Phonology The sound patterns of language What is Phonology? Peter what is Phonology? the study about how speech sounds form pattern is Phonology. the linguistic knowledge that speakers have about the sound patterns of their language. the description of that knowledge that linguists try to produce. Phonology tell us... sounds in our language v.s. foreign language. impossible combination of sounds *Mbick. certain phonetic features can identify a word. adjust our pronunciation and morphemes the pronunciation of morphemes how we pronounce words and parts of words we call morpheme. certain morphemes are pronounced differently depending on their contexts. for example... the pronunciation of Plurals cat/cats, dog/dogs, house/houses, ox/oxen what’s the difference of their plural forms pronunciation? How do we know how to pronounce these plural nouns ? The final sound of the plural nouns column A is a [z], a voiced alveolar fricative column B is a [s], a voiceless alveolar fricative column C is a [əz] column D are a hodge-podge of special cases. What is allomorph ? the more technical term for variant of a morpheme. phonological analysis phonological analysis there are some property of the environment associated with each group of allomorph. [kæb] or [lʌv] / [bʌs] or [bʌz] [z] or [əz] minimal pairs two words with different meanings that are identical except for one sound segment that occurs in the same place in each word. cat [kæt] and mat [mæt] , initial segment bus [bʌs] / buzz [bʌz] , final segment the distribution of plural allomorphs conditioned by the final segment of the singular form. inspection [əz] : after sibilants segment [s] : after voiceless nonsibilant segment [z] : after voiced nonsibilant segment Rules - phonetic forms of all plurals we assume that the basic or underlying form of the plural morpheme is /z/, with the meaning ‘plural.’ This is the “default” pronunciation. Rules - phonetic forms of all plurals if no rule applies, then the plural morpheme will be realized as [z] conclusion The particular phonological rules that determine the phonetic form of the plural morpheme and other morphemes of the language are morphophonemic rules. Such rules concern the pronunciation of specific morphemes. conclusion That the rules of phonology are based on properties of segments rather than on individual words is one of the factors . Once the rule is learned, thousands of word forms are automatically known. conclusion English is not the only language that has morphemes that are pronounced differently in different phonological environments. Many languages have morpheme variation that can be described by rules similar to the ones we have written for English. Phonemes and Allophone Cheng Yu (Feather) what is a Phoneme? The phonological unit of languge You use phoneme to build words Abstract basic form of a sound as sensed mentally rather than spoken or heard Manifested physically by one or more actual sounds, called “allophone” what are Allophones ? the perceptible sounds corresponding to the phoneme in various environment the sounds we can actually feel and sense physically, depending on the phoneme in different environment Example The phoneme [p] aspirated allophone [pʰ]: pit without aspirated allophone[p]: spit Example Nasality Vowels are nasalized before a nasal consonant within the same syllable how to find Phoneme Minimal pair substituting one sound for another in a word to see if it makes a difference how to find Phoneme [i] [ɪ] [e] [ɛ] [æ] [u] [ʊ] [ɑ] [o] [ɔ] [ɑ] [ʌ] [aɪ] [aʊ] [ɔɪ] each vowel phoneme have at least 2 allophones, which means they have two ways of being pronounced. ex. orally or nasally. the choice of the allophone is not random but rule-governed What is a Phone? a particular realization (pronuncation) of a phoneme the aggregate of phones that are the realization of the same phoneme are called the allophones of that phoneme phonemic transcription slashes // are for phonemes brackets [ ] are for allphones or phones Complementary Distribution Where A occur, B do not occur, vise versa. Ex. Where oral vowels occur, nasal vowels do not occur, vise versa. In this sense, the phones are said to complement each other——to be in complementary distribution. Complementary Distribution Contrastive: can be used to make different words ex. big/dig Non-contrastive: cannot be used to make different words ex. letter(tʰ/ɾ) Complementary Distribution Contrastive sounds constitute the phonemes of the language “the allophones of a phoneme”do not contrast with each other, and are in a complementary distribution---never occurring in identical environments the Need for Smilarity When sounds are in complementary distribution, the replacement of one sound for another will not change the meaning of the word →A phoneme can be viewed as a set of phonetically similar sounds that are in complementary distribution the choice of one or another allophone (pronunciation) is determined by its phonetic environment the Need for Smilarity Complementary distribution alone is not sufficient to determine the allophones of a phoneme, the phones must be phonetically similar Ex. velar nasal[ŋ] and glottal fricative [h] are in complementary distribution, but [ŋ] does not occur wordinitially and [h] does not occur word-finally. Other than that, they also share very few phonetic features: [ŋ]is a voiced velar nasal stop; [h] is a voiceless glottal fricative. Therefore, they are allophones of different phonemes. (sort of a)conclusion Speakers of a language generally percieve the different allophones of a single phoneme as the same sound or phone. For example, bead/bean, because mentally, speakers produce and hear phonemes, not phone. Distinctive features of Phonemes Sharon Distinctive Features of Phonemes Phonetics Phonology 1.provides the means to describe the phones (sounds) of language tells us how various sounds form patterns to create phonemes and their allophones 2.showing how they are produced and vary Distinctive Features of Phonemes [s] & [z] represent two contrasting phonemes in English seal [sil] & zeal[zil] allophone of one phoneme because one cannot replace the [s] with the [z] without changing the meaning of the word Distinctive Features of Phonemes seal [sil] & zeal[zil] > The fact that you can find a minimal pair also means they are not in complementary distribution as both occur word initially before the vowel [i] > They are therefore allophones of the two different phonemes /s/ and /z/ Distinctive Features of Phonemes seal [sil] & zeal[zil] voicing phonetic feature feel & veal: [f] / [v] [s]:voiceless / [z]:voiced cap & cab: [p] / [b] Distinctive Features of Phonemes when a feature distinguishes one phoneme from another, hence one word from another, it is a distinctive feature or a phonemic feature Feature Value Sharon Feature Values one can think of voicing and voicelessness as the presence or absence of a single feature, voiced. plus(+): presence , minus(-): absence ex: [b]:[+voiced], [p]:[-voiced] Feature Values The presence or absence of nasality can similarly be designated as [+nasal] or [-nasal] plus(+): presence , minus(-): absence ex: [m]:[+nasal][b]:[+nasal], [p]:[-nasal] Feature Values We consider the phonetic and phonemic symbols to be cover symbols for sets of distinctive features They are a shorthand method of specifying the phonetic properties of segments. Feature Values Phones and phonemes are not indissoluble units; They are composed of phonetic features molecules (phones & phonemes) atoms (phonetic features) Feature Values p b m Labial + + + Voiced – + + Nasal – – + Feature Values Aspiration listed as a phonemic feature in the specification of these units for English because [p] and [pʰ] do not represent different phonemes in English Feature Values phonetic feature is distinctive when + & – values of that feature in certain words with different meanings ex: [+voiced] on [z] in [zIp] [–voiced] on [s] in [sIp] Feature Values b m d n g ŋ voiced + + + + + + Labial + + – – – – Alveolar – – + + – – Velar – – – – + + Nasal – + – + – + each phoneme in this chart differs from all the other phonemes by at least one distinctive feature Feature Values Vowels have distinctive features too ex: [±back] [±tense] look[lʊk] ([+back]) lick[lɪk] ([–back]) beat[i] bit[ɪ] Nondistinctive Features Sharon Nondistinctive Features distinctive feature of English consonants Aspiration nondistinctive/redundant/predictable feature some features may be distinctive for one class of sounds but predictable for another (means predictable by rule) Nondistinctive Features ex: nasality is a distinctive feature of English consonants but not for English vowels predict when an /m/ or an /n/ will occur in an English word predict the nasality feature value of the vowels in bean, mean, comb, and sing (because the nasalized vowels occur before nasal consonants) the feature nasal is nondistinctive for vowels Nondistinctive Features Nasality on vowels is phonemic in Portuguese Nasalization is a distinctive feature for vowels in Akan [ka] “bite” [fi] “come from” [tu] “pull” [nsa] “hand” [tʃi] “hate” [pam]“sew” [kã] “speak” [f ĩ] “dirty” [tũ] “den” [nsã] “liquor” [tʃ ĩ] “squeeze” [pãm] “confederate” Nondistinctive Features Another nondistinctive feature in English is aspiration for voiceless stops. The voiceless aspirated stops [pʰ],[tʰ] and [kʰ] and the voiceless unaspirated stops [p], [t], and [k] are in complementary distribution. Phonemic Patterns May Vary across Languages Sharon Aspiration is a phonetic feature in Thai just as in English, but it functions differently in the two languages. Voiceless Unaspirated Voiceless Aspirated [paa] forest [tam] to pound [kat] to bite [pʰaa]to split [tʰam] to do [kʰat] to interrupt In English they are allophones of the phonemes /p/, /t/, and /k/; in Thai they represent the distinct phonemes /p/, /t/, /k/,/pʰ/, /tʰ/, and /kʰ/. aspiration is a distinctive feature in Thai; it is a nondistinctive redundant feature in English The phonetic facts alone do not reveal what is distinctive or phonemic: The phonetic representation of utterances shows what speakers know about the pronunciation of sounds. The phonemic representation of utterances shows what speakers know about the patterning of sounds. Korean il “day” seda“to count” kul “oyster” i:l “work” se:da“strong” ku:l “tunnel” Italian grandfather = nonno/non:o/ ninth = nono/nono/ consonant length is phonemic African language grow up = /kula/ treasure = /k:ula/ consonant length is phonemic and unpredictable In ASL phonology, signs can be decomposed into handshape, movement, and location. “candy”,”apple”,”jealous”: articulated at the same location on the face involve the same movement but contrast minimally in hand configuration “summer”,”ugly”,”dry”: a minimal set contrasting only in place of articulation “tape”,”chair”,”train”: contrast only in movement Like sounds, signs can be decomposed into smaller minimal units that contrast meaning. Some features are non-distinctive. Whether a sign is articulated on the right or left hand does not affect its meaning. Natural classes of speech sound Ryan Natural classes of speech sound Phonological rules determine vowels-- nasalized, voiceless stop aspirated. Apply to made- up words Sint, peeg, sparg phonemically /sɪnt/, /pig/, /sparg/ phonetically. [sĩnt], [phig], [sparg] Similar phonological rules involve the same classes of sounds such as nasal and voiceless stop. Nasalized vowel before nasal consonant. [ +nasal ] [ +voiced ], [ -voiced ] Ex. Aspirated sounds [ -voiced ] Voiceless Natural classes A group of sounds described by a small number of distinctive features. [–voiced], [–continuant] [–voiced], [–continuant] /p/, /t/, /k/, /ʧ/. However... /p/ [–voiced], [–continuant], [+labial]. Feature-Changing Rules Danny rules change features from one value to its opposite or even add features not present rule in English aspirates voiceless stops at the beginning of a syllable rule : aspirates voiceless stops at the beginning of a syllable /p/ /p/ pit , repeat inspect , compass aspirated [pʰ] unaspirated [p] We can now state the rule A voiceless noncontinuant has [+aspirated] added to its feature matrix at the beginning of a syllable when followed by a stressed vowel with an optional intervening consonant. Assimilation Rules The vowel nasalization rule is an assimilation rule that makes neighboring segments more similar by adding the feature [ +nasal] to the vowel. the vowel nasalization rule Vowels are nasalized before a nasal consonant within the same syllable. This rule specifies the class of sounds affected by the rule: Vowels It states what phonetic change will occur by applying the rule: Change phonemic oral vowels to phonetic nasal vowels. and it specifies the context or phonological environment. Before a nasal consonant within the same syllable short hand V → [+nasal] / _ [+nasal] $ nasal vowels nasalized before within a syllable segments became when den/dɛn/ [dɛn] deck /dɛk/ [dek] not affected!! “bob” phonemic representation Nasality: phonemic feature value /b a b/ /b u m/ - O* - - O* + - + + [b ~ u m] Apply nasal rule Nasality: phonetic feature value phonemic representation “boom” NA - [b - a - b] *The O means not present on the phonemic level. dissimilation Rules dissimilation similar sounds become less similar to be easy to say “the sixth sheik's sixth sheep is sick” harder than "the second sheik's tenth sheep is sick" fricative dissimilation rule applies to sequences to be a stop /fθ/ and /sθ/ fifth sixth into into [ft] and [st]. fift sikst -al ment-al annu-al spiritu-al ven-al -ar angul-ar annul-ar simil-ar vel-ar all of the -ar contain /l/ Segment Insertion and Deletion Rules Alesa Phonoligical rules may add or delete entire segment. Epenthesis The process of inserting a consonant or vowel. The rules for forming regular plurals, possessive forms, and third-person singular verb agreement in English all require an epenthesis rule. Example ●insert a /ə/ before morpheme /z/ ➝[əz] eg. kiss➝kisses ●add a [z] to squeeze to form its plural➝[skwizː] But it would be hard for English speakers to distinguish from [skwiz], because they do not contrast long and short consonants. Segment deletion One such rule occurs in casual or rapid speech. Example mystery➝mystry general➝genral memory➝memry funeral➝funral vigorous➝vigrous Barbara➝Barbra Segment deletion The silent g that torments spellers in such words. Example A B sign design paradigm [sãɪn] [dəzãɪn] [pʰærədãɪm] a phonetic [g] signature designation paradigmatic ↓ [sɪgnəʧər] [dεzɪgneʃ�n] [pʰærədɪgmæɾək] a [g] occurs a /g/ that will be deleted by the regular rule if a prefix or suffix is not added. From One to Many and from Many to One Phonemic to Phonetic representation FUNCTION EXAMPLE 1. Change feature values 1.Nasal consonant assimilation rules in Akan and English 2.Aspiration in English 3. g-deletion before nasals in English 4. Schwa insertion in English plural and past tense 2. Add new features 3. Delete segments 4. Add segments The same phone may be the realization of several different phonemes. ↓ All the boldfaced vowels are stressed vowels with a variety of vowel phones. ↓ The boldfaced vowels are without stress, or reduced, and are pronounced as schwa [ə]. The word meaning ‘bundle,’ Bund /bʊnd/; In German, ‘colorful,’bunt /bʊnt/ ↓ [+voiced] The phonemic representation of the final stop in Bund is /d/, specified as [+voiced]; it is changed by rule to [–voiced] to derive the phonetic [t] in word-final position. No simple relationship between phonemes and their allophones. The Function of Phonological Rules The function is to provide the phonetic information necessary for the pronunciation of utterances. Derivation Slips of the Tongue: Evidence for Phonological Rules Slips of the Tongue = speech errors Example Intended Utterance Actual Utterance 1. gone to seed [gãn tə sid] 2.stick in the mud [stɪk ɪ̃n ðə mʌd] 3.speech production [spiʧ phrədʌkʃə̃n] 1. god to seen [gad tə sĩn] 2.smuck in the tid [smʌk ɪ̃n ðə thɪd] 3.preach seduction [phriʧ sədʌkʃə̃n] → → → nasalization rule aspiration rule aspiration rule Prosodic Phonology Melody Syllable Structure one or more phonemes a phonological unit one or more syllables words Every syllable has a nucleus usually a vowel (but can be a syllabic liquid or nasal) may be preceded and/or followed by one or more phonemes called the syllable onset and coda. Syllable Structure Jack and Jill Went up the hill To fetch a pail of water. Jack fell down And broke his crown And Jill came tumbling after. In rhyming words, the nucleus and the coda of the final syllable of both words are identical. the nucleus + coda constitute the subsyllabic unit called a rime (note the spelling). Using the IPA symbol σ for the phonological syllable, the hierarchical structure of the monosyllabic word splints can be shown: Word Stress one or more of the syllables in every content word are stressed Be marked by an acute accent (') Example: pérvert (noun) as in "My neighbor is a pervert." pervért (verb) as in "Don't pervert the idea." súbject (noun) as in "Let's change the subject." subiéct (verb) as in "He'll subject us to criticism. adjective inválid (not valid) /the noun invalid (a sickly person). Word Stress The vowel that receives primary stress is marked by an acute accent (´). The other stressed vowels are indicated by grave accents (`) over the vowels Example: rèsignátion lìnguístics sỳstemátic fûndaméntal ìntrodúctory rèvolútion vowels in unstressed syllables are pronounced as schwa [ǝ]in English except at the ends of certain words such as confetti, laboratory, and motto. Word Stress láboratòry American English : British English : =>English vowels generally reduce to schwa or delete when they are not stressed British version the fourth vowel is deleted because it is not stressed. property of the syllable prosodic or suprasegmental feature. Sentence and Phrase Stress Is capable by varied vocal inflexions of expressing all states of mind. only one of the vowels in a phrase (or sentence) receives primary stress or accent. we place primary stress on the adjectival part of a compound noun (which may be written as one word, two words separated by a hyphen, or two separate words)=>chapter3 Sentence and Phrase Stress we place the stress on the noun when the words are a noun phrase consisting of an adjective followed by a noun. Example: Sentence and Phrase Stress 1. stress may be predictable from the morphology and syntax. 2. The stress differences between the noun and verb pairs are also predictable from the syntactic word category. Intonation Pitch still plays an important role, but only in the form of the pitch contour or intonation of the phrase or sentence. =>is also a phonemic feature in tone languages such as Chinese, Thai, and Akan. Intonation may reflect syntactic or semantic difference. Intonation Tristram wanted Isolde to follow him Tristram wanted Isolde to read and follow a set of directions, Sequential Constraints of Phonemes Patrick Phonotactic Constraints Limitations on sequences of phonemes that exist in all languages, albeit with different constraints. What makes “explicit” a possible English word but not “condstluct”? Explanation: Syllables are the basis of phonotactic constraints, so only clusters that can begin a syllable can begin a word and only a cluster that can end a syllable can end a word. The former does NOT violate any rules because of a clear syllable boundary between /k/ and /spl/ The latter does violate this rule because the second syllable starts with the impermissible onset of /stl/ or /tl/ Accidental gaps Creck, cruck Xanax, Kodak Why Do Phonological Rules Exist? page 258 “Many linguists believe that phonological rules exist to ensure that the surface or phonetic forms of words do not violate phonotactic constraints. If underlying forms remained unmodified, they would often violate the phonotactics of the language.” page 259 “Thus phonological rules exist because languages have general principles that constrain possible sequences of sounds. The rules specify minimal modifications of the underlying forms that bring them in line with the surface constraints. Therefore, we find different variants of a particular underlying form depending on the phonological context.” Constraints in different languages Twi, a language spoken in Ghana /pik/ is not a possible word in Twi but /mba/ is because words in Twi may end only in a vowel or nasal consonant (e.g. /mba/, /bam/, /ban/) Constraints in different languages Polish language /zl/ and /kt/ - which are impermissible in English - are permissible syllable-initial combinations in Polish Constraints in different languages Croatian language /ml/ in Mladen - which is impermissible in English - is a permissible sequence in Croatian Constraints in different languages Japanese language Severe constraints In Japanese on what may begin a syllable means very few combinations are permissible (usually a consonant and a vowel with /sh/ being one exception) English Obstruent sequences may not differ with respect to their voice feature at the end of a word (e.g. [kasb], [kabs] VS [kasp], [kɛbz]) Consonants cannot be followed by another stop consonant (e.g. *bkli) If a word begins with an /l/ or an /r/, the next segment MUST be a vowel (e.g. lamp, road) Sequences of obstruents that differ at most with respect to voicing are not permitted within English words (Constraint A) Optimality Theory This theory proposes that a universal set of phonological constraints exists. This set is ordered and some constraints are more highly ranked than others. The higher the constraint is ranked, the more influence it exerts on the language. As we’ve seen through the past-tense rule, English highly ranks constraint A In Modern Hebrew, constraint A is highly ranked, so suffixes that begin with /t/ are always separated from stems ending in /t/ or /d/ by inserting [e] as in /kiʃat + ti/ ([kiʃatetɪ]) In Berber, constraint A is NOT highly ranked, so /tt/, /dd/, and /ss/ consonants can surface Phonological Analysis Time :D