theory proposed by Johnston and Heinz, and Posner’s paradigm. In this paragraph, we describe the first mentioned theory. 1.2.1. Anne Treisman’s Attenuation Theory Anne Treisman (1964) proposed the Attenuation Theory to explain how unattended stimuli sometimes came to be processed in a more rigorous manner than what Broadbent’s Filter model could account for. Treisman agreed with Broadbent that there was a bottleneck but disagreed on the location. Attenuation Theory added layers of sophistication to Broadbent’s original idea of how selective attention might operate; claiming that instead of a filter which barred unattended inputs from ever entering awareness, it was a process of attenuation (Nketesia, 2013). Treisman (1964) argued that secondary channels of information are not completely filtered but attenuated. Attenuated information would be passed to higher levels of analysis only if it passed a threshold test. This test would identify words that had learned importance (e.g., one’s name or a warning such as “Look out!”) or that were favoured by contextual probabilities or recent use (Cohen, 2014). Treisman, in her Attenuation Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 19 Theory, assumed that there was a decrease in the perceived loudness of an unattended message. This message will usually not be loud enough to reach its threshold unless it has a very low threshold to begin with (your name), or there is a general momentary decrease for all messages. Thus the attenuation of unattended stimulus would make it difficult, but not impossible, to exact meaningful context from irrelevant inputs, so long as stimuli possessed sufficient “strength” after attenuation to make it through a hierarchical analyzation process (Beneli, 1997). Treisman’s model overcomes some of the problems associated with Broadbent’s Filter model (1958), such as semantic analysis, and explains the “Cocktail party phenomenon.” However, this model does not explain the meaning of the “attenuation concept” and how semantic analysis works. In addition, the nature of the attenuation process has never been precisely specified. 1.2.2. Deutsch and Deutsch Model Deutsch and Deutsch (1963) proposed the “Late Selection Theory” and postulated that all information, both attended and unattended, are analysed for “importance.” All signals receive extensive analysis and the most important signal is selected for further processing at a later stage. This further processing is what enters awareness. Further processing of a given message may not occur at all, however, if a person’s level of arousal is low, particularly when the message is not of great importance (Cohen, 2014). In the Late Selection Theory, another factor that has a major effect on selecting the input is the relevance of the information during the time of processing. Deutsch and Deutsch (1963) claimed that all messages are routinely processed for at least some aspects of meaning – the selection of message for response happens later. At low level of alertness, only very important messages capture attention, whereas at higher level of alertness, less important messages can be processed. Moreover, the authors suggested that both channels of information are recognized but are quickly forgotten unless they hold personal pertinence to the individual. For example, in shadowing experiments, the participant is asked to repeat a Complimentary Contributor Copy 20 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. certain message, that would create the personal significance needed in attention. However, Deutsch and Deutsch’s model has some limits. Firstly, the system that assigns “importance” to some information is not well described. Secondly, the model is vague regarding the stages of allocation. It does not explain how the analysis is automatically allocated to all stimuli, where the late stage is located in the stream of information processing (Cohen, 2014). Comparing Treisman’s model and Deutsch and Deutsch Model, the Late Selection approach appears wasteful with its thorough processing of all information before selection of admittance into working memory. 1.2.3. Donald Broadbent’s Filter Theory The British psychologist Donald E. Broadbent (1958) created the Filter Theory, based on findings from the shadowing and dichotic listening tasks, introduced by Cherry (1953). In a dichotic listening experiment an individual wears a set of earphones through which two different messages are played simultaneously. The listener is usually asked to monitor one of the two messages, repeating it word for word as it is presented. This spoken repetition is called shadowing the message. If one message is presented to one ear, and a second message to the other ear, then it is easy for the listener to shadow the first message and ignore the other. If the delivery rate of the messages is doubled, it is still possible to shadow effectively. Broadbent started his research with air traffic controllers during the war. In this situation, a number of competing messages from departing and incoming aircrafts arrive continuously, all requiring attention. According to Broadbent (1958) information from the stimuli present at any time enters a sensory buffer. One of the inputs is then selected on the basis of its physical characteristics for further processing by being allowed to pass through a filter. Since we have only a limited capacity to process information, this filter is designed to prevent the information processing system from being overloaded. The inputs not initially selected by the filter Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 21 remain briefly in the sensory buffer, and if they are not processed they decay rapidly. Therefore, Broadbent’s theory assumed that “the stimuli come through the various channels to the sensory system where they are stored for a short time and analysed in parallel, based on their elemental physical characteristics, tone or intensity of voice (S). This stage is followed by a more advanced processing phase performed by the perceptive system (P), which operates serially and is characterized by limited capacity, which only has access to some of the stimuli. A filter between the S system and the P system selects the stimuli that have access to a more sophisticated processing level” (Figure 4). Figure 4. Broadbent’s Model. As shown in Figure 4, Broadbent described two processes: filtering and pigeonholing. Filtering is involved in the selection of a stimulus for attention or further processing because it possesses a particular feature, usually a simple physical characteristic. Pigeonholing is the process that sorts stimuli that differ by multiple sensory attributes into response categories. If no category is appropriate, a stimulus is usually ignored. Because pigeonholing requires the detailed analysis of a stimulus on a number of dimensions, it occurs later in time. Therefore, in this model, short-term storage precedes the filter system and allows the system to store Complimentary Contributor Copy 22 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. input from secondary channels for later analysis. Consequently, multichannel monitoring of complex information can be carried out. In summary, the key assumptions in the Filter Theory are as follows: • • • Two stimuli or messages presented at the same time gain access in parallel (at the same time) to a sensory buffer. This holds information for a short period before it is attended to or disappears from the processing system; One of the inputs is then allowed through a filter on the basis of its physical characteristics, with the other input only briefly in the buffer for later processing; This filter prevents overloading of the limited-capacity mechanism beyond the filter; this mechanism processes the input thoroughly. However, Broadbent’s theory was criticized because as in his experiments participants report after the entire message has been played it is possible that the unattended message is analysed thoroughly but participants forget. In light of these findings, Moray (1959) introduced the following concept “the intrusion of ignored stimuli.” He argued that the ignored information is implicitly elaborated, for example, we cancatch our name even if we are not careful in listening because we are able to discriminate and orient our attention to this information. 1.2.4. Kahneman’s Capacity Model Kahnemann (1973) first postulated the nature of attentional capacity and proposed the Capacity Model of attention. The author maintained that attentional capacity depends on a variety of factors that limit performance possible for particular behavioural and cognitive functions. As described by Kahnemann (1973), attentional capacity is constrained by both structural and energetic limitations. Precisely, attention capacity is limited by some characteristics inherent to the person and they are specific for each individual (energetic capacity limitations), i.e., arousal, effort generation, motivation and momentary disposition of the person. Structural Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 23 capacity limitations refer to inherent properties of the person’s nervous system and physiology, i.e., memory/encoding, cognitive resources and processing speed. However, extrinsic factors in the environment are also important capacity limitations of attention, e.g., whether or not adequate reward or stimulation is provided in a situation. Kahnemann’s Capacity Theory was based on the idea of mental effort. Kahneman argued that some activities are more demanding and therefore require more mental effort than others, the total amount of available processing capacity may be increased or decreased by other factors such as arousal, and that several activities can be carried out at the same time provided that their total mental effort does not exceed the available capacity, and finally that rules or strategies exist which determine allocation of resources to various activities and to various stages of processing. Attention capacity will therefore reflect the demands made at the perceptual level, the level at which the input is interpreted or committed to memory and the response selection stage. However, Kahneman’s Capacity Model of attention had two limits: 1. Psychological measures do not allow to distinguish between different levels of processing: there is failure to determine whether the effect being measured is, for example, the compilation of relevant or non-relevant information for the task if the task is it difficult or not. These distinctions must be initiated in the light of structures and processes; 2. The close interweaving between effort and activation makes it impossible to distinguish between the two concepts. Today, the relationship between attention and underlying capacity limitations is well established. This link is decidedly evident in natural settings and relatively easy to demonstrate experimentally by manipulating specific organismic and environmental factors during task performance. Complimentary Contributor Copy 24 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. 1.2.5. Cherry’s Cocktail Party Theory Colin Cherry (1953) was the first to study the “cocktail party effect” which is the human ability to follow a single conversation in the midst of other conversations and background noises. We can imagine a chaotic context, such as a party. People move on one side to the environment, groups talk about each other, they hear the voices of individuals. At the same time, during a party people hear a lot of background noise, such as taffy, clinking of glasses, doors or windows being slammed, music; therefore, they are in a situation full of stimuli. However, while each person has the perception of being a in chaotic environment, nevertheless the individual can perfectly understand the topic of a conversation, even if someone talks about a different topic with a higher voice. This human ability allows us to follow a single conversation while many other conversations are proceeding at the same time. Cherry (1953) carried out some studies to investigate the “Cocktail party phenomenon.” He employed a shadowing task, in which one auditory message had to be shadowed (repeated back out aloud) while a second auditory message was presented to the other ear. Little information seemed to be obtained from the second or non-attended message. Listeners rarely noticed even when that message was spoken in a foreign language or in reversed speech. In contrast, physical changes (e.g., the insertion of a pure tone) were usually detected, and listeners noticed the sex of the speaker and the intensity of the sound of unattended messages (Figure 5). Cherry’s experiments are quite important because it was determined that subjects are able to distinguish two messages that come from the same spatial source as long as they are different in meaning. Gutschalk, Micheyl and Oxenham (2008) noted that the cocktail party phenomenon originated in the secondary auditory cortex in the temporal lobe. He used magnetoencephalography and monitored the area that was activated when a subject identified a noise among many indistinct sounds. These findings confirmed the existence of the Cocktail Party Phenomenon. Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 25 Figure 5. Example of Dicotic listening: the actor repeats the sequence perfectly reported by the attentive ear, the other ear suffers the shadowing. 1.2.6. Multimode Theory Johnston and Heinz (1978) proposed a broader model in the form of the ‘Multimode Theory,’ which viewed attention as a flexible system that allows selection of a message over others at several different points. Later selection requires more processing, capacity, and effort. In their model, Johnston and Heinz (1978) described the selective attention processing in three stages; Stage 1 is the initial stage where sensory representations of stimuli are recognized which corresponds to Broadbent’s filter theory. Stage 2 is the stage where semantic representations (meanings) are constructed and this corresponds to the Deutsch and Deutsch model of attention. The final stage is the stage where both sensory and semantic representations enter consciousness. Therefore, the Multimode theory combines both physical and semantic inputs in one theory. As mentioned above, within this model, attention is assumed to be flexible following different depths of perceptual analysis. The feature that gathers awareness is dependent upon the person’s needs at the time. Switching from physical and semantic features as a basis for selection yields costs and benefits. Stimulus information will be attended to via an early selection through sensory analysis, then, as it increases in complexity, semantic analysis is involved, compensating for attention limited capacity. Complimentary Contributor Copy 26 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. 1.2.7. Posner’s Paradigm Posner (1980) first demonstrated that attention can be summoned to a location using a peripheral cue. Posner, with his studies, wanted to demonstrate in a certain way that attention could move in space. He argued that if “it is actually possible to move attention, then it must also be possible to measure the effects of this shift in the performance of the subject.” He started by observing the results obtained by Cherry, dichotic listening, and by Treisman, sensorial processing at a selective level, from which it emerged how attention gave greater importance to the selected information than that which was acquired by the “non-attentive” channel, in the sense that, if an experimental subject was asked to signal a stimulus, through for example a cursor, it was noticed that he was more reactive only if this stimulus was present in the range of action in which his attention is focused. Posner used a simple detection paradigm in his experimental studies. In this paradigm, a subject has to detect a target appearing in one of the two place holders that are equidistant from a central fixation cross (Figure 4). The subject, seated in front of a computer, will be told that his task will be to indicate in a short time in which part of the screen a target stimulus will appear; on the computer monitor the target will appear in two positions delimited by two squares aligned to the left and right of a central fixation point, on which the subject’s gaze must be kept fixed. These two quadrants represent the possible positions in which the target stimulus will appear, and as soon as it is displayed, the subject must report it by pressing a button as fast as possible, since the response speed is recorded from the computer. Before it appears on the screen, a signal stimulus called cue (or suggestion) will be presented, which will suggest in which part of the screen the target will appear to the subject, for example, the arrow pointing towards the left or right hemisphere, so it is as if the subject were encouraged to direct his attention to this position. Finally, there will be three possible experimental situations: the “valid tests” or “congruent,” in which the cue will be perfectly in the position in which the target will appear; the “invalid tests” or “incongruent,” in which the cue will be in a different position from the point in which the target will appear; the Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 27 “neutral tests,” in which there is no cue and the attention will be given to all the possible positions in which the target can appear on the screen. The Posner paradigm also provides two possible suggestions: the exogenous cue, which allows the attention to be directed automatically and independently of the will of the subject, and the endogenous cue, which merely provides a suggestion that the subject freely decides to follow or not. The Posner paradigm, therefore, which represents a compromise solution between the hypothesis of a unique attentional system or characterized by separate selection operations, has managed to discover, through targeted studies, how spatial attentive orientation works. Figure 6. Posner’s Paradigm. In the last century, therefore, attention played a privileged and exclusive role within the international scientific community. Until now, the investigation of the phenomenon of attention has highlighted, with countless studies based on the experimental tradition, observations based on the neural basis of the attentive processes, the attention deficit of neuropsychological patients and the development of attention from childhood to old age. Complimentary Contributor Copy 28 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Given the vivacity of this research in the future, surely the understanding of attention will grow thanks to the knowledge deriving from neuroanatomical studies and studies based on the simulations of attentive computer processes. 1.3. Current Theories of Attention Figure 7. Current theories of attention. The theory of attention continues to evolve. This evolution is due two methodological approaches: neuroimaging techniques for measuring brain structure and function, and computational neurosciences for studying the neural networks of attention. The main aim of cognitive neuroscience is getting further into the theoretical and biological nature of attention using simple stimuli (Matei, Ferrera, Riche & Taylor, 2016). From the late 1990s, the arrival of advanced tools such as functional imaging, EEG, MEG, PET, single-cell recordings in awake, transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation (TMS and tDCS) have allowed to study the relationship of neural activities with behavioural correlates of attention. A rich body of neuroimaging studies has now examined functional brain response associated with attention in both healthy subjects and patients (Fabio, Gangemi, Caprì, Budden, & Falzone, 2018; Fabio, Magaudda, Caprì, Towey, Martino, 2018; Fabio, Caprì, Campana, & Buzzai, 2018). Consequently, new theories of attention have been developed. Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 29 1.3.1. The Current Neuropsychological Framework of Attention The current neuropsychological framework of attention is based on specific assumptions about attentional processes. These principles are universally accepted and empirically demonstrated. It is universally accepted that people cannot simultaneously process and respond to an infinite quantity of information (Caprì, Gugliandolo, Iannizzotto, Nucita, & Fabio, 2019). This means that attention enables information reduction (Cohen, 2014). Moreover, attention selects the information. In other words, attending not only reduces the quantity of information but also establishes a priority of particular information for further processing (Cohen, 2014). It is widely accepted that attentional processes are connected with higherorder cognition, such as memory, language, and perception. Hence, attention is not a unitary process, it is determined by multiple factors and most likely by a network of neural systems. In addition, attention is temporally and spatially distributed. This principle claims that the spatial characteristics of the environment influence the attentional processes underlying the particular situation. Attention also produces a tendency toward a temporal continuity of response to a stimulus, but this continuity is subject to competing influences. Attention has a limited capacity, which is due to the energetic state of person, motivational factors, and natural differences across individuals. Although there is data to debate, current neuropsychological studies support the idea that attention is simultaneously governed in both bottom up and a top-down processes. When attention is oriented by environmental events, additional focusing often occurs subsequently. After an initial response to a stimulus, we may either tune our attention to a finer level of featural resolution or draw our attention to a less specific level of detail. To realize such attentional tuning, neural systems must exist that are capable of exerting supervisory control over other neural systems that are responsible for more basic sensory processes (Cohen, 2014). Lastly, attention is not localized at one brain site, it is governed by a network of neural systems. This principle is very important as it confirms Complimentary Contributor Copy 30 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. that attention is not a unitary process, both theoretically and empirically. The neuroanatomic system of attention will be described in the third chapter of this book. 1.3.2. The Computational Model of Attention The applications of visual attention models in computer vision emerged during the 1990s. Early applications were for object detection and recognition, first in natural images and then in complex satellite imagery (i.e., remote sensing and radar imagery). Recently, object detection methods have been combined with the visual attention concept, called salient object detection. Saliency can be defined by the combinations of values of image parameters such as intensity, colour, orientation, size and others. Particular local structures, such as edges, curvature, corners, shape and location are also considered relevant measures of saliency (Matei, Ferrera, Riche & Taylor, 2016). Gao and Vasconcelos (2009) have used the feature decomposition of Itti and Koch (2001). In this approach, saliency is determined by the discrimination obtained from the mutual information between centre and surround. Zhang et al. (2008) have created a set of Boolean feature maps using the Lab colour space and have gathered statistics from a set of natural scenes to train sets of features used to estimate saliency. In this method, saliency is indicated if features in a region are comparatively rare in the background.Valenti, Sebe, and Gevers (2009) have employed features based on the edges of colour regions and their curvature. Achanta, Hamami, Astrada, and Susstrunk (2009) has also used the Lab colour space but blurs the images with a Gaussian kernel and has used the difference with the original image to identify salient regions. Lastly, Cheng, Mitra, Huang, Torr, and Hu (2015) have used a distance metric in Lab colour space to measure contrast between regions and estimate saliency. The number of colours is minimised to reduce computation. This approach has been used to segment salient objects. It is difficult to gather all research in the field of computational attention as there is a large body of studies. We have cited the main works in which the feature-based methods have been used. This approach Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 31 incorporates some form of top-down input either through supervised learning or a targeted choice of features (Matei, Ferrera, Riche & Taylor, 2016). CONCLUSION The multidisciplinary nature of attention leads to many different definitions. Overall, attention can be defined as the allocation of cognitive resources to prioritize incoming information to bring it to a conscious state, update a scene model, update memory, and influence behaviour. In this chapter, we have presented the classical taxonomy of attention. We have also gathered the historical theories of attention. In the late 1950s, D. Broadbent proposed a “bottleneck” model in which he described the selective properties of attention. His idea was that attention acts like a filter (selector) of relevant information based on basic features, such as colour or orientation for images. However, Broadbent’s theory has been criticized. Deutsch and Deutsch (1964) introduced a “late selection” model, where attentional selection is basically a matter of memory processing and response selection. The idea is that all information is acquired, but only that which fits semantic or memory-related objects is selected to reach awareness. Treisman (1960) proposed the Attenuation Theory, where attention occurs in two distinct steps. First, a pre-attentive parallel effortless step analyses objects and extracts features from those objects. In a second step, those features are combined to obtain a hierarchy of focus attention which pushes information towards awareness. Posner (1956) supported a spatial selection approach and Kahneman introduced the theory of capacity supporting the idea of mental effort. In the last part of this chapter, we have introduced the current theories of attention. In particular, we have focused on neuropsychological studies and applications of visual attention models in computer sciences. Complimentary Contributor Copy 32 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. REFERENCES Achanta, R., Hamami, S., Astrada, F., & Susstrunk, S. (2009). Frequencytuned salient region detection. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, Miami Beach. Allport, A. (1989). Visual attention. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 631-682). Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press. Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359. Betts, J., McKay, J., Maruff, P., & Anderson, V. (2007). The Development of Sustained Attention in Children: The Effect of Age and Task Load. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3), 205-221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 09297040500488522. Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and Communication. London: Pergamon Press. Caprì, T., Gugliandolo, M. C., Iannizzotto, G., Nucita, A., & Fabio, R.A. (2019). The influence of media usage on family functioning. Current Psychology, 1-10. DOI: 10.1007/s12144-019-00204-1 Cheal, M. L., & Lyon, D. R. (1991). Central and peripheral precuing of forced-choice discrimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 43A, 859 – 880. Cheng, M. M., Mitra, N. J., Huang, X., Torr, P. H. S., & Hu, S.-M. (2015). Global contrast based salient region detection. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 37(3), 569–582. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some Experiments on the Recognition of Speech, with One and with Two Ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), 975. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. Cohen, R. A. (2014). The neuropsychology of attention (2nd ed.). New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/978-0-387-72639-7. Colquhoun, W. P., & Baddeley, A. D. (1967). Influence of signal probability during pretraining on vigilance decrement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(1), 153-155. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/h0024087. Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 33 Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 95, 831-838. Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215. Coull, J. T. (1998). Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging, and psychopharmacology. Progress in Neurobiology, 55, 343-361. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00011-2. De Brigard, F. & Prinz, J. (2010). Attention and consciousness. WIREs: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 51–59. De Gangi, G., & Porges, S. (1990). Neuroscience Foundation of Human Performance. Rockville, MD: American Occupational Therapy Association Inc. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222. Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some Theoretical Considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80–90. Fabio, R. A. (2009). Relationship between automatic and controlled processes of attention and leading to complex thinking. New York, Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Accession Number: WOS: 000273473000008. Fabio, R. A. (2017). The study of automatic and controlled processes in ADHD: a reread and a new proposal. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 5, 1-33. Fabio, R. A., & Antonietti, A. (2012). Effects of hypermedia instruction on declarative, conditional and procedural knowledge in ADHD students. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2028–2039. Fabio, R. A, & Caprì, T. (2015). Autobiographical memory in ADHD subtypes. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 40, 2636. DOI: 10.3109/13668250.2014.983057. Complimentary Contributor Copy 34 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Fabio, R. A., & Caprì, T. (2017). The executive functions in a sample of Italian adults with ADHD: attention, response inhibition and planning/organization. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 5 (3), 1-17. DOI: 10.6092/2282-1619/2017.5.1636. Fabio, R.A., Caprì, T., Campana, R., & Buzzai, C. (2018). The construction and validation of an Italian Dsyfunctional Belifies Questionnaire. Current Psychology, 1-11. DOI: 10.1007/s12144-0189958-8 Fabio, R. A, Caprì, T., Mohammadhasani, N., Gangemi, A., Gagliano, A., Martino, G. (2018). Frequency bands in seeing and remembering: comparing ADHD and typically developing children, Neuropsychological Trends, 24, 97-116. Fabio, R.A., Caprì, T., Lotan, M., Towey, G.E., & Martino, G. (2018). Motor abilities are related to the specific genotype in Rett Syndrome. In Kevin V. Urbano (Ed). Advances in Genetics Research. Volume 18. New York: Nova Science Publisher. ISBN: 978-153613265-6;978153613264-9 Fabio R. A., Caprì, T., & Romano, M. (in press). From controlled to automatic processes and back again the role of contextual features. Europe’s journal of Psychology. Fabio, R. A., Castriciano, C., & Rondanini, A. (2015). Auditory and visual stimuli in automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 771–778. Fabio, R.A., Martino, G., Caprì, T., Giacchero, R., Giannatiempo, S., La Briola, F., Banderali, G., Canevini M.P. & Vignoli, A. (2018). Long chain poly-unsaturated fatty acid supplementation in Rett Syndrome: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Asian Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 10, 37-46. DOI: 10.3923/ajcn.2018.37.46 Fabio, R.A., Magaudda, C., Caprì, T., Towey, G., Martino, G. (2018). Choice Behavior in Rett Syndrome, the consistency parameter. Life Span and Disability, XXXI, 1, 47-62. Fabio, R. A., Piran, S. G., & Antonietti, A. (2005). Auditory vigilance in typically developing subjects and subjects with attention deficit- Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 35 hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clinical and Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 151-157. Fabio, R. A., & Urso, M. F. (2014). The analysis of Attention Network in ADHD, attention problems and typically developing subjects. Life Span and Disability, 17 (2), 199-221. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030-1044. Gangemi, A., Caprì, T., Fabio, R.A., Puggioni, P., Falzone, A. M. & Martino, G. (2018). Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) and Cognitive Empowerment for the functional recovery of diseases with chronic impairment and genetic etiopathogenesis. In Kevin V. Urbano (Ed). Advances in Research. Volume 18. New York: Nova Science Publisher. ISBN: 978-153613265-6;978-153613264-9 Gao, D., Han, S., & Vasconcelos, N. (2009). Discriminant saliency, the detection of suspicious coincidences, and applications to visual recognition. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence. Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2017). Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79, 45-62. Gutschalk, A., Micheyl, C., Oxenham, A. J. (2008), Neural correlates of auditory perceptual awareness under informational masking. PLoS Biology, 6, e138. Hawkins, H. L., Hillyard, S. A., Luck, S. J., Mouloua, M., Downing, C. J., & Woodward, D. P. (1990). Visual attention modulates signal detectability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 802– 811. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior; a neuropsychological theory. Oxford, England: Wiley. Hickey, C., McDonald, J. J., & Theeuwes, J. (2006)., Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 604 – 613. Complimentary Contributor Copy 36 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., & Shimojo, S. (1993). Voluntary and stimulus induced attention detected as motion sensation. Perception, 22, 517– 526. Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2003a). Covert and overt voluntary attention: Linked or independent? Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 102– 105. Itti, L., & Koch, C., (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(3), 194 –203. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: H. Holt and Company. Johnston, W. A., & Heinz, S. P. (1978). Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107(4), 420-435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.107.4.420. Kahneman D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Klein, R. M. (1980). Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 259–276). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Klein, R. M., & Pontefract, A. (1994). Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? In C. Umilta` & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 333– 350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Martino, G., Caprì, T., Castriciano, C., & Fabio, R. A. (2017). Automatic deficits can lead executive deficits in ADHD. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 5 (3), 1-32. DOI: 10.6092/22821619/2017.5.1669. Martino, G., Catalano, A., Bellone, F., Sardella, A.,Lasco, C., Capri', T., Langher, V., Caputo, A., Fabio, R.A., & Morabito, N. (2018). Vitamin D status is associated with anxiety levels in postmenopausal women evaluated for osteoporosis. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 6, 1-16. DOI: 10.6092/2282-1619/2018.6.1740 Marzocchi, G. M., Molin, A., & Poli, S. (2000). Attenzione e metacognizione. Come migliorare la concentrazione della classe. Trento : Erickson. Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 37 Matei, M., Vincent, F., Nicolas, R., & Taylor, J. G. (2016). From Human Attention to Computational Attention - A Multidisciplinary Approach. Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems. Mazer, J. (2011). Spatial Attention, Feature-Based Attention, and Saccades: Three Sides of One Coin? Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), 1147–1152. Mohammadhasani, N., Fabio, R. A., Fardanesh, H., & Hatami, J. (2015). The link between visual attention and memory in ADHD and normally developing students: seeing is remembering? Reti, Saperi e LinguaggiItalian Journal of Cognitive Science, 1(2), 89-102. Mohammadhasani, N., Fardanesh, H., Hatami, J., Mozayani, N., & Fabio R.A. (2018). The pedagogical agent enhances mathematics learning in ADHD students. Education and Information Technologies, 23(6), 2299-2308. Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56–60. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289. Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326. Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vision Research, 29(11), 1631– 1647. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behaviour. In: R. J. Davidson., G. E. Schwartz, & D. E. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation (pp. 1-14). New York: Plenum Press. Noudoost, B., & Moore, T. (2011). A reliable microinjectrode system for use in behaving monkeys. Journal of Neurosciensces and Methods, 194, 218 223. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.10.009,pmid:20951736. Parasuraman, A. (2000). Technology Readiness Index (TRI) a MultipleItem Scale to Measure Readiness to Embrace New Technologies. Journal of Service Research, 2, 307-320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 109467050024001. Complimentary Contributor Copy 38 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Pestilli, F., & Carrasco, M. (2005). Attention enhances contrast sensitivity at cued and impairs it at uncued locations. Vision Research, 45, 1867– 1875. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2, 3–25. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbanm Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. P. (2001). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Sustained Attention: Where Top-Down Meets Bottom-Up. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01650173(01)00044-3. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66. Stevens, C., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1), S30-S48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.11.001. Stevens, C., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1), S30-S48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.11.001. Treisman, A. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3(6), 449–459. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(64)80015-3. Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 49, 83-90. Theeuwes, J. (1994). Bottom-up capture and attentional set: Selective search for colour and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 799-806. Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 65-70. Complimentary Contributor Copy Current Theory 39 Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99. Theeuwes, J., De Vries, G.-J., & Godijn, R. (2003). Attentional and oculomotor capture with static singletons. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(5), 735–746. Theeuwes, J., & Chen, C. Y. D. (2005). Attentional capture and inhibition (of return): The effect on perceptual sensitivity. Perception and Psychophysics, 67 (8), 1305-1312. Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2011). Attention on our mind: the role of spatial attention in visual working memory. Acta psychologica, 137 2, 248-5. Theeuwes, J., Van Der Burg, E., & Belopolsky, A. (2008). Detecting the presence of a singleton involves focal attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15 (3), 555-560. Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571-590. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97-136. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. Titchener, E. B. (1908). Lectures on the Elementary Psychology of Feeling and Attention. New York: Macmillan. doi:10.1037/10867-000. Umiltà, C. (2001). Mechanisms of attention. In B. Rapp (Ed.), Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology (pp. 135-158). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Valenti, R., Sebe, N., & Gevers, T. (2009). Isocentric color saliency in images. In Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Image Processing, Cairo. Zhang, L., Tong, M. H., Marks, T. K., Shan, H., & Cottrell, G. W. (2008). SUN: A Bayesian framework for saliency using natural statistics. Journal of Vision, 8(7), 32, 1–20. Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459-482. Complimentary Contributor Copy 40 Tindara Caprì, Rosa Angela Fabio, Giulia Emma Towey et al. Yantis, S. (1996). Attentional capture in vision. In A. Kramer, M. Coles, & G. Logan (Eds.), Converging operations in the study of selective visual attention (pp. 45–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Yantis, S. (2000). Goal directed and stimulus driven determinants of attentionalcontrol. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention & performance XVIII (pp. 73-104). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Yantis, S., & Johnson, D. N. (1990). Mechanisms of attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 812– 825. Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (1998). Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature, 396, 5, 72–75. Complimentary Contributor Copy View publication stats